You are on page 1of 13

Gifted and Talented International

ISSN: 1533-2276 (Print) 2470-9565 (Online) Journal homepage: https://www.tandfonline.com/loi/ugti20

Creative ideation and motivated strategies for


learning of academically talented students in
Greek secondary school

Dimitrios Zbainos & Vassiliki Beloyianni

To cite this article: Dimitrios Zbainos & Vassiliki Beloyianni (2018): Creative ideation and
motivated strategies for learning of academically talented students in Greek secondary school,
Gifted and Talented International, DOI: 10.1080/15332276.2018.1547620

To link to this article: https://doi.org/10.1080/15332276.2018.1547620

Published online: 25 Dec 2018.

Submit your article to this journal

Article views: 7

View Crossmark data

Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at


https://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=ugti20
GIFTED AND TALENTED INTERNATIONAL
https://doi.org/10.1080/15332276.2018.1547620

Creative ideation and motivated strategies for learning of academically talented


students in Greek secondary school
Dimitrios Zbainos and Vassiliki Beloyianni
Harokopio University of Athens

ABSTRACT KEYWORDS
A consistent body of research has indicated that intrinsic motivation, self-regulation, and creative Motivational learning
ideation tend to facilitate academic performance. This article examines differences in self- strategies; self-regulation;
regulated learning strategies, motivational beliefs, and creative ideation among academically motivational beliefs;
creativity; ideation;
talented students, high achievers, and ordinary achieving students in Greek secondary school.
academic performance
To assess the relationship between motivational strategies for learning, ideational behavior, and
academic performance, a sample of 287 students between the ages of 13 and 18 completed the
Runco Ideational Behavior Scale and the Motivated Strategies for Learning Questionnaire.
Furthermore, their school grades were used to provide data about their academic performance.
The results indicated that academically talented students tended to use more self-regulated
learning strategies and displayed higher self-efficacy and stronger motivational beliefs. Creative
ideation was found to be negatively correlated with academic achievement, especially for low and
moderate achievers. In conclusion, according to the results, high academic performance appeared
to be related to higher academic self-efficacy, intrinsic motivation, self-regulation, and use of
cognitive strategies. Nevertheless, it appeared to be related to lower creative ideation.

Introduction soon after youngsters have begun learning a new set


of skills, it becomes possible to assess their perfor-
Academic achievement refers to the outcomes of mances normatively, comparing them with others
education and, specifically, to the extent to which who have been learning for an approximately equal
a student reaches the main goals of an educational amount of time. In schools, such assessments can
system (Ward, Stoker, & Murray-Ward, 1996). begin as early as kindergarten… During their first
years in school, some students can obtain grades
Usually, academic performance is measured by
within the top 10 per cent of their class and, conse-
continuous assessment and/or exams and is quently, be labeled academically talented” (p. 124)
coded as a grade point average (O’Connor,
Chassie, & Walther, 1980, Ward et al., 1996). In this regard, a consistent body of research has
However, despite the variety of academic objec- attempted to determine the psychological qualities
tives in different educational contexts, every school and environmental factors that influence academic
has its own high and low achievers (Allen, 2005, performance and set exceptionally high achievers
Ward et al., 1996). or “academically talented students” apart (e.g.,
According to Gagne’s Differentiated Model of Anwar, Shamim-ur-Rasool, & Haq, 2012;
Giftedness and Talent (DMGT), the term talent Grolnick, Ryan, & Deci, 1991; Kosnin, 2007).
describes the advanced mastery of abilities and In relation to the possible psychological con-
skills that are systematically developed in at least structs, motivational beliefs and self-regulated stra-
one specific field of activity to a degree that places tegies in learning have been extensively studied as
the individual in the top 10% in comparison with variables that strongly affect academic achievement
their peers. Children with high achievement can be (e.g., Kosnin, 2007; Kusurkar, Ten Cate, Vos,
characterized as academically talented (Gagné, Westers, & Croiset, 2013; Lemos & Verissimo, 2013).
2001, 2005). According to Gagné (2004), academic According to Pintrich’s expectancy-value model
talent can be identified even in early years: of motivation, there are three fundamental concepts

CONTACT Vassiliki Beloyianni vbeloyianni@hua.gr Venizelou 70, Kallithea 17676, Athens, Greece
© 2018 World Council for Gifted and Talented Children
2 D. ZBAINOS AND V. BELOYIANNI

that determine students’ general motivational two students of their class. The remaining parade
response to specific tasks (Pintrich, 1988, 1989; consists of moderate or low achievers, who have
Pintrich & DeGroot, 1990), namely (1) an expec- never received any distinction for their academic per-
tancy component, (2) a value component, and (3) formance (Vouyoukas, 2007).
an affective component. The expectancy component According to the most recent report of Hellenic
applies to students’ perceptions and beliefs about Statistical Authority (2015), in 2014 general sec-
their ability to accomplish tasks and reach goals, ondary education included 1,794 lower secondary
generally known as self-efficacy (Pintrich & and 1,306 upper secondary schools with 310,389
DeGroot, 1990; Pintrich, Marx, & Boyle, 1993). The and 241,905 students, respectively. Since only the
value component relates to students’ beliefs about two highest achieving students of each school bear
the significance and interest of the task (Markou & the national flag each school year, it is estimated
Philippou, 2005; Pintrich & DeGroot, 1990), and the that flag bearers represent only 1.16% (N = 3,588)
affective component refers to the specific emotional of the lower secondary students and 1.08%
responses to a task (Pintrich & DeGroot, 1990). Of (N = 2,612) of the upper secondary students,
course, there are a variety of affective responses that approximately. Therefore, flag bearers undoubt-
might be expressed in response to a task. However, edly belong to the top 10% of achievers of each
in school settings, test anxiety, defined as a condition school and can be considered to be academically
of severe worry that negatively affects performance talented according to Gagne’s definition of talent.
during a test, is the most common (Metallidou & Although flag bearers have attracted social and
Vlachou, 2007; Pintrich et al., 1993). media attention in Greece, they have never been on
Within school context, self-regulation and motiva- the focus of psycho-educational research. In this
tional beliefs are customarily considered factors that respect, this study examined the relationships between
facilitate academic performance. A consistent body of self-regulation, intrinsic motivation, and creative
research has indicated substantive links between self- ideation among these three subgroups of secondary
regulating learning and academic performance in school students. Moreover, the possible relationship
young children and adolescents (e.g., Broadbent & between academic achievement, self-regulated learn-
Poon, 2015; McClelland & Cameron, 2011; Pintrich ing, motivational beliefs, and creativity was investi-
& DeGroot, 1990). Other authors have identified the gated. Since research about the relationship between
strong and positive predictive effects of intrinsic moti- these factors is still limited, this study sought to
vation on the academic performance of adolescents answer the following research questions:
(Kusurkar et al., 2013; Lemos & Verissimo, 2013).
● Do flag bearers, attendants, and regular students,
i.e., students without any distinctions, vary sig-
Academic classification of Greek secondary
nificantly in their academic achievement?
students
● Do flag bearers, attendants, and regular stu-
It is a common practice around the world for high dents vary in terms of their motivational beliefs,
achievers to receive distinctions and prizes from their creative ideation, and self-regulated learning?
schools and local community. In Greece, the most ● What are the inter-relationships among self-
prestigious distinction for high achievers is to bear regulated learning strategies, motivational
the Greek flag in parades during the two major beliefs, creative ideation, and academic per-
national celebrations. Specifically, flag bearers repre- formance of secondary school students?
sent a small minority of the total school population, ● Do self-regulated learning strategies, motiva-
characterized by their exceptionally high academic tional beliefs, and creative ideation have an
achievements. Each flag bearer in Greek parades is effect on the academic achievement of flag
surrounded by a group of five attendants, who are also bearers, attendants, and regular students?
distinguished for their high academic performance. ● Do motivational beliefs, self-regulated learn-
However, attendants represent a wider part of the ing strategies, and academic achievement
student population, and, although they are high aca- explain the creative ideation of flag bearers,
demic achievers, they have never been among the top attendants, and regular students?
GIFTED AND TALENTED INTERNATIONAL 3

Results from this study may indicate some of brainstorming (Fink & Benedek, 2014; Fink,
the psychological differences between low and Benedek, Grabner, Staudt, & Neubauer, 2007;
high achievers and reveal some key recommenda- Runco & Jaeger, 2012).
tions for ensuring continued academic success in Thus creative ideation is a key process that,
teaching practice. among others, determines the qualitative and quan-
titative features of the creative responses of an
individual (Fink & Benedek, 2014; Runco, 1992;
Creativity and creative ideation
Runco et al., 2001). However, since creativity is
In recent years, some research has suggested that a complex and multifactorial phenomenon, many
creativity may be related to academic achievement. authors have highlighted the difficulties and chal-
It is not clear, however, the extents to which creative lenges in assessing creative ideation (Plucker &
abilities are being taken into account in academic Renzulli, 1999; Runco et al., 2001). Although
assessments (e.g., Anwar et al., 2012, Kuncel, Hezlett, many different measuring instruments of divergent
& Ones, 2004). Creativity and innovation are becom- thinking have been recommended as a means to
ing more valued in modern knowledge societies, assess creative ideation, self-reported scales have
since they are recognized as essential prerequisites been found to possess a moderate predictive validity
for economic, social, technological, and scientific when listing criteria related to ideational behaviors
progress within different cultural and working con- (Batey & Furnham, 2006; Runco & Basadur, 1993).
texts (Craft, 2005; Kozbelt, Beghetto, & Runco, 2010; In fact, some self-reported scales that can measure
Leung & Wang, 2015). Hence, as creativity and creative ideation assess many different types of
innovation are deemed to be both social values and ideational behavior, while non-self-reported mea-
skills by modern socio-educational systems, their sures, such as batteries and experiments, frequently
development holds a prime position in the hierarchy report specific expressions and focus on limited
of objectives in school curricula (Amabile, 1996; types of creative ideation (Runco et al., 2001). Yet
Kozbelt et al., 2010). self-reported inventories also have their limitations,
Assuredly, numerous definitions and theories since they are assessing participants’ beliefs about
have been offered to explain the mechanism of crea- their behavior, rather than behaviors themselves.
tive expression. In general, these various theoretical According to previous research, creative idea-
approaches to creativity can be considered in terms tion can predict academic performance, especially
of the particular aspect of creativity they emphasize in specific domains, such as open problem solving
(Kozbelt et al., 2010; Sternberg, 2006). According to or creative writing (Asha, 1980; Bentley, 1966;
the most commonly used definition, creativity is the Kuncel et al., 2004; Runco, 1992). The Munich
ability to produce appropriate and original ideas that Model of Giftedness also confirms that high levels
are also useful within a social context (Brown, 1989, of creativity and creative ideation are common
Runco & Jaeger, 2012; Sternberg & Lubart, 1999). traits among gifted students (Heller, Perleth, &
Generally, although creative thinking tends to Lim, 2005).
involve most of the common cognitive processes, Additionally, motivation has been frequently
such as memory, attention, and conjunction, it is shown to be positively correlated to both academic
based on a complex process called divergent thinking performance and creative ideation. For example, crea-
(Plucker & Renzulli, 1999; Runco, 1992; Silvia et al., tive individuals who are high achievers also tend to be
2008). Divergent thinking, also known as creative highly motivated (Amabile, 1996; Bergendahl,
ideation, is described as the process of creating Magnusson, & Bjork, 2015; Kusurkar et al., 2013).
a variety of different novel ideas to specified open Still, even if motivation is a common space between
problems (Runco, 1992; Runco, Plucker, & Lim, creativity and academic achievement, there is lack of
2001). In other words, creative ideation refers to research evincing that academically talented students
the ability to produce numerous alternative and ori- are also creative (Amabile, 1985, 1996).
ginal answers during creative problem solving, Things are complicated when examining the spe-
which is the core for developing strategies to cific relationship between high academic achieve-
improve creative performance, such as ment and creativity, a relationship defined
4 D. ZBAINOS AND V. BELOYIANNI

primarily by the prevailing ideologies in education To ensure that students could participate in the
policy, the organizational structure of schools, and research on only one occasion, each received
other sociocultural factors (Davies et al., 2013). In a personal disposable password to access the ques-
fact, many studies have established that creativity tionnaire online. All told, the response rate of the
influences academic achievement, since specific target population was low, as only 287 of the 600
creative abilities such as flexibility and elaboration, students (47,83%) who received a password filled
which are crucial for problem solving, may enhance out the questionnaires.
success in academic settings (e.g., Ai, 1999; Anwar, The sample consisted of 287 Greek students from
Shamim-ur-Rasool, & Haq, 2012; Naderi, Abdullah, both lower secondary (32.1%, N = 92) and upper
Aizan, Sharir, & Kumar, 2010; Nami, Marsooli, & secondary schools (67.9%, N = 195). The mean age
Ashouri, 2014). However, a similar number of stu- of respondents was 15.9 years, and the gender divi-
dies revealed a nonsignificant negative correlation sion was 64.5% female (N = 185) and 35.5% male
between creativity and academic achievement (e.g., (N = 102). For the purpose of analysis, since the
Edwards & Tyler, 1965; Olatoye, Akintunde, & study focused on differences between academically
Ogunsanya, 2010; Wallach & Kogan, 1965; talented students and their peers, participants were
Yamamoto & Chimbidis, 1966). divided into three subgroups: (1) flag bearers, (2)
In this regard, Niu and Sternberg (2003) argued attendants, and (3) regular students.
that some environmental and cultural factors may In particular, flag bearers constitute 25.4%
affect the relationship between the two variables. (N = 73) of the sample, attendants 36.5%
This indicates that creativity may not be assessed (N = 105), and regular students, who also constituted
as an academic qualification in some cultural and the control group for this study, accounted for 38%
educational contexts and, therefore, is not pro- (N = 109). According to their place of residence,
moted systematically as a specific objective of cur- 26.5% (Ν = 76) of the students were living in the
ricula. Nevertheless, even in contexts where larger urban areas, 45.6% (Ν = 131) in smaller towns,
creativity is a common trait among high achievers, and 9.1% (Ν = 26) in semi-rural locations. Only
such as in Asian countries, the exact relationship 18.8% (Ν = 54) of the sample were living in rural
between creative ideation and academic perfor- communities.
mance is neither clear nor linear (Anwar et al.,
2012). Furthermore, we should not ignore the
Instruments
fact that highly creative students may be under-
achieving (Kim, 2008; Whitmore, 1980), as they Creative ideation
frequently experience difficulties in traditional In order to assess creative ideation, the sample
school environments (Amabile, 1989). filled out the Runco Ideational Behavior Scale
(RIBS) (Runco et al., 2001), a self-reported inven-
tory that contains 23 items (a = .93) on a 5-point
Method Likert scale (1 = never to 5 = very often). All
reported Cronbach’s alphas have been estimated
Participants
for the sample of the current study.
The sampling procedure commenced with forma-
tive letters being sent to various Greek Regional Motivated strategies for learning
Education Authorities during academic year Both motivational beliefs and self-regulated learning
2014–2015. These letters were subsequently dis- strategies were assessed by adapting the Motivated
tributed to school principals informing them of Strategies for Learning Questionnaire (MSLQ;
the objectives of the research and inviting them Pintrich & DeGroot, 1990). This is a self-reported
to forward a request to potential participants with inventory of 44 items divided into two subscales,
similar demographic profiles from each of the each of them examining different aspects of moti-
three categories of students required for this vated learning strategies. Specifically, the subscale of
research: flag bearers, attendants, and regular “Motivational beliefs” examines (1) self-efficacy
students. (nine items; a = .95), (2) intrinsic value (nine items;
GIFTED AND TALENTED INTERNATIONAL 5

a = .92), and (3) test anxiety (four items; a = .87), efficacy, F(2, 284) = 24.98, p < .01, η2 = .15, and on
whereas “Self-regulated learning strategies” assesses intrinsic value, F(2, 284) = 22.36, p < .01, η2 = .14. In
(4) cognitive strategies use (13 items; a = .89) and (5) order to investigate the source of the main effects,
self-regulation (nine items; a = .83). Students were Bonferroni’s post hoc tests for multiple comparisons
instructed to respond to the items on a 7-point Likert were performed at the .05 significance level. These
scale (1 = not at all true of me to 7 = very true of me). results demonstrated that in relation to their self-
Cronbach’s alphas reported above have been esti- efficacy and intrinsic value, there were significant
mated for the sample of the present study. differences between regular students and attendants
Furthermore, academic performance was mea- and between regular students and flag bearers. Thus
sured by the school grades each participant there was no significant difference between flag
received in the academic year prior to this research bearers and attendants (see Table 2).
commencing. Afterwards, data were further analyzed to
investigate possible differences between sub-
groups concerning their self-regulated learning
Results strategies. The analysis of variances demonstrated
a significant effect of subgroups on cognitive
Initial statistics
strategy use, F(2, 284) = 21.73, p < .01, η2 = .13,
Initially, in order to examine the potential differ- and self-regulation, F(2, 284) = 29.20, p < .01,
ences in academic achievement between the three η2 = .17. Bonferroni’s post hoc test for multiple
subgroups of the sample, the last school grades of comparisons was performed at the .05 signifi-
the students were analyzed using SPSS 20.0 soft- cance level and revealed that there was no signif-
ware. The results, as presented in Table 1, revealed icant difference between flag bearers and
that there were statistically significant differences attendants, both in terms of cognitive strategy
between each subgroups’ mean scores, as deter- use and self-regulation (see Table 3), whereas
mined by 1-way ANOVA, F(2, 284) = 54.22, p there were significant differences between both
< .01, η2 = .27. Levene’s test of homogeneity of these groups and students without distinction.
variances showed that the variances between the
subgroups were unequal, F(2, 284) = 43.86, p < .01.
Table 2. Descriptives of subgroups’ motivational beliefs.
Bonferroni’s test for post hoc comparisons demon-
95% CI
strated that flag bearers and attendants differed Sub Groups N M SD SE LB UB
significantly to regular students, but not to each Self-efficacy Regular students 109 4.12a 1.37 .13 3.86 4.38
other. Therefore, these results indicated that there Attendants 105 5.14b 1.15 .11 4.92 5.36
Flag bearers 73 5.18b .96 .11 4.95 5.40
was one group of academically talented students,
Total 287 4.77a 1.30 .08 4.61 4.91
which includes both the flag bearers and atten- Intrinsic value Regular students 109 4.14a 1.19 .11 3.92 4.37
dants, and another group of ordinary achievers, Attendants 105 4.95b 1.00 .09 4.76 5.15
Flag bearers 73 5.11b 1.02 .11 4.87 5.35
comprising the regular students. Total 287 4.70a 1.17 .07 4.55 4.82
Further analysis investigated whether the three Test anxiety Regular students 109 3.56 1.60 .15 3.25 3.86
subgroups varied in terms of their motivational Attendants 105 3.60 1.55 .15 3.30 3.90
Flag bearers 73 3.34 1.50 .17 2.99 3.70
beliefs. Total 287 3.52 1.57 .09 3.34 3.70
In general, the analysis of variances demonstrated Note: Means with the same superscripts are significantly different from
that the subgroups had a significant effect on self- each other.

Table 1. Descriptives of subgroups’ academic achievement.


95% CI
Sub Groups N M SD SE LB UB
Academic Achievement Regular students 109 16.69ab 2.35 .22 16.24 17.13
Attendants 105 18.68ac 1.31 .13 18.43 18.94
Flag bearers 73 19.12bc 1.08 .13 18.86 19.37
Total 287 18.04 2.04 .12 17.80 18.27
Note: Means with the same superscripts are significantly different from each other.
6 D. ZBAINOS AND V. BELOYIANNI

Table 3. Descriptives of subgroups’ self-regulated learning Table 5. Pearson’s correlation between motivational beliefs,
strategies. self-regulated learning strategies, creative ideation, and aca-
95% CI demic achievement.
Sub Groups N M SD SE LB UB Goals 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Cognitive strategy Regular 109 4.27a 1.18 .11 4.04 4.50 1. Self-efficacy 1.00
use students 2. Intrinsic .78** 1.00
Attendants 105 5.09b .93 .09 4.91 5.27 value
Flag bearers 73 5.11b .90 .10 4.89 5.32 3. Test anxiety .03 .07 1.00
Total 287 4.79a 1.10 .06 4.66 4.91 4. Cognitive .75** .78** .12* 1.00
Self- regulation Regular 109 3.76a 1.12 .10 3.55 3.98 Strategy Use
students 5. Self- .71** .74** .04 .77** 1.00
Attendants 105 4.71b .96 .09 4.52 4.90 regulation
Flag bearers 73 4.78b 1.03 .12 4.53 5.02 6. Creative .32** .28** .01 .32** .17** 1.00
Total 287 4.37a 1.15 .07 4.27 4.51 ideation
7. Academic .70** .63** .00 .63** .61** .18** 1.00
Note: Means with the same superscripts are significantly different from
Achievement
each other.
Note: *p < .05, **p < .01.

Potential differences between the mean scores of regulation strategies appeared to be highly corre-
each subgroups’ creative ideation were also exam- lated with each other in addition to academic
ined. The analysis of variance demonstrated achievement, while self-reported ideation was low
a significant effect of the subgroups on ideation, or remotely correlated with the other factors. This
F(2, 284) = 6.28, p < .01, η2 = .04. Using indicated that academic performance tends to
Bonferroni’s test at the .05 significance level and relate more with motivational and self-regulating
making multiple comparisons revealed significant abilities than creative ideation, with which there is
differences between regular students and both little correlation.
attendants and flag bearers in relation to creative
ideation, but there were no significant differences
between flag bearers and attendants (see Table 4). Regression analysis
Motivational beliefs, self-regulated learning, crea-
tive ideation, and academic achievement were
Correlations found to be inter-correlated. Specifically, intrinsic
value, self-efficacy, cognitive strategy use, and self-
In order to examine the inter-relationships among regulation were shown to be strongly correlated
self-regulated learning strategies, motivational with academic achievement, while there was a low
beliefs, creative ideation, and academic perfor- correlation between creative ideation and respon-
mance of respondents, the collected data were dents’ academic achievement. In this respect, the
further investigated via Pearson’s r correlations relationships mentioned above were further ana-
and multiple regressions. As seen in Table 5, lyzed to examine whether students’ perception of
there were significant positive correlations among their motivational beliefs, self-regulated learning
all factors, except test anxiety, which was found to strategies, and creative ideation could predict stu-
be significantly and positively correlated only with dents’ academic achievement.
cognitive strategy use. Besides this, it is notable A series of multiple regression analyses indi-
that the rest of the motivational beliefs and self- cated that self-efficacy can be used to explain the
extent of variation in academic performance in the
Table 4. Descriptives of subgroups’ creative ideation. total sample. On the other hand, ideation was
95% CI
found to predict a small percentage of variance
Sub Groups N M SD SE LB UB
in academic performance, but only for regular
Creative Regular 109 2.96a .80 .07 2.81 3.11
ideation students students. Moreover, in relation to self-regulation,
Attendants 105 3.26b .71 .07 3.12 3.40 intrinsic value and cognitive strategy use, regres-
Flag bearers 73 3.28b .58 .06 3.15 3.42
Total 287 3.15 .73 .04 3.07 3.24
sion was found to be insignificant, which suggests
Note: Means with the same superscripts are significantly different from that variance of these factors cannot predict any
each other. differences in academic performance (see Table 6).
GIFTED AND TALENTED INTERNATIONAL 7

Table 6. Multiple regression analysis for variables predicting variance of academic performance of regular students
subgroups’ academic achievement. (sr2 = .26, p < .01), 29% of variance for attendants
Total (N = 287)
(sr2 = .29, p < .01), and 34% for flag bearers (sr2 = .34,
Dependent Predicting SE
Sub Groups Variable variables B B Β p = .01). While ideation was found to negatively
Regular Academic Self-efficacy .86 .84 .50** explain 13% of academic performance’s variation for
students Achievement Intrinsic value .33 .26 .17 regular students (sr2 = −.13, p = .05), it appears to have
(N = 109) Cognitive .37 .26 .19
strategy use no significant predicting effect on the other sub-
Self-regulation −.05 .23 −.03 groups’ academic grades. This suggests that high self-
Creative −.46 .22 −.15* efficacy tends to be a common trait among academi-
ideation
R2 = .52 cally talented and high-achieving students. On the
F = 20.19** other hand, creative ideation appears to negatively
Attendants Self-efficacy .49 .13 .43**
(N = 105) Intrinsic value .82 .16 .06 affect academic performance, but this negative effect
Cognitive .10 .18 .08 was reduced for low and moderate achievers. This
strategy use indicates that ideation may be an underappreciated
Self-regulation .22 .17 .16
Creative −.25 .15 −.13 quality when rating students’ achievement.
ideation The next research question related to whether
R2 = .37
F = 11.38** or not motivational beliefs, self-regulated learning,
Flag bearers Self-efficacy .56 .16 .50** and academic achievement explain students’ crea-
(N = 73) Intrinsic value −.12 .16 −.12 tive ideation. For a deeper investigation of the
Cognitive .33 .20 .28
strategy use relationships between participants’ motivational
Self-regulation −.05 .17 −.04 beliefs, self-regulated learning strategies, academic
Creative −.08 .20 −.04
ideation
achievement, and ideation, a multiple regression
R2 = .29 analysis was performed with ideation as the depen-
F = 5.88** dent variable (see Table 7).
Total Self-efficacy .74 .11 .47**
(N = 287) Intrinsic value .16 .13 .09 On the complete sample, squared semi-partial
Cognitive .29 .14 .16* correlations indicated that academic self-efficacy
strategy use
Self-regulation .16 .13 .09
can explain 16% of creative ideation’s variation
Creative −.18 .12 −.07 (sr2 = .16, p < .01), cognitive strategy use can predict
ideation 19% (sr2 = −.19, p = .001), and self-regulated learning
R2 = .53
F = 65.70** explains approximately17% (sr2 = .17, p < .01).
Note: *p < .05, **p < .01. Furthermore, to see if the model of influences was
akin for all three subgroups, the collected data were
further analyzed. The analysis for each of the sub-
All prediction models were statistically significant groups revealed that cognitive strategy use can explain
in Table 6. To investigate whether the models of effects a similar percentage of variance of ideation of 21% for
were analogous for flag bearers, attendants, and reg- regular students (sr2 = −.21, p < .05) and attendants
ular students, we calculated the specific percentage of (sr2 = −.21, p < .05), but explains only 5% for flag
variation of academic performance. Each of the sig- bearers (sr2 = −.05, p < .05). Other variables have
nificantly predicting variables can be explained by significant predictive strength only for the subgroup
performing squared semi-partial correlations (sr2). of regular students. Namely, academic self-efficacy
The regression analysis revealed that self- was found to predict 28% of variance of ideation
efficacy predicted 27% of variance in academic (sr2 = .28, p = .001), self-regulation 22% (sr2 = −.22, p
performance (sr2 = .27, p < .01), while cognitive < .05) and academic achievement 17% (sr2 = .17,
strategy use explained only 8% of the total sample p < .05).
(sr2 = .08, p < .05).
Therefore, self-efficacy was found to be the stron-
Discussion
ger predictor of academic performance, with similar
predictive strength for all three subgroups. Namely, As discussed in the Results section, flag bearers and
self-efficacy appeared able to explain a 26% of attendants appeared not to differ in any variables
8 D. ZBAINOS AND V. BELOYIANNI

Table 7. Multiple regression analysis for variables predicting in terms of their motivational beliefs, self-regulated
subgroups’ creative ideation. learning strategies, and creative ideation. However,
Total (N = 287)
it is worth noting that all three groups displayed no
Dependent Predicting
Sub Groups Variable variables B SE B Β significant differences in their test anxiety. Previous
Regular Creative Self-efficacy .32 .10 .57** research has also identified that academically
Students ideation Intrinsic value −.04 .12 −.06 talented students tend to display higher motivation
(N = 109) Cognitive .27 .11 .35*
strategy use and self-regulated strategies use (e.g., Broadbent &
Self-regulation −.25 .10 −.34* Poon, 2015; McClelland & Cameron, 2011; Pintrich
Academic −.09 .04 −.25*
Achievement
& DeGroot, 1990), while there has been conflicting
R2 = .20 findings on the creativity of academically talented
F = 5.71** students (Amabile, 1985, 1996). Findings from this
Attendants Self-efficacy .04 .09 .06
(N = 105) Intrinsic value .09 .11 .12 study and previous literature indicated that motiva-
Cognitive .24 .12 .31* tional beliefs, self-regulated learning, and creative
strategy use ideation were factors broadly recognized as critical
Self-regulation −.10 .11 −.13
Academic −.11 .07 −.20 for academic achievement (e.g., Amabile, 1996;
Achievement Pintrich & DeGroot, 1990; Pintrich et al., 1993).
R2 = .07
F = 2.30* Further examination of the relationships between
Flag bearers Self-efficacy .15 .11 .25 motivational beliefs, self-regulated learning strate-
(N = 73) Intrinsic value .07 .10 .13 gies, creative ideation, and academic achievement
Cognitive .16 .12 .25
strategy use revealed a series of positive correlations among all
Self-regulation −.24 .10 −.44* variables, with the exception of test anxiety, which
Academic −.03 .08 −.06
Achievement was found to have a weak positive correlation with
R2 = .04 cognitive strategy use only. Generally, these results
F = 1.44 verify the findings of the established literature,
Total Self-efficacy .16 .06 .29**
(N = 287) Intrinsic value .05 .06 .08 which accepts that creativity, motivation, and self-
Cognitive .23 .07 .35** regulation for learning are related to academic suc-
strategy use
Self-regulation −.19 .06 −.29**
cess (e.g. Asha, 1980; Kuncel et al., 2004; Runco,
Academic −.04 .03 −.12 1992). The insignificant relationship found between
Achievement academic achievement and test anxiety appears to
R2 = .14
F = 10.56** be contrary to previous research, according to
Note: *p < .05, **p < .01. which test anxiety is understood as an affective
component that is slightly, but significantly, related
to academic performance (Metallidou & Vlachou,
examined, which indicated that they tend to behave, 2007; Pintrich et al., 1993).
more or less, as a unified group of academically Moreover, the complicated relationship between
talented students. The results did not totally con- test anxiety and cognitive strategy use has been
firm the academic classification, since there were no contested in previous research (e.g., Bugge, 1977;
statistically significant differences between all sub- Entwistle, 1988; Kondo, 1997). As a result,
groups’ means of academic performance. In this Entwistle (1988) posited that highly anxious stu-
regard, the classification of academically talented dents tend to display increased need for structure
students as flag bearers and attendants appears to and often seek learning strategies and routines to
serve traditional purposes. However, regular stu- improve their performance. However, in practice,
dents were found to differ significantly from both these students eventually adopt a surface approach
flag bearers and attendants in terms of their aca- to cognitive strategy use.
demic achievement. Further analysis revealed that Previous research has also suggested that crea-
academic performance was not the only factor on tive ideation is a predictor of a specific domains of
which academically talented students and regular academic performance (e.g., Asha, 1980; Kuncel
students were found to differ. High achievers and et al., 2004; Runco, 1992). Nevertheless, as findings
regular students also appeared to differ significantly in the literature are at variance with one another,
GIFTED AND TALENTED INTERNATIONAL 9

more research into the relationship between aca- Moreover, this research was based on self-reported
demic success and creativity is needed (Amabile, instruments that have been frequently found to
1985, 1996). suffer from various biases, such as social desirability
Further examination of the data revealed that bias, which may affect the accuracy of participants’
creative ideation and academic performance may responses. Additionally, the cross-sectional data
be related in various ways. In particular, self- collection prevented causal inferences regarding
perception of creative ideation was found to nega- the relationship between creative ideation and aca-
tively affect the academic performance of each demic achievement for Greek secondary students.
group, with academically talented students least Finally, in the absence of other formal methods
likely to be affected. to identify academically talented students in Greek
In this respect, cognitive strategy use and self- secondary education, we defined academically stu-
regulated learning were found to negatively predict dents in terms of their relative academic achieve-
creative ideation. Taking into account that self- ment, which placed them to the top 10% compared
regulated strategies were found to a larger extent to their peers. Talent identification would be more
in high achievers, it could be assumed that acade- valid if it was supported by standardized testing.
mically talented students of the present sample may Considering these limitations, further investiga-
tend to consider themselves less creative. This nega- tion into the wider population of students using
tive relationship could possibly be explained as the a variety of research methods and instruments
effect of specific cultural contexts and educational may be a better means to fully understand the
systems on students. According to Mclnerney psychological background of academically talented
(2008), some educational systems promote self- students.
regulated learning as conformity to norms, engage-
ment to more conventional models of thinking, less
Conclusion and recommendations
experimentation, and more frequent avoidance of
original thinking. In this regard, the Greek educa- To conclude, high academic performance reflects
tional system may have affected highly self- higher academic self-efficacy, intrinsic motivation
regulated learners to have a low perception of and self-regulation, more frequent use of cognitive
their creative ideation. However, this assumption strategies, and rare use of creative ideation, among
requires further investigation and cannot be sup- other factors. Specifically, the findings suggest that
ported by the present study. academic self-efficacy exerts a critical motivational
Academic self-efficacy was also found to be influence on students’ academic achievement. This
a crucial and direct predictor of academic success suggests that self-belief has a vital role in academic
that uniformly affects low, moderate achieving, success. In this regard, psycho-educational strate-
and academically talented students. As an expec- gies for students that promote the ability to suc-
tancy component involving students’ perceptions ceed should be included in teaching practice to
and beliefs about their capacity to accomplish increase academic potential. For instance, model-
tasks and reach goals, academic self-efficacy has ing, verbal persuasion, goal setting, and frequent
been consistently studied as a variable able to performance feedback have been found to increase
explain much about academic performance self-efficacy and self-regulation in academic tasks
(Pintrich & DeGroot, 1990; Pintrich et al., 1993). (Schunk & Zimmerman, 2007).
As mentioned above, even if creativity is a desired
skill for the 21st century, creative ideation was found
Limitations
not to significantly predict academic achievement in
There are certain limitations of this study that secondary school. Few studies have reported
restrict the extent to which the findings can be a relationship between academic achievement and
generalized. First, since the sample for this research creativity. This indicates that in Greek secondary
was selected by convenience, (i.e., selection was schools, teachers need to restrict students’ creative
based on accessibility and proximity to the ideation in order to facilitate their academic achieve-
researchers), the results cannot be generalized. ment, as creative ideation is rated as a non-
10 D. ZBAINOS AND V. BELOYIANNI

appreciated quality in Greek educational system, Amabile, T. M. (1985). Motivation and creativity: Effects of
even if it holds a high position in the hierarchy of motivational orientation on creative writers. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 48(2), 393–399.
aims in school curricula. On the other hand, restrict-
doi:10.1037/0022-3514.48.2.393
ing creativity in school is impractical, as it is cur- Amabile, T. M. (1989). Growing up creative: Nurturing
rently a highly valued skill for social, personal, and a lifetime of creativity. Williston, VT: Crown House.
economic development. Under this scope, cultivat- Amabile, T. M. (1996). Creativity in context. Boulder, CO:
ing creative skills and innovation has to be promoted Westview Press.
within the overall educational process. In this regard, Anwar, M. N., Shamim-ur-Rasool, S., & Haq, R. (2012). A
comparison of creative thinking abilities of high and low
teachers have to be trained to include educational
achievers secondary school students. International
methods and strategies that contribute to cultivating Interdisciplinary Journal of Education, 1(1), 1–6.
a school environment that respects, promotes, and Anwar, M. N., Shamim-ur-Rasool, S., & Haq, R. (2012).
rewards creativity as a value. A comparison of creative thinking abilities of high and
low achievers secondary school students. International
Interdisciplinary Journal of Education, 1(1), 1–6.
Disclosure statement Asha, C. B. (1980). Creativity and academic achievement
among secondary school children. Asian Journal of
No potential conflict of interest was reported by the authors.
Psychology and Education, 6, 1–4.
Batey, M., & Furnham, A. (2006). Creativity, intelligence, and
personality: A critical review of scattered literature.
Notes on contributors Genetic, Social and General Psychology Monographs, 132
Dimitrios Zbainos is an Assistant Professor in Harokopio (4), 355–429. doi:10.3200/MONO.132.4.355-430
University. He graduated the Pedagogic department of Bentley, J. C. (1966). Creativity and academic achievement.
University of Thrace and continued his studies at a postgraduate Journal of Educational Research, 59(1), 269–272.
level at the Institute of education, University of London, doi:10.1080/00220671.1966.10883353
(Diploma in Education, M.A. in Psychology of Education) Bergendahl, M., Magnusson, M., & Bjork, J. (2015). Ideation
where he was awarded a PhD. He has taught in Primary high performers: A study of motivational factors.
Schools, in the Department of Psychology of the University of Creativity Research Journal, 27(4), 361–368. doi:10.1080/
Crete, and in postgraduate courses in the School of Philosophy of 10400419.2015.1088266
University of Athens. His articles have been published in Greek Broadbent, J., & Poon, W. L. (2015). Self-regulated learning
and international journals and he has participated in Greek and strategies and academic achievement in online higher edu-
international conferences. His research interests include themes cation learning environments: A systematic review. The
in Psychology of Education, Creativity, Motivation, Giftedness, Internet and Higher Education, 27, 1–13. doi:10.1016/j.
Individual Differences, Assessment in Education and Educational iheduc.2015.04.007
Curricula. He is the secretary of the International Association for Brown, R. T. (1989). Creativity: What are we to measure? In
Cognitive Education and Psychology and member to the execu- J. A. Glover, R. R. Ronning, & C. R. Reynolds (Eds.),
tive committee of the Hellenic Association of Educational Handbook of creativity (pp. 3–32). New York, NY:
Assessment. Springer Science & Business Media LLC.
Bugge, I. D. (1977). The use of cognitive strategies to attenu-
Vassiliki Beloyianni is a doctorate student in Harokopio ate test anxiety: Attribution of normality self-instructions
University of Athens. She graduated the Classics department and distraction. Theses, Dissertations, Professional Papers.
of University of Peloponnese and also holds a Diploma in Paper 6014.
Special Education and Psychometric Assessment (University Craft, A. (2005). Creativity in schools: Tensions and dilemmas.
of Thessaly) and a Master’s degree in Educational Psychology London, UK: Routledge.
and Practice (Harokopio University of Athens). She is currently Davies, D., Jindal-Snape, D., Collier, C., Digby, R., Hay, P., &
working as a special education teacher. Her research interests Howe, A. (2013). Creative learning environments in education.
span both special education and educational psychology. A systematic literature review. Thinking Skills and Creativity, 8,
80–91. doi:10.1016/j.tsc.2012.07.004
Edwards, M. P., & Tyler, L. E. (1965). Intelligence, creativity,
References and achievement in a nonselective public junior high
school. Journal of Educational Psychology, 56, 96–99.
Ai, X. (1999). Creativity and academic achievement: Αn Entwistle, N. (1988). Motivation and learning strategies: Effective
investigation of gender differences. Creativity Research learning. Educational and Child Psychology, 5(3), 5–20.
Journal, 12(4), 329–337. doi:10.1207/s15326934crj1204_11 Fink, A., & Benedek, M. (2014). EEG alpha power and crea-
Allen, J. D. (2005). Grades as valid measures of academic tive ideation. Neuroscience &Biobehavioral Reviews, 44,
achievement of classroom learning. Thc, 78(5), 218–223. 111–123. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.12.002
GIFTED AND TALENTED INTERNATIONAL 11

Fink, A., Benedek, M., Grabner, R. H., Staudt, B., & Oxford handbook of creativity, innovation and entrepreneur-
Neubauer, A. C. (2007). Creativity meets neuroscience: ship (pp. 245–266). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Experimental tasks for the neuroscientific study of crea- Markou, A., & Philippou, G. (2005). Motivational beliefs, self-
tive thinking. Methods, 42(1), 68–76. doi:10.1016/j. regulated learning and mathematical problem solving. In
ymeth.2006.12.001 H. L. Chick & J. L. Vincent (Eds.), Proceedings of the 29th
Gagné, F. (2001). Transforming gifts into talents: The DMGT conference of the international group for the psychology of
as a developmental theory. High Ability Studies, 15(2), mathematics education (Vol. 3, pp. 297–304). Melbourne,
119–147. doi:10.1080/1359813042000314682 Australia: PME.
Gagné, F. (2004). Transforming gifts into talents: The DMGT McClelland, M. M., & Cameron, C. E. (2011). Self-regulation
as a developmental theory. High Ability Studies, 15(2), and academic achievement in elementary school children.
119–147. doi:10.1080/1359813042000314682 New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 133,
Gagné, F. (2005). From gifts to talents. The DMGT as 29–44. doi:10.1002/cd.302
a developmental model. In R. J. Sternberg & Mclnerney, D. M. (2008). The motivational roles of cultural
J. E. Davidson (Eds.), Conceptions of Giftedness (pp. differences and cultural identity in self-regulated learning.
98–119). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. In D. H. Schunk & B. Zimmermann (Eds.), Motivation and
Grolnick, W. S., Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (1991). The inner self-regulated learning: Theory, research, and applications (pp.
resources for school performance: Motivational mediators of 369–399). New York, NY: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
children’s perceptions of their parents. Journal of Educational Metallidou, P., & Vlachou, A. (2007). Motivational beliefs, cogni-
Psychology, 83, 508–517. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.83.4.508 tive engagement, and achievement in language and mathe-
Hellenic Statistical Authority. (2015). Living Conditions in matics in elementary school children. International Journal of
Greece. Athens, GR: Ministry of Education. Retrieved Psychology, 42(1), 2–15. doi:10.1080/00207590500411179
from http://www.minedu.gov.gr/publications/docs2016/ Naderi, H., Abdullah, R., Aizan, T. H., Sharir, J., & Kumar, V.
27-01-16_elstat%CE%9D%CE%95%CE%9F.pdf (2010). Relationship between creativity and academic
Heller, K. A., Perleth, C., & Lim, T. K. (2005). The Munich achievement: A study of gender differences. Journal of
model of giftedness designed to identify and promote American Science, 6(1), 181–190.
gifted students. In R. J. Sternberg & J. E. Davidson Nami, Y., Marsooli, H., & Ashouri, M. (2014). The relation-
(Eds.), Conceptions of giftedness (pp. 147–170). ship between creativity and academic achievement.
Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences, 114, 36–39.
Kim, K. H. (2008). Underachievement and creativity: Are doi:10.1016/j.sbspro.2013.12.652
gifted underachievers highly creative? Creativity Research Niu, W., & Sternberg, R. J. (2003). Social and school influ-
Journal, 20(2), 234–242. doi:10.1080/10400410802060232 ences on student creativity: The case of China. Psychology
Kondo, D. S. (1997). Strategies for coping with test anxiety. in the Schools, 40(1), 103–114. doi:10.1002/pits.10072
Anxiety, Stress & Coping, 10(2), 203–215. doi:10.1080/ O’Connor, E. J., Chassie, M. B., & Walther, F. (1980).
10615809708249301 Expended effort and academic performance. Teaching of
Kosnin, J. M. (2007). Self-regulated learning and academic Psychology, 7, 231–233. doi:10.1207/s15328023top0704_10
achievement in Malaysian undergraduates. International Olatoye, R. A., Akintunde, S. O., & Ogunsanya, E. A. (2010).
Education Journal, 8(1), 221–228. Relationship between creativity and academic achievement
Kozbelt, A., Beghetto, R. A., & Runco, M. A. (2010). Theories of business administration students in south western poly-
of creativity. In R. J. Sternberg & J. C. Kaufman (Eds.), The technics, Nigeria. African Research Review, 4(3), 134–149.
Cambridge handbook of creativity (pp. 20–47). Cambridge, doi:10.4314/afrrev.v4i3.60164
UK: Cambridge University Press. Pintrich, P. R. (1988). A process-oriented view of student
Kuncel, N. R., Hezlett, S., & Ones, D. S. (2004). Academic motivation and cognition. In J. S. Stark & L. Mets (Eds.),
performance, career potential, creativity, and job perfor- Improving teaching and learning through research. New
mance: Can one construct predict them all? Journal of directions for institutional research (Vol. 57, pp. 55–70).
Personality and Social Psychology, 86(1), 148–161. Greenwich, CT: Jossey-Bass.
doi:10.1037/0022-3514.86.1.148 Pintrich, P. R. (1989). The dynamic interplay of student
Kusurkar, R. A., Ten Cate, T. J., Vos, C. M., Westers, P., & motivation and cognition in the college classroom. In
Croiset, G. (2013). How Motivation affects academic per- C. Ames & M. Maehr (Eds.), Advances in motivation and
formance: A structural equation modelling analysis. achievement: Vol. 6. Motivation enhancing environments
Advances in Health Science Education, 18(1), 57–69. (pp. 117–160). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.
doi:10.1007/s10459-012-9354-3 Pintrich, P. R., & DeGroot, E. (1990). Motivational and
Lemos, M., & Verissimo, L. (2013). The relationships between self-regulated learning components of classroom academic
intrinsic motivation, extrinsic motivation, and achievement, performance. Journal of Educational Psychology, 82(1),
along elementary school. Procedia - Social and Behavioral 33–40. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.82.1.33
Sciences, 112, 930–938. doi:10.1016/j.sbspro.2014.01.1251 Pintrich, P. R., Marx, R. W., & Boyle, R. A. (1993). Beyond cold
Leung, K., & Wang, J. (2015). A cross-cultural analysis of conceptual change: The role of motivational beliefs and class-
creativity. In C. Shalley, M. A. Hitt, & J. Zhou (Eds.), The room contextual factors in the process of conceptual change.
12 D. ZBAINOS AND V. BELOYIANNI

Review of Educational Research, 63(2), 167–199. doi:10.3102/ creativity with divergent thinking tasks: Exploring the reliabil-
00346543063002167 ity and validity of new subjective scoring methods. Psychology
Plucker, J. A., & Renzulli, J. S. (1999). Psychometric approaches to of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 2, 68–85. doi:10.1037/
the study of human creativity. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), 1931-3896.2.2.68
Handbook of creativity (pp. 35–61). New York, NY: Sternberg, R. J. (2006). The nature of creativity. Creativity
Cambridge University Press. Research Journal, 18(1), 87–98. doi:10.1207/s15326934crj1
Runco, M. A. (1992). Children’s divergent thinking and crea- 801_10
tive ideation. Developmental Review, 12(3), 233–264. Sternberg, R. J., & Lubart, T. (1999). The concept of creativ-
doi:10.1016/0273-2297(92)90010-Y ity. prospects and paradigms. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.),
Runco, M. A., & Basadur, M. (1993). Assessing ideational and Handbook of creativity (pp. 3–16). Cambridge, UK:
evaluative skills and creative styles and attitudes. Creativity Cambridge University Press.
and Innovation Management, 2, 166–173. doi:10.1111/ Vouyoukas, C. (2007). An analysis of equality, legislation,
caim.1993.2.issue-3 attitudes and values in education. The case of Greece.
Runco, M. A., & Jaeger, G. J. (2012). The standard definition Mediterranean Journal of Educational Studies, 12(2),
of creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 24(1), 92–96. 115–134.
doi:10.1080/10400419.2012.650092 Wallach, M. A., & Kogan, N. (1965). Models of thinking in
Runco, M. A., Plucker, J. A., & Lim, W. (2001). Development and young children. New York, NY: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.
psychometric integrity of a measure of ideational behavior. Ward, Α., Stoker, H. W., & Murray-Ward, M. (1996).
Creativity Research Journal, 13, 393–400. doi:10.1207/ Achievement and ability tests - definition of the domain.
S15326934CRJ1334_16 Educational Measurement, 2, 2–5.
Schunk, D. H., & Zimmerman, B. J. (2007). Influencing Whitmore, J. R. (1980). Giftedness, conflict, and underachie-
children’s self-efficacy and self-regulation of reading and vement. Boston, MA: Allyn and Bacon.
writing through modeling. Reading and Writing Quarterly, Yamamoto, K., & Chimbidis, M. E. (1966). Achievement,
23, 7–25. doi:10.1080/10573560600837578 intelligence, and creative thinking in fifth grade children:
Silvia, P. J., Winterstein, B. P., Willse, J. T., Barona, C. M., A correlational study. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 12,
Cram, J. T., Hess, K. I., … Richard, C. A. (2008). Assessing 233–241.

You might also like