You are on page 1of 16

Sex Roles (2016) 74:512–526

DOI 10.1007/s11199-015-0534-5

ORIGINAL ARTICLE

The Price Mothers Pay, Even When They Are Not Buying
It: Mental Health Consequences of Idealized Motherhood
Angie Henderson 1 & Sandra Harmon 1 & Harmony Newman 1

Published online: 2 September 2015


# Springer Science+Business Media New York 2015

Abstract Drawing on previous work on the relationship be- Keywords Motherhood . Mental health . Gender . Intensive
tween intensive mothering ideologies and mental health out- mothering . Feminist sociology
comes, the present study analyzed the relationship between
the pressure to be the perfect mother and psychological well-
being for modern mothers. Specifically, this study suggests Introduction
that even women who do not subscribe to these ideologies
are at-risk for experiencing increased stress and anxiety, and While the last 50 years have demonstrated progress towards
decreased self-efficacy in the face of the pressure to be perfect equality for U.S. women, such as improved educational attain-
and guilt for not living up to high mothering expectations. The ment and higher participation in the workforce, equality in
sample for this study was recruited using a snowball sampling other areas has arguably stalled, especially when it comes to
technique via e-mail and an online survey instrument. The the dominant ideologies of perfection associated with mother-
final sample included 283 mothers aged 18–50 mostly from hood (e.g., Hays 1996; Henderson et al. 2010; Newman and
the West (45 %) and Midwest (29 %) regions of the U.S., but Henderson 2014). Feminist scholars in psychology, human
also including the South (18 %) and Northeast (8 %). development, and sociology – among others – have demon-
Hierarchical linear regression results indicate that mothers strated that families exist in two spheres, both public and pri-
who experience the pressure to be perfect experience lower vate (Ferree 1990; Walker 1999), which means that mothers’
self-efficacy and higher levels of stress. Mothers who experi- experiences cannot be analyzed without taking prevailing gen-
ence guilt for not meeting parenting expectations also experi- der ideologies into account (c.f., Arendell 1999, 2000; Bobel
ence lower self-efficacy, higher levels of stress and higher 2002; Douglas and Michaels 2004; Newman and Henderson
levels of anxiety. Contrary to prior research, intensive moth- 2014; Warner 2005). Although the data for this project are
ering beliefs were not a significant predictor of poorer mental embedded within the dominant ideology of motherhood in
health. The results from this study indicate that internalizing the United States, these ideologies are undoubtedly prevalent
guilt and the pressure to be the perfect mother are detrimental in other countries as well, including but not limited to the
for mothers regardless of whether or not they subscribe to United Kingdom (Duberley and Carrigan 2012; Vincent
intensive motherhood ideologies. This study also emphasizes 2010) and Australia (Johnson 2014; Lupton 2011, 2012;
the importance of framing motherhood with a feminist socio- Williams et al. 2013). Indeed, many scholars around the globe
logical lens to critique the dominant ideologies of motherhood have identified that this hegemonic ideology of motherhood is
and the detrimental effects on women. based on what Freidan (1963) first called the feminine mys-
tique – women’s passive acceptance of the limited roles of
wife and mother (Allen et al. 2013).
* Angie Henderson Thirty years after Freidan (1963) named Bthe problem that
angela.henderson@unco.edu has no name^ (p. 151), Hays (1996) analyzed the emotional
labor and demanding expectations of motherhood in the U.S.
1
Department of Sociology, University of Northern Colorado, Campus She included the experiences of working mothers in her sam-
Box 142, Greeley, CO 80639, USA ple, named the practice Bintensive mothering,^ and defined
Sex Roles (2016) 74:512–526 513

maternal expectations as child-centered, expert-guided, emo- alternative perspectives of motherhood have emerged among
tionally absorbing, labor intensive, and financially expensive such groups which Arendell (1999) refers to as Bdeviancy
(p. 46). Feminist theorists argue that these expectations have discourses^ (p. 1195). Arendell utilized interview data from
intensified even since Hays (1996) first wrote about them, mothers to suggest that the presence of such discourses –
referring to the contemporary U.S. experience of motherhood articulations of motherhood developed in response to the
as new momism (Douglas and Michaels 2004). They argue dominant model – highlights the hegemonic power of inten-
that mothers are influenced by media messages about good sive mothering. That is, even when mothers are not drawing
and bad celebrity moms, which forces them to put on Bthe specifically on the ideology of intensive motherhood, they are
doting, self-sacrificing mother and wear it at all times^ often responding to it as the dominant ideal (Newman and
(Douglas and Michaels 2004; p. 6). Warner (2005) echoes this Henderson 2014). In order to capture the variation among
analysis in her work examining the mommy mystique, where women’s responses to this project, we control for age,
mothers find themselves consumed with stress, anger, and race/ethnicity, marital and work status, income, and highest
guilt for not meeting the unrealistically high expectations of level of education.
motherhood. These socio-cultural theories provide us the lens It is clear from this body of work that the discourse of U.S.
with which to understand how mothers reflect upon their motherhood has implications for mothers’ well-being, partic-
sense of self and performance as a mother in a particular social ularly for those who identify as perfectionist parents and also
context, a context framed by powerful institutions such as the those who subscribe to intensive mothering. However, from a
mass media (c.f., Reese et al. 2003). It is upon this theoretical feminist sociological perspective, it is important to identify the
foundation that we control for the media as an influencing limitations of this earlier work. First, the focus on
variable. perfectionistic parenting has focused on both mothers and
Since the publication of these seminal works critiquing the fathers, which suggests that fathers experience a similar
public discourse of motherhood, there has been an abundance pressure to be perfect as mothers. Garey and Arendell
of research documenting the stressful, isolating, and anxiety (2001) argue that to use the term Bparent^ or Bparenting^
producing consequences of this ideology in U.S. women’s serves to obscure the social reality of the gendered nature of
private lives (c.f., Christopher 2012; Johnston and Swanson parenting (p. 295). A feminist framework confronts the op-
2006, 2007; Liss et al. 2013). Researchers have also examined pression of women in families, which does not suggest we
the concept of mother-blame, when U.S. mothers (compared ignore fathers, but instead acknowledges that there is a
to fathers) are disproportionately blamed for their children’s socio-historical context that has historically placed the respon-
actions, behaviors, health and well-being (Jackson and sibility for children’s problems squarely on the shoulders of
Mannix 2004; Singh 2002, 2004). More recent work – and mothers (Garey and Arendell 2001). In fact, the U.S. research
the first quantitative study of intensive motherhood in the U.S. on mother-blame (to be discussed below) highlights the very
– has also identified the link between intensive mothering gendered nature of parenting, such that mothers carry a par-
practices and increased depression and stress (Rizzo et al. ticular, gendered pressure that is not experienced by fathers.
2013). Other work based in psychology has found a link be- Second, we argue that the current research on intensive
tween U.S. individuals who identify as perfectionist parents mothering focuses too closely on individual-responsibility,
and negative outcomes like anxiety, depression and reduced overlooking the power that the dominant ideology has over
self-esteem and self-efficacy (Flett et al. 2005; Lee et al. 2012; all modern U.S. mothers. Indeed, Rizzo et al. (2013) posed the
Powers et al. 2005; Randles et al. 2010). We use the theoretical following question at the conclusion of their study on inten-
contributions of these findings to control for the effects of sive motherhood: BSo, if intensive mothering is related to so
subscribing to the intensive mothering ideology. many negative mental health outcomes, why do women do
Although some scholars have critiqued the ideology of it?^ (p. 619). This question assumes that the choice to mother
intensive motherhood for coming from a place of privilege intensively is truly a choice, and not an inescapable cultural
(e.g., Baca Zinn 1990; Collins 1994), researchers have iden- mandate. From a psychological perspective, it makes sense to
tified the omnipresent state of these maternal expectations conclude that if women want to decrease the negative conse-
across populations such that even if a group questions a par- quences of intensive parenting and parental perfectionism,
ticular aspect of the ideology, the hegemony of these maternal they should reconsider their approach to parenting.
standards continue to affect how women parent (Arendell Accordingly, perhaps mothers could choose to ignore media
1999; Newman and Henderson 2014). Indeed, this model of influence when it comes to parenting decisions, or shift their
mothering has been critiqued for overlooking the very differ- own thinking and beliefs, perhaps lowering expectations and
ent ways of doing motherhood, including the voices of lesbi- freeing themselves from the strictures of intensive mother-
an, bisexual, or transgendered mothers, those from varying hood. However, we suggest that from a feminist sociological
socioeconomic backgrounds, as well as mothers of color perspective, it is important to shift the focus away from
(Baca Zinn 1990; Collins 1994). In response to this silence, individual-responsibility instead to analyzing the dominant
514 Sex Roles (2016) 74:512–526

discourse of motherhood. We propose that regardless of researchers have established that although social changes have
choice, the pressure to be a perfect affects all women. afforded women agency in the public sphere, the same choices
Indeed, Douglas and Michaels (2004) argue that the façade are not necessarily available in the private domain, the home
of a Bchoice^ is what makes the dominant ideology of moth- (Douglas and Michaels 2004; Walzer 1998; Warner 2005;
erhood so powerful. They argue: Zelizer 1985). According to this mothering ideal: 1) the moth-
er must be the primary caregiver of children because men
the only truly enlightened choice to make as a woman, cannot be relied upon for the duty; 2) child rearing logically
the one that proves, first, that you are a ‘real’ woman, requires extensive time, energy, and material resources; and 3)
and second, that you are a decent, worthy one, is to the children are priceless and incompatible with paid labor
become a ‘mom’ and to bring to child rearing a combi- (Hays 1996).
nation of selflessness and professionalism that would Studies done in the U.S. provide numerous examples of
involve the cross cloning of Mother Teresa with Donna how the dominant ideologies of motherhood affect women.
Shalala. (Douglas and Michaels 2004, p. 5). Newman and Henderson (2014) used interviews and partic-
ipant observations of women participating in a mothering
This public ideology of motherhood, then, mandates that support group to demonstrate that the requirements of inten-
women not only become mothers, but that they approach sive mothering set women up for failure, even those coming
motherhood in a way that put their own needs low on the from a privileged status. Indeed, women in this study were
priority list. Therefore, one could argue that gender equality unhappy with the strictures of intensive motherhood,
surrounding U.S. motherhood has not only stalled, but has insomuch that they sought out a support group to deal with
shifted into reverse. We should no longer be asking why wom- the frustrations.
en subscribe to intensive mothering ideologies or perfection- Similarly, Stone (2007) utilized interviews with highly suc-
istic parenting practices, but instead we should identify the cessful U.S. women who left the paid workforce to mother
power of this ideology and how it affects all U.S. women. full-time to demonstrate the inflexibility of contemporary so-
The present study, grounded in feminist sociology (Garey cial structures that prevent women from having it Ball.^ These
and Arendell 2001), is a step in this direction as we take into findings support Hays (1996) now classic articulation of the
account the ubiquitous pressure to be perfect for all U.S. cultural contradictions of motherhood whereby she identified
mothers, not just those who practice intensive mothering or the effects of contemporary U.S. capitalist culture on most
identify as perfectionist parents. Indeed, we ask: how do pub- families. She argues that in addition to feeling a constant pres-
lic messages influence mothers’ psychological well-being, sure to be an intensive mother, families need to have dual
even among women who do not subscribe to such ideologies? incomes. Therefore, mothers are not immune from the social
Do mothers even need to Bbuy in^ to intensive motherhood or pressures to be productive citizens who also contribute to the
perfectionistic parenting to be affected by its messages? This economy. In addition, women are pressured to conform to the
approach contrasts with previous research in this body of work seemingly ubiquitous ideology of perfect motherhood, despite
because it broadens the discussion from the micro- to the a general sense of dissatisfaction with it (Newman and
macro-level of analysis. By conceptualizing the issue in this Henderson 2014). It is worth noting here that while this ide-
way, we can shift the lens of this body of work by re- ology has been documented around the globe, the empirical
imagining idealized motherhood as a public social issue and studies covered here are all based on U.S. samples, unless
not a personal choice. otherwise stated.
In summary, the present study builds on previous literature
by suggesting that women need not subscribe to perfectionis- Psychology of Motherhood
tic parenting or the strictures of intensive mothering in order to
experience the pressure to be perfect. In fact, we suggest that Researchers have long examined the psychological effects of
all mothers are subject to the pressure to be the perfect mother, parenting on both mothers and fathers, suggesting that parent-
which in turn, negatively impacts mothers’ psychological hood can be detrimental to mental health (Baumeister and
well-being; specifically, self-efficacy, stress, and anxiety. Vohs 2002). Specifically, parents have been found to experi-
ence increased stress (Nomaguchi and Milkie 2003), anxiety
Literature Review (Warner 2005), guilt and shame (Guendouzi 2006; Liss et al.
2013) and depression (Evenson and Simon 2005). The major-
Despite numerous social changes (e.g., Gerson 2010; O'Reilly ity of research on parental self-efficacy, or the belief parents
2010) that have occurred in the U.S. since Freidan (1963) first hold about their capabilities to organize and execute a set of
identified Bthe problem that has no name^ (p. 151), the image tasks related to parenting a child, has examined the transition
of what it means to be a good mom has consistently been a to parenting (Cutrona and Troutman 1986; Gross et al. 1994;
part of a prescribed performance of gender for women. That is, Leahy-Warren et al. 2011; Teti and Gelfand 1991). This focus
Sex Roles (2016) 74:512–526 515

on the transition to parenthood makes sense, because becom- Similarly, Stoeber and Otto (2006) conducted a meta-
ing a parent can be associated with a major shift in one’s analysis of studies on perfectionism to define perfectionistic
beliefs about their abilities to be able to manage the new role. behavior as occurring when one is highly concerned over
It is no surprise, then, that this body of work has found that mistakes, has fear of negative evaluation from others, has
low self-efficacy can be problematic for new parents (Gross doubts about her/his actions, and has concern about parental
et al. 1994; Maciejewski et al. 2000). Particularly, new criticism. This type of perfectionism has been consistently
mothers have been found to experience more self-doubt associated with negative outcomes like depression and re-
(Wilkins 2006) and lower confidence (Moran et al. 2006), duced self-esteem in a wide variety of samples (Flett et al.
both of which Bandura (2006) conceptualized as indicators 2005; Hewitt and Flett 1991; Powers et al. 2005). When ap-
of low self-efficacy. Given the modern ideology of mother- plied to parenthood, researchers have demonstrated that per-
hood which perpetuates the pressure to be the perfect mother, fectionist mothers and fathers are more likely than the average
struggles with self-efficacy could last much longer than earlier parent to experience higher levels of anxiety, overprotection,
research has indicated. That is, do mothers continue to wrestle and have harsher interactions with their children (Flett et al.
with self-efficacy after the transition to parenthood is over? 2002; Randles et al. 2010). First-time mothers who subscribe
Does the current ideology of motherhood exacerbate the ef- to perfectionistic parenting have also been found to experience
fects of parenthood on self-efficacy beyond those first tenuous higher levels of anxiety as they transition to parenthood (Lee
few years as a new parent? et al. 2012).
Another psychological consequence of modern mother- While the link between perfectionistic standards and nega-
hood – that scholars have argued is actually an inherent part tive outcomes has been established, less is known about how
of motherhood – is guilt (Liss et al. 2013; Sutherland 2010). the dominant ideology of being a Bperfect mother^ affects
Evolutionary psychologists Rotkirch and Janhunen (2009) ar- mothers’ psychological well-being. Research on working
gued that guilt Bfocuses on wrongful behavior, and is connect- mothers has argued that they utilize cognitive acrobatics in
ed to a concern for others and how they are affected by one’s order to manage the tension between employment and the
behavior^ (p. 92). Therefore, when it comes to mothering, dominant mothering ideology (Johnston and Swanson
guilt can emerge frequently as a result of the pressure to be 2007). Rizzo et al. (2013) found similar results whether
perfect and the perpetual feelings of inadequacy in attaining mothers work part-time, full-time, or stay at home. In a quan-
perfection. Indeed, Rotkirch and Janhunen (2009) found in titative study of 181 women, they discovered that mothers
their qualitative study of Finnish mothers that guilt was among who subscribe to the intensive mothering ideology experience
the most frequently mentioned feelings associated with moth- lower life satisfaction.
erhood. Mothers in this study discussed the presence of neg- Each of these studies highlights how internalizing the
ative feelings (e.g., anger, aggression) and how guilt was a Bmotherhood myth^ (the dominant discourse of motherhood)
direct result of those feelings. Similarly, Liss et al. (2013) is problematic for women, because they are subscribing to
found in their quantitative U.S.-based study of mothers that unrealistic expectations. However, framing the issue in this
an interaction existed between mothers’ self-discrepancy, the way overlooks the difficulty in rejecting the dominant dis-
perceived difference between one’s actual and ideal selves, course of motherhood. The present study, framed with a fem-
and fear of negative evaluation. That is, women who experi- inist sociological imagination (Garey and Arendell 2001), ar-
ence guilt and shame also fear negative evaluation from gues that the pressure to be a perfect mother (which leads to
others, and experience discrepant feelings between their self-discrepancy, maternal guilt, and shame) is often inescap-
Bideal^ self as a mother and their Breal^ self. That is, guilt able, and particularly hard to reject for mothers. Indeed, the
and shame stem from the very hegemonic discourse of moth- following section details a phenomenon called mother-blame,
erhood that measures women against an unattainable, and because mothers are held responsible for not only their own
therefore problematic, ideal of perfection. Thus, for the pur- well-being, but for their children’s as well.
poses of this study, we also ask how guilt is experienced by
modern mothers who feel unable to live up to both societal Mother Blame: Connecting Macro and Micro
and personal expectations.
For several decades, scholars have noted the stressful, isolat-
Parenting Perfection ing, and anxiety producing nature of mothering (e.g., Arendell
2000; Boulton 1983; Oakley 1992; Ribbens 1994; Ross 1995;
Researchers have examined the mental health effects of sub- Roxburgh 1997; Wolf 2001). One aspect of this work has
scribing to perfectionist standards. For example, Hewitt and focused on mother-blame, which describes holding mothers
Flett (1991) used both clinical and non-clinical data samples accountable for the actions, behavior, health and well-being of
to define societal-oriented perfectionism as the belief that their (even adult) children (Jackson and Mannix 2004; Rich
others hold oneself to exceptionally high standards. 1976; Singh 2002, 2004). Mother-blame is a pervasive
516 Sex Roles (2016) 74:512–526

mothering ideology that contains essentialized and idealized and historical discourses about motherhood. In doing so, we
notions of mother and mothering behaviors (O’Reilly 2001; take into account how the public discourses of motherhood
Chase and Rogers 2001; Ladd-Taylor and Umansky 1998). It negatively affect mothers’ self-efficacy, and potentially in-
is this concept of mother-blame that illustrates the relationship crease rates of anxiety and stress, even among mothers who
between macro-level discourses and micro-level psychologi- may not fully subscribe to the ideology. We also examine
cal well-being. guilt, defined as mothers’ negative evaluation of not fulfilling
For example, Singh (2004) analyzed interviews with U.S. their own expectations, and its relationship to stress, anxiety,
mothers of boys with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder and self-efficacy.
(ADHD) to demonstrate mothers’ feelings of inadequacy and In addition, given that prior research argues that the media
self-blame for their sons’ health; feelings that are reinforced (Douglas and Michaels 2004) and subscribing to intensive
by interactions with other moms in public settings. In local, mothering beliefs (Rizzo et al. 2013) can lead to negative
every day encounters on the street and in restaurants, church, psychological outcomes for mothers, the present study strate-
shopping malls, movie theaters, and supermarkets, mothers gically controlled for these variables first in the analysis. In
reported experiencing reproach and judgment from onlookers doing so, we keep the analysis focused on the broader moth-
witnessing their sons’ behavior (Singh 2004). Other research ering ideology as opposed to the idea that women truly have a
using interviews with U.S. mothers (Jackson and Mannix choice in whether or not the intensive mothering ideologies
2004) suggests that mother-blaming is ever-present and a affect them. The analysis follows a feminist sociological
powerful influence over moms’ feelings of adequacy and framework, which broadens the discussion away from insin-
worth. In particular, this line of research focused on mothers’ uating that women can choose to not be affected by the media,
feelings of blame as experienced through formal health care and that they can also choose to not subscribe to intensive
settings (e.g., nurses, pediatricians; Jackson and Mannix mothering ideologies. Instead, we argue that the cultural pres-
2004). sure to be perfect and associated guilt for not meeting moth-
This body of work on mother-blame begins the bridging of ering expectations are much more powerful indicators of mod-
psychosocial feelings of inadequacy and guilt as a result from ern mothers’ psychological well-being.
the hegemonic discourse of motherhood. These feelings are Therefore, the hypotheses guiding this project include:
developed and reinforced on several levels, both interperson-
ally (i.e., mother-to-mother) and more formally (i.e., health H1: The pressure to be perfect is significantly associated
care settings). However, researchers have not yet brought with lower levels of self-efficacy for contemporary
these two approaches together to examine how macro-level mothers, regardless of media influence and intensive
standards of perfection articulated in the ideologies of moth- mothering beliefs.
erhood may subject mothers to similar psychological feelings H2: Guilt for not meeting parenting expectations is sig-
regarding their self-worth, even if they do not subscribe fully nificantly associated with lower levels of self-efficacy for
to its ideology. contemporary mothers, regardless of media influence and
intensive mothering beliefs.
The Price Women Pay, Even When They Are Not Buying It H3: The pressure to be perfect is significantly associated
with an increase in stress for contemporary mothers, re-
This project attends to the reality that perceptions of failure in gardless of media influence and intensive mothering
an all-encompassing role like Bmother^ may actually contrib- beliefs.
ute to adverse global psychological well-being. In other H4: Guilt for not meeting parenting expectations is sig-
words, what if mothers do not particularly believe in the ide- nificantly associated with an increase in stress for con-
ology of intensive motherhood, but are still influenced by it as temporary mothers, regardless of media influence and
a part of the cultural ideology? As discussed above, the be- intensive mothering beliefs.
havioral and psychological expectations of intensive mother- H5: The pressure to be perfect is significantly associated
ing are ongoing, and arguably inescapable at multiple levels. with an increase in anxiety for contemporary mothers,
We examine how these discourses and expectations may neg- regardless of media influence and intensive mothering
atively affect self-efficacy and potentially increase rates of beliefs.
anxiety and stress. H6: Guilt for not meeting parenting expectations is sig-
This study extends the literature by utilizing what Garey nificantly associated with an increase in anxiety for con-
and Arendell (2001) identify as a feminist sociological imag- temporary mothers, regardless of media influence and
ination. That is, Bour ideas about children and families are intensive mothering beliefs.
historically and culturally situated^ (p. 293), which means that
we cannot understand individuals without measuring their These models will be tested using hierarchical regression,
perceptions of and experiences with the dominant cultural wherein variables are entered into the regression model in
Sex Roles (2016) 74:512–526 517

Bblocks.^ This approach is designed to elucidate any specific Table 1 Demographics (N = 283)
and/or moderating effects any of the control variables have on Characteristics n %
the independent variables of interest. Therefore, Block 1 in-
cluded demographic variables, Block 2 included media con- Race/ethnicity
trol variables, and Block 3 included intensive mothering var- Non-Hispanic White 257 90.8
iables, and Block 4 included the pressure to be perfect and African American 3 1.0
guilt variables. These control variables are discussed below. Hispanic 45 15.9
Education
Some high school 1 .84
High school diploma/GED 36 12.7
Method Some college 14 4.9
Associate’s degree 28 9.9
Data were collected through online data collecting software Bachelor’s degree 118 41.6
once our study obtained Institutional Review Board approval. Master’s degree 53 18.7
A snowball sampling technique was used in this study, which Doctoral degree 28 9.9
means that the sample size snowballed, or grew, as partici- Marital Status
pants responded to the survey questions. Participants were Married or living with partner 254 89.7
invited, upon completion of the survey, to forward the link Divorced 14 4.9
to the survey on to mothers over the age of 18 who they Separated 3 1.0
thought would be interested in participating in the study. Single, never married 11 3.8
Therefore, the original database of personal contacts compiled Age
by the researchers, which consisted of 164 mothers over the 18–25 22 7.9
age of 18, resulted in a total sample of 283 mothers. 26–33 130 46.2
It is important to note that the majority of e-mail 34–40 93 32.9
addresses generated from the research team’s personal 41–47 23 8.2
contacts included women in the Midwest and West re-
Over 47 15 5.3
gions of the U.S. Participants were not given any incen-
Age of children
tives for filling out the survey or for forwarding it on to
< 1 year old 67 23.7
other mothers they knew. Although participants could
2–4 years old 178 62.9
send the link to women anywhere, the final sample in-
5–9 years old 101 35.7
cluded an overrepresentation of mothers from the West
10–15 years old 56 19.8
(45 %) and Midwest (29 %) regions in the U.S. The
16–20 years old 19 6.7
South (18 %) and Northeast (8 %) regions of the United
> 21 years old 17 6.0
States were represented, though in smaller proportions.
Employment Status
These categorizations were made based on the U.S.
Full-time 121 42.7
Census Bureau’s definitions of regions (BCensus
Part-time 70 24.7
Regions^ 2010).
Stay at home parent 65 22.9
The survey was open for approximately two weeks and, in
Number of children M = 1.82 SD = 1.11
total, we received 283 completed surveys. Respondents were
asked about their demographic background (i.e., age, Incomea M = 2.99 SD = 2.13
race/ethnicity, marital and work status, income, and highest (25,000–34,000)
education level). Respondents were also asked about their a
Range = 1 (below 25,000) to 8 (over 80,000)
media consumption, surveillance of their behavior, surveil-
lance of fellow mothers, and sources of their expectations
for parenting. The demographics for the sample (N = 283) Media Influence
are displayed in Table 1.
Based on the argument that the media is influential in perpet-
uating the hegemonic discourse of motherhood (Douglas and
Measures Michaels 2004), and that societal-oriented perfectionism im-
portant in understanding parental expectations and criticism,
Summary statistics for key study variables are found in we controlled for media influence to tease out the Bpublic^
Table 2. Both control variables and dependent variables are aspect of the pressure to be perfect. Media influence was mea-
described below. sured using seven separate types of media. Respondents were
518 Sex Roles (2016) 74:512–526

Table 2 Means and standard deviations of key variables

Variable Mean Standard Deviation Range

Self-efficacy 4.78 1.28 1–7


Number of days stressed in past week 3.73 2.05 1–7
Anxiety level 2.99 1.99 1–7
Media Consumption 21.69 8.19 1–49
I am the best caregiver for my children. 6.01 1.37 1–7
I should attend to all of my child(ren)’s needs. 5.12 1.73 1–7
Motherhood is the most important role in my life. 6.16 1.26 1–7
I feel guilty for not meeting my expectations for parenting. 5.26 2.07 1–8
I feel pressure to be the perfect mother. 5.77 1.94 1–8

asked to Bindicate how influential each source of information considered acceptable for internal consistency (Nunnally and
is when making decisions about purchases for child/ren. Bernstein 1994; Bland and Altman 1997).
Respondents answered on a Likert scale (0-not influential at To measure stress, respondents were asked to indicate how
all, 7-very influential) for seven types of media: television many days in the past week they felt stressed, on a scale of 1–
commercials, television news, parenting magazines, newspa- 7. The anxiety measure used in this study asked respondents to
pers, internet websites or blogs, radio and self-help books. The indicate how true (on a scale of 1–7) the following statement
Cronbach’s Alpha for the index was 0.82 (M = 21.69, is: BI have felt anxious or worried for no good reason^ in the
SD = 8.19). past 7 days.

Intensive Mothering & The Pressure To Be Perfect


Results
The present study utilized three items, measured on a Likert
scale of 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree), to measure Table 2 presents the appropriate means, standard errors, and
beliefs about intensive mothering. The statements were: (1) I ranges of the dependent and independent variables used in this
feel that I am the best person to be the primary caregiver for study. The means for the outcome variables, self-efficacy,
my child/ren; (2) It is important for me to personally attend to number of days stressed, and anxiety level, indicate that the
all of my child’s physical and emotional needs; (3) mothers in our sample report high levels of self-efficacy and
Motherhood is the most important role of my life. on average, experience moderate levels of stress and anxiety.
Based on previous research (Henderson et al. 2010), we Average media influence scores are not as high as we expect-
used two items to assess the existence of the pressure to be ed, but the mothers in our sample score at the moderate or high
perfect, on a 1–7 scale (1-strongly disagree, 7-strongly agree). levels of agreement on the intensive mothering and pressure to
The first item asked respondents to react to the following be perfect indicators.
statement: BSometimes as moms, we feel pressured to be ‘per- Table 3 presents Pearson correlations for several of the key
fect’ in our roles as mothers. How true is this for you?^ variables used in the central analysis. As predicted, the bivar-
Responses ranged from 1 (not at all) to 8 (strongly agree). iate analyses show that the mothers’ self-efficacy scores were
The second item read: BI feel guilty when I cannot live up to lower if their media influence scores were high. This means
the expectations for my parenting that I set for myself.^ that for this group of mothers, their belief in their own ability
to achieve set goals is linked to their media consumption.
Dependent Variables However, stress and anxiety levels were not related to media
influence. Further, correlations show that only one of the in-
Four items made up the index for measuring self-efficacy, tensive mothering hypotheses is supported. Self-efficacy is
based on Bandura (2006). The items used to measure this lower if mothers agree that motherhood is their most impor-
construct, on a scale of 1–7, were: (1) BI have little control tant role; consequently, simply believing in the intensive
over the things that happen to me^; (2) BI feel self-conscious^; mothering ideology does not lead to higher levels of stress
(3) BI feel inferior to others at the moment^; and (4) BI am and anxiety. However, as expected, mothers who score higher
worried about what other people think of me.^ Higher scores on the pressure to be perfect and guilt for not meeting parent-
indicate higher levels of self-efficacy. The Cronbach’s Alpha ing expectations indicators, experience lower levels of self-
for the index was 0.71 (M = 4.78, SD = 1.28). In most psy- efficacy and higher levels of stress and anxiety. These specific
chometric theory research, values between 0.70 to 0.90 are findings were most important as we tested the final set of
Sex Roles (2016) 74:512–526

Table 3 Correlation matrix for key variables (N = 283)

(1) (2) (3) (4) (5) (6) (7) (8) (9) (10) (11) (12) (13) (14) (15)

(1) Self-efficacy – – – – – – – – – – – – – – –
(2) Days stressed −.33*** – – – – – – – – – – – – – –
(3) Anxiety −.42*** .34*** – – – – – – – – – – – – –
(4) Age .09 .05 −.04 – – – – – – – – – – – –
(5) White −.05 −.04 .01 .01 – – – – – – – – – – –
(6) Education .20*** −.08 −14* .13* −.02 – – – – – – – – – –
(7) Work Full-time −.01 .09 −.04 .07 .15** .07 – – – – – – – – –
8) Work Part-time −.01 −.01 .04 −.03 .15** .03 −.49*** – – – – – – – –
(9) Married −.01 −.07 .06 −.12* .43*** .08 .13** .12* – – – – – – –
(10) Media Influence −.13* −.02 .10 .02 .03 −.11* .11* −.05 .04 – – – – – –
(11) I am best caregiver for my child(ren) −.02 −.10 .04 −.14* .01 −.18** −.22** .05 .01 −.06 – – – – –
(12) I should attend to all child(ren)’s needs −.08 −.06 .05 −.18** −.07 −.23*** −.08 .01 −.02 .09 .44*** – – – –
(13) Motherhood most important role −.15** −.01 .09 −.07 −.01 −.19** .08 −.06 −.02 .10 .34*** .37*** – – –
(14) Guilt for not meeting expectations −.43*** −.44*** .35*** −.02 −.09 −.01 .03 .03 −.05 .13* .03 .07 .18*** – –
15) Pressure to be perfect −.31*** .32*** .20*** −.09 .03 .10 −.07 .08 −.01 .07 .08 .05 .15* .53*** –

*p < .05,**p < .01, ***p < .001


519
520 Sex Roles (2016) 74:512–526

hypotheses because we felt confident that the mothers’ psy- parenting expectations, the significant relationship be-
chological well-being seemed most related to the pressure to tween education and self-efficacy persists (p < 0.01);
be perfect and experiencing maternal guilt. women with higher levels of education have higher
Hierarchical linear regression was conducted in STATA levels of self-efficacy. On the other hand, mothers who
statistical software (2013). Hypothesis 1, which predicted that feel the pressure to be perfect (p < 0.01) and experience
the pressure to be perfect is significantly associated with lower guilt for not meeting their own parenting expectations
levels of self-efficacy for contemporary mothers, regardless of (p < 0.001) have lower levels of self-efficacy. For this
media influence and intensive mothering beliefs, is supported set of models, all but one of the F- Incremental tests are
by the data. Hypothesis 2, which argued that mothers’ guilt for statistically significant at the 0.05 alpha level. First,
not meeting parenting expectations is significantly associated adding media influence, F(6, 259) = 3.19, p = 0.04,
with lower levels of self-efficacy for contemporary mothers, improves the ability to predict self-efficacy. However,
regardless of media influence and intensive mothering beliefs, the intensive mothering variables, F(7, 256) = 3.19,
was also supported by the data. The models are described p = 0.07, do not improve the model. As hypothesized,
below. adding the pressure to be perfect variables, F(10,
Results for the first set of hierarchical linear regres- 254) = 30.52, p = 0.00, greatly improves the ability to
sion models predicting self-efficacy (Hypotheses 1 & 2) predict self-efficacy of the mothers in the study. In ad-
are presented in Table 4. In Model 1, education was dition, no evidence of multicollinearity was found
significantly related to higher levels of self-efficacy (mean VIF = 1.24) in this set of models.
(p < 0.01). This finding persisted in Model 2 Hypothesis 3 predicted that the pressure to be perfect is
(p < 0.05), which controlled for media influence, as significantly associated with an increase in stress for contem-
well as in Model 3 (p < 0.05), which controlled for porary mothers, regardless of media influence and intensive
intensive mothering beliefs. In Model 4, which controls mothering beliefs. Hypothesis 4 predicted that mother’s guilt
for the pressure to be perfect and guilt for not meeting for not meeting parenting expectations is significantly

Table 4 Hierarchical linear regression predicting self-efficacy (N = 281)

Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 Model 4

B (SE) β B (SE) β B (SE) β B (SE) Β

Age .04 .06 .05 .07 −.05 .07 .03 .04


(.04) (.04) (.04) (.04)
White −.18 −.04 −.15 −.03 −.18 −.04 −.18 −.04
(.28) (.28) (.28) (.26)
Education .15** .17 .14* .16 .12* .14 .17** .19
(.05) (.06) (.06) (.05)
Work full-time −.12 −.04 −.05 −.02 .01 .01 −.01 −.01
(.18) (.19) (.19) (.18)
Work part-time −.25 −.08 −.20 −.07 −.19 −.07 −.10 −.04
(.21) (.21) (.21) (.19)
Married .05 .01 −.02 −.01 −.01 −.01 −.19 −.05
(.25) (.26) (.26) (.24)
Media influence – – −.02 −.12 −.02 −.10 −.01 −.04
(.01) (.01) (.01)
Best caregiver for my children – – – – .06 .06 .05 .05
(.07) (.06)
I must attend to all of my children(s) needs – – – – −.03 −.04 −.01 −.02
(.05) (.05)
Motherhood most important role in life – – – – −.13 −.13 −.06 −.06
(.07) (.06)
Pressure to be perfect – – – – – – −.12** −.17
(.04)
Guilt for not meeting expectations – – – – – – −.20*** −.32
(.04)
R2 .05 .06 .08 .26
R2 Change .01 .02 .18***
df (6, 260) df (7, 259) df (10, 256) df (12, 254)

*p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001


Sex Roles (2016) 74:512–526 521

associated with an increase in for contemporary mothers, re- in this study. In addition, no evidence of multicollinearity was
gardless of media influence and intensive mothering beliefs. found (mean VIF = 1.25) in this set of models.
Both hypotheses are supported by the data. The models are Hypothesis 5 predicted that the pressure to be perfect is
described in detail below. significantly associated with an increase in anxiety for con-
Results for the second set of regressions predicting stress temporary mothers, regardless of media influence and inten-
(Hypotheses 3 & 4) are presented in Table 5. In Model 1, sive mothering beliefs. Hypothesis 6 predicted that mother’s
none of the demographic variables are significantly related guilt for not meeting parenting expectations is significantly
to higher levels of stress. Model 2, which controls for media associated with an increase in anxiety for contemporary
influence, also reveals no significant relationship between mothers, regardless of media influence and intensive mother-
stress and the control variables. Model 3 controls for inten- ing beliefs. In this set of models, only Hypothesis 6 is sup-
sive parenting beliefs and again, shows no significant rela- ported by the data. The models are described in detail below.
tionship to stress. Results for the third and final set of regression models
In Model 4, women with higher levels of education expe- predicting anxiety (Hypotheses 5 & 6) are presented in
rience lower levels of stress. Similar to the models predicting Table 6. In Model 1, lower education levels are significantly
self-efficacy, both the pressure to be perfect (p < 0.01) and related to an increase in anxiety (p < 0.05). This effect disap-
guilt for not meeting parenting expectations (p < 0.001) are pears in Model 2, controlling for media influence. In Model 3,
significantly related to an increase in stress. Incremental R- which controls for intensive parenting beliefs, no control var-
squared test results were not as strong in this set of models; iables emerge as significantly related to anxiety. In Model 4,
adding media influence, F(7, 257) = 0.81, p = 0.14, and inten- however, lower education levels are significantly related to an
sive mothering indicators, F(10, 254) = 0.48, p = 0.35, does increase in anxiety (p < 0.05), along with guilt for not meeting
not improve the model. However, as hypothesized, the pres- parental expectations (p < 0.001). In addition, no evidence of
sure to be perfect variables significantly improves the ability multicollinearity was found (mean VIF = 1.56) in this set of
to predict stress, F(12, 252) = 37.53, p = 0.00, for the mothers models. Incremental R-squared tests reveal a similar pattern of

Table 5 Hierarchical linear regression predicting stress (N = 268)

Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 Model 4

B (SE) β B (SE) β B (SE) β B (SE) β

Age .04 .03 .01 .01 −.01 −.01 .03 .03


(.07) (.07) (.07) (.07)
White −.51 −.07 −.42 −.06 −.46 −.06 −.49 −.07
(.44) (.45) (.45) (.40)
Education −.11 −.07 −.11 −.08 −.14 −.10 −.22** −.16
(.08) (.09) (.09) (.09)
Work full-time .46 .11 .50 .12 .45 .11 .51 .12
(.29) (.30) (.31) (.28)
Work part-time .51 .11 .67 .14 .64 .14 .52 .11
(.33) (.34) (.34) (.31)
Married −.38 −.06 −.40 −.06 −.40 −.06 −.09 −.01
(.40) (.41) (.41) (.37)
Media influence – – .01 .06 .01 .06 −.01 −.11
(.02) (.02) (.01)
Best caregiver for my children – – – – −.05 −.04 −.03 −.02
(.11) (.10)
I must attend to all of my children(s) needs – – – – −.08 −.07 −.10 −.09
(.08) (.08)
Motherhood most important role in life – – – – .03 .02 −.09 −.06
(.11) (.10)
Pressure to be perfect – – – – – – .20** .18
(.07)
Guilt for not meeting expectations – – – – – – .35*** .36
(.06)
R2 .03 .04 .04 .26
R2 Change .01 .01 .22***
df (6, 258) df (7, 257) df (10, 254) df (12, 252)

*p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001


522 Sex Roles (2016) 74:512–526

Table 6 Hierarchical linear regression predicting anxiety (N = 271)

Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 Model 4

B (SE) β B (SE) β B (SE) β B (SE) β

Age −.01 −.01 .01 .01 .01 .01 .02 .02


(.06) (.07) (.07) (.07)
White −.12 −.02 −.21 −.03 −.23 −.03 −.29 −.04
(.43) (.45) (.45) (.42)
Education −.19* −.14 −.15 −.11 −.13 −.09 −.17* −.13
(.08) (.09) (.09) (.09)
Work full-time −.09 −.02 −.21 −.05 −.22 −.06 −.28 −.07
(.28) (.30) (.31) (.29)
Work part-time .11 .02 .04 .01 .07 .01 −.07 −.02
(.32) (.33) (.34) (.32)
Married .26 .04 .20 .03 .19 .03 .42 .07
(.39) (.41) (.41) (.38)
Media influence – – .02 .07 .02 .07 .01 .02
(.02) (.02) (.01)
Best caregiver for my children – – – – .04 .03 .05 .03
(.11) (.10)
I must attend to all of my children(s) needs – – – – −.05 −.04 −.08 −.07
(.08) (.08)
Motherhood most important role in life – – – – .12 .07 .04 .03
(.11) (.10)
Pressure to be perfect – – – – – – .02 −.02
(.07)
Guilt for not meeting expectations – – – – – – .34*** .34
(.07)
R2 .04 .05 .05 .16
R2 Change .01 .01 .12***
df (6, 261) df (7, 260) df (10, 257) df (12, 255)

*p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001

results as earlier models; only adding the pressure to be perfect The present study provides a quantitative examination of
variables significantly improves the ability to predict anxiety, forces that previous researchers have argued contribute to the
F(12, 255) = 18.47, p = 0.000, for mothers in this study. hegemonic discourse of motherhood, such as the media
(Douglas and Michaels 2004), and intensive mothering beliefs
(Hays 1996; Rizzo et al. 2013; Warner 2005). We also extend
the examination of the pressure to be perfect beyond role-
Discussion specific (maternal) strain (Liss et al. 2013). Instead, we hy-
pothesized that even those who may not subscribe to the dom-
The purpose of the present study was to apply a feminist inant discourse of motherhood are subject to adverse psycho-
perspective to the study of the public discourse of motherhood logical consequences. We hypothesized that mothers who ex-
to women’s private experiences of psychological well-being. perience the pressure to be perfect would have higher levels of
Using a feminist sociological imagination (Garey and stress and anxiety, and lower levels of self-efficacy. Similarly,
Arendell 2001), we analyzed how the public discourses of we also hypothesized that mothers who experience guilt for
motherhood affect women’s lived realities, connecting how not meeting parenting expectations would have higher levels
structural-level discourses (i.e., intensive motherhood) affect of stress and anxiety, and lower levels of self-efficacy. The
even mothers who may not fully subscribe to the ideology. data supported our hypotheses; mothers who feel the pressure
Although the demands of parenthood can be exhausting for to be perfect and guilt for not meeting parenting expectations
both men and women, this feminist perspective allows us the have lower self-efficacy and higher levels of stress, and
ability to understand the ways in which women are uniquely mothers who feel guilty for not meeting parenting expecta-
impacted by the cultural constructions of motherhood as we tions experience higher levels of anxiety.
problematize the exhaustive demands of intensive mothering Results of this study support the notion that standards of
in the U.S. (see Douglas and Michaels 2004; Hays 1996; contemporary motherhood are inescapable; mothers are being
Warner 2005). negatively affected by the presence of unattainable standards
Sex Roles (2016) 74:512–526 523

of perfection regardless of their beliefs about intensive These findings suggest that the experience of the pressure to
mothering. Not only do our statistical models show a be perfect is widespread and not just experienced by mothers
significant relationship between stress, anxiety and of young children.
self-efficacy and guilt for not meeting expectations As noted above, many researchers and theorists have
(and the pressure to be perfect), but the increment expressed concerns over the high standards associated with
change in R2 is also significant in of our models once contemporary motherhood (Arendell 2000; Douglas and
we add these explanatory variables. The R2 increased Michaels 2004; Newman and Henderson 2014; Rizzo et al.
significantly between models for all three outcomes, 2013). In part, our findings confirm previous findings on the
which indicates that neither the media nor women’s be- negative psychological effects of perfectionist standards (Flett
liefs about intensive mothering has more explanatory et al. 2005; Hewitt and Flett 1991; Stoeber and Otto 2006).
power than the pressure to be perfect and the guilt for For example, similar to Rizzo et al. (2013), our data demon-
not meeting parenting expectations. These findings chal- strate how intensive mothering ideology can negatively im-
lenge previous literature, which has argued that women pact one’s life satisfaction. However, our study enhances this
who subscribe to the intensive mothering ideology knowledge by suggesting that on its own, the hegemonic dis-
(Rizzo et al. 2013), perfectionistic parenting practices course of motherhood – not the actual beliefs or practices
(Flett et al. 2005; Lee et al. 2012; Powers et al. 2005; associated with the ideology – is detrimental to women. That
Randles et al. 2010), or mother-blame (Jackson and is, women who report simply feeling the pressure to be the
Mannix 2004; Rich 1976; Singh 2002, 2004) suffer ad- perfect mother, even if they do not internalize the myth, are at
verse consequences to their psychological well-being. risk of experiencing negative mental health outcomes like
Instead, we suggest that the ideology of being the stress, anxiety and decreased self-efficacy. These findings em-
Bperfect mother^ is inescapable; it is far-reaching and pirically illustrate arguments made above regarding mother
negatively affects mothers who do not even buy into blame (e.g., Singh 2004; Jackson and Mannix 2004) in that
it. These findings are important in many ways. First, these data link the macro-level discourses with micro-level
this work shifts the lens away from mothers who active- psychological outcomes.
ly subscribe to intensive mothering ideologies. In doing A feminist sociological imagination allows us to link indi-
so, this broadens the discussion away from the suppo- vidual processes within families with patterns in the larger
sition that a woman’s choice to parent in a certain way is social world (Garey and Arendell 2001). Given that perspec-
detrimental. Instead, our results suggest that it does not tive, the findings of these analyses are problematic. In partic-
matter whether or not mothers believe in intensive moth- ular they indicate that the hegemonic discourse of motherhood
ering; all mothers are subject to the pressure to be a per- is far-reaching. Perhaps the strictures of intensive mothering
fect mother. By analyzing the issue in this way, we ac- are no longer necessary to oppress women in their families;
tively confront the oppression of women in families by women no longer have to believe that intensive mothering is
acknowledging that the modern ideology of motherhood the only way to parent. Instead, the existence of the pressure to
is pervasive. This grounds the discussion within a femi- be perfect seems to be infiltrating the ways in which women
nist sociological perspective by acknowledging women’s view themselves. Additionally, the fact that mothers are
feelings of inadequacy in the role of mothers, and the experiencing such high levels of the pressure to be perfect
subsequent effects on stress, anxiety, and self-efficacy. (M = 5.77, SD = 1.94), and guilt (M = 5.26, SD = 2.07)
Therefore, the present study pays attention to how indi- indicates that women are perhaps involved in a cycle of self-
vidual lives intersect with the larger social, political, eco- blame for not meeting such high standards, which echoes
nomic, and historical contexts within which they are situ- previous research (Henderson et al. 2010; Rizzo et al. 2013;
ated (Garey and Arendell 2001). Rotkirch and Janhunen 2009).
Second, this project accessed groups of mothers who were
both employed full-time, part-time, and those who stayed at Limitations
home to care for children full-time. The results of this research
suggest that the pressure to be the perfect mother is pervasive While our study presents many important findings to the
enough to cut across employment status, media influence, or literature and theoretical underpinnings of the study of
beliefs about intensive mothering; in fact, the mothers in our motherhood, there are several limitations that need to be
sample constituted a wider range of both employment status addressed. First, though based on Hays (1996) work on inten-
and life course stage. While the majority of mothers in our sive mothering, the items used to capture these beliefs in this
sample had very young children (younger than 4 years old), study are limited. Two items could be categorized as
the second highest age range of children was between 5 and Bessentialist,^ according to Liss et al. (2013), which captures
15 years old. Therefore, our sample included women with mothers’ beliefs that they are uniquely qualified to care for
very young children as well as women with older children. children (e.g., BI feel that I am the best person to be the
524 Sex Roles (2016) 74:512–526

primary caregiver for my child/ren^ and BIt is important for standards of intensive mothering, the pressure to be
me to personally attend to all of my child’s physical and emo- the perfect mother and associated guilt for not meeting
tional needs^). The other item (BMotherhood is the most im- such high standards are alive and well.
portant role of my life^), aligns with fulfillment items validat-
ed by Liss et al. (2013). The mothers in our study did not
Conflict of Interest The authors declare that they have no conflict of
answer an exhaustive intensive mothering questionnaire.
interest.
Therefore, results of this research can only explain the rela-
tionship between a few intensive mothering beliefs, as op- Ethical Approval All procedures performed in studies involving hu-
posed to a thorough investigation of each construct and its man participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the
relationship to the pressure to be perfect. institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964
Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical
Our measures of media influence are also limited.
standards.
The measures asked about each source of media when The authors of the study received IRB approval at their home institu-
mothers Bmake decisions about purchases.^ These items tion before data collection and analysis.
were drawn from Douglas and Michaels (2004) argu-
ment that the media is the most powerful source of Informed Consent Informed consent was obtained from all individual
participants included in the study.
the omnipresent pressure to be perfect. However, given
how rapidly technology changes, our items could over-
look the relationship between recent types of social me-
References
dia (e.g., twitter, Instagram, Facebook) and the influence
such instantaneous sources of information influence par-
Allen, K. R., Walker, A. J., & McCann, B. R. (2013). Feminism and
ents in their decision-making processes. families. In G. W. Peterson & K. R. Bush (Eds.), Handbook of
Furthermore, our sample is not representative of all marriage and the family (3rd ed., pp. 139–158). New York, NY:
mothers. Our snowball sample represents only White, Springer. doi:10.1007/978-1-4614-3987-5.
mostly married, upper-middle class, highly educated Arendell, T. (1999). Hegemonic motherhood: Deviancy discourses and
employed mother’s accounts of out-of-school time issues. Center for
mothers. In addition, the nature of the survey may have
Working Families Working Paper No. 9. Berkeley, CA: University
attracted mothers who are more prone to experiencing of California.
the pressures to be perfect and guilt associated with Arendell, T. (2000). Conceiving and investigating motherhood: The de-
unattainable mothering expectations; that is, they might cade’s scholarship. Journal of Marriage and Family, 62, 1192–
1207. doi:10.1111/j.1741-3737.2000.01192.x.
have been more likely to fill out a survey on something
Baca Zinn, M. (1990). Family, feminism, and race in America. Gender
that they are passionate about or are experiencing and and Society, 4, 68–92. doi:10.1177/089124390004001006.
frustrated with. Also, the fact that our survey was online Bandura, A. (2006). Guide for constructing self-efficacy scales. In T.
automatically restricts the sample to mothers who have Urdan & F. Pajares (Eds.), Self-efficacy beliefs of adolescents (pp.
access to internet and are technologically literate, which 307–338). Greenwich, CT: Information Age Publishing.
Baumeister, R. F., & Vohs, K. D. (2002). The pursuit of meaningfulness
inherently excludes mothers who are not and may re- in life. In C. R. Snyder & S. J. Lopez (Eds.), The handbook of
strict the sample to moms who are naturally high con- positive psychology (pp. 608–618). New York: Oxford University
sumers of internet media and rhetoric on motherhood or Press.
parenting advice. Bland, J., & Altman, D. (1997). Statistics notes: Cronbach's alpha. British
Medical Journal, 314, 572. doi:10.1136/bmj.314.7080.572.
In addition, these data are cross-sectional, which Bobel, C. (2002). The paradox of natural mothering. Philadelphia, PA:
limits the causality that can be inferred from these re- Temple University Press.
sults. Our data do not allow us to tease out whether or Boulton, M. G. (1983). On being a mother: A study of women with pre-
not the pressure to be perfect leads to poorer psycho- school children. London: Tavistock.
Chase, S., & Rogers, M. (2001). Mothers and children: Feminist analyses
logical well-being or whether or not low psychological
and personal narratives. Rutgers, NJ: Rutgers University Press.
well-being reinforces the pressure to be perfect and guilt Christopher, K. (2012). Extensive mothering: Employed mothers’ con-
for not meeting parenting expectations. structions of the good mother. Gender and Society, 26, 73–96. doi:
In conclusion, this study contributes to the sociolog- 10.1177/0891243211427700.
ical study of how women experience family life and Collins, P. H. (1994). Shifting the center: Race, class and feminist theo-
rizing about motherhood. In E. Glenn, G. Chang, & L. Forcey
adds a dimension to our understanding of how macro- (Eds.), Mothering: Ideology, experience, and agency (pp. 45–65).
level messages (e.g., the media’s influence) and micro- New York: Routledge.
level beliefs (e.g., intensive mothering) are related to Cutrona, C. E., & Troutman, B. R. (1986). Social support, infant temper-
maternal mental health. It also adds to the existing body ament, and parenting self-efficacy: A mediational model of postpar-
tum depression. Child Development, 57, 1507–1518. doi:10.2307/
of knowledge on the unattainable standards of perfec- 1130428.
tion associated with modern motherhood and suggests Douglas, S. J., & Michaels, M. (2004). The mommy myth. New York:
that even for women who do not conform to the Free Press.
Sex Roles (2016) 74:512–526 525

Duberley, J., & Carrigan, M. (2012). The career identities of Individual Differences, 52, 454–457. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2011.10.
‘mumpreneurs’: Women’s experiences of combining enterprise 047.
and motherhood. International Small Business Journal, 31, 629– Liss, M., Schiffrin, H. H., & Rizzo, K. M. (2013). Maternal guilt and
651. doi: 10.1177/0266242611435182. shame: The role of self-discrepancy and fear of negative evaluation.
Evenson, R. J., & Simon, R. W. (2005). Clarifying the relationship be- Journal of Child and Family Studies, 22, 1112–1119. doi:10.1007/
tween parenthood and depression. Journal of Health and Social s10826-012-9673-2.
Behavior, 46, 341–358. doi:10.1177/002214650504600403. Lupton, D. A. (2011). "The best thing for the baby": Mothers' concepts
Ferree, M. M. (1990). Beyond separate spheres: Feminism and family and experiences related to promoting their infants' health and devel-
research. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 52, 866–884. doi:10. opment. Health, Risk and Society, 13, 637–651. doi:10.1080/
1111/j.1741-3737.2010.00711.x. 13698575.2011.624179.
Flett, G. L., Hewitt, P. L, Oliver, J. M., & Macdonald, S. (2002). Lupton, D. (2012). BI’m always on the lookout for what could be going
Perfectionism in children and their parents: A developmental anal- wrong^: Mothers' concepts and experiences of health and illness in
ysis. In G. L. Flett & P. L. Hewitt (Eds.), Perfectionism: Theory, their young children. Sydney Health and Society Group Working
research, and treatment (pp. 89–132). Washington D.C.: American Paper No. 1. Sydney: Sydney Health and Society Group. doi: 10.
Psychological Association. 1177/0891243205278010
Flett, G. L., Besser, A., & Hewitt, P. L. (2005). Perfectionism, ego defense Maciejewski, P. K., Prigerson, H. G., & Mazure, C. M. (2000). Self-
styles, and depression: A comparison of self-reports versus infor- efficacy as a mediator between stressful life events and depressive
mant rating. Journal of Personality, 73, 1355–1396. doi:10.1111/j. symptoms differences based on history of prior depression. The
1467-6494.2005.00352.x. British Journal of Psychiatry, 176, 373–378. doi:10.1192/bjp.176.
Freidan, B. (1963). The feminine mystique. New York: W. W. Norton & 4.373.
Company. Moran, V. H., Dykes, F., Burt, S., & Shuck, C. (2006). Breastfeeding
Garey, A. I., & Arendell, T. (2001). Children, work, and family: Some support for adolescent mothers: Similarities and differences in the
thoughts on "motherblame". In R. Hertz & N. Marshall (Eds.), approach of midwives and qualified breastfeeding supporters.
Working families: The transformation of the American home (pp. International Breastfeeding Journal, 1(1), 23. doi: 10.1186/1746-
293–303). Berkeley: University of California Press. 4358-1-23
Gerson, K. (2010). The unfinished revolution: How a new generation is Newman, H. D., & Henderson, A. C. (2014). The modern mystique:
reshaping family, work, and gender in America. New York: Oxford Institutional mediation of hegemonic motherhood. Sociological
University Press. Inquiry, 84, 472–491. doi:10.1111/soin.12037.
Gross, D., Conrad, B., Fogg, L., & Wothke, W. (1994). A longitudinal Nomaguchi, K. M., & Milkie, M. A. (2003). Costs and rewards of chil-
model of maternal self-efficacy, depression, and difficult tempera- dren: The effects of becoming a parent on adults' lives. Journal of
ment during toddlerhood. Research in Nursing and Health, 17, 207– Marriage and Family, 65, 356–374. doi:10.1111/j.1741-3737.2003.
215. doi:10.1002/nur.4770170308. 00356.x.
Guendouzi, J. (2006). BThe guilt thing^: Balancing domestic and profes- Nunnally, J., & Bernstein, L. (1994). Psychometric theory. New York:
sional roles. Journal of Marriage and Family, 68, 901–909. doi:10. McGraw-Hill Higher. doi:10.2307/1161962.
1111/j.1741-3737.2006.00303.x. Oakley, A. (1992). Social support in pregnancy: Methodology and find-
Hays, S. (1996). The cultural contradictions of motherhood. New Haven: ings of a 1-year follow-up study. Journal of Reproductive and Infant
Yale University Press. Psychology, 10, 219–231. doi:10.1080/02646839208403955.
Henderson, A. C., Harmon, S. M., & Houser, J. (2010). A new state of O’Reilly, A. (2001). Mothers and sons: Feminism, masculinity, and the
surveillance? An application of Michel Foucault to modern mother- struggle to raise our sons. New York, NY: Routledge, Taylor and
hood. Surveillance & Society, 7, 231–247. Francis. doi:10.1177/0265407502195012.
Hewitt, P. L., & Flett, G. L. (1991). Perfectionism in the self and social O'Reilly, A. (Ed.) (2010). Twenty-first-century motherhood: Experience,
contexts: Conception, assessment, and the association with psycho- identity, policy, agency. New York, NY: Columbia University Press.
pathology. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 456– Powers, T. A., Koestner, R., & Topciu, R. A. (2005). Implementation,
470. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.60.3.456. intentions, perfectionism, and goal progress: Perhaps the road to hell
Jackson, D., & Mannix, J. (2004). Giving voice to the burden of blame: A is paved with good intentions. Personality and Social Psychology
feminist study of mothers’ experiences of mother blaming. Bulletin, 31, 902–912. doi:10.1177/0146167204272311.
International Journal of Nursing Practice, 10, 150–158. doi:10. Randles, D., Flett, G. L., Nash, K. A., McGregor, I. D., & Hewitt, P. L.
1111/j.1440-172X.2004.00474.x. (2010). Dimensions of perfectionism, behavioral inhibition, and ru-
Johnson, S. A. (2014). BMaternal devices^, social media and the self- mination. Personality and Individual Differences, 49, 83–87. doi:10.
management of pregnancy, mothering and child health. Societies, 1016/j.paid.2010.03.002.
4, 330–350. doi:10.3390/soc4020330. Reese, S. D., Gandy, O. H., & Grant, A. E. (2003). Framing public life:
Johnston, D. D., & Swanson, D. H. (2006). Constructing the Bgood Perspectives on media and our understanding of the social world.
mother^: The experience of mothering ideologies by work status. Mahwah, N.J.: Erlbaum. doi:10.1080/10584600500477385.
Sex Roles, 54, 509–519. doi:10.1007/s11199-006-9021-3. Ribbens, J. (1994). Mothers and their children: A feminist sociology of
Johnston, D. D., & Swanson, D. H. (2007). Cognitive acrobatics in the childrearing. London: Sage Publications.
construction of worker–mother identity. Sex Roles, 57, 447–459. Rich, A. (1976). Of woman born: Motherhood as experience and
doi:10.1007/s11199-007-9267-4. institution. New York: Bantam Books.
Ladd-Taylor, M., & Umansky, L. (Eds.). (1998). Bad mothers: The pol- Rizzo, K. M., Schiffrin, H. H., & Liss, M. (2013). Insight into the parent-
itics of blame in 20th century America. New York: NYU Press. hood paradox: Mental health outcomes of intensive mothering.
Leahy-Warren, P., McCarthy, G., & Corcoran, P. (2011). Postnatal depres- Journal of Child and Family Studies, 22, 614–620. doi:10.1007/
sion in first-time mothers: Prevalence and relationships between s10826-012-9615-z.
functional and structural social support at 6 and 12 weeks postpar- Ross, E. (1995). New thoughts on "The oldest vocation:" Mothers and
tum. Archives of Psychiatric Nursing, 25, 174–184. doi:10.1016/j. motherhood in recent feminist scholarship. Signs: Journal of Women
apnu.2010.08.005. in Culture and Society, 20, 397–413. doi:10.1086/494980.
Lee, M. A., Schoppe-Sullivan, S. J., & Kamp Dush, C. M. (2012). Rotkirch, A., & Janhunen, K. (2009). Maternal guilt. Evolutionary
Parenting perfectionism and parental adjustment. Personality and Psychology, 8, 90–106.
526 Sex Roles (2016) 74:512–526

Roxburgh, S. (1997). The effect of children on the mental health of wom- Vincent, C. (2010). The sociology of mothering. In M. W. Apple, S. J.
en in the paid labor force. Journal of Family Issues, 18, 270–289. Ball, & L. A. Gandin (Eds.), The Routledge international handbook
doi:10.1177/019251397018003003. of the sociology of education (pp. 109–120). New York: Routledge.
Singh, I. (2002). Bad boys, good mothers and the Bmiracle^ of Ritalin. Walker, A. J. (1999). Gender and family relationships. In M. Sussman, S.
Science in Context, 15, 577–603. doi:10.1017/S0269889702000650. K. Steinmetz, & G. W. Peterson (Eds.), Handbook of marriage and
Singh, I. (2004). Doing their jobs: Mothering with ritalin in a culture of the family (2nd ed., pp. 439–474). New York: Plenum Press. doi:10.
mother-blame. Social Science and Medicine, 59, 1193–1205. doi: 1007/978-1-4757-5367-7_17.
10.1016/j.socscimed.2004.01.011. Walzer, S. (1998). Thinking about the baby: Gender and transitions into
StataCorp. (2013). Stata Statistical Software: Release 13. College parenthood. Philadelphia, PA: Temple University Press.
Station, TX: StataCorp LP. Warner, J. (2005). Perfect madness: Motherhood in the age of anxiety.
Stoeber, J., & Otto, K. (2006). Positive conceptions of perfectionism: New York: Riverhead Books.
Approaches, evidence, challenges. Personality and Social Wilkins, C. (2006). A qualitative study exploring the support needs of
Psychology Review, 10, 295–319. doi:10.1207/s15327957pspr1004_2. first-time mothers on their journey towards intuitive parenting.
Stone, P. (2007). Opting out?: Why women really quit careers and head Midwifery, 22, 169–180. doi:10.1016/j.midw.2005.07.001.
home. Los Angeles: University of California Press.
Williams, K., Donaghue, N., & Kurz, T. (2013). BGiving guilt the flick^?
Sutherland, J. A. (2010). Mothering, guilt and shame. Sociology
An investigation of mothers’ talk about guilt in relation to infant
Compass, 4, 310–321. doi:10.1111/j.1751-9020.2010.00283.x.
feeding. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 37, 97–112. doi:10.
Teti, D. M., & Gelfand, D. M. (1991). Behavioral competence among
1177/0361684312463000.
mothers of infants in the first year: The mediational role of maternal
self-efficacy. Child Development, 62, 918–929. doi:10.2307/1131143. Wolf, N. (2001). Misconceptions: Truth, lies and the unexpected on the
U.S. Census Bureau, Geography Division. (2010). Census regions and journey to motherhood. New York: Random House.
divisions of the United States. Retrieved from http://www2.census. Zelizer, V. (1985). Pricing the priceless child: The changing social value
gov/geo/pdfs/maps-data/maps/reference/us_regdiv.pdf of children. New York: Basic Books, Inc.
Sex Roles is a copyright of Springer, 2016. All Rights Reserved.

You might also like