You are on page 1of 13

Journal of Child and Family Studies (2022) 31:2631–2643

https://doi.org/10.1007/s10826-022-02352-8

ORIGINAL PAPER

Perceived Strengths and Difficulties in Emotional Awareness and


Accessing Emotion Regulation Strategies in Early Adolescents
1
Caroline Cummings ●
Amy Hughes Lansing2 Christopher D. Houck3

Accepted: 9 June 2022 / Published online: 2 July 2022


© The Author(s), under exclusive licence to Springer Science+Business Media, LLC, part of Springer Nature 2022

Abstract
Emotional awareness (EA) and access to emotion regulation strategies (ER) are often conceptualized as being on a single
continuum, yet discrepancies in these associations are found across samples. We conducted a person-centered analysis to identify
distinct profiles of adolescents’ perceived EA and ER. Secondary analyses were conducted to compare demographic variables,
distress tolerance, and parent- and teacher-report emotional functioning across profiles. Participants (N = 414) completed two
subscales of the Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale and a behavioral measure of distress tolerance. A parent and teacher
completed the Emotion Regulation Checklist. Adolescent mental health symptoms were measured, with responses dichotomized
1234567890();,:
1234567890();,:

based on adolescent- and/or parent-report of clinically significant symptoms of any disorder. Exploratory two-step cluster
analysis identified three clusters: Varied EA/Low ER, Low EA/High ER, and High EA/High ER. Gender differences emerged,
with a greater proportion of girls in the Varied EA/Low ER cluster and boys in the Low EA/High ER cluster. The Varied EA/
Low ER and Low EA/High ER clusters demonstrated greater mental health symptoms, and greater parent- and teacher-reported
negativity/lability and problems with emotion regulation, than adolescents in the High EA/High ER cluster. The Low EA/High
ER cluster tolerated less distress on the behavioral task than the Varied EA/Low ER or High EA/High ER clusters, which
suggests problems with emotional awareness may contribute to inaccurate self-evaluation of one’s related emotional abilities. A
person-centered approach was useful in identifying patterns of adolescent perceptions of strengths and difficulties in emotional
development and their associations with other-report and performance on a behavioral task.
Keywords Emotion Regulation Emotional Awareness Cluster Analysis Adolescent Development Psychological
● ● ● ●

Functioning

Highlights
● Use of person-centered analyses can help us better understand how to assess and provide tailored support for emotion
development in adolescents.
● Perceived problems with emotional awareness alone, without perceived challenges with emotion regulation or perceived
problems with access to effective emotion regulation strategies, regardless of level of emotional awareness, may confer
risk of experiencing mental health symptoms and emotion regulation disturbances.
● Perceived problems with emotional awareness while perceiving few challenges with accessing emotion regulation
strategies were related to poorer distress tolerance on a behavioral task.

Adolescents’ ability to regulate emotions is associated with


* Caroline Cummings better psychosocial and physical health and is theorized to be
carolicu@ttu.edu
an important mediator of positive developmental outcomes
1
Department of Psychological Sciences, Texas Tech University, into adulthood (Campos et al., 2010; Crowell et al. 2013,
Box 42051, Lubbock, TX 79409, USA Diamond & Aspinwall, 2003; Eisenberg et al., 2005; Gar-
2
Department of Psychological Science, University of Vermont, 2 nefski et al., 2005; Vasilev et al., 2009). For successful
Colchester Avenue, Burlington, VT 05405, USA emotion regulation, that is to engage in emotion modulation
3
Rhode Island Hospital/Alpert Medical School at Brown in the pursuit of goals, the process theory of emotion reg-
University, 1 Hoppin St., Providence, RI 02903, USA ulation suggests adolescents must not only learn how to apply
2632 Journal of Child and Family Studies (2022) 31:2631–2643

emotion regulation strategies, but also have awareness of distinct, yet related aspects of adolescent emotion regulation
their emotional experience (Cole et al., 2004; Gratz & Roe- development (Neumann et al., 2010; Perez et al., 2012). Yet,
mer, 2004; Gross, 2015). This is consistent with develop- findings from studies that utilize the DERS and variable-
mental systems theory models of the emergence of emotion centered analytic approaches (i.e., examining how two vari-
regulation across childhood and into adolescence which ables relate or predict another) demonstrate inconsistent
suggest that growth in self-awareness, and in particular associations between emotional awareness and access to
emotional awareness and understanding, facilitates growth in effective emotion regulation strategies in adolescence.
Dizer aqui
the ability to effectively apply specific regulatory strategies For example, Neumann et al. (2010) found that greater que há
algumas
(Ciarrochi et al., 2008; Kranzler et al., 2016; Riley et al., adolescent-reported problems with emotional awareness contradiçõe
2019; Roth et al., 2019; Thompson, 2011; Van Beveren et al., were associated with fewer problems with access to emotion s, mas
que...
2019). At the same time, there is evidence that, while access regulation strategies in a community-based sample of ado-
to effective emotion regulation strategies (i.e., the ability to lescents attending secondary school. In contrast, Weinberg
modify your emotional experience) is generally linearly and Klonsky (2009) found no significant correlation
improved across adolescence—a result of shifts in neuro- between adolescent reports on the two factors in a
biological development that undergird self-regulation (Guyer community-based sample of adolescents attending high
et al., 2016)—in many adolescents, emotional awareness (i.e., school, and Sarıtaş-Atalar et al. (2015) found a significant
the ability to monitor one’s own emotional experience) may but diminutive correlation in a community-based sample of
decline before improving into emerging adulthood (Nook adolescents attending high school. In even further contrast,
et al., 2018; Rith-Najarian et al., 2014; Weissman et al., Perez et al. (2012) found a significant and positive correla-
2020). This suggests that some adolescents may report tion between adolescent reports on the two factors in an
positive associations between use of emotion regulation skills inpatient psychiatric unit sample. The discrepancies among
and level of emotional awareness, while others may experi- these studies in the strength and direction of these associa-
ence a null or negative association between emotion regula- tions for clinical inpatient and non-clinical samples support
tion skills and emotional awareness. Which of these patterns that multiple profiles of adolescents’ perceptions of emotion
are present in early adolescence and which might indicate development may exist and be linked with emotional
greater risk for challenges in emotional development and developmental outcomes. A person-centered approach (i.e.,
associated outcomes remains unclear. Person-centered ana- how variables cluster together differently within different
lytic approaches are needed to identify these potential dif- types of individuals) may further broaden our understanding
ferential associations between emotion regulation and of which specific profiles of strengths and difficulties in
emotional awareness within samples and then link those emotion development present during early adolescence and
patterns with emotional development outcomes. This manu- which adolescent characteristics (e.g., demographic vari-
script describes the use of a person-centered approach ables, mental health, distress tolerance) might be most clo-
towards identifying distinct profiles of strengths and diffi- sely associated with those distinct profiles.
culties with emotional awareness and access to emotion Moreover, discrepancies in findings across these samples
regulation strategies in a sample of middle school students. might also be attributed to the inclusion of individuals
Consistent with research indicating that the association across all stages of adolescence within study samples,
between emotional awareness and emotion regulation may which does not account for the nonlinearity of emotion
not present linearly across adolescence, the strength and development across adolescence (i.e., rapid gains found
direction of the association of self-reported emotional during early adolescence and stagnancy then growth in the
awareness and access to effective emotion regulation strate- latter stages) (Hollenstein & Lanteigne, 2018). Although
gies in adolescence differs across studies and samples. The studies have used person-centered analyses to examine
most common multifactorial assessment tool for these con- emotion regulation development, these studies largely do
structs is the Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale not consider emotional awareness and are conducted with
(DERS; Gratz & Roemer, 2004). The DERS is a self-report young adults or adolescents broadly, not during early ado-
measure that evidences good construct validity and internal lescence. For example, person-centered analyses were used
consistency in assessing emotion regulation, including to better understand profiles of emotion regulation strategy
among adolescents (Adrian et al., 2011; Neumann et al., use and mental health symptoms in a sample of under-
2010; Perez et al., 2012). As a multifactorial measure of graduate students (Dixon-Gordon et al., 2015), and distinct
emotion regulation, the DERS has six subscales, including profiles of emotion regulation problems and distress toler-
Lack of Emotional Awareness and Limited Access to Emo- ance in young adults (Van Eck et al., 2017). In the context
tion Regulation Strategies subscales. Studies of the factor of adolescent emotion development, defined emotion reg-
structure of the DERS highlight that emotional awareness ulation profiles in adolescents based upon level of global
and access to effective emotion regulation strategies are emotion regulation, as well as emotionality. Across all
Journal of Child and Family Studies (2022) 31:2631–2643 2633

studies, distinct profiles of strengths and difficulties of the make specific hypotheses as to the number or nature of
studied emotion-related processes emerged, suggesting this clusters. Our secondary aim was to investigate whether
person-centered approach may yield unique profiles, but the distinct profiles would capture unique groups of adoles-
relationship between emotion regulation skills and emo- cents. Specifically, exploratory analyses were conducted to
tional awareness in early adolescents remains unclear. examine if differences in demographic variables between
Given the changes in emotional awareness and emotion clusters emerged. We also assessed for differences in parent
regulation systems that are theorized to occur beginning in and teacher measures of emotion development (i.e., nega-
early adolescence, an investigation of profiles of perceived tivity/lability and emotion regulation) and a behavioral
strengths and weaknesses in emotional awareness combined measure of distress tolerance between clusters/profiles.
with emotion regulation strategies is warranted. For exam-
ple, it is likely that some early adolescents who perceive
challenges with emotional awareness may still have access Methods
to strategies to regulate emotions. At the same time, other
early adolescents may perceive fewer problems in emo- Participants
tional awareness but persistent deficits in accessing effective
emotion regulation strategies—though it is possible this Adolescents (N = 414) were recruited from five urban public
discrepancy may be in part due to biased self-evaluation middle schools in the northeastern United States to partici-
(Kruger & Dunning, 1999). Still yet, profiles with varying pate in a larger emotion regulation and sexual health pro-
levels of emotional awareness or accessing emotion reg- motion randomized control trial intervention. All procedures
ulation strategies might be similarly or differentially related and measures described were conducted as a part of that
to developmental outcomes. For example, developmental study (Houck et al., 2016). Briefly, recruitment occurred
cascades theory (Masten & Cicchetti, 2010) explains that between September 2009 and February 2012. Adolescents
adaptive functioning in a specific developmental process were eligible to participate if they were aged 12–14 years, in
(e.g., strengths in emotional awareness and/or access to the 7th grade, and spoke English. Adolescents deemed at-risk
emotion regulation strategies) contributes to positive due to symptoms of emotional or behavioral problems were
downstream effects on related domains of development targeted (see the procedures subsection below for further
(e.g., mental health functioning; distress tolerance). Con- details). Adolescents who had a history of being sexually
versely, maladaptive functioning serves as a pathway for aggressive, were currently HIV-infected, had developmental
impairment across related facets of development. It is delays or were currently pregnant were excluded.
unknown whether strengths in one domain (i.e., emotional Descriptive statistics of the sample are documented in
awareness only or access to emotion regulation strategies Table 1. About half (47%) of participants self-identified as
only) may counteract the negative cascading effects of female, and 32.6% of participants self-identified as White,
difficulties in development of another domain (i.e., emo- 27.3% as African American/Black, 17.9% as multi-race,
tional awareness only or access to emotion regulation 3.4% as Native Hawaiian/Pacific Islander, 2.4% as Amer-
strategies only). Accordingly, identifying person-centered ican Indian/Alaskan Native and 1.0% as Asian. Almost half
profiles of perceived strengths and deficits in emotional (44%) were reported as experiencing symptoms above a
awareness and access to emotion regulation strategies, clinical cut-off for at least one psychiatric illness, including
along with their connections with emotion development and externalizing and internalizing disorders per adolescent
mental health outcomes in early adolescence, is essential. In (Youth Inventory-4; Gadow et al., 2002)- and/or parent-
doing so, researchers may generate a more nuanced report (Adolescent Symptom Inventory; Gadow & Sprafkin,
understanding of how various facets of emotion develop- 1997). Specifically, 18.6% of adolescents met the clinical
ment may contribute to adolescent psychopathology and symptom cut-off for conduct disorder, 13.5% for opposi-
socioemotional functioning. Such research may ultimately tional defiant disorder, 13.3% for attention-deficit/
help develop more targeted interventions to support positive
developmental outcomes in adolescence. Table 1 Descriptive statistics for entire sample
The primary purpose of the current study was to explore Lack of Emotional Awareness 3.05 (1.10)
profiles of perceived strengths and difficulties in emotional Limited Access to Emotion Regulation Strategies 2.10 (0.83)
awareness and access to emotion regulation strategies in a Age 12.96 (0.55)
sample of early adolescents. Given the paucity of consistent Family Income (median) $25,000
findings regarding the association between perceived emo- Race (% White) 33%
tional awareness and access to emotion regulation strategies Gender (% female) 47%
in early adolescents, and lack of theory describing person-
Mental Health Diagnosis Clinical Cut-off 44%
centered profiles combining these two factors, we did not
2634 Journal of Child and Family Studies (2022) 31:2631–2643

hyperactivity (ADHD), hyperactive or impulsive type, p = 0.26) and access to emotion regulation strategies
12.3% for ADHD, inattentive type, 11.6% for dysthymic (t(412) = 2.41, p = 0.12), parent-reports of negativity/labi-
disorder, 8.7% for ADHD, combined type, 8.5% for bipolar lity (t(386) = 1.07, p = 0.30) and emotion regulation
disorder, 5.8% for generalized anxiety disorder, and 5.8% (t(386) = 0.66, p = 0.42), or teacher-reports of negativity/
for major depression disorder. The primary caregivers of lability (t(400) = 0.82, p = 0.37) and emotion regulation
participants mostly self-identified as female (79%), with an (t(400) = 3.08, p = 0.08).
average age of 38.74 years, and reported a median family At baseline, adolescents completed an audio-assisted
income of $20,000 to $29,000. Teachers of participants computer self-interview on a laptop computer for all mea-
were primarily English teachers, which was chosen because sures, in a quiet location in the school, over multiple ses-
most students had a consistent English class during the sions if needed. Parents completed assessments on laptop
study period. For more information about this sample, and computers in the language of their choice (English, Spanish,
the screening process, please see Houck et al. (2016). or Portuguese). Teachers completed paper and pencil
questionnaires. Post-intervention, adolescents completed
Procedure another computerized assessment that included a behavioral
distress tolerance measure.
All procedures were approved by the Rhode Island Hospital
institutional review board. School staff were asked to pro- Measures
vide information on study participation to parents of stu-
dents exhibiting mental health symptoms. School staff were Adolescent behavioral and self-report measures
encouraged to refer to a counseling referral sheet for
examples of such symptoms, such as hyperactivity or dis- Baseline measures Difficulties in Emotion Regulation
ruptive behavior, withdrawal or internalization, declining Scale (DERS): Adolescents completed two subscales of the
grades, emotional lability, and concern for substance use or DERS; these assessed problems with emotional awareness
sexual activity. Approximately 27% of students attending and access to emotion regulation strategies (Gratz & Roe-
the seventh grade during the three study years were referred, mer, 2004). The other 4 subscales were not administered.
consistent with national prevalence data for adolescent Adolescents rated statements about emotion regulation on a
mental health disorders (Merikangas et al., 2010). Parents of 5-point scale (1 = almost never to 5 = almost always). The
potentially eligible adolescents were sent a consent-to- Lack of Emotional Awareness subscale included 6 items
contact form, which provided contact information for study about emotion awareness, e.g., “When I’m upset, I recog-
staff, who then called the parent to conduct eligibility nize my emotions” and “I pay attention to how I feel.” The
screening by phone. Adolescents who were reported by Limited Access to Emotion Regulation Strategies subscale
their parents as being sexually aggressive, currently HIV- included 8 items about accessing emotion regulation stra-
infected, having developmental delays, or currently preg- tegies, e.g., “When I’m upset, it takes me a long time to feel
nant were deemed ineligible at this point. Following the better” and “When I’m upset, my emotions feel over-
screening process, a face-to-face meeting was arranged (at whelming.” Item scores were summed and then divided by
the family’s home or at a community location), during the number of items to create an average score for each
which parental consent and adolescent assent were subscale. Higher scores indicated greater problems with
obtained, and eligible adolescents began participation. This emotional awareness and greater problems with accessing
study utilizes data from the baseline intake assessment prior emotion regulation strategies, respectively. The DERS,
to participation in the intervention. There is one exception including the subscale structure, has been previously vali-
to this; the behavioral distress tolerance measure was only dated in adolescent samples (Neumann et al., 2010; Vasilev
administered at the completion of the intervention, et al., 2009; Weinberg & Klonsky, 2009) and had strong
approximately 8 weeks after the baseline data were col- reliability in our sample for each subscale, Lack of Emo-
lected. To avoid possible intervention influences as a con- tional Awareness, α = 0.88, and Limited Access to Emotion
found for distress tolerance task performance, analyses Regulation Strategies, α = 0.83.
involving this measure included only participants (n = 195) Youth Inventory-4: Adolescents completed selected sub-
that were randomized to the control group, which received a scales from the YI-4 (Gadow et al., 2002) to screen for
health promotion only intervention without emotion reg- symptoms of common emotional and behavioral disorders
ulation content. Adolescents in the control group did not based on DSM-IV symptomatology. These included both
differ from the active treatment group at baseline in rates of externalizing (attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder- inatten-
psychiatric symptoms meeting cut-offs for clinical disorder tive and hyperactive type, conduct disorder, and oppositional
(X2 = 0.40, p = 0.53), gender (X2 = 0.13, p = 0.72), ado- defiant disorder) and internalizing (generalized anxiety
lescent reports of emotional awareness (t(412) = 1.25, disorder, major depressive disorder, dysthymia, and bipolar
Journal of Child and Family Studies (2022) 31:2631–2643 2635

disorders) psychopathology. We then generated a dichot- of emotion, emotional understanding, and empathy; Shields
omous variable where a score of 1 indicated the adolescent & Cicchetti, 1997). Statements were rated on a 4-point scale
met the symptom criteria based on the clinical cutoffs (1 = rarely to 4 = almost always). The negativity/lability
(compared to normative samples) for any disorder by either subscale included 16 items, e.g., “Has wide mood swings,”
adolescent or parent-report (see below), and a score of 0 “Can manage excitement,” and “Responds angrily to limit-
indicated the adolescent did not meet criteria for any disorder. setting by adults.” The emotion regulation subscale inclu-
This approach allows for a more parsimonious approach ded 8 items, e.g., “Is a cheerful child” and “Can say when
towards measuring adolescents’ mental health functioning feeling sad/mad/fearful.” Item scores were summed to
and accounts for potential discrepancies in adolescent self- generate a total score for each subscale, with higher scores
report and parent-report. The YI has been found to be reliable indicating more negativity/lability and better emotion reg-
across multiple samples (e.g., Gadow et al., 2002; Fanti et al., ulation, respectively. The ERC has been previously vali-
2018). In the current study, across all disorder scales, dated in child and early adolescent samples (Gratz et al.,
reliability was good to strong (0.71 < α < 0.95). 2009; Shields & Cicchetti 1998 2001) and had moderate to
good reliability in our sample for parent-report, Negativity/
Post-treatment measure Behavioral Indicator of Resi- Lability, α = 0.81, and Emotion Regulation, α = 0.61.
liency to Distress (BIRD): Post-intervention, adolescents
completed the BIRD, a computerized task that measures Adolescent symptom inventory (ASI) Primary caregivers
resiliency to distress by inducing emotional distress in par- completed subscales of the ASI (Gadow & Sprafkin, 1997)
ticipants through incrementally increasing the difficulty of a to screen for symptoms of emotional and behavioral dis-
task, then examining how long a participant persists. Parti- orders. These included both externalizing (attention deficit/
cipants were asked to click on a box below a moving dot on hyperactivity disorder inattentive and hyperactive type,
the computer screen in a video game like format. An ani- conduct disorder, and oppositional defiant disorder) and
mated “bird” responds to mistakes with an irritating noise, internalizing (generalized anxiety disorder, major depres-
thus increasing distress. In the third and final trial of the sive disorder, dysthymia, and bipolar disorders) psycho-
BIRD, the task becomes extremely difficult and participants pathology. As noted above, scores were dichotomized,
can use a “quit game” button that ends the task. Participants with a score of 1 indicating the adolescent met the symp-
were informed that they could end the task early, but that the tom criteria based on the clinical cutoffs (compared to
magnitude of their prize (amount of candy) was dependent normative samples) for any disorder by either adolescent
upon how well they did on the task. The maximum time that or parent-report, and a score of 0 indicated the adolescent
any participant can play in the final trial is 300 sec. In this did not meet criteria for any disorder. The ASI has been
study, we utilized a metric of how many sec an adolescent found to be reliable across multiple samples (e.g., Gadow
persisted in the final trial of the BIRD before giving up and et al., 2002; Pardini et al., 2012). In the current study,
quitting the program, referred to as Time to Quit (from 1 to across all scales, reliability was good to strong
300 sec). The BIRD has been validated as a behavioral (0.74 < α < 0.95), apart from the parent-report scale for
measure of distress tolerance in samples of adolescents symptoms of dysthymia (α = 0.66).
(Amstadter et al., 2012; Daughters et al., 2009; MacPherson
et al., 2010), and those studies found that around 50% of Teacher report measures (completed at baseline)
adolescents quit the task early. Across studies, adolescents
reported varying levels of distress during the task, yet level Emotion regulation checklist (ERC) Teachers completed
of distress did not predict whether adolescents quit early. the ERC. See above for further measure details. The ERC
Therefore, this task likely measures how much adolescents had strong reliability in our sample for teacher-report
tolerate distress, not merely the amount of distress (i.e., (α’s = 0.95 and 0.85).
negative affect) an adolescent experienced. In our sample of
early adolescents with mental health symptoms, 69% of the Analyses
participants quit the game early.
Data were examined for normality and outliers prior to all
Parent report measures (all completed at baseline) analyses. All statistical analyses were performed using the
Statistical Package for the Social Sciences for Windows.
Emotion regulation checklist (ERC) Parents completed an
emotion regulation measure that consisted of 24 items with Primary aim: clusters of distinct profiles
two subscales designed to examine both adolescent nega-
tivity/lability (i.e., mood swings, reactivity and intensity of Person-centered analysis was conducted through a two-step
emotion) and emotion regulation (i.e., adaptive regulation cluster analysis, where data are first grouped into
2636 Journal of Child and Family Studies (2022) 31:2631–2643

subclusters based on the Scharz’s Bayesian Criterion, a Emotional Awareness and Limited Access to Emotion
distance measure, and then a hierarchical process is utilized Regulation Strategies (Largest ratio of BIC distance mea-
to group subclusters into clusters (Bulger et al., 2007). The sures = 2.93). We labeled these clusters based upon visual
number of clusters in the final solution is identified as the and statistical inspection as Perceived Varied Emotional
solution with the highest ratio of distance measures between Awareness (scores spread across rating scale) and Low
the final number of clusters against the previous number of Emotion Regulation Access (Varied EA/Low ER), Per-
clusters. This type of cluster analysis does not require pre- ceived Low Emotional Awareness and High Emotion
specification of the number of clusters to identify nor does it Regulation Access (Low EA/High ER), and Perceived High
specify a latent process or structures that would underlie the Emotional Awareness and High Emotion Regulation
cluster structure of the data (we did not have a priori theory Access (High EA/High ER) clusters. It should be noted that,
for a latent structure) and therefore was best suited for the for ease of interpretation and readability, the labels for
exploratory cluster analysis of the current study. Using clusters describe participants’ perceived abilities in both
visual interpretation, we identified distinct clusters of pro- domains (e.g., Low EA indicates low perceived emotional
files of emotional awareness and access to emotion reg- awareness, not low perceived problems with emotional
ulation strategies. We then ran one-way ANOVAs with awareness as measured). The three clusters are represented
post-hoc Games-Howell corrections—due to non- visually in Fig. 1. Average levels and standard deviations of
homogenous variances in cluster groups on dependent Lack of Emotional Awareness and Limited Access to
variables—and univariate ANOVAs. Both analyses were Emotion Regulation Strategies for each cluster are provided
used to further characterize significant differences in the in Table 2.
associations between Lack of Emotional Awareness and Further supporting the identified clusters as being distinct
Limited Access to Emotion Regulation Strategies between profiles, one-way ANOVAs with post-hoc tests for differ-
the distinct clusters. The latter analysis provides regression ences by cluster indicated that each cluster was significantly
parameter estimates to identify the direction and strength of different from the others in scores on Lack of Emotional
the relationship between the subscales for each cluster. Awareness (Welch: F (2, 185.70) = 478.75, p < 0.01, partial
η2 = 0.63) and Limited Access to Emotion Regulation
Secondary aims Strategies (Welch: F (2, 200.18) = 264.91, p < 0.01, partial
η2 = 0.64), with all post-hoc Games-Howell comparisons
Profiles associations with demographic and parent- and between clusters significant (p’s < 0.03; Cohen’s d’s,
teacher-report measures We conducted two secondary 0.37 > d < 3.35). Moreover, results of a univariate ANOVA
analyses after completing the cluster analyses. First, we indicated cluster membership significantly moderated the
utilized one-way ANOVAs with post-hoc corrections and association of Lack of Emotional Awareness and Limited
chi square analyses to examine differences in demographic Access to Emotion Regulation Strategies (F(2, 408) = 9.84,
and descriptive variables for the clusters, including gender, p < 0.001, partial η2 = 0.05; see Fig. 2). Specifically, for
family income, race, age, and mental health symptoms. adolescents in the Varied EA/Low ER cluster, the slope of
Second, we utilized one-way ANOVAs with parent- and the association between Lack of Emotional Awareness and
teacher-report of emotion lability and emotion regulation as Limited Access to Emotion Regulation Strategies was
the dependent variables in order to examine if differences in small, but positive, and significantly different from zero
emotion development across social domains were found (b = 0.14, p = 0.01), such that greater perceived problems
between clusters. with emotional awareness were associated with greater
perceived problems with access. Also, adolescents within
Profiles associations with the post-treatment behavioral the Varied EA/Low ER cluster reported an average score on
task measure The association between cluster membership the problems with emotional awareness (M = 2.49, SD =
and Time to Quit on the BIRD (post-treatment) was analyzed 1.03) and high average score on limited access to strategies
through a Kruskal-Wallis one-way ANOVA test, as the Time (M = 3.42, SD = 0.63) subscales. For the adolescents in the
to Quit variable had a non-normal distribution in our data. Low EA/High ER cluster, the slope was negative and sig-
nificantly different from zero (b = −0.23, p = 0.001), such
that greater perceived problems with emotional awareness
Results were associated with fewer perceived problems with access.
Also, adolescents within the Low EA/High ER cluster
Clusters of Distinct Profiles reported a high average score on the problems with emo-
tional awareness subscale (M = 4.03, SD = 0.52) but a low
Exploratory two-step cluster analysis indicated that there score on the limited access to strategies subscale (M = 1.86,
were three clusters of adolescent self-reported Lack of SD = 0.48). For adolescents in the High EA/High ER
Journal of Child and Family Studies (2022) 31:2631–2643 2637

Fig. 1 Scatterplot of the Clusters


Varied EA/Low ER
Low EA/High ER
High EA/High ER

Table 2 Descriptive statistics for perceived problems with emotion income (X2 = 8.98, p = 0.53), or race (X2 = 4.98,
regulation clusters
p = 0.89). However, there were significant differences
Clusters M (SD) between clusters in gender (X2 = 12.70, p = 0.002) and
Varied EA/ Low EA/ High EA/ mental health symptoms (X2 = 22.77, p < 0.001). The
Low ER High ER High ER Varied EA/Low ER cluster included more females (n = 112
out of 182; 62%), whereas the Low EA/High ER cluster
Lack of EA 2.49 (1.03)a 4.03 (0.52)b 2.18 (0.58)c
a b
included more males (n = 51 out of 83; 61%). Similar rates
Limited ER 3.42 (0.63) 1.86 (0.48) 1.66 (0.44)c
of males (n = 75 out of 149; 50%) and females (n = 74 out
Age 12.98 (0.51) 12.97 (0.58) 12.94 (0.53)
of 149; 50%) were found in the High EA/High ER cluster.
Family Income $25,000 $25,000 $25,000 With regards to mental health symptoms, adolescents in
(median)
both the Varied EA/Low ER (n = 88 out of 182; 48%) and
Race (% White) 38% 40% 37%
Low EA/High ER (n = 50 out of 83; 60%) clusters were
Gender (% female) 62%a 39%b 50%a,b
more likely to include those with mental health symptoms
Mental Health 60%a 48%a 30%b
significant enough to reach a clinical cut-off for symptoms
Diagnosis Clinical
Cut-off (% at or of at least one diagnostic category assessed, in comparison
above clinical cut- to the High EA/High ER cluster (n = 45 out of 149; 30%).
off) One-way ANOVAs demonstrated that cluster member-
Means in the same row that do not share superscripts differ at a ship was significantly associated with parent-report of
p < 0.05 negativity/lability (F(2, 385) = 3.41, p = 0.03, partial
EA emotional awareness, ER access to emotion regulation strategies η2 = 0.02) and emotion regulation (Welch: F(2,
194.96) = 7.53, p = 0.001, partial η2 = 0.03), as well as
teacher-report of negativity/lability (F(2, 399) = 4.15,
cluster, the slope was positive, but not significantly different p = 0.02, partial η2 = 0.02) and emotion regulation (F(2,
from zero (b = 0.11, p = 0.07) and adolescents within the 399) = 3.42, p = 0.03, partial η2 = 0.02). Table 3 contains
cluster reported low average scores on the problems with cluster means and standard deviations for parent- and tea-
emotional awareness (M = 2.18, SD = 0.58) and limited cher- report of emotion regulation. Post-hoc t-tests found
access to strategies (M = 1.66, SD = 0.44) subscales. that adolescents in the High EA/High ER cluster were
reported as having lower Negativity/Lability compared to
Profiles Associations with Demographic and Parent- the Varied EA/Low ER Cluster (teacher: t(138.47) = 2.42,
and Teacher-Report Measures p = 0.02, Cohen’s d = 0.34) and the Low EA/High ER
Cluster (parent: t(311) = 2.49, p = 0.01, Cohen’s d = 0.29;
Demographic differences between the three clusters were teacher: t(322) = 2.50, p = 0.01, Cohen’s d = 0.28). There
also explored (see Table 2). The clusters did not differ in was one exception, such that adolescents in the High EA/
age (F(2, 410) = 0.11, p = 0.90, partial η2 < 0.001), family High ER cluster were not reported by parents as
2638 Journal of Child and Family Studies (2022) 31:2631–2643

Fig. 2 The Association of Lack


of Emotional Awareness and Varied EA/Low ER
Limited Access to Emotion
Low EA/High ER
Regulation Strategies Differed
High EA/High ER
by Cluster Membership

Table 3 Cluster membership


Clusters M (SD)
predicted differences in other
metrics of emotion regulation Varied EA/Low ER Low EA/High ER High EA/High ER

Negativity/Lability- Parent 1.93 (0.50) 1.93 (0.48) 1.80 (0.41)a


Negativity/Lability- Teacher 2.01 (0.73) 1.97 (0.72) 1.78 (0.62)a,b
Emotional Regulation-Parent 3.18 (0.44) 3.11 (0.47) 3.29 (0.36)a,b
Emotional Regulation-Teacher 2.89 (0.62) 2.91 (0.59) 3.06 (0.58)a,b
Time to Quit (median) 207.00 sec 116.50 sec 176.00 seca
EA emotional awareness, ER access to emotion regulation strategies
a
High EA/High ER cluster significantly differed from Low EA/High ER cluster
b
High EA/High ER cluster significantly differed from Varied EA/Low ER cluster

significantly different on Negativity/Lability from adoles- ER cluster (mean rank = 68.55) had shorter median Time to
cents in the Varied EA/Low ER cluster (t(127.64) = 1.86, Quit than adolescents in the High EA/High ER cluster
p = 0.07, Cohen’s d = 0.28); however, the general direction (mean rank = 83.80; U = 2154.50, p = 0.03, r = 0.18),
of the effect was consistent with the other findings. Also, highlighting difficulties in distress tolerance for the former
adolescents in the High EA/High ER cluster were reported cluster. There was not a significant difference in median
as having greater Emotion Regulation compared to the Time to Quit between the Varied EA/Low ER (mean
Varied EA/Low ER Cluster (parent: t(215) = −2.00, rank = 47.60) and High EA/High ER clusters (mean
p = 0.05, Cohen’s d = 0.27; teacher: t(220) = −2.01, rank = 46.71; U = 927.00, p = 0.88, r = 0.02). There also
p = 0.05, Cohen’s d = 0.28) and the Low EA/High ER was not a significant difference in median Time to Quit
Cluster (Parent: t(309.06) = −3.83, p < 0.001, Cohen’s between the Low EA/High ER (mean rank = 67.87) and
d = 0.43; Teacher: t(322) = −2.41, p = 0.02, Cohen’s Varied EA/Low ER clusters (mean rank = 55.23;
d = 0.26). U = 1009.00, p = 0.073, r = 0.22); however, the general
direction of the effect was consistent with the other findings.
Profiles Associations with the Post-Treatment
Behavioral Task Measure
Discussion
For the subset of adolescents in the control group (n = 195),
we found that cluster membership predicted significant This study explored profiles of strengths and difficulties in
differences in adolescent Time to Quit on the hardest block self-reported emotional awareness and access to emotion
they attempted of the BIRD (Kruskal-Wallis Test = 6.06, regulation strategies in early adolescents with mental health
p = 0.05, partial η2 = 0.03). Table 3 also contains cluster symptoms and examined how these profiles related to
medians for Time to Quit on the BIRD. Post-hoc Mann- demographic variables and emotional development. Three
Whitney U tests found that adolescents in the Low EA/High clusters were identified: adolescents reporting relatively
Journal of Child and Family Studies (2022) 31:2631–2643 2639

fewer problems in awareness and access (High EA/High In examining demographic differences between clusters,
ER); adolescents reporting fewer problems with accessing there was a greater proportion of adolescent girls in the
emotion regulation strategies, but notable difficulties with Varied EA/Low ER cluster and adolescent boys in the Low
emotional awareness (Low EA/High ER); and adolescents EA/High ER cluster. This is consistent with prior literature
reporting problems with accessing strategies, who reported (Bender et al., 2012; Neumann et al., 2010) and may be
a range of abilities related to emotional awareness (Varied explained by gender socialization theory, as parents often
EA/Low ER). These clusters were related to demographic promote emotional expressiveness (Brody & Hall, 2008)
variables, mental health functioning, and other facets of and use more emotion-related words when interacting with
emotion development (i.e., parent- and teacher-report their daughters compared to sons (Adams et al., 1995;
negativity/lability and emotion regulation; distress Fivush, 1989) which yields greater emotional awareness in
tolerance). girls in comparison to boys. Conversely, boys are expected
Our findings clarified the discrepant associations to experience more externalized emotions and parents often
between the Lack of Emotional Awareness and Limited shape boys to down-regulate those emotions rather than
Access to Emotion Regulation Strategies subscales on the express them (Brody, 1999; Weinberg et al., 1999), thus
DERS as reported in the literature (Neumann et al., 2010; boys may have reported fewer problems with access to
Vasilev et al., 2009; Weinberg & Klonsky, 2009). There emotion regulation strategies because they frequently
were three distinct categories and the association between practice downregulation of negative emotions, i.e., inhibi-
the Lack of Emotional Awareness and Limited Access to tory control of “negative” emotion. Given the findings,
Emotion Regulation Strategies subscales differed for each there may be important psychosocial factors (e.g., sociali-
cluster. The positive correlation between both domains of zation) to address to reduce adolescent girls’ risk for
emotion regulation functioning, which was reported by delayed development in accessing emotion regulation stra-
adolescents in the Varied EA/Low ER cluster, is con- tegies and boys’ risk for impaired emotion awareness.
sistent with prior studies of clinical, adolescent samples Moreover, given that boys within our sample were just as
(e.g., Perez et al., 2012). Moreover, the inverse relation- likely as girls to fall within the High EA/High ER cluster,
ship found within the Low EA/High ER cluster and the there is a need to further understand what differentiates
nonsignificant association found within the High EA/High adolescents who experience delayed functioning in a sin-
ER cluster are consistent with studies of non-clinical, gular domain versus those who experience healthy emotion
adolescent samples (Neumann et al., 2010; Weinberg & regulation development across both domains (e.g., differ-
Klonsky, 2009, respectively). Further, these findings are ences in brain development, influence of hormones).
consistent with multidimensional theories of emotion Our findings were also consistent with research and
regulation purporting that there are multiple dimensions theories that suggest that both emotional awareness and
of emotion regulation skill development that need to be ability to access various emotion regulation strategies are
assessed separately to fully understand adolescent emo- necessary to guide healthier mental and behavioral func-
tional competency and psychosocial development (Cole tioning (Cole et al., 2004; Izard, 2009; Izard et al., 2011).
et al., 2004; Gratz & Roemer, 2004). Moreover, findings Adolescents in the Varied EA/Low ER cluster and the Low
support the onset of differences in patterns of strengths EA/High ER cluster were both more likely to have clinical
and difficulties in emotional awareness and access to levels of mental health symptoms and were perceived by
emotion regulation strategies during early adolescence, teachers and parents as having poorer emotion regulation
which is likely related to changes in neurobiology and compared to the High EA/High ER cluster. Therefore, while
social-emotional functioning that are theorized to occur all adolescents had some noted problems in mental health or
during this key developmental period (Guyer et al., 2016; socioemotional functioning per school-personnel report,
Nook et al., 2018). For example, though it was not within having a perceived deficit in either emotional awareness or
the scope of the current study, it is possible that adoles- access to emotion regulation strategies was a strong pre-
cents within the current sample demonstrated high varia- dictor of clinically significant symptoms. This is consistent
bility in their neural responsiveness to emotion-related with the extended process model of emotion regulation
stimuli and level of neural reactivity might have predicted (Gross, 2015), which asserts that impairments with emo-
concurrent differences in emotion regulation development tional processing and regulation may serve as mechanisms
(Guyer et al., 2016). Building upon current study findings, explaining the onset and maintenance of most psycho-
it is imperative to explore possible mechanisms explain- pathology. Moreover, these deficits were notable across
ing the emergence of different patterns of emotional multiple social domains (i.e., home and school), as indi-
awareness and emotion regulation abilities during early cated by corroborating emotion regulation data collected
adolescence. from parents and teachers, thus such emotion regulation
2640 Journal of Child and Family Studies (2022) 31:2631–2643

impairments might have been pervasive and affecting ado- appropriate emotion regulation strategy during the BIRD
lescent functioning at a global level. task. Therefore, these findings highlight the importance of
Exploratory analyses of performance on the BIRD task assessing emotion regulation via multiple modalities (i.e.,
yielded more complex findings. Adolescents in the Low other-report and performance tasks), to ensure deficits are
EA/High ER cluster exhibited poorer distress tolerance on adequately addressed and compensate for a potential bias in
the BIRD task when compared to the High EA/High ER self-report.
cluster. Yet, those adolescents in Varied EA/Low ER cluster
did not differ significantly in performance on the BIRD
from the High EA/High ER cluster. Moreover, despite Limitations
differences in mean time-to-quit between the Varied EA/
Low ER and Low EA/High ER cluster, this difference was The findings of this study need to be considered in the
not significant, which may be attributed to high variability context of some limitations. First, while a strength of this
found in performance on the BIRD within the Low EA/ sample is that our population included a heterogeneous
High ER cluster. Nonetheless, given that better performance sample of urban youth with differing levels of socio-
on the BIRD task has been associated with both decreased emotional and/or mental health problems, it is unclear if the
internalizing and externalizing symptoms, including fewer findings would generalize to a general sample of adoles-
delinquent behaviors (e.g., substance use, getting in physi- cents or to a clinical sample, i.e., additional profiles might
cal altercations, carrying a weapon) and alcohol use emerge in a true clinical, non-clinical, or population-based
(Cummings et al., 2013; Daughter et al., 2009), adolescents sample. Moreover, school personnel referred potentially
in the Low EA/High ER cluster might be at a greater risk for eligible students to our research staff based on their obser-
delinquent behaviors in addition to internalizing symptoms. vations or knowledge of those students who exhibited
One hypothesis that requires further examination is that symptoms of emotional or behavioral problems. It is pos-
awareness of emotions might protect adolescents with sible that there are additional adolescents who may have
regards to distress tolerance and engagement in risky been eligible based on these criteria but were not referred
behaviors, even if they are experiencing elevated mental because school personnel did not detect their emotional or
health symptoms. behavioral problems. Second, since this study did not
Last, although adolescents in the Low EA/High ER include non-adolescent reporter measures of emotional
cluster reported few problems with accessing emotion reg- awareness and access to emotion regulation strategies, it
ulation strategies, parent, teacher, and behavioral measures was not feasible to test questions pertaining to the con-
of emotion regulation suggested to the contrary. Adoles- cordance of adolescent self-report with other reporters.
cents in this cluster exhibited poorer distress tolerance on Third, the full DERS was not administered, thus the validity
the BIRD task, greater parent and teacher reported negative of the instrument may have been affected by modification of
lability, and poorer parent and teacher reported emotion the item set. Fourth, the findings of this cluster analysis
regulation when compared to the High EA/High ER cluster. would benefit from replications with larger sample sizes to
While it may be that deficits in emotional awareness por- increase the reliability of the results. Although we did uti-
tend less effective emotion regulation despite having access lize an objective indicator of distress tolerance, the BIRD
to emotion regulation strategies, we hypothesize that ado- was only administered in our sample post-intervention, and
lescents in the Low EA/High ER cluster were also less able the sample size for the analyses with the BIRD was much
to accurately self-evaluate their emotion regulation skills. smaller than the sample size used for the other analyses.
Kruger and Dunning (1999) explain that self-assessments of Fifth, more recently, the construct validity of the awareness
social and intellectual abilities become most biased when subscale of the DERS has been critiqued, (e.g., Bardeen
people have significant deficits, as they are usually unaware et al., 2016). However, high internal consistency of the
of the extent of such deficits. Lack of emotional awareness items was found in our sample, thus awareness is likely to
likely limits these adolescents’ abilities to accurately rate have been reliably captured in the current study, perhaps a
access to strategies to regulate emotions. In fact, develop- benefit of the shortened measure set. Sixth, the DERS
mental systems theories suggest the development of self- captures trait emotion regulation abilities, thus the findings
awareness, including emotional awareness, should precede might not generalize to adolescents who struggle with
the development of broader self-regulatory processes, such emotional awareness or accessing effective emotion reg-
as the development of an emotion regulation repertoire ulation strategies only within confined and specific contexts,
(Thompson, 2011). This notion is supported by this cluster and therefore would fit in differing profiles depending upon
performing the worst on the BIRD distress tolerance task, the context of interest. Last, our measure of mental health
which suggested that a lack of awareness of their emotional symptoms collapsed all categories of clinical disorders. It is
state may have resulted in difficulty in skillfully utilizing an unknown whether the distinct profiles may be differentially
Journal of Child and Family Studies (2022) 31:2631–2643 2641

related to specific categories of clinical disorders (e.g., and emotion. regulation. It is also possible that the use of a
externalizing versus internalizing disorders). modular approach towards treatment planning, based on
strengths and difficulties at baseline, may also be efficacious
in supporting emotion development in adolescents partici-
Conclusion and Future Directions pating in interventions. It will also be important to
acknowledge the potential for within-person malleability in
The findings of the study allow multiple recommendations emotional awareness and access to emotion regulation
for future research. Foremost, given the increasing number strategies across contexts when examining how these
of interventions that target emotion regulation, researchers associations might change within interventions. By taking a
studying these interventions would benefit from collecting more contextualized approach we can better understand the
and utilizing data on both emotional awareness and access emotion-behavior chain and bolster treatment effects, thus
to effective emotion regulation strategies. These data may improving adolescent emotional functioning and overall
help researchers understand if these skills both change in wellbeing across the lifespan.
response to intervention and if there are particular profiles
of adolescents with skills or deficits in emotional awareness Funding This research was supported by a grant from National
Institute of Nursing Research – R01NR011906.
and accessing emotion regulation strategies who benefit
from emotion regulation interventions (e.g., emotion
focused coping skills, mindfulness training), including
Compliance with Ethical Standards
those that utilize chain analysis to better understand the
Conflict of Interest The authors declare no competing interests.
degree to which clients have deficits in specific domains of
emotion regulation (e.g., DBT skills training). While this Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to
study provides the framework for future studies to examine jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
profiles of emotion regulation, our sample consisted of
adolescents with varying levels of socioemotional and
References
mental health functioning, thus future studies should
explore this using both clinical and non-clinical samples to Adams, S., Kuebli, J., Boyle, P. A., & Fivush, R. (1995). Gender
examine whether differences exist between the two popu- differences in parent–child conversations about past emotions: A
lations. Further an examination of concordance of adoles- longitudinal investigation. Sex Roles, 33(5), 309–322.
cent, and parent- and teacher- report on the same constructs, Adrian, M., Zeman, J., & Veits, G. (2011). Methodological implica-
tions of the affect revolution: A 35-year review of emotion reg-
as well as concordance with other more objective indicators ulation assessment in children. Journal of Experimental Child
of emotion regulation (e.g., physiological processes), is Psychology, 110(2), 1–27.
warranted. Amstadter, A. B., Daughters, S. B., MacPherson, L., Reynolds, E. K.,
Danielson, C. K., & Wang, F., et al. (2012). Genetic associations
with performance on a behavioral measure of distress tintoler-
Practical Implications ance. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 46(1), 87–94.
Bardeen, J. R., Fergus, T. A., Hannan, S. M. & Orcutt, H. K. (2016).
The findings also suggest that adolescents reporting pro- Addressing psychometric limitations of the Difficulties in Emo-
blems with accessing effective emotion regulation strategies tion Regulation Scale through item modification. Journal of
Personality Assessment, 98(3), 298–309.
and adolescents reporting problems with emotional aware- Bender, P. K., Reinholdt-Dunne, M. L., Esbjørn, B. H., & Pons, F.
ness may benefit from uniquely targeted interventions. For (2012). Emotion dysregulation and anxiety in children and ado-
example, it may be that adolescents reporting problems with lescents: Gender differences. Personality and Individual Differ-
access to emotion regulation strategies lack self-efficacy for ences, 53(3), 284–288.
Brody, L. R. (1999). Gender, emotion, and the family. Cambridge,
utilizing available emotion regulation strategies and need MA: Harvard University Press.
support in implementing and monitoring the success of Brody, L. R., & Hall, J. A. (2008). Gender and emotion in context. In
emotion regulation attempts. In contrast, those adolescents M. Lewis, J. M. Haviland-Jones & L. F. Barrett (Eds.), Handbook
reporting problems with emotional awareness were less able of emotions. 3rd ed. (pp. 395–408). New York, NY: The Guilford
Press.
to tolerate distress and may have unique emotion regulation Bulger, C. A., Matthews, R. A., & Hoffman, M. E. (2007). Work and
deficits such that interventions will need to target increasing personal life boundary management: Boundary strength, work/
awareness and understanding of their emotional experience personal life balance, and the segmentation-integration con-
as well as training emotion-focused regulation skills (e.g., tinuum. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 12(4),
365–375.
tolerating distress). Interventions such as Dialectical Beha- Campos, J. J., Campos, R. G., & Barrett, K. C. (2010). Emergent
vior Therapy allow for such individualization based on themes in the study of emotional development and emotion
adolescent strengths and difficulties in emotional awareness regulation. Developmental Psychology, 25(3), 394–402.
2642 Journal of Child and Family Studies (2022) 31:2631–2643

Ciarrochi, J., Heaven, P. C., & Supavadeeprasit, S. (2008). The link Guyer, A. E., Silk, J. S., & Nelson, E. E. (2016). The neurobiology of
between emotion identification skills and socio-emotional func- the emotional adolescent: From the inside out. Neuroscience &
tioning in early adolescence: a 1-year longitudinal study. Journal Biobehavioral Reviews, 70, 74–85.
of Adolescence, 31(5), 565–582. Hollenstein, T., & Lanteigne, D. M. (2018). Emotion regulation
Cole, P., Martin, S., & Dennis, T. (2004). Emotion regulation as a dynamics in adolescence. In P. M. Cole & T. Hollenstein (Ed.),
scientific construct: Methodological challenges and directions for Emotion Regulation: A Matter of Time. New York, NY:
child development research. Child Development, 75(2), 317–333. Rutledge.
Crowell, S. E., Skidmore, C. R., Rau, H. K., & Williams, P. (2013). Houck, C. D., Hadley, W., Barker, D., Brown, L. K., Hancock, E., &
Psychosocial Stress, Emotion Regulation, and Resilience in Almy, B. (2016). An emotion regulation intervention to reduce
Adolescence. In W. T. O’Donohue, L. T. Benuto & L. Woodward risk behaviors among at-risk early adolescents. Prevention Sci-
Tolle (Eds.), Handbook of adolescent health psychology (pp. ence, 17(1), 71–82.
129–141). New York, NY: Springer. Izard, C. E. (2009). Emotion theory and research: Highlights, unan-
Cummings, J. R., Bornovalova, M. A., Ojanen, T., Hunt, E., Mac- swered questions, and emerging issues. Annual Review of Psy-
Pherson, L., & Lejuez, C. (2013). Time doesn’t change every- chology, 60, 1–25.
thing: The longitudinal course of distress tolerance and its Izard, C. E., Woodburn, E. M., Finlon, K. J., Krauthamer-Ewing, E. S.,
relationship with externalizing and internalizing symptoms during Grossman, S. R., & Seidenfeld, A. (2011). Emotion knowledge,
early adolescence. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 41(5), emotion utilization, and emotion regulation. Emotion Review,
735–748. 3(1), 44–52.
Daughters, S. B., Reynolds, E. K., MacPherson, L., Kahler, C. W., Kranzler, A., Young, J. F., Hankin, B. L., Abela, J. R., Elias, M. J., &
Danielson, C. K., & Zvolensky, M., et al. (2009). Distress tol- Selby, E. A. (2016). Emotional awareness: A transdiagnostic
erance and early adolescent externalizing and internalizing predictor of depression and anxiety for children and adolescents.
symptoms: The moderating role of gender and ethnicity. Beha- Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 45(3),
viour Research and Therapy, 47(3), 198–205. 262–269.
Diamond, L., & Aspinwall, L. (2003). Emotion regulation across the Kruger, J., & Dunning, D. (1999). Unskilled and unaware of it: how
life span: An integrative perspective emphasizing self-regulation, difficulties in recognizing one’s own incompetence lead to
positive affect, and dyadic processes. Motivation and Emotion, inflated self-assessments. Journal of Personality and Social
27(2), 125–156. Psychology, 77(6), 1121.
Dixon-Gordon, K. L., Aldao, A., & De Los Reyes, A. (2015). MacPherson, L., Reynolds, E. K., Cassidy, J., Daughters, S. B.,
Repertoires of emotion regulation: A person-centered approach to Mayes, L. C., & Wang, F. (2010). Positive and negative rein-
assessing emotion regulation strategies and links to psycho- forcement underlysing risk beahvior in early adolescents. Pre-
pathology. Cognition and Emotion, 29(7), 1314–1325. vention Science, 11(3), 331–342.
Eisenberg, N., Sadovsky, A., & Spinrad, T. L. (2005). Associations of Masten, A. S., & Cicchetti, D. (2010). Developmental cascades.
emotion-related regulation with language skills, emotion knowl- Development and Psychopathology, 22(3), 491–495.
edge, and academic outcomes. New Directions for Child and Merikangas, K. R., He, J. P., Burstein, M., Swanson, S. A., Avenevoli,
Adolescent Development, 109, 109–118. S., Cui, L., & Swendsen, J. (2010). Lifetime prevalence of mental
Fanti, K. A., Kyranides, M. N., Lordos, A., Colins, O. F., & Ander- disorders in US adolescents: results from the National Comor-
shed, H. (2018). Unique and interactive associations of callous- bidity Survey Replication–Adolescent Supplement (NCS-A).
unemotional traits, impulsivity and grandiosity with child and Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psy-
adolescent conduct disorder symptoms. Journal of Psycho- chiatry, 49(10), 980–989.
pathology and Behavioral Assessment, 40(1), 40–49. Neumann, A., Van Lier, P., Gratz, K., & Koot, H. (2010). Multi-
Fivush, R. (1989). Exploring sex differences in the emotional content dimensional Assessment of Emotion Regulation Difficulties in
of mother–child conversations about the past. Sex Roles, 20(11), Adolescents Using the Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale.
675–691. Assessment, 17(1), 138–149.
Gadow, K., & Sprafkin, J. (1997). Adolescent Symptom Inventory-4 Nook, E. C., Sasse, S. F., Lambert, H. K., McLaughlin, K. A., &
(ASI-4). Stonybrook, NY: Checkmate Plus. Somerville, L. H. (2018). The nonlinear development of emotion
Gadow, K. D., Sprafkin, J., Carlson, G. A., Schneider, J., Nolan, E. E., differentiation: Granular emotional experience is low in adoles-
& Mattison, R. E., et al. (2002). A DSM-IV-referenced, adoles- cence. Psychological Science, 29(8), 1346–1357.
cent self-report rating scale. Journal of the American Academy of Pardini, D., Stepp, S., Hipwell, A., Stouthamer-Loeber, M., & Loeber,
Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 41(6), 671–679. R. (2012). The clinical utility of the proposed DSM-5 callous-
Garnefski, N., Kraaij, V., & van Etten, M. (2005). Specificity of unemotional subtype of conduct disorder in young girls. Journal
relations between adolescents’ cognitive emotion regulation of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry,
strategies and internalizing and externalizing psychopathology. 51(1), 62–73.
Journal of Adolescence, 28(5), 619–631. Perez, J., Venta, A., Garnaat, S., & Sharp, C. (2012). The difficulties in
Gratz, K., & Roemer, L. (2004). Multidimensional assessment of emotion regulation scale: Factor structure and association with
emotion regulation and dysregulation: Development, factor nonsuicidal self-injury in adolescent inpatients. Journal of Psy-
structure, and initial validation of the difficulties in emotion chopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 34(3), 393–404.
regulation scale. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Riley, T. N., Sullivan, T. N., Hinton, T. S., & Kliewer, W. (2019).
Assessment, 26(1), 41–54. Longitudinal relations between emotional awareness and
Gratz, K., Tull, M. T., Reynolds, E. K., Bagge, C. L., Latzman, R. D., expression, emotion regulation, and peer victimization among
& Daughters, S. B., et al. (2009). Extending extant models of urban adolescents. Journal of Adolescence, 72, 42–51.
pathogenesis of borderline personality disorder to childhood Rith-Najarian, L. R., McLaughlin, K. A., Sheridan, M. A., & Nock, M.
borderline personality symptoms: The roles of affective dys- K. (2014). The biopsychosocial model of stress in adolescence:
function, disinhibition, and self- and emotion-regulation deficits. self-awareness of performance versus stress reactivity. Stress,
Developmental Psychopathology, 21(4), 1263–1291. 17(2), 193–203.
Gross, J. J. (2015). Emotion regulation: Current status and future Roth, G., Vansteenkiste, M., & Ryan, R. M. (2019). Integrative
prospects. Psychological inquiry, 26(1), 1–26. emotion regulation: Process and development from a self-
Journal of Child and Family Studies (2022) 31:2631–2643 2643

determination theory perspective. Development and Psycho- depressive symptoms in youth. European Child & Adolescent
pathology, 31(3), 945–956. Psychiatry, 28(3), 389–398.
Sarıtaş-Atalar, D., Gençöz, T., & Özen, A. (2015). Confirmatory factor Van Eck, K., Warren, P., & Flory, K. (2017). A variable-centered and
analyses of the difficulties in emotion regulation scale (DERS) in person-centered evaluation of emotion regulation and distress
a Turkish adolescent sample. European Journal of Psychological tolerance: links to emotional and behavioral concerns. Journal of
Assessment, 31, 12–19. Youth and Adolescence, 46(1), 136–150.
Shields, A., & Cicchetti, D. (1997). Emotion regulation among school- Vasilev, C., Crowell, S., Beauchaine, T., Mead, H., & Gatzke-Kopp,
age children: The develpment and validation of a new criterion L. (2009). Correspondence between physiological and self-report
Q-sort scale. Developmental Psychology, 33(6), 906–916. measures of emotion dysregulation: A longitudinal investigation
Shields, A., & Cicchetti, D. (1998). Reactive aggression among mal- of youth with and without psychopathology. Journal of Child
treated children: The contributions of attention and emotion Psychology and Psychiatry, 50(11), 1357–1364.
dusregulation. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 27(4), Weinberg, A., & Klonsky, E. (2009). Measurement of emotion dys-
709–723. regulation in adolescents. Psychological Assessment, 21(4),
Shields, A., & Cicchetti, D. (2001). Parental maltreatment and emotion 616–621.
dysregulation as risk factors for bullying and victimization in Weinberg, M. K., Tronick, E. Z., Cohn, J. F., & Olson, K. L. (1999).
middle childhood. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 30(3), Gender differences in emotional expressivity and self-regulation
349–363. during early infancy. Developmental Psychology, 35(1), 175–188.
Thompson, R. A. (2011). Emotion and emotion regulation: Two sides Weissman, D. G., Nook, E. C., Dews, A. A., Miller, A. B., Lambert,
of the developing coin. Emotion Review, 3(1), 53–61. H. K., Sasse, S. F., & McLaughlin, K. A. (2020). Low emotional
Van Beveren, M. L., Goossens, L., Volkaert, B., Grassmann, C., awareness as a transdiagnostic mechanism underlying psycho-
Wante, L., Vandeweghe, L., & Braet, C. (2019). How do I feel pathology in adolescence. Clinical Psychological Science, 8(6),
right now? Emotional awareness, emotion regulation, and 971–988.

You might also like