You are on page 1of 7

Developmental Psychology

© 2019 American Psychological Association 2019, Vol. 55, No. 8, 1768 –1774
0012-1649/19/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/dev0000744

BRIEF REPORT

Emotion Regulation and Relations to Well-Being Across the Lifespan


Kalee De France and Tom Hollenstein
Queen’s University

The specific strategies that individuals use to regulate their emotions have shown strong associations with
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

various indices of well-being. However, theoretical accounts suggest that strategy use, and the associ-
ations between strategy use and well-being, may change across the life span. Attempts have been made
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

to assess whether levels of strategy use, and the association between strategy use and well-being, change
across development; however, studies typically do not take into account potential differences in base rates
of item endorsement across the life span. Therefore, the current study had two objectives. First, we sought
to examine whether relative ER strategy reliance, or the proportional degree to which an individual relies
on various ER strategies, varied across three developmental periods: early adolescence, young adulthood,
and adulthood. Second, we sought to identify whether the associations between relative strategy use and
well-being differed significantly across these developmental periods. Results showed that relative
reliance on distraction, rumination, relaxation, and suppression differed significantly across age groups.
Moreover, results showed that the association between relative use of reappraisal and suppression
interacted with age group to predict relationship quality. Relative strategy use and age did not interact
significantly to predict anxiety and depressive symptoms. Theoretical and practical implications are
discussed.

Keywords: emotion regulation, ER strategies, life span development, relative ER strategy reliance

Emotion regulation (ER) involves the modification of processes The extent to which individuals implement various ER strate-
involved in the generation or manifestation of emotion (Campos, gies is consistently associated with well-being (Aldao, Gee, De
Frankel, & Camras, 2004). The specific strategies that individuals Los Reyes, & Seager, 2016). For example, high levels of suppres-
use to regulate their emotions have strong associations with key sion use are associated with high levels of depressive symptoms,
indices of well-being, such as interpersonal relationship quality, whereas high levels of reappraisal use are associated with low
anxiety, and depressive symptoms (e.g., Aldao, Nolen-Hoeksema, levels of depressive symptoms (Gross & John, 2003). Although the
& Schweizer, 2010). However, strategy use, and the associations associations between strategy use and well-being have been quite
between strategy use and well-being, may change across develop- well established (e.g., Aldao et al., 2010), scant attention has been
mental stage (e.g., Carstensen, 1995). Attempts have been made to paid to how these associations may manifest differentially at
assess strategy use across development, yet studies typically do not different stages of development (for exceptions see Zimmermann
take into account potential differences in base rates of item en- & Iwanski, 2014). This omission is problematic as evidence
dorsement across the life span. Therefore, it remains largely un- suggests considerable differences in cognitive ability, social
clear how proportional ER strategy use, and the associations be- and emotional goals, and physiological reactivity across the life
tween proportional ER strategy use and well-being, compare span (Scheibe & Carstensen, 2010), which likely influence how
across the life span. The current study aimed to address this gap in
strongly individuals rely on various ER strategies, as well as
the literature by assessing how individuals rely on various strate-
how strongly ER strategy use is associated with well-being
gies relative to how much they use all possible strategies to
(e.g., Carstensen, 1995).
identify how individual differences in strategy use, and the asso-
The moderating influence of age on the association between
ciations between strategy use and well-being, differ across the life
strategy use and well-being has been explored more thoroughly,
span.
however, in theoretical contexts. For example, the Socioemotional
Selectivity Theory (SST; Carstensen, 1995), the Selection, Opti-
mization, and Compensation with Emotion Regulation framework
This article was published Online First May 2, 2019. (SOC-ER; Urry & Gross, 2010) and the Strength and Vulnerability
Kalee De France and Tom Hollenstein, Department of Psychology,
Integration model (SAVI; Charles & Luong, 2013; Charles, 2010)
Queen’s University.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Kalee De each focus on how developmental changes in emotional experi-
France, Department of Psychology, Queen’s University, Humphrey Hall, ences, processing, and resources may influence which ER strate-
62 Arch street, Kingston, Ontario K7L 3N6, Canada. E-mail: kalee.defrance@ gies individuals rely on, and the propensity to benefit from utiliz-
queensu.ca ing these strategies. Whereas the SST suggests that a systematic

1768
ER AND WELL-BEING ACROSS THE LIFESPAN 1769

shift in goals across development is responsible for a shift in the ER strategy use across individuals in various developmental
relation between ER strategies and well-being (Carstensen & stages, and the associations between these reports and well-being
Mikels, 2005; Carstensen, Fung, & Charles, 2003; Löckenhoff & outcomes (e.g., John & Gross, 2004). However, utilizing raw
Carstensen, 2004), both the SOC-ER framework and the SAVI strategy scores does not take into account individual differences in
model posit that individuals benefit from ER strategies differen- overall strategy reporting style (Vitaliano, Maiuro, Russo, &
tially because of the internal and external resources available to Becker, 1987). Importantly, while individuals in various develop-
them (Charles, 2010; Charles & Luong, 2013; Urry & Gross, mental stages may differ in the extent to which they report imple-
2010). As individuals gain or lose regulatory resources in one menting a specific regulatory strategy, they likely also differ in
domain, such as cognitive control, physiological flexibility, or their overall style of ER reporting. Evidence suggests that levels of
social support, they may benefit more from ER strategies that emotional awareness, regulatory effort, and regulatory awareness
maximize their available resources. For example, Urry and Gross may differ across the life span (Scheibe & Carstensen, 2010),
(2010) suggest that increases in levels of cognitive control into indicating that comparing raw levels of self-report strategy use
adulthood may lead to increases in benefit gained from cognitively across cohorts may fail to account for differences in how strategy
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

taxing strategies such as reappraisal. In conclusion, theoretical reliance is reported across the life span.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

accounts suggest that as individuals move through various stages To better understand how ER strategy reliance differs across the
of development the benefit gained from ER strategies may vary as life span, and how strategy reliance is associated with well-being,
a result of changes in emotion-related goals and internal and we can instead assess relative strategy reliance: determining the
external resources. extent to which an individual relies on a given strategy in relation
Consistent with theoretical suppositions, nascent empirical ev- to how much they report implementing ER strategies (Folkman et
idence does suggest that strategy use, as well as the benefit gained al., 1987; Scheibe et al., 2015; Vitaliano et al., 1987). This relative
from habitual implementation of various strategies, may change score is generated by creating a proportional score for each indi-
throughout the life span. First, evidence suggests that strategy use vidual based only on their own responses, thereby avoiding
may differ across the life span; however, specific results have been sample-specific results. This approach allows for the identification
conflicting. For example, whereas several studies have found that of whether individuals rely differentially on various strategies,
heightened reappraisal use was associated with older age (Folk- while reducing the impact of extraneous variables that may inflate
man, Lazarus, Pimley, & Novacek, 1987; John & Gross, 2004; or reduce overall levels of ER strategy reporting. This approach is
Zimmermann & Iwanski, 2014), others found higher reappraisal in line with typical practices in the coping literature (e.g., Kim,
levels in younger participants (Blanchard-Fields, Jahnke, & Camp, Neuendorf, Bianco, & Evans, 2016), which examines the propor-
1995). Study results have also conflicted regarding differences in tion that individuals rely on various strategies, rather than their
suppression use across development, as some find that suppression raw, self-reported agreement with various strategy descriptions, in
use decreased with age (John & Gross, 2004), whereas others an attempt to control for differences in base rates of item endorse-
found no age differences in suppression use (Folkman et al., 1987). ment (Connor-Smith, Compas, Wadsworth, Thomsen, & Saltz-
Second, evidence also suggests that several changes in emo- man, 2000). Moreover, this method is also in line with recent
tional generation and processing across the life span may lead to approaches within the ER literature that attempt to place strategy
changes in the association between strategy use and well-being. In use within a larger context of overall regulation efforts. For ex-
line with theoretical accounts, evidence suggests that changes in ample, several recent studies have instead reported the proportion
emotion-related processes and resources may influence the asso- that individuals endorse each strategy to control for systematic
ciation between strategy use and well-being. For example, older patterns of responding related to the total number of items that are
adults seem to receive greater benefit from strategies that intervene endorsed, such as a tendency to overendorse or underreport items
early in the emotion generative process as physiological flexibility (Connor-Smith et al., 2000; De France & Hollenstein, 2017; Mur-
decreases with age and increases the difficulty of downregulating phy et al., 2017; Vitaliano et al., 1987). However, the authors are
arousal once it occurs (Charles & Carstensen, 2010). This has been unaware of any study examining ER strategy use across develop-
noted repeatedly in studies comparing distraction use and reap- mental stage that has taken relative strategy use into account.
praisal use. As distraction likely blocks emotional information The current study, therefore, sought to examine developmental
earlier in the emotion generative process than reappraisal (Shep- differences in ER strategy reliance, as well as the associations
pes, Brady, & Samson, 2014), a greater reliance on distraction may between ER strategy reliance and well-being, by answering the
assist in reducing the generation of physiological arousal in re- following research questions: (a) What is the relative reliance on
sponse to emotionaly evocative events (Scheibe, Sheppes, & various ER strategies across the life span? and (b) to what extent
Staudinger, 2015). Indeed, several studies have shown that dis- does the relation between strategy use and well-being differ across
traction use was more strongly associated with well-being among developmental stages? Using a cross-sectional design, the current
older participants (Opitz, Rauch, Terry, & Urry, 2012; Phillips, study examined six core ER strategies (De France & Hollenstein,
Henry, Hosie, & Milne, 2008; Scheibe et al., 2015). 2017) and well-being across three developmental stages: adoles-
cence, young adulthood, and middle adulthood. Well-being was
operationalized using three indices: depressive symptoms, anxiety
Reported Versus Relative Strategy Use
symptoms, and relationship quality. These three components of
Although empirical results show trends in line with tenets of the well-being were chosen as they have been studied extensively
developmental theories of ER strategy use and well-being, there is in the ER literature and tend to show consistent associations
a key limitation to most previous approaches: previous studies with ER strategy use (e.g., see Aldao et al., 2010 for a meta-
have relied heavily on comparing raw, self-reported levels of each analytic review).
1770 DE FRANCE AND HOLLENSTEIN

Method the mean across all items and demonstrated high internal consis-
tency across samples (Cronbach’s alpha ! .91 to .94).
Anxiety. The Beck Anxiety Inventory (BAI; Beck, Epstein,
Participants
Brown, & Steer, 1988) is a 21-item self-report inventory designed
Participants included 306 individuals from three distinct stages to measure the severity of anxiety symptoms. Respondents indi-
across the life span: adolescents (n ! 99, age range ! 12–15, cated on a 4-point scale (0 ! not at all; 3 ! severely) the severity
mean age ! 13.4 years, SD ! 1.05, 55% female), young adults of anxiety symptoms they have experienced recently (e.g., heart
(n ! 103, age range ! 20 –25; mean age ! 21.8 years, SD ! 1.5, pounding, unable to relax, nervousness). Anxiety was calculated as
85% female), and adults (n ! 104, age range 40 – 60, mean age ! the mean across all items and demonstrated strong internal reli-
48.1 years, SD ! 5.4, 68% female). Adolescent participants were ability across samples (Cronbach’s alpha ! .90 to .95).
recruited from a large database of families within the community Relationship quality. The Inventory of Parent and Peer At-
maintained by the developmental psychologists at the university. tachment (IPPA; Armsden & Greenberg, 1987) is a 53-item self-
Families have been added to the database through a wide range of report measure assessing specific elements of an individual’s re-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

methods, including birth announcements, advertisements, posters, lationships to index the strength of relationships with parents and
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

community events, and participation in previous studies. Young peers. Items are scored on a 5-point Likert Scale from one (almost
adults were recruited via a university studies FaceBook page, which never or never true) to five (almost always or always true). In the
advertises available research studies at the author’s University which current sample, only the Peer Relationship Quality was used,
are currently recruiting participants. The adult sample was recruited which was calculated as the mean across peer items, and demon-
using Prolific, an online research portal that allows researchers to post strated high internal consistency (Cronbach’s alpha ! .84 to .95).
links to current studies. All studies were approved by the Queen’s
University Internal Review Board under the project name Emotion Results
Regulation and Well Being (Study Number GPSYC-817–17).
All variables were assessed for missing values, normality of
distributions, and outliers. No variables presented any systematic
Procedure
missing data, all variables were within normal ranges of skewness
Participants were provided with a link to a secure online survey and kurtosis, and no individual scores were identified as outliers.
site, where they completed questionnaires which took approximately Table 1 shows the intercorrelations among all variables, their raw
25–30 min to complete. Surveys were presented in the same order to means with confidence intervals, SDs, and minimum and maxi-
each cohort. Seven individuals (three adolescents, four young adults) mum values.
were removed from the dataset because they completed less than 75%
of the questionnaires. Both the adolescents and young adults were Relative Strategy Reliance
compensated with $5 for their participation in the study. The adult
sample was compensated with $4.70. To answer our first research question, what are the differences
in relative reliance on various ER strategies across developmental
cohorts, a new variable was generated for each participant: average
Measures
strategy use. This variable was calculated as the mean of all RESS
Emotion regulation strategies. The Regulation of Emotion items and reflected the average amount that an individual reported
Systems survey (RESS; De France & Hollenstein, 2017) is a using ER strategies in general. An ANOVA was run comparing
38-item self-report measure assessing the extent to which an average strategy use scores across the three cohorts to assess
individual relies on six core emotion regulation strategies to down- whether the cohorts differed in overall strategy reporting. Results
regulate negative emotions in general (Distraction, Rumination, indicated that cohorts differed significantly on how much they
Reappraisal, Relaxation, Engagement, Suppression). Items are implemented ER strategies in general, F(2) ! 28.98, p " .001.
rated on a 5-point Likert scale from 1 (never) to 5 (always). Scores Strategy use seemed to increase across the life span, as the Adult
for each strategy are generated by taking the mean of items cohort (M ! 16.51) and the Young Adult cohort (M ! 15.63)
belonging to that subscale, with higher scores indicating higher reported significantly higher ER strategy use than the youngest
levels of use of that strategy to down-regulate negative emotions. cohort (M ! 13.30), and the Adult cohort reported significantly
Subscales showed high internal consistency (on all subscales and higher use than the Young Adults.
across cohorts, Cronbach’s alpha ! .83 to .95). To control for these significant differences in overall ER strat-
Depression. The Beck Depression Inventory, second edition egy reporting, we then created six new scores for each participant,
(BDI-II; Beck, Steer, & Brown, 1996) is a 21-item self-report one for each strategy, reflecting how much their reliance on each
questionnaire assessing the extent to which someone has experi- ER strategy deviated from their own personal mean reporting of all
enced depressive symptoms in the past two weeks. Participants are six ER strategies. These new scores were calculated by subtracting
asked to choose one of four statements that best reflect the extent each subscale score from the new average strategy use variable. As
to which they have experienced various symptoms of depression in a result, the proportional scores were calculated within individual,
the last two weeks (e.g., 0 ! I do not feel sad; 1 ! I feel sad much and were therefore not influenced by reporting trends in the cohort
of the time; 2 ! I am sad all of the time; and 3 ! I am so sad or or larger sample. Higher relative use scores reflected a higher
unhappy that I cannot stand it). One item regarding suicidal reliance, whereas lower scores reflected a lower reliance, on that
thoughts was excluded at the request of the institutional ethics strategy in comparison with the individual’s own strategy use
review board. In the current study, Depression was calculated as reporting. Relative use scores were checked for normality of
ER AND WELL-BEING ACROSS THE LIFESPAN 1771

Table 1
Correlations, Mean Values, Confidence Intervals, SDs, and Minimum and Maximum Values for All Study Variables

Variable 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9

1. Anxiety 1.00
2. Depression .53!! 1.00
3. Relationship quality .50!! .30!! 1.00
4. Distraction .15!! .08 .05 1.00
5. Reappraisal .13! #.04 .17!! .42!! 1.00
6. Rumination .45!! .38!! .27!! .11! .22!! 1.00
7. Relaxation .23!! .07 .00 .38!! .38!! .15!! 1.00
8. Suppression .16!! .25!! .17!! .39!! .21!! .31!! .23!! 1.00
9. Engagement .29!! .07 .16!! .07 .18!! .25!! .13! #.35 1.00
Mean .65 .59 2.53 2.65 2.30 3.12 2.07 2.71 2.38
Standard deviation .61 .52 .83 .96 .93 1.01 .96 .99 .85
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Minimum score .00 .00 1.12 1.00 1.00 1.00 1.00 1.00 1.00
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Maximum score 3.00 2.40 4.52 5.00 5.00 5.00 4.75 5.00 5.00
95% CI Upper .72 .65 2.62 2.76 2.41 3.24 2.19 2.81 2.47
Lower .59 .53 2.44 2.53 2.19 3.01 1.96 2.60 2.28
Note. CI ! confidence interval.
!
p " .05. !! p " .01.

distributions. Each score was normally distributed with acceptable stage, we ran a series of MANCOVAs. Each MANCOVA was run
levels of skewness and kurtosis. Figure 1 shows the average with two predictors: relative reliance on one ER strategy and cohort.
relative reliance of each ER strategy, broken down by cohort. Depression, Anxiety, and Relationship Quality were included as de-
Six univariate ANOVAs were run to compare the levels of pendent variables. This analytical approach allows for the testing of
relative strategy reliance across cohorts. Each ANOVA was fol- interactions between cohort and strategy use to predict indices of
lowed by a Simple contrast, comparing the youngest cohort to the well-being. As we were not interested in examining the linear com-
middle and oldest cohorts separately. Significant differences were bination of these well-being indices, only the univariate effects were
found between cohorts for relative reliance on Distraction, Rumi- interpreted. To control for family wise error levels, only findings with
nation, Relaxation, and Suppression (see Table 2). Contrast results p values smaller than .01 were interpreted. Significant interactions
showed that Adolescents reported significantly higher relative between cohort and ER strategy were followed by a Pearson’s cor-
reliance on Distraction and Relaxation, and significantly lower relation between the well-being measure and relative ER strategy use
relative reliance on Rumination, compared with Young Adults.
separately for each cohort, to illustrate how the association between
Compared with Adults, Adolescents reported significantly higher
well-being and strategy use differed across cohort. See Table 3 and
relative reliance on Relaxation and Suppression, and significantly
Figure 2 for a summary of results.
less relative reliance on Rumination.
Significant interactions between cohort and strategy use were
found only for Reappraisal and Suppression, and only in the
Associations With Well-Being
prediction of Relationship Quality. Although relative Reappraisal
To test our second research question, whether the association use was positively associated with Relationship Quality for adults,
between strategy use and well-being varied across developmental the association was nonsignificant for early adolescents and young

0.25

0.2
Porportional Reliance

0.15

0.1

0.05

0
Early Adolescent Young Adult Adult
Distraction Reappraisal Rumination Relaxation Suppression Engagement

Figure 1. Relative strategy reliance scores for each strategy across three age groups. Relative strategy reliance
was calculated as the average strategy score divided by the sum of all strategy scores.
1772 DE FRANCE AND HOLLENSTEIN

Table 2
Differences in Relative Reliance on ER Strategies Across Cohort

Contrast mean differences


Strategy 1. Adolescence 2. Young adult 3. Adult F 1 versus 2 1 versus 3

Distraction .18 .17 .17 2.93! .02! .01


Reappraisal .14 .15 .15 2.03 #.01 #.01
Rumination .19 .22 .21 7.95!!! #.03!! #.03!!
Relaxation .15 .13 .13 5.78!! .02!! .02!!
Suppression .19 .18 .17 4.12! .01 .02!!
Engagement .16 .15 .17 1.84 .01 #.01
Note. 1 ! adolescence, 2 ! young adult, 3 ! adult.
!
p " .05. !! p " .01. !!! p " .001.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

adults. Moreover, relative Suppression use was positively associ- found no significant differences in relative reappraisal use across
ated with Relationship Quality for the early adolescent and young developmental stage. These discrepancies could stem from several
adult groups, but negatively associated for the adult group. study design differences, such as the assessment of relative reli-
ance on ER strategies rather than raw ER strategy scores, the use
Discussion of differing ER strategy conceptualizations and measurement
tools, and the sampling of different age ranges.
The current study had two main objectives. First, we aimed to
The second aim of the current study was to identify whether the
assess differences across developmental stage in ER strategy reli-
association between ER strategy use and well-being was moder-
ance. Results indicated that, compared with young adults, adoles-
ated by developmental stage. Results showed that developmental
cents relied less heavily on rumination, and more heavily on
distraction and relaxation, to regulate their emotions. Compared stage and strategy use interacted to predict well-being, however,
with adults, adolescents relied less heavily on rumination, and only in the case of relative reappraisal and suppression use pre-
more heavily on relaxation and suppression. The results of the dicting relationship quality. Relative reappraisal use was positively
current study are consistent with previous studies that found that associated with relationship quality, but only in the oldest cohort.
suppression use decreased across the life span (John & Gross, There are two possible explanations for this finding. First, engag-
2004). However, the current results also present some conflicting ing in reappraisal to regulate emotions is a cognitively taxing
results. For example, Sütterlin, Paap, Babic, Kübler, and Vögele process, and individuals with higher levels of executive function-
(2012) found no significant differences in rumination use across ing tend to show higher rates of reappraisal use (Lantrip, Isquith,
individuals who were between 15 and 60 years old. Moreover, Koven, Welsh, & Roth, 2016). Because executive functioning
John and Gross (2004) and Folkman et al. (1987) found that tends to improve into adulthood (Zelazo, Craik, & Booth, 2004), it
reappraisal increased across the life span, while the current study is possible that the adults in our study were able to implement

Table 3
Univariate Results of Relative ER Strategy Use, Cohort, and Their Interaction to Predict Well-Being

Anxiety Depression Relationship quality


2 2
Strategy Predictors F $ F $ F $2

Distraction Distraction 4.95 .02 .62 ".01 6.38!! .02


Cohort 17.13!!! .10 1.57 .01 10.73!!! .07
Distraction % Cohort 3.46 .02 1.75 .01 1.21 ".01
Reappraisal Reappraisal 12.96!!! .04 19.26!!! .06 .02 ".01
Cohort 13.04!!! .08 1.81 .01 1.13 ".01
Reappraisal % Cohort 1.14 .01 .35 ".01 5.01!! .03
Rumination Rumination 12.64!!! .04 18.20!!! .06 9.56!! .03
Cohort 2.56 .02 .41 ".01 19.99!!! .12
Rumination % Cohort .81 ".01 .01 ".01 3.44 .02
Relaxation Relaxation 7.31!! .02 .01 ".01 3.33 .03
Cohort 5.00!! .03 1.77 .01 7.79!!! .05
Relaxation % Cohort 3.47 .02 .65 ".01 .70 .01
Suppression Suppression .10 ".01 6.61!! .02 6.80!! .02
Cohort 16.21!!! .10 2.59 .02 34.61!!! .19
Suppression % Cohort 2.21 .02 .62 ".01 8.04!!! .05
Engagement Engagement .78 ".01 4.24 .01 .44 ".01
Cohort 4.64!! .03 1.24 .01 2.36 .02
Engagement % Cohort 1.89 .01 .05 ".01 3.75 .03
!!
p " .01. !!!
p " .001.
ER AND WELL-BEING ACROSS THE LIFESPAN 1773

0.4 Limitations and Future Directions


*
* * The current study was not without limitations. First, for the
0.3
middle and oldest cohorts, recruitment and data collection took
0.2 place purely online, which limited the type of sample that could
access the study. Second, common method bias is a concern as the
Correlation Value

0.1 study relied solely on self-report measures. Future studies would


benefit from utilizing alternative assessment techniques, such as
0
semistructured interviews or multiinformant methods, to assess
-0.1
ER, depression, anxiety, and relationship quality. Third, the cur-
rent study relied solely on a community sample, and therefore did
-0.2 not assess associations between ER and clinical levels of depres-
sion, anxiety, or relationship problems. Fourth, the current study
-0.3
*
used a cross-sectional design, selecting representative cohorts
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Reappraisal & Relationship Quality Suppression & Relationship Quality


across the life span. Studies implementing a more continuous
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Early Adolescent Young Adult Adult


sampling across the life span, or ideally a longitudinal methodol-
ogy, will be better able to answer the questions posed by the
Figure 2. Zero-order correlation values assessing the relation between
relative strategy use and well-being indices. ! Significant correlation, p " current study in a more detailed manner. Finally, the current study
.05. did not assess the impacts of strategy reliance in relation to the
specific emotion being regulated. Because the association between
strategy use and well-being may depend on the specific emotion
reappraisal with greater effectiveness when managing interper- being regulated (Zimmermann & Iwanski, 2014), future studies
sonal conflict than the younger cohorts. Second, reappraisal is less should focus on how the results of the current study extend when
effective and more difficult to implement when used on high examining emotion-specific regulation.
intensity emotions (Sheppes, Catran, & Meiran, 2009). Older
Conclusion
individuals report experiencing lower levels of intense emotions,
particularly in interpersonal conflict (Birditt, Fingerman, & Almeida, Overall, the current study showed that levels of reliance on
2005). Therefore, it is possible that older individuals are able to various ER strategies do tend to differ across the life span, and that
implement reappraisal with more ease when managing interpersonal specific strategies, such as reappraisal and suppression, may have
problems, and thereby can more easily utilize practices indicative of differential effects on well-being depending on the developmental
reappraisal, such as seeing other perspectives, when experiencing stage of the individual employing them. These findings are impor-
interpersonal conflict. tant for researchers and clinicians alike, as they provide evidence
Suppression use, alternatively, was positively associated with against the uniform efficacy of ER strategy use and should inform
relationship quality in the adolescent and young adult cohorts, and our assumptions regarding the perceived benefits or harm of var-
was negatively associated in the adult cohort. The differential ious ER strategies. These results also contribute to mounting
association may be a result of the types of relationships that typify evidence suggesting that strategy use should be placed in a larger
these developmental cohorts. Older adults tend to report more context of individual strategy reporting by assessing relative,
consistent relationships and a more discerning relationship style, rather than raw, strategy reliance. Moreover, these results empha-
whereas social network size and social contact both shrink with size the considerable benefits that could be gained by shifting
age (Berscheid & Regan, 2016; Wright & Patterson, 2006). There- attention toward individual factors that may account for variations
fore, the style and nature of friendships across early adolescents to in the associations between ER strategy use and well-being.
adults may be distinct. Moreover, although suppression use is
typically associated with disrupting communication and reducing References
interpretations of authenticity (Butler et al., 2003), many of the
Aldao, A., Gee, D. G., De Los Reyes, A., & Seager, I. (2016). Emotion
studies asserting that suppression use has negative implications on regulation as a transdiagnostic factor in the development of internalizing
relationship quality have used unacquainted pairs of individuals and externalizing psychopathology: Current and future directions. De-
(e.g., Butler et al., 2003; Gross & John, 2003). When studies ask velopment and Psychopathology, 28, 927–946. http://dx.doi.org/10
individuals to report on their relationship quality with preexisting .1017/S0954579416000638
peers, findings are more in line with the current study. For exam- Aldao, A., Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Schweizer, S. (2010). Emotion-
ple, De France and Hollenstein (2017) found that late adolescents regulation strategies across psychopathology: A meta-analytic review.
and young adults with high reliance on suppression showed height- Clinical Psychology Review, 30, 217–237. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j
ened levels of self-reported relationship quality when compared .cpr.2009.11.004
with individuals with average levels of ER strategy use. While Armsden, G. C., & Greenberg, M. T. (1987). The inventory of parent and
peer attachment: Individual differences and their relationship to psycho-
suppression use may make it difficult to develop rapport and form
logical well-being in adolescence. Journal of Youth and Adolescence,
new relationships (Butler et al., 2003), suppressing negative emo- 16, 427– 454. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF02202939
tions among preexisting peers may function as an adaptive way to Beck, A. T., Epstein, N., Brown, G., & Steer, R. A. (1988). An inventory
prioritize goals of affiliation, as individuals choose to not express for measuring clinical anxiety: Psychometric properties. Journal of
their negative emotions to maintain positive relationship quality Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 56, 893– 897. http://dx.doi.org/10
(Butler & Gross, 2004). .1037/0022-006X.56.6.893
1774 DE FRANCE AND HOLLENSTEIN

Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., & Brown, G. K. (1996). Manual for the Beck role of coping strategies. Stress and Health, 32, 494 –502. http://dx.doi
Depression Inventory–II. San Antonio, TX: Pearson. .org/10.1002/smi.2646
Berscheid, E., & Regan, P. (2016). The psychology of interpersonal rela- Lantrip, C., Isquith, P. K., Koven, N. S., Welsh, K., & Roth, R. M. (2016).
tionships. New York, NY: Routledge. http://dx.doi.org/10.4324/97813 Executive function and emotion regulation strategy use in adolescents.
15663074 Applied Neuropsychology: Child, 5, 50 –55. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/
Birditt, K. S., Fingerman, K. L., & Almeida, D. M. (2005). Age differences 21622965.2014.960567
in exposure and reactions to interpersonal tensions: A daily diary study. Löckenhoff, C. E., & Carstensen, L. L. (2004). Socioemotional selectivity
Psychology and Aging, 20, 330 –340. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0882- theory, aging, and health: The increasingly delicate balance between
7974.20.2.330 regulating emotions and making tough choices. Journal of Personality,
Blanchard-Fields, F., Jahnke, H. C., & Camp, C. (1995). Age differences 72, 1395–1424. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.2004.00301.x
in problem-solving style: The role of emotional salience. Psychology Murphy, L. K., Bettis, A. H., Gruhn, M. A., Gerhardt, C. A., Vannatta, K.,
and Aging, 10, 173–180. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.10.2.173 & Compas, B. E. (2017). Resilience in adolescents with cancer: Asso-
Butler, E. A., Egloff, B., Wlhelm, F. H., Smith, N. C., Erickson, E. A., & ciation of coping with positive and negative affect. Journal of Develop-
Gross, J. J. (2003). The social consequences of expressive suppression. mental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 38, 646 – 653. http://dx.doi.org/10
Emotion, 3, 48 – 67. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.3.1.48 .1097/DBP.0000000000000484
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Butler, E. A., & Gross, J. J. (2004). Hiding feelings in social contexts: Out Opitz, P. C., Rauch, L. C., Terry, D. P., & Urry, H. L. (2012). Prefrontal
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

of sight is not out of mind. In P. Phillipot & R. S. Feldman (Eds.) The mediation of age differences in cognitive reappraisal. Neurobiology of
regulation of emotion (pp. 101–126). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Aging, 33, 645– 655. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2010.06
Campos, J. J., Frankel, C. B., & Camras, L. (2004). On the nature of .004
emotion regulation. Child Development, 75, 377–394. http://dx.doi.org/ Phillips, L. H., Henry, J. D., Hosie, J. A., & Milne, A. B. (2008). Effective
10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00681.x regulation of the experience and expression of negative affect in old age.
Carstensen, L. L. (1995). Evidence for a life-span theory of socioemotional The Journals of Gerontology Series B, Psychological Sciences and Social
selectivity. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 4, 151–156. Sciences, 63, 138 –145. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/geronb/63.3.P138
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-8721.ep11512261 Scheibe, S., & Carstensen, L. L. (2010). Emotional aging: Recent findings
Carstensen, L., Fung, H., & Charles, S. (2003). Socioemotional selectivity and future trends. The Journals of Gerontology: Series B, 65B, 135–144.
theory and the regulation of emotion in the second half of life. Motiva- http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gbp132
tion and Emotion, 27, 103–123. http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/A:1024569 Scheibe, S., Sheppes, G., & Staudinger, U. M. (2015). Distract or reap-
803230 praise? Age-related differences in emotion-regulation choice. Emotion,
Carstensen, L. L., & Mikels, J. A. (2005). At the intersection of emotion and 15, 677– 681. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0039246
cognition: Aging and the positivity effect. Current Directions in Psycho- Sheppes, G., Brady, W. J., & Samson, A. C. (2014). In (visual) search for
logical Science, 14, 117–121. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.0963-7214.2005 a new distraction: The efficiency of a novel attentional deployment
.00348.x versus semantic meaning regulation strategies. Frontiers in Psychology,
Charles, S. T. (2010). Strength and vulnerability integration: A model of 5, 346. http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00346
emotional well-being across adulthood. Psychological Bulletin, 136, Sheppes, G., Catran, E., & Meiran, N. (2009). Reappraisal (but not dis-
1068 –1091. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0021232 traction) is going to make you sweat: Physiological evidence for self-
Charles, S. T., & Carstensen, L. L. (2010). Social and emotional aging. control effort. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 71, 91–96.
Annual Review of Psychology, 61, 383– 409. http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/ http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2008.06.006
annurev.psych.093008.100448 Sütterlin, S., Paap, M. C. S., Babic, S., Kübler, A., & Vögele, C. (2012).
Charles, S. T., & Luong, G. (2013). Emotional experience across adult- Rumination and age: Some things get better. Journal of Aging Research,
hood: The theoretical model of strength and vulnerability integration. 2012, 267327. http://dx.doi.org/10.1155/2012/267327
Current Directions in Psychological Science, 22, 443– 448. http://dx.doi Urry, H. L., & Gross, J. J. (2010). Emotion regulation in older age. Current
.org/10.1177/0963721413497013 Directions in Psychological Science, 19, 352–357. http://dx.doi.org/10
Connor-Smith, J. K., Compas, B. E., Wadsworth, M. E., Thomsen, A. H., .1177/0963721410388395
& Saltzman, H. (2000). Responses to stress in adolescence: Measure- Vitaliano, P. P., Maiuro, R. D., Russo, J., & Becker, J. (1987). Raw versus
ment of coping and involuntary stress responses. Journal of Consulting relative scores in the assessment of coping strategies. Journal of Behav-
and Clinical Psychology, 68, 976 –992. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022- ioral Medicine, 10, 1–18. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF00845124
006X.68.6.976 Wright, K. B., & Patterson, B. R. (2006). Socioemotional selectivity theory
De France, K., & Hollenstein, T. (2017). Assessing emotion regulation and the macrodynamics of friendship: The role of friendship style and
repertoires: The Regulation of Emotion Systems Survey. Personality communication in friendship across the lifespan. Communication Re-
and Individual Differences, 119, 204 –215. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j search Reports, 23, 163–170. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/088240906
.paid.2017.07.018 00796377
Folkman, S., Lazarus, R. S., Pimley, S., & Novacek, J. (1987). Age Zelazo, P. D., Craik, F. I., & Booth, L. (2004). Executive function across the
differences in stress and coping processes. Psychology and Aging, 2, life span. Acta Psychologica, 115, 167–183. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j
171–184. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.2.2.171 .actpsy.2003.12.005
Gross, J. J., & John, O. P. (2003). Individual differences in two emotion Zimmermann, P., & Iwanski, A. (2014). Emotion regulation from early
regulation processes: Implications for affect, relationships, and well- adolescence to emerging adulthood and middle adulthood: Age differ-
being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 348 –362. ences, gender differences, and emotion-specific developmental varia-
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.85.2.348 tions. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 38, 182–194.
John, O. P., & Gross, J. J. (2004). Healthy and unhealthy emotion regu- http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0165025413515405
lation: Personality processes, individual differences, and life span de-
velopment. Journal of Personality, 72, 1301–1334. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1111/j.1467-6494.2004.00298.x Received November 2, 2018
Kim, P., Neuendorf, C., Bianco, H., & Evans, G. W. (2016). Exposure to Revision received February 16, 2019
childhood poverty and mental health symptomatology in adolescence: A Accepted March 20, 2019 !

You might also like