You are on page 1of 13

633562

research-article2016
AUT0010.1177/1362361316633562AutismPan et al.

Original Article
Autism

The impacts of physical activity 1­–13


© The Author(s) 2016
Reprints and permissions:
intervention on physical and sagepub.co.uk/journalsPermissions.nav
DOI: 10.1177/1362361316633562

cognitive outcomes in children aut.sagepub.com

with autism spectrum disorder

Chien-Yu Pan1, Chia-Hua Chu1, Chia-Liang Tsai2,


Ming-Chih Sung1, Chu-Yang Huang1 and Wei-Ya Ma1

Abstract
This study examined the effects of a 12-week physical activity intervention on the motor skill proficiency and executive
function of 22 boys (aged 9.08 ± 1.75 years) with autism spectrum disorder. In Phase I of the 12 weeks, 11 boys with
autism spectrum disorder (Group A) received the intervention, whereas the other 11 boys with autism spectrum
disorder (Group B) did not (true control, no intervention). The arrangement was reversed in Phase II, which lasted an
additional 12 weeks. The Bruininks–Oseretsky Test of Motor Proficiency, Second Edition, and the Wisconsin Card Sorting
Test were conducted three times for each participant (Group A, primary grouping: baseline (T1), post-assessment (T2),
and follow-up assessment (T3); Group B, control grouping: T1−T2; intervention condition, T2−T3). The main findings
were that both groups of children with autism spectrum disorder significantly exhibited improvements in motor skill
proficiency (the total motor composite and two motor-area composites) and executive function (three indices of the
Wisconsin Card Sorting Test) after 12 weeks of physical activity intervention. In addition, the effectiveness appeared to
have been sustained for at least 12 weeks in Group A. The findings provide supporting evidence that physical activity
interventions involving table tennis training may be a viable therapeutic option for treating children with autism spectrum
disorder.

Keywords
autism spectrum disorder, executive function, motor skill proficiency, physical activity intervention

Introduction
Children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) experience domains. However, a frequently overlooked area in the
challenges in social communication and interactions and early intervention literature for children with ASD is motor
exhibit a restricted pattern of behavior and interests skills. Motor skills, including locomotor (e.g. running,
(American Psychiatric Association (APA), 2013). hopping, and jumping), object control (e.g. catching,
Identifiers for ASD-related motor skill impairment are throwing, and striking), gross motor (e.g. coordination,
lacking in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental balance, and agility), and fine motor skills (e.g. precision,
Disorders (APA, 2013); however, the International integration, and manual dexterity), are necessary for
Classification of Functioning, Disability and Health lists engaging in physical activities related to the development
motor skill deficits as an ASD-associated symptom (World
Health Organization, 2001). Impaired or delayed motor 1National Kaohsiung Normal University, Taiwan
skills (e.g. movement skills, motor coordination, and gross 2National Cheng Kung University, Taiwan
and fine motor skills) have been reported in numerous
studies on children with ASD (Fournier et al., 2010; Green Corresponding author:
Chien-Yu Pan, Department of Physical Education, National Kaohsiung
et al., 2009; Lloyd et al., 2013; Staples and Reid, 2010). Normal University, No. 116, He-Ping First Road, Kaohsiung 802,
Traditional interventions are primarily focused on core Taiwan.
challenges in the social, communicative, and behavioral Email: chpan@nknucc.nknu.edu.tw

Downloaded from aut.sagepub.com at R.M.I.T. Libraries Bundoora on April 11, 2016


2 Autism

of healthy lifestyles; however, participation in physical decreased independence and poor outcomes in adulthood
activities is necessary for promoting the development of (Hume et al., 2009). Other long-term effects of executive
these skills as well. Therefore, developing proficient motor function deficits have been found in middle-childhood
skills may be even more critical for children with ASD girls with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder; specifi-
because it empowers them with the opportunity to engage cally, impaired global executive function predicted a
in physically active activities with peers, enabling them to higher number of suspensions and expulsions from school.
derive the developmental benefits of physical activities. Because executive function contributes to success in
A range of motor skill impairments has been identified school and employment and enables people to inhibit inap-
in studies on people with ASD, and these include gross propriate behaviors as well as manage stress and barriers
motor, fine motor, postural control, and imitation or praxis related to activities in daily life, assessing and developing
impairments (Bhat et al., 2011). There is no one-size-fits- interventions that target executive function impairments
all approach to motor skill assessment (Staples et al., early in life to prevent long-term difficulties across a range
2012); however, researchers and pediatric clinicians com- of important functional domains is critical.
monly assess gross and fine motor skill performance using Evidence supports the beneficial effects of physical
standardized measures such as the Bruininks–Oseretsky activity on executive function and suggests that effects
Test of Motor Proficiency, Second Edition (BOS-2; Pan, might be particularly large for children (Gapin et al.,
2014; Wuang et al., 2010), the Movement Assessment 2011). The rationale is that exercise-induced neural plas-
Battery for Children (Ament et al., 2015; Whyatt and ticity is not merely restricted to areas of the brain serving
Craig, 2012), or the Test of Gross Motor Development motor function and may therefore translate into enhanced
(MacDonald et al., 2013; Staples and Reid, 2010) in chil- executive function (Verburgh et al., 2014). However, few
dren with ASD. The most comprehensive study directly studies have explored physical activity as a means of
measured the motor skills of 25 school-aged children with improving executive function of children with ASD. To
ASD compared with three typically developing (TD) com- date, data on the effects of physical activity intervention
parison groups, each individually matched according to (1) addressing executive function for children with ASD are
chronological age, (2) movement skill performance, and promising. Hilton et al. (2014) conducted a single-group
(3) mental age (Staples and Reid, 2010). That cross- pretest–posttest design in a group of seven children diag-
sectional study found that children with ASD exhibited nosed with ASD (aged 6–14 years) and reported improved
significantly poorer locomotor and object control scores motor skill proficiency and executive function following
compared with TD children in the chronological- and men- 10 weeks of an exergaming program; significant improve-
tal-age-matched groups; however, no significant differ- ments were noted in working memory, metacognition, and
ences emerged between the children with ASD with a the motor skill areas of strength and agility. Because motor
mean age of 11.15 years and TD children with a mean age skill and executive function deficits, in addition to core
of 5.87 years. The significant motor delays experienced by symptoms in children with ASD, may contribute to low
school-age children with ASD indicate the need for creat- rates of physical activity participation by presenting a
ing motor skill interventions in order to minimize motor greater challenge for such children, interventions directly
delays and promote optimal overall development. targeting motor skill proficiency and executive function in
In addition to the motor skill deficits reported in numer- children with ASD may be helpful.
ous studies on children with ASD, executive dysfunction Studies have indicated that physical activity interven-
has consistently been observed in people diagnosed with tions can be effective for improving motor skills in children
ASD (Robinson et al., 2009; Sachse et al., 2013). Executive with ASD (aged 4–18  years) (Bremer et al., 2015;
function constitutes a set of cognitive processes that com- MacDonald et al., 2012; Wuang et al., 2010). A recent study
prise distinct yet highly interrelated components such as on optimal outcomes for young children with ASD aged
cognitive flexibility, inhibitory control, and working mem- 14–33 months indicated that motor skills were a significant
ory (McClelland et al., 2014). Because executive function predictor of calibrated autism severity (i.e. a severity
is involved in the regulation of both thought and action, the marker less influenced by verbal intelligent quotient (IQ;
behavioral manifestations of executive function can be Gotham et al., 2009)) at that age, implying that young chil-
assessed (Becker et al., 2014), including difficulty switch- dren with superior motor skills demonstrated fewer social
ing between tasks, difficulty initiating new nonroutine communicative skill deficits (MacDonald et al., 2014).
actions, and a lack of impulse control (Rajendran and Therefore, physical activity interventions can potentially
Mitchell, 2007). Low cognitive and behavioral flexibility improve the overall development of young children with
is a specific executive function impairment in many chil- ASD, in addition to their motor skills. Bass et al. (2009)
dren with ASD (Reed et al., 2013), and this difficulty is indicated that 12-week therapeutic horseback riding was
correlated with core symptom presentation (Reed et al., effective in improving sensory skills and social responsive-
2013) and adaptive behaviors in ASD (Pugliese et al., ness in children with ASD ranging from 5 to 10 years. Pan
2015). Problems with flexibility and initiation are linked to (2010) reported that a 10-week aquatic exercise regimen

Downloaded from aut.sagepub.com at R.M.I.T. Libraries Bundoora on April 11, 2016


Pan et al. 3

improved aquatic skills and has social improvement poten- 2. Does executive function measured using the com-
tial for children with ASD (aged 6–9 years). Anderson- puter version of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test
Hanley et al. (2011) found significant improvements in the (WCST; Heaton and PAR Staff, 2003) improve
measures of attention and working memory and significant after participation in the physical activity interven-
decreases in repetitive behaviors in 12 children aged 10– tion through table tennis exercise in two groups of
18 years with a diagnosis of an ASD immediately after they children with ASD?
participated in a 20-min exergaming intervention. Chan 3. Does the intervention effect sustained for at least
et al. (2013) investigated the efficacy of a 4-week (60 min 12 weeks in primary-grouping children with ASD
per session, twice per week) mind–body exercise, nei yang (i.e. Group A)?
gong, in 20 children with ASD aged 6–17 years, and found
positive enhancements in self-control and reductions in We hypothesized that (1) physical activity intervention
typical autistic symptoms and daily emotional and behavio- through table tennis exercise would improve both motor
ral problems. Although the mentioned data support the skill proficiency and executive function in both groups of
positive effects of these novel interventions, whether the children with ASD, and (2) the intervention effect would
treatment efficacies can be sustained or cumulate remains sustain for at least 12 weeks in primary-grouping children
largely inconclusive. with ASD. To date, no study has examined these physical
Numerous studies have been published on the value of and cognitive measures concurrently in a realistic and
employing moderate to vigorous aerobic exercise in exec- dynamic environment (i.e. table tennis setting) in ASD;
utive function improvement in TD children (Crova et al., therefore, our study was partly exploratory.
2014; Diamond and Lee, 2011; Verburgh et al., 2014);
however, the effects of using alternative forms of physical
activity intervention have only begun to be studied. Pesce Method
(2012) indicated that whether and how the qualitative
Study design
aspects of physical activity intervention (e.g. open-skill
exercise or a combination of complex skills) affect short- Ethical approval was received from a university research
and long-term physical and cognitive performance must be ethics committee for human behavioral sciences. All par-
examined. Recent evidence suggests that ASD-associated ents and children provided informed consent before the
motor skill deficits may not be pervasive, but are apparent study began. Before intervention, all children were
in activities requiring complex, interceptive actions or core screened for eligibility and paired on the basis of age, dis-
balance abilities (Whyatt and Craig, 2012), or in skills ability type, and comorbidity and then they were randomly
necessitating the coupling of visual and temporal feedback split into Group A (n = 11; primary grouping: baseline
(Ament et al., 2015). Therefore, the purpose of this study (T1), post-assessment (T2), and follow-up assessment
was twofold: (1) to evaluate the effects of a 12-week phys- (T3)) and Group B (n = 11; control grouping: T1−T2; inter-
ical activity intervention through table tennis exercise on vention condition, T2−T3). This enabled testing the effec-
motor skill proficiency and executive function in two tiveness of the intervention (Group A children with ASD
groups (A and B) of children with ASD and (2) to examine receiving the first phase of a 12-week intervention, and
the possible sustained intervention effects in improving Group B children with ASD acting as the control) in addi-
motor skill proficiency and executive function of primary- tion to the effectiveness of a second phase of a 12-week
grouping (i.e. Group A) children with ASD. We employed intervention in Group B children with ASD as well as the
a physical activity intervention through table tennis exer- potential sustainment of treatment effectiveness in Group
cise because it merges motor skill training (i.e. locomotor A children with ASD.
and object control skills) with executive function training Regardless of group assignment, all participants
(more details are in physical activity intervention session), received the intervention for the same number of sessions
and the characteristics of this type of exercise are expected and hours. Each participant also attended three assess-
to affect response selection and execution in children with ments, where we assessed motor skill proficiency and
ASD (e.g. the training of visual information in a table ten- executive function: once at study enrollment to serve as
nis task as perceived by the player, and the manipulation of the baseline (T1), a second time after 12 weeks of physical
the temporal and spatial characteristics of ball flight). The activity intervention or regular treatment (T2), and a third
research questions asked were as follows: time after another 12 weeks (T3). Assessments 1 and 2 rep-
resented the pre- and post-assessment, respectively, of the
1. Does motor skill proficiency assessed using the experimental (Group A) and control (Group B) groups.
BOT-2 (Bruininks and Bruininks, 2005) improve Assessment 3 served as the 12-week follow-up for Group
after participation in the physical activity interven- A and the posttest for Group B following the intervention.
tion through table tennis exercise in two groups of The progression of the participants throughout the trial is
children with ASD? depicted in Figure 1.

Downloaded from aut.sagepub.com at R.M.I.T. Libraries Bundoora on April 11, 2016


4 Autism

Figure 1.  Participant flow diagram describing the progress through the enrollment, allocation, follow-up, and data analysis.

Participants Autism Behavior Checklist (ABC; Krug et al., 2008) and


Social Responsiveness Scale, Second Edition (SRS-2;
The participant inclusion criteria were as follows: (1) cur- Constantino and Gruber, 2012) to screen for autistic traits
rent age between 6 and 12 years, (2) a diagnosis of ASD and autism and/or Asperger behaviors. The ABC Chinese
based on Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental version manual documents good reliability (test–retest
Disorders, 4th edition, text revision (DSM-IV-TR) criteria reliability, 0.89; Cronbach’s alpha, 0.95) for the measure
at a high-functioning level, (3) the ability to follow direc- as a whole as well as content, construct, and criterion-
tions and perform requested motor skill proficiency and related validity. The Chinese version of the SRS-2 used in
executive function measures, and (4) no history of reading this study was a version obtained from the publisher. We
disabilities according to parents. We recruited participants did not use the Autism Diagnostic Interview–Revised
through an autism society by promoting the study at local (Lord et al., 1994) and the Autism Diagnostic Observation
ASD support group meetings, from the local clinical Schedule (Lord et al., 2002) because these instruments
center, and through word of mouth in a large urban city were unavailable in Chinese-language versions for
with a high level of socioeconomic deprivation in a devel- research. However, it is believed that expert clinicians of
oping Asian country. A multidisciplinary team from a the multidisciplinary team are reliable and accurate in
developmental clinic at a hospital in that Asian country diagnosing high-functioning ASD.
evaluated and determined the ASD diagnosis, and written Parents enrolled 22 children with ASD, aged
confirmation of ASD diagnosis had to be provided. In 6–12 years, in our study. The children were randomly
addition to a formal diagnosis by the physicians, we col- assigned to one of two groups on the basis of age, disabil-
lected parent ratings, which we confirmed through paren- ity type, and comorbidity. We collected information for
tal reports using the traditional Chinese version of the each child from the parents, including records on

Downloaded from aut.sagepub.com at R.M.I.T. Libraries Bundoora on April 11, 2016


Pan et al. 5

Table 1.  Participant descriptive characteristics.

Group A (n = 11) Group B (n = 11) t p

  M ± SD M ± SD
Age (years) 9.68 ± 1.61 8.49 ± 1.76 1.64 0.17
Height (cm) 137.88 ± 12.25 134.55 ± 12.99 0.62 0.54
Weight (kg) 35.93 ± 11.45 32.00 ± 12.47 0.77 0.45
BMI (kg/m2) 18.56 ± 3.73 17.02 ± 3.66 0.98 0.34
ABCT total raw score 13.45 ± 6.39 14.09 ± 7.23 −0.22 0.83
SRS-2 total raw score 113.82 ± 19.34 111.64 ± 19.33 0.27 0.79
Type of ASD (n, %)
 Autism  6  6  
  Asperger’s syndrome  5  5  
Co-existing symptoms (n, %)
 None  8  8  
 ADHD  3  3  
Medication (n, %)
 None  8  8  
 Yes  3  3  

BMI: body mass index; ABCT: Autism Behavior Checklist–Taiwan version; SRS-2: Social Responsiveness Scale-2–Chinese version; ASD: autism
spectrum disorder; ADHD: attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder; M: mean; SD: standard deviation.

after-school therapy, comorbidity, and medication usage. education, special education, or adapted physical educa-
All but one regularly received after-school group therapy, tion, had experience with children with ASD.
speech therapy, occupational therapy, and physical ther- Each intervention session was conducted with an iden-
apy. After-school group therapy involved speaking freely tical format, consisting of five activities: warm-up (5 min),
in a group, providing feedback to others, learning each motor skills (20 min), particular type of motor skill train-
other’s manners of interacting, and requesting support; ing that was expected to relate to executive function
speech therapy involved language comprehension, expres- (20 min), group games (20 min), and cool-down (5 min).
sion, and communication training; occupational therapy The second and third activities were the two main ses-
consisted of sensory motor integration and general skill sions of the intervention involving specific components
building for participating in activities of daily living, play that are designed to increase the child’s motor skills and
and school; and physical therapy included a passive range executive function. Motor skill training included techni-
of motion exercises, postural control activities, balance cal skills specific to table tennis (e.g. basic forehand and
training, sensory processing and organization skills, and backhand push strokes, serves, and returns) and body
neurodevelopment training. The descriptive characteris- movement skills related to table tennis exercise (e.g. hand
tics are listed in Table 1. bounce, racquet bounce, footwork movement, body con-
trol, balance training, and hand-eye coordination). The
Physical activity intervention first 4 weeks involved focusing on only simple table ten-
nis ball skills followed by 8 weeks wherein simple table
Despite differences in the intervention arrangement, both tennis ball skills were repeated and more complex table
groups undertook 12 weeks of physical training for 24 ses- tennis ball exercises and ball games were added.
sions (two sessions per week, 70 min per session) in a mul- Participants did not learn the rules of the game in the cur-
tipurpose room at the university. Each intervention session rent intervention because they had not yet mastered the
was conducted by the primary investigator, assisted by a necessary skills to play a regulation table tennis match.
trained undergraduate and graduate research assistants; the The particular type of training that was expected to relate
instructor-to-child ratio for both groups ranged from 1:2 to to executive function consisted of the manipulation of the
1:1, depending on attendance. The primary researcher task (e.g. the colors, direction, interval, and speed of the
trained one primary coach and five research assistants and balls) and the social environment (e.g. peers and coaches).
provided a written treatment protocol for administering the An example of how to execute the components of the
intervention. The primary coach was an elite national table treatment approach is described as follows: the coach or
tennis player for 5 years, with 5 years of experience teach- automatic ball projection machine tossed balls of two
ing table tennis exercise to children with and without dis- colors (white and orange); the participants hit the balls
abilities. All research assistants, majoring in physical according to the indicated color in a random order.

Downloaded from aut.sagepub.com at R.M.I.T. Libraries Bundoora on April 11, 2016


6 Autism

Table 2.  Physical activity training program protocol.

Activity Length (min) Content Goal


1. Warm-up  5 Slowly jog around, stretch each major muscle Warm-up and stretching
group
2. Motor skills 20 Table tennis technical skills (forehand and Individually learn and practice the
  backhand counterstroke/serve/return/block/ skill
attack, etc.), locomotor skills (footwork 1-to-2 to 1-to-1 instructor-to-child
movement, e.g., one-, two-, and cross-step), ratio
and object control skills (ball- and racquet-
handling drills, etc.)
3. Motor skills  + executive 20 Individually learn particular task and practice Each child would prepare to
function training (i.e. specific skills assumed to be related to respond by processing actual
inhibitory control and executive function (e.g. white and orange balls visual information, planning his
attention) projected randomly from a specified direction, response, and then programming
and the child was taught to hit only the the appropriate action or inhibition
orange ball so that the child could inhibit their response
movement programming when a white one 1-to-2 to 1-to-1 instructor-to-child
was projected, and vice versa) ratio
4. Group games 20 Cooperative games/activities (table tennis Social interaction and
  balancing shuttle-run, etc.) sportsmanship development
  Fun and challenging activities (running, wind Integrating previously learned skills
sprints, jumping rope, line jumps, etc.) Fitness training
5. Cool-down  5 Slow walk/job/stretch, make comments and Review, reward, and help with
announcements transition

The participants were required to read aloud the number (manual dexterity and upper-limb coordination), (3) body
of balls from 1 to 10 and hit the indicated number (or coordination (bilateral coordination and balance), and (4)
color) of balls, concurrently ignoring the color (or num- strength and agility (running speed and agility and
ber) of the balls. The primary coach guided the transitions strength). Each composite generates gender- and age-spe-
between the five activities. The activities, content, and cific standard subscale scores; the four composite scores
goals of the intervention are listed in Table 2. We mainly were combined to obtain a total motor composite score.
adopted the training protocols from previous studies (Pan The BOT-2 was chosen for this study because it is one
et al., 2015; Tsai et al., 2012); a more detailed training of the most widely used performance-based measures
manual is available on request from the primary researcher. (Bhat et al., 2011) and provides norms for the age group of
children included in this study (Bruininks and Bruininks,
2005). Its validity and reliability were demonstrated in an
Procedures evaluation of children with development coordination dis-
All measurements were conducted in the gym, with a orders, mild intellectual disabilities, and high-functioning
parent or caregiver present. At the initial assessment, all autism or Asperger disorder (Bruininks and Bruininks,
parents completed a supplemental information form 2005). Bruininks and Bruininks (2005) established strong
providing demographic data and a brief developmental internal subtest consistency reliability (r = 0.70–0.80),
history of their child. internal composite consistency reliability (r = 0.80–0.90),
test–retest reliability (r = 0.69–0.80), and interrater relia-
Anthropometric measurements. Height and weight were bility (r = 0.92–0.99) for the BOT-2 manual.
measured to the nearest 0.1 kg and 0.1 cm, respectively,
using a bioelectrical impedance analyzer (MF-BIA8, Executive function.  The computer version of the WCST was
InBody 720, Biospace). used to assess executive function (Heaton and PAR Staff,
2003). The WCST is a measure of abstract reasoning abil-
Motor skill proficiency.  The BOT-2 (Bruininks and Bruin- ity necessitating the ability to shift strategies (Heaton
inks, 2005) measures an array of motor skills in people et al., 1993). The WCST comprises 4 stimulus cards and
aged 4–21 years. It uses a composite structure organized 128 response cards. The stimulus cards display one red tri-
around the muscle groups and limbs involved in move- angle, two green stars, three yellow crosses, or four blue
ments. The following four motor-area composites exist, circles. The 128 response cards contain images combining
and each composite comprises two subscales, each com- various shapes (triangles, stars, crosses, and circles), colors
posed of five to nine items: (1) fine manual control (fine (red, blue, yellow, and green), and numbers (one, two,
motor precision and integration), (2) manual coordination three, or four). In the computer version of the WCST,

Downloaded from aut.sagepub.com at R.M.I.T. Libraries Bundoora on April 11, 2016


Pan et al. 7

the cards are displayed on a screen. The four stimulus Intervention consistency
cards are identical to the card version, except that the par-
ticipants had to choose a response card that matches one of To ensure quality in program implementation, the physical
the four key cards on the basis of its potential characteris- activity intervention was implemented as prescribed and
tics. The basic concept of the WCST is that participants supervised by the primary researcher for each session. If
match response cards to key cards according to a nonspeci- the instructor did not implement the instructional sequence
fied matching rule, which changes each time 10 (of a max- of each component of the intervention as intended, the pri-
imum of 128) response cards have been sorted correctly. mary researcher intervened to assist the instructor. If any
We used this computerized WCST version to reduce the participant engaged in off-task behavior during the pro-
complexity of WCST administration and to increase the gram, the instructor managed the behavior, and the primary
efficiency of data collection. We used six raw scores from researcher assisted the instructor when required.
the WCST indices for analysis: total correct (i.e. an indica- Additionally, anecdotal information (e.g. conversations and
tor of overall performance), perseverative responses (i.e. e-mail correspondence) was collected informally from
number of responses that were perseverative, regardless of instructors when the primary researcher was contacted by
whether they were correct), perseverative errors (i.e. num- instructors to discuss the intervention program. Furthermore,
ber of errors made after a rule change, reflecting tendency each instructor completed a self-report checklist of their
toward perseveration), conceptual-level response (i.e. con- compliance with the teaching steps after each session for
secutive number of correct responses in series of three or assessing whether each component of the intervention was
more, providing insight into the correct sorting principles), implemented precisely and consistently among the partici-
categories completed (i.e. an indicator of correct set shift- pants. By the end of the study, the instructors had com-
ing), and non-perseverative errors (i.e. the number of pleted 100% of the checklist forms, and all of them
errors that were not perseverative, reflecting efficiency in responded “yes” to the steps for each component session.
incorrect set shifting). The first 5 indices reflect concep-
tual formation and flexibility, and the final index repre- Results
sents unsuccessful problem solving with an ineffective
sorting strategy (Polgár et al., 2010). There was no significant difference in the participation
rate between Group A (90.53% ± 6.48%) in Phase I of the
12 weeks and Group B (88.26% ± 6.93%) in Phase II of
Data analysis the 12 weeks (t = 0.80, p = 0.44).
To assess the effects of the physical activity intervention,
analysis of variance (ANOVA) and analysis of covariance Effects of physical activity intervention on motor
(ANCOVA) with a 2 (time: T1 vs T2) × 2 (group: A vs B)
skill proficiency
mixed-model factorial design were conducted for the
BOT-2 and executive function outcomes, respectively. A The results for motor skill proficiency performance are
potential confounding variable (i.e. child age) was used as listed in Table 3. We found no differences between groups
a covariate for the raw scores of the executive function at the study entry (T1) on all demographic (Table 1) and
outcomes because it was considered as an important outcome variables of motor skill proficiency (Table 3).
developmental factor. Age was not used as a covariate
because age-specific standard scores were used for the Phase I (T1−T2).  The ANOVA analysis revealed a signifi-
BOT-2 measures. The least significant difference post hoc cant main effect of time on three motor-area composites (i.e.
test was performed if differences in group or time were manual coordination: F = 23.90, p < 0.01, η2 = 0.46; body
deemed significant. We conducted a follow-up by testing coordination: F = 42.95, p < 0.01, η2 = 0.57; strength and
for simple main effects, with significant interaction agility: F = 63.57, p < 0.01, η2 = 0.61) and the total motor
effects. The effect size was computed and reported as a η2 composite (F = 28.95, p < 0.01, η2 = 0.45) (Table 4). These
value for ANOVA and ANCOVA evaluations. Next, paired scores post-intervention (T2) were significantly higher than
t tests were performed to examine the physical activity those pre-intervention (T1). No significant group differ-
intervention effect in Group B (T2 vs T3) and the poten- ences were found for any of the BOT-2 composite scores.
tial for a sustained physical activity intervention effect in We observed a significant interaction effect for the total
Group A (T2 vs T3). The effect size was computed and motor composite (F = 15.31, p < 0.01, η2 = 0.24) and three
reported as a Cohen’s d for paired t tests. All statistical BOT-2 composites (manual coordination: F = 8.52, p < 0.01,
analyses were conducted using SPSS (version 18.0) for η2 = 0.16; body coordination: F = 11.96, p < 0.01, η2 = 0.16;
Windows (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, USA). To control for strength and agility: F = 15.31, p < 0.01, η2 = 0.20).
possible type I error inflation caused by multiple com- As shown in Figure 2, a follow-up of the simple main
parisons in repeated-measures ANOVA, ANCOVA, and effects revealed that Group A exhibited improved total
paired t tests, the alpha level was set at p < 0.01. motor composite after the physical activity intervention

Downloaded from aut.sagepub.com at R.M.I.T. Libraries Bundoora on April 11, 2016


8 Autism

Table 3.  Motor skill proficiency and executive function by group at three assessments.

T1 Group A (n = 11) Group B (n = 11)

  t p T1 T2 T3 T1 T2 T3
BOT-2
 FMC −0.22 0.83 47.00 ± 11.64 45.18 ± 17.43 58.55 ± 12.60 48.36 ± 16.94 45.91 ± 18.89 54.64 ± 17.70
 MC 0.59 0.56 47.27 ± 10.56 57.00 ± 11.83 55.00 ± 11.23 44.64 ± 10.48 47.09 ± 11.32 53.00 ± 11.40
 BC 0.49 0.63 49.91 ± 9.62 59.91 ± 10.63 55.73 ± 10.98 47.91 ± 9.42 51.00 ± 8.75 58.73 ± 10.58
 SA 0.25 0.80 54.36 ± 5.99 67.64 ± 9.71 66.27 ± 9.47 53.36 ± 11.75 57.00 ± 9.71 66.27 ± 8.33
 TMC 0.43 0.67 49.73 ± 8.39 60.09 ± 10.11 63.64 ± 10.95 48.48 ± 8.51 49.82 ± 5.86 61.55 ± 8.61
WCST
  TC (+) 0.43 0.67 71.64 ± 10.51 80.73 ± 9.37 79.73 ± 8.97 70.00 ± 7.17 70.09 ± 6.12 76.00 ± 5.42
  PR (−) 1.37 0.19 27.00 ± 15.50 16.36 ± 12.58 18.64 ± 12.81 19.55 ± 9.26 17.27 ± 8.44 13.82 ± 9.50
  PE (−) 1.06 0.30 24.36 ± 17.81 18.18 ± 12.73 12.91 ± 11.55 17.82 ± 10.31 13.63 ± 10.28 9.00 ± 9.77
  NPE (−) −0.21 0.84 15.64 ± 8.95 13.45 ± 8.63 9.18 ± 7.55 16.36 ± 7.55 18.27 ± 14.95 7.00 ± 3.61
  CLR (+) −0.14 0.89 55.36 ± 23.19 76.55 ± 10.42 63.27 ± 15.02 56.45 ± 9.83 58.00 ± 11.48 70.09 ± 7.42
  CC (+) −1.94 0.07 4.09 ± 2.26 5.18 ± 1.60 4.91 ± 2.02 5.55 ± 1.04 5.41 ± 1.26 6.00 ± 0.00

BOT-2: Bruininks–Oseretsky Test of Motor Proficiency, Second Edition; FMC: fine motor control; MC: manual coordination; BC: body coordina-
tion; SA: strength and agility; WCST: Wisconsin Card Sorting Test; TMC: total motor composite; TC: total correct; PR: perseverative responses;
PE: perseverative errors; NPE: non-perseverative errors; CLR: conceptual-level response; CC: categories completed; (+): higher scores represent
better performance; (−): lower scores represent better performance.

Table 4.  Summary of analyses from two-way (group × time) ANOVA (BOT-2) and ANCOVA controlling for age (WCST) with
repeated measures on one factor (time).

Group (G) A B Time (T) T1 T2 Statistically significant


interactions (p < 0.01)
BOT-2
 FMC NS 46.09 ± 4.30 47.14 ± 4.30 NS 47.68 ± 3.10 45.55 ± 3.88 NS
 MC NS 52.14 ± 3.22 45.86 ± 3.22 p < 0.01 45.96 ± 2.24 52.05 ± 2.47 p < 0.01
 BC NS 54.91 ± 2.82 49.46 ± 2.82 p < 0.01 48.91 ± 2.03 55.46 ± 2.08 p < 0.01
 SA NS 61.00 ± 2.77 55.18 ± 2.77 p < 0.01 53.86 ± 1.99 62.32 ± 2.07 p < 0.01
 TMC NS 54.91 ± 2.39 49.00 ± 2.39 p < 0.01 48.96 ± 1.80 54.96 ± 1.76 p < 0.01
WCST
  TC (+) NS 75.65 ± 2.56 70.57 ± 2.56 NS 70.82 ± 1.93 75.41 ± 1.71 p < 0.01
  PR (−) NS 21.99 ± 3.68 18.10 ± 3.68 NS 23.27 ± 2.79 16.82 ± 2.33 p < 0.01
  PE (−) NS 21.51 ± 3.92 15.49 ± 3.92 NS 21.09 ± 3.16 15.91 ± 2.53 NS
  NPE (−) NS 14.94 ± 2.85 16.92 ± 2.85 NS 16.00 ± 1.81 15.86 ± 2.64 NS
  CLR (+) NS 64.67 ± 4.22 58.51 ± 4.22 NS 55.91 ± 3.71 67.27 ± 2.36 p < 0.01
  CC (+) NS 4.47 ± 0.41 5.75 ± 0.41 NS 4.82 ± 0.36 5.41 ± 0.27 NS

BOT-2: Bruininks–Oseretsky Test of Motor Proficiency, Second Edition; FMC: fine motor control; MC: manual coordination; BC: body coordina-
tion; SA: strength and agility; WCST: Wisconsin Card Sorting Test; TMC: total motor composite; TC: total correct; PR: perseverative responses;
PE: perseverative errors; NPE: non-perseverative errors; CLR: conceptual-level response; CC: categories completed; NS: not significant; (+): higher
scores represent better performance; (−): lower scores represent better performance.

(T2) compared with Group B (+10.27, F = 8.50, p < 0.01, Phase II (T2−T3). For Group B, the comparison between
η2 = 0.30). Group A had a significantly higher score after T2 and T3 (i.e. intervention condition) revealed signifi-
the intervention (T2) compared with T1 (manual coordina- cant differences for manual coordination (+5.91,
tion: +9.73, F = 15.86, p < 0.01, η2 = 0.49; body coordina- t = 4.21, p < 0.01, Cohen’s d = 0.52), body coordination
tion: +10.00, F = 47.41, p < 0.01, η2 = 0.21; strength and (+7.73, t = 5.56, p < 0.01, Cohen’s d = 0.80), strength
agility: +13.27, F = 50.44, p < 0.01, η2 = 0.43; total motor and agility (+9.27, t = 5.07, p < 0.01, Cohen’s d = 1.02),
composite: +10.36, F = 29.06, p < 0.01, η2 = 0.25). We and the total motor composite (+11.73, t = 5.58, p < 0.01,
found a significant improvement in Group B for manual Cohen’s d = 1.59), indicating a greater improvement in
coordination (+2.46, F = 24.80, p < 0.01, η2 = 0.12) and scores after the physical activity intervention. For Group
strength and agility (+3.64, F = 13.16, p < 0.01, η2 = 0.03) A, the comparison between T2 and T3 (i.e. follow-up
at T2 compared with T1. assessment) revealed no significant difference in any of

Downloaded from aut.sagepub.com at R.M.I.T. Libraries Bundoora on April 11, 2016


Pan et al. 9

Figure 2.  The BOT-2 standard score of two groups of children with ASD before (T1) and after (T2) intervention.
MC: manual coordination; BC: body coordination; SA: strength and agility; TMC: total motor composite.
**p < 0.01.

the BOT-2 composite scores, suggesting that interven- Phase II (T2−T3).  We found a significant improvement in
tion effects were sustained. the total correct (+5.91, t = 5.94, p < 0.01, Cohen’s
d = 1.02) and conceptual-level response (+12.09, t = 4.55,
Effects of physical activity intervention on p < 0.01, Cohen’s d = 1.25) at T3 compared with T2 (i.e.
intervention condition) for Group B. No significant differ-
executive function ence was noted for any of the WCST scores between T2
The results for executive function are listed in Table 3. No and T3 (i.e. follow-up assessment) for Group A, suggest-
differences were noted between groups at the study entry ing that intervention effects were sustained.
(T1) on all demographic (Table 1) and executive function
outcome variables (Table 3).
Discussion
Phase I (T1−T2).  Our ANCOVA results revealed that none We recruited 22 children with ASD in two periods of a
of the WCST indices exhibited any significant main effects physical activity intervention, during which the motor skill
of group. There were no significant main effects of time on proficiency and executive function performance increased
all indices of WCST (Table 4). We noted interactions in after training. For the first period (T1−T2), 11 children
group-by-time differences for the total correct (F = 19.84, with ASD received the intervention (Group A), and the
p < 0.01, η2 = 0.48), perseverative responses (F = 14.21, other 11 children with ASD served as the control (Group
p < 0.01, η2 = 0.41), and the conceptual-level response B). We found significant interaction effects and noted that
(F = 19.98, p < 0.01, η2 = 0.43). the intervention induced improvements in the experimen-
A follow-up of the simple main effect revealed that tal group (Group A) compared with the control group,
Group A performed higher in the total correct (+10.64, which did not receive the intervention (Group B); in the
F = 9.93, p < 0.01, η2 = 0.33) and conceptual-level response total motor composite and three motor-area composites
(+18.55, F = 15.73, p < 0.01, η2 = 0.44) compared with (i.e. manual coordination, body coordination, and strength
Group B after the intervention (Figure 3). For within- and agility); as well as in three indices of the WCST per-
group differences between T1 and T2, Group A had a sig- formance (i.e. total correct, perseverative response, and
nificantly higher score for the total correct (+9.09, conceptual-level response). For the second period (T2−T3),
F = 26.98, p < 0.01, η2 = 0.19) as well as for the concep- we observed improvements in a comparison between T2
tual-level response (+21.18, F = 21.19, p < 0.01, η2 = 0.28), and T3 scores in Group B for the total motor composite
but a significantly lower score for the perseverative and three motor-area composites (i.e. manual coordina-
response (−10.64, F = 61.44, p < 0.01, η2 = 0.14), whereas tion, body coordination, and strength and agility) as well
the performance of Group B was similar both at T1 and as for two of the WCST performance indices (i.e. total cor-
after the intervention period (T2). rect and conceptual-level response). In addition, the

Downloaded from aut.sagepub.com at R.M.I.T. Libraries Bundoora on April 11, 2016


10 Autism

Figure 3.  The WCST raw score of two groups of children with ASD before (T1) and after (T2) intervention.
TC: total correct; PR: perseverative responses; CLR: conceptual-level response.
**p < 0.01.

intervention effect appeared to be sustained for at least skills that were more mature to emerge from the enhanced
12 weeks for Group A. These results are encouraging use of feedback. Likewise, the ability to control the trunk
because the motor skill impairment in ASD acts as a bar- and shift bodyweight led to improvements in running abil-
rier to social interactions and community integration when ity as well as strength and agility in the children. The sig-
diagnosed children engage in physical activities with nificant enhancements in motor skills following the
peers. Furthermore, the positive impact of the current intervention may be relevant for the overall functioning of
physical activity intervention on executive function may children with ASD. MacDonald et al. (2013, 2014) have
provide additional cognitive control for students coping found that motor skills significantly predicted the social
with the restricted behaviors of ASD. Because of the sim- communicative skills of children with ASD; in other
plicity and effectiveness of this physical exercise, it can be words, children with superior motor skills were more
considered an alternative or complementary intervention likely to have greater social communication skills. If the
for improving motor skill proficiency and executive func- motor skills of young children with ASD are improved
tion in children with ASD. through early intervention, they may have greater opportu-
Several studies on children with ASD (Bass et al., 2009; nities for social communicative interactions (e.g. playing
Bremer et al., 2015; MacDonald et al., 2012; Pan, 2010; with peers and siblings) throughout childhood.
Wuang et al., 2010) have found improvements in motor The potential benefits of physical exercise in promot-
skills following long-term physical activity intervention. ing increased executive function in our study are consist-
The cumulative effects from a series of training steps that ent with the findings reported in recent empirical studies
are goal directed, structured, progressive, and interrelated on regular aerobic activity and open-skill exercise in chil-
may account for significant improvements in motor skills. dren with and without disabilities (Chang et al., 2014;
For instance, once a child learned to return the ball (basic Guiney and Machado, 2013; Hilton et al., 2014; Pan et al.,
forehand and backhand return), we introduced dynamic 2015; Tsai et al., 2012). For instance, Hilton et al. (2014)
tasks that were more challenging, such as continually adopted a single-group pretest–posttest research design
returning a ball delivered at various speeds, and from dif- and found improvements in the working memory and
ferent directions by either the coach or an automatic ball metacognition (i.e. the ability to initiate, plan, organize,
projection machine. After mastering these tasks, the child and sustain future-oriented problem solving in working
was more able to utilize body feedback to understand the memory) of seven children with ASD according to the
outcome of movements (feedback), anticipate upcoming parent-reported Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive
events (feedforward), and plan alternative strategies. The Function after 10 weeks of exergaming (i.e. Makoto arena
child was also encouraged to rely on self-evaluations of training). In addition, Tsai et al. (2012) reported a signifi-
performance. Adjusting the speed and direction allowed cant increase in the inhibitory function of 16 children with
patterns of manual coordination and body coordination developmental coordination disorder after 10 weeks of

Downloaded from aut.sagepub.com at R.M.I.T. Libraries Bundoora on April 11, 2016


Pan et al. 11

soccer training compared with no-exercise control groups for underlying deficits (Geurts et al., 2009). The enhanced
composed of children with and without developmental flexibility induced by the current physical activity inter-
coordination disorder. Pan et al. (2015) adopted a between- vention may lead to improvements in early repetitive and
subject experimental design and reported a significant inflexible behaviors associated with ASD. Cognitive fac-
increase in the Stroop Color-Word condition (i.e. the tors are inevitably involved in movement control, espe-
selective attention or inhibition of a dominant response), cially in the sporting context, necessitating greater
WCST total correct, perseverative errors, and other cate- accuracy in decision making and quicker action anticipa-
gories completed in 15 children with attention-deficit tion by players (Chen et al., 2015). Table tennis players
hyperactivity disorder after 12 weeks of table tennis train- must gauge the speed of a moving ball with precision as
ing. The effectiveness of open-skill exercise training may well as the timing for passing the ball. This physical
be related to the concurrent emphasis on physical as well activity intervention appeared to motivate the children
as cognitive training and psychological activation from and was both quick and easy to implement. It may be use-
social interactions with peers during physical activity, ful as a support for traditional therapy, with the potential
which tends to be focused on executive function. Similarly, for greater improvements in motor skill and executive
the table tennis exercise adopted in this study was devel- function performance, which are critical for daily activity
oped on the basis of the principles of constraint-induced participation in children with ASD.
movement therapy (Bonnier et al., 2006), and the children One important limitation of our study was the small
were guided to practice with repetitions and experiment sample, although it represents an improvement over the
with varied strategies and tasks to undercover their latent past predominant reliance on case studies. It would be ben-
motor potential. This may explain the distinctive effects eficial for future studies to adopt a randomized design with
of the table tennis exercise in enhancing attention and a larger sample. Another limitation of this study was the
concentration in the children. use of the BOT-2 and WCST tests, which limited our abil-
According to a review of relevant literature, this study ity to assess other motor skills and executive functions. By
is the first to explore the effect of a chronic open-skill considering only one measure each for motor skills and
physical activity intervention on motor skill and execu- executive functions, we did not assess or control for the
tive function parameters in a sample of children with full range of children’s motor skills and executive func-
ASD. Because of the improvements observed in three tions. Including measures of brain activity that are more
WCST performance indices (i.e. total correct, persevera- direct (e.g. electroencephalograms or functional magnetic
tive responses, and conceptual-level response) for Group resonance imaging) would further support the scientific
A in the comparison between T1 and T2 scores, in addi- rigor of the findings and strengthen their generalizability.
tion to two WCST performance indices (i.e. total correct Moreover, the specific components of the current physical
and conceptual-level response) for Group B in the com- activity intervention that yielded the primary benefits have
parison between T2 and T3 scores, the current physical yet to be clarified (e.g. motor skill training vs motor skills
activity intervention may serve as a valuable addition to and executive function training). A follow-up study could
other forms of therapy for children with ASD with execu- conduct a comparison of traditional table tennis exercise
tive function impairments. Although significant improve- with the current intervention program, which could sepa-
ments in executive function were limited to three indices rate the factors contributing to the motor and cognitive
in Group A, we noted similar gains for the remaining effects. Furthermore, we lacked a quantitative measure-
three indices, but they did not reach a level of signifi- ment for exertion or exercise intensity, which has been
cance compared with improvements in the control group. linked to the effect magnitude in certain cases. Future stud-
A larger sample or a longer intervention duration may ies could quantify exertion according to intensity, perhaps
yield improvements in the other indices of executive by measuring the heart rate and energy expenditure (e.g.
function. It is also possible that a larger effect could be using an accelerometer). Finally, investigating additional
observed if ASD-diagnosed participants were to demon- outcomes such as improved social skills may yield useful
strate a greater degree of executive function impairment findings. As noted, Bass et al. (2009) found improved
at the baseline. Nevertheless, our findings directly imply social functioning in 19 children with ASD after a 12-week
that children with ASD may derive advantages in flexi- horseback riding intervention, and Pan (2010) found
bility performance after participating in a physical activ- improvements in social behaviors in 16 children with ASD
ity intervention through table tennis exercise. In our after 10 weeks of aquatic group exercise. Examining the
study, we considered the WCST indices to reflect flexi- potential benefits of the current physical activity training
bility in reasoning and executive functioning. Lopez in the social domain for ASD may be beneficial because it
et al. (2005) noted that the ability to shift rules during a is possible to control several variables in table tennis exer-
card-sorting task is correlated with restricted behavioral cise that can moderate outcomes (e.g. social environment:
patterns in people with ASD. Greater inflexibility in individual vs collaborative vs competitive play).
behaviors among people with ASD acts as the foundation MacDonald et al. (2013, 2014) found that object control

Downloaded from aut.sagepub.com at R.M.I.T. Libraries Bundoora on April 11, 2016


12 Autism

motor skills as well as fine and gross motor skills predicted skills and early academic achievement. Early Childhood
calibrated autism severity significantly, implying that Research Quarterly 29: 411–424.
social and motor skills should be considered in early inter- Bhat AN, Landa RJ and Galloway JC (2011) Current perspec-
ventions focused on the phenotypic characteristics of tives on motor functioning in infants, children, and adults
with autism spectrum disorders. Physical Therapy 91:
ASD, because motor skills can hinder success in early
1116–1129.
interventions. This implication underscores the need for
Bonnier B, Eliasson AC and Krumlinde-Sundholm L (2006)
additional well-designed comparative studies that clarify Effects of constraint-induced movement therapy in ado-
the role of potential factors. Such additional information lescents with hemiplegic cerebral palsy: a day camp
might guide the development of interventions that are model. Scandinavian Journal of Occupational Therapy
more refined, perhaps enabling greater precision in tailor- 13: 13–22.
ing interventions to meet the needs of individual patients Bremer E, Balogh R and Lloyd M (2015) Effectiveness of
(e.g. emphasizing social behavioral changes or cognitive a fundamental motor skill intervention for 4-year-old
and motor skill performance as required). children with autism spectrum disorder: a pilot study.
Our study provides initial evidence that 12 weeks of Autism: International Journal of Research and Practice
physical activity intervention through table tennis exercise 19: 980–991.
Bruininks RH and Bruininks BD (2005) Bruininks-Oseretsky
exerted a positive effect in enhancing motor skill profi-
Test of Motor Proficiency. 2nd ed. Minneapolis, MN: NCS
ciency and executive function in children with ASD. These
Pearson.
motor skill and executive function enhancements further Chan AS, Sze SL, Siu NY, et al. (2013) A Chinese mind-body
elucidate the potential application of table tennis training exercise improves self-control of children with autism: a
as a complementary intervention for rehabilitating motor randomized controlled trial. PLoS ONE 8(7): e68184.
skill disturbances and for increasing executive function in Chang YK, Hung CL, Huang CJ, et al. (2014) Effects of an
children with ASD or other motor skill and executive func- aquatic exercise program on inhibitory control in children
tion problems. Additional research should be conducted to with ADHD: a preliminary study. Archives of Clinical
replicate and extend these findings for identifying the Neuropsychology 29: 217–223.
physical activity components potentially influencing Chen YC, Wu CC and Shih YL (2015) Effects of volleyball train-
motor skill and executive function outcomes and for exam- ing on visual time perception. Physical Education Journal
48: 105–116.
ining the applied utility of physical activity intervention
Constantino JN and Gruber CP (2012) Social Responsiveness
through table tennis exercise for ASD populations.
Scale. 2nd ed. Torrance, CA: Western Psychological
Services.
Acknowledgements
Crova C, Struzzolino I, Marchetti R, et al. (2014) Cognitively
The authors gratefully thank all the children who participated in challenging physical activity benefits executive function
this study, parents of children for their supports, and research assis- in overweight children. Journal of Sports Sciences 32(3):
tants who helped with data collection and other contributions. 201–211.
Diamond A and Lee K (2011) Interventions shown to aid execu-
Funding tive function development in children 4 to 12 years old.
Science 333: 954–969.
This research was supported by MOST 103-2410-H-017-
Fournier KA, Hass CJ, Naik SK, et al. (2010) Motor coordina-
026-MY3.
tion in autism spectrum disorders: a synthesis and meta-
analysis. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders
References
40: 1227–1240.
Ament K, Mejia A, Buhlman R, et al. (2015) Evidence for speci- Gapin JI, Labban JD and Etnier JL (2011) The effects of physi-
ficity of motor impairments in catching and balance in chil- cal activity on attention deficit hyperactivity disorder symp-
dren with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental toms: the evidence. Preventive Medicine 52: S70–S74.
Disorders 45: 742–751. Geurts HM, Corbett B and Solomon M (2009) The paradox of
American Psychiatric Association (APA) (2013) Diagnostic and cognitive flexibility in autism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences
Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 5th ed. Washington, 13: 74–82.
DC: APA. Gotham K, Pickles A and Lord C (2009) Standardizing ADOS
Anderson-Hanley C, Tureck K and Schneiderman RL (2011) scores for a measure of severity of autism spectrum disor-
Autism and exergaming: effects on repetitive behaviors and ders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 39:
cognition. Psychology Research and Behavior Management 693–705.
4: 129–137. Green D, Charman T, Pickles A, et al. (2009) Impairment in
Bass MM, Duchowny CA and Llabre MM (2009) The effect of movement skills of children with autistic spectrum disor-
therapeutic horseback riding on social functioning in children ders. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology 51(4):
with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 311–316.
39: 1261–1267. Guiney H and Machado L (2013) Benefits of regular aerobic
Becker DR, Miao A, Duncan R, et al. (2014) Behavioral self- exercise for executive functioning in healthy populations.
regulation and executive function both predict visuomotor Psychonomic Bulletin and Review 20: 73–86.

Downloaded from aut.sagepub.com at R.M.I.T. Libraries Bundoora on April 11, 2016


Pan et al. 13

Heaton RK and PAR Staff (2003) Wisconsin Card Sorting Test: Pan CY, Tsai CL, Chu CH, et al. (2015) Effects of physical
Computer Version 4, Research Edition (WCST: CV4). exercise intervention on motor skills and executive func-
Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources. tions in children with ADHD: a pilot study. Journal of
Heaton RK, Chelune GJ, Talley JL, et al. (1993) Wisconsin Card Attention Disorders. Epub ahead of print 2 February. DOI:
Sorting Test Manual: Revised and Expanded. Lutz, FL: 10.1177/1087054715569282.
Psychological Assessment Resources. Pesce C (2012) Shifting the focus from quantitative to qualitative
Hilton CL, Cumpata K, Klohr C, et al. (2014) Effects of exer- exercise characteristics in exercise and cognition research.
gaming on executive function and motor skills in children Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology 34: 766–786.
with autism spectrum disorder: a pilot study. American Polgár P, Réthelyi JM, Bálint S, et al. (2010) Executive func-
Journal of Occupational Therapy 68: 57–65. tion in deficit schizophrenia: what do the dimensions of
Hume K, Loftin R and Lantz J (2009) Increasing independence in the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test tell us? Schizophrenia
autism spectrum disorders: a review of three focused inter- Research 122: 85–93.
ventions. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders Pugliese C, Anthony L, Strang J, et al. (2015) Increasing adap-
39: 1329–1338. tive behavior skill deficits from childhood to adolescence in
Krug DA, Arick JR and Almond PJ (2008) Autism Behavior autism spectrum disorder: role of executive function. Journal
Checklist-Taiwan Version (ABCT) (trans. YY Wu and GY of Autism and Developmental Disorders 45: 1579–1587.
Huang). Austin, TX: PRO-ED. Rajendran G and Mitchell P (2007) Cognitive theories of autism.
Lloyd M, MacDonald M and Lord C (2013) Motor skills of tod- Developmental Review 27: 224–260.
dlers with autism spectrum disorders. Autism: International Reed P, Watts H and Truzoli R (2013) Flexibility in young
Journal of Research and Practice 17: 133–146. people with autism spectrum disorders on a card sort task.
Lopez BR, Lincoln AJ, Ozonoff S, et al. (2005) Examining the Autism: International Journal of Research and Practice
relationship between executive functions and restricted 17(2): 162–171.
repetitive symptoms of autistic disorder. Journal of Autism Robinson S, Goddard L, Dritschel B, et al. (2009) Executive
and Developmental Disorders 35: 445–460. functions in children with autism spectrum disorders. Brain
Lord C, DiLavorne PC and Risi S (2002) Autism Diagnostic and Cognition 71: 362–368.
Observation Schedule. Los Angeles, CA: Western Sachse M, Schlitt S, Hainz D, et al. (2013) Executive and visuo-
Psychological Services. motor function in adolescents and adults with autism
Lord C, Rutter M and LeCouteur A (1994) Autism Diagnostic spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental
Interview-Revised: a revised version of a diagnostic Disorders 43: 1222–1235.
interview for caregivers of individuals with possible per- Staples KL and Reid G (2010) Fundamental movement skills
vasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and and autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and
Developmental Disorders 24: 659–685. Developmental Disorders 40: 209–217.
McClelland MM, Cameron CE, Duncan R, et al. (2014) Staples KL, MacDonald M and Zimmer C (2012) Assessment of
Predictors of early growth in academic achievement: the motor behavior among children and adolescents with autism
head-toes-knees-shoulders task. Frontiers in Psychology 5: spectrum disorders. International Review of Research in
Article 599. Developmental Disabilities 42: 179–214.
MacDonald M, Esposito P, Hauck J, et al. (2012) Bicycle train- Tsai CL, Wang CH and Tseng YT (2012) Effects of exercise
ing for youth with Down syndrome and autism spectrum intervention on event-related potential and task perfor-
disorders. Focus on Autism and Other Developmental mance indices of attention networks in children with devel-
Disabilities 27(1): 12–21. opmental coordination disorder. Brain and Cognition 79:
MacDonald M, Lord C and Ulrich DA (2013) The relationship of 12–22.
motor skills and social communicative skills in school-aged Verburgh L, Königs M, Scherder EJA, et al. (2014) Physical
children with autism spectrum disorder. Adapted Physical exercise and executive functions in preadolescent chil-
Activity Quarterly 30: 271–282. dren, adolescents and young adults: a meta-analysis. British
MacDonald M, Lord C and Ulrich DA (2014) Motor skills and cali- Journal of Sports Medicine 48(12): 1–8.
brated autism severity in young children with autism spectrum Whyatt CP and Craig CM (2012) Motor skills in children aged
disorder. Adapted Physical Activity Quarterly 31: 95–105. 7–10 years, diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder. Journal
Pan CY (2010) Effects of water exercise swimming program on of Autism and Developmental Disorders 42: 1799–1809.
aquatic skills and social behaviors in children with autism World Health Organization (2001) International Classification
spectrum disorders. Autism: International Journal of of Functioning, Disability and Health. Geneva: World
Research and Practice 14(1): 9–28. Health Organization.
Pan CY (2014) Motor proficiency and physical fitness in ado- Wuang YP, Wang CC, Huang MH, et al. (2010) The effective-
lescent males with and without autism spectrum disorders. ness of simulated developmental horse-riding program in
Autism: International Journal of Research and Practice 18: children with autism. Adapted Physical Activity Quarterly
156–165. 27: 113–126.

Downloaded from aut.sagepub.com at R.M.I.T. Libraries Bundoora on April 11, 2016

You might also like