You are on page 1of 9

Journal of Personality Assessment

ISSN: 0022-3891 (Print) 1532-7752 (Online) Journal homepage: https://www.tandfonline.com/loi/hjpa20

The Narcissistic Personality Inventory: A Useful


Tool for Assessing Pathological Narcissism?
Evidence From Patients With Narcissistic
Personality Disorder

Aline Vater , Michela Schröder-Abé , Kathrin Ritter , Babette Renneberg ,


Lars Schulze , Jennifer K. Bosson & Stefan Roepke

To cite this article: Aline Vater , Michela Schröder-Abé , Kathrin Ritter , Babette Renneberg ,
Lars Schulze , Jennifer K. Bosson & Stefan Roepke (2013) The Narcissistic Personality
Inventory: A Useful Tool for Assessing Pathological Narcissism? Evidence From Patients With
Narcissistic Personality Disorder, Journal of Personality Assessment, 95:3, 301-308, DOI:
10.1080/00223891.2012.732636

To link to this article: https://doi.org/10.1080/00223891.2012.732636

Published online: 26 Oct 2012.

Submit your article to this journal

Article views: 2579

View related articles

Citing articles: 24 View citing articles

Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at


https://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=hjpa20
Journal of Personality Assessment, 95(3), 301–308, 2013
Copyright C Taylor & Francis Group, LLC
ISSN: 0022-3891 print / 1532-7752 online
DOI: 10.1080/00223891.2012.732636

The Narcissistic Personality Inventory: A Useful Tool for Assessing


Pathological Narcissism? Evidence From Patients With
Narcissistic Personality Disorder
ALINE VATER,1,2 MICHELA SCHRÖDER-ABÉ,3 KATHRIN RITTER,4 BABETTE RENNEBERG,4
LARS SCHULZE,4 JENNIFER K. BOSSON,5 AND STEFAN ROEPKE1,2
1
Cluster of Excellence Languages of Emotion, Freie Universität Berlin, Germany
2
Department of Psychiatry, Charité—Universitätsmedizin Berlin, Campus Benjamin Franklin, Germany
3
Department of Psychology, University of Bamberg, Germany
4
Department of Educational Science and Psychology, Freie Universität Berlin, Germany
5
Department of Psychology, University of South Florida

The Narcissistic Personality Inventory (NPI) has dominated research on narcissism in the field of social and personality psychology. Surprisingly,
it is unclear whether the NPI is useful for identifying pathological narcissism in patients with Narcissistic Personality Disorder (NPD). The goal of
this study was to close this research gap. We used an extreme-group approach by including NPD patients and healthy controls and comparing their
narcissism scores. We further investigated whether explicit self-esteem (assessed with the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale) suppressed the relationship
between group membership and NPI narcissism. According to our results, NPD patients do not score higher on the NPI in comparison to healthy
controls. Analysis of indirect effects revealed that differences in NPI scores are suppressed by NPD patients’ low self-esteem. Our results indicate
that the NPI is not a valid indicator of NPD, unless one controls for self-esteem. Implications for future research are discussed.

Narcissism as a construct is as old as psychology itself: Have- criticism or defeat.” In the current literature, pathological narcis-
lock Ellis first introduced narcissism as an “autoerotic disorder” sism is defined as a broad dysfunctional personality trait ranging
in 1898. Psychoanalytic theorists subsequently contributed to from mild to severe expressions. Here, we use the term patho-
the popularization of narcissism as a clinical phenomenon (i.e., logical narcissism to refer to a diagnosis of Narcissistic Person-
Freud, 1914/1957; Kernberg, 1975; Kohut, 1966). Since then, ality Disorder (NPD) as defined in the DSM–IV–TR and normal
scientists and laypersons alike have maintained an enduring in- narcissism to refer to nonclinical levels of narcissistic traits.
terest in narcissism. However, there is still substantial disagree- In social and personality psychology, the majority of studies
ment on the conceptualization and assessment of narcissism use the Narcissistic Personality Inventory (NPI) to capture nar-
between and within different disciplines. In one common con- cissism as a personality trait (Cain, Pincus, & Ansell, 2008).
ceptualization, narcissism is defined as a personality trait and Raskin and Hall (1979) designed the NPI as a measure of sub-
described dimensionally (Foster & Campbell, 2007). Many re- clinical narcissism. Starting with a list of 220 items, these re-
searchers in social and personality psychology use the terms searchers conducted a series of validation studies with students
normal or subclinical narcissism to refer to nonclinical levels that resulted in a 40-item final version with seven subscales:
of narcissistic tendencies (e.g., Miller & Campbell, 2008; Pin- authority, self-sufficiency, superiority, exhibitionism, exploita-
cus & Lukowitsky, 2010; Zeigler-Hill, Green, Arnau, Sisemore, tiveness, vanity, and entitlement (Raskin & Hall, 1979; Raskin
& Myers, 2011). In contrast, another conceptualization defines & Terry, 1988). Although the construction of the NPI is based on
narcissism as a personality disorder, characterized by “a perva- the clinical definition of NPD according to the third edition of
sive pattern of grandiosity, need for admiration, and a lack of the DSM (American Psychiatric Association, 1980), it is still un-
empathy” that leads to psychological distress (American Psy- clear whether the NPI captures pathological narcissism. Raskin
chiatric Association, 2000). Especially in clinical psychology and Terry (1988) themselves admitted that the NPI fails to cap-
and psychiatry, narcissism is primarily treated as a diagnostic ture the clinical picture of NPD. They stated that the validation
category and assessed with diagnostic interviews. The current process of the NPI led to a reduction of important psychological
Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (4th and behavioral dimensions inherent in pathological narcissism.
ed. [DSM–IV–TR]; American Psychiatric Association, 2000) Moreover, several researchers state that the NPI primarily
emphasizes grandiose aspects of narcissism, but also acknowl- concentrates on the grandiose facets of normal narcissism and
edges that a “vulnerability in self-esteem makes individuals with fails to capture the vulnerable qualities inherent to the disorder
Narcissistic Personality Disorder very sensitive to ‘injury’ from (e.g., feelings of shame and insecurity; e.g., Cramer, 2011). For
example, individuals with high scores on the NPI seem to present
a nondistressed, self-confident version of normal narcissism
(Pincus et al., 2009). In contrast, some researchers assume that
Received January 15, 2012; Revised July 2, 2012.
Address correspondence to Aline Vater, Cluster of Excellence Languages
the NPI assesses subclinical narcissism that is close to the defini-
of Emotion, Freie Universität Berlin, Habelschwerdter Allee 45 14195, Berlin, tion of NPD according to the DSM–IV–TR (Miller & Campbell,
Germany; Email: aline.vater@fu-berlin.de 2008; Paulhus & Williams, 2002; Wallace & Baumeister, 2002).

301
302 VATER ET AL.

This assumption is based on evidence that narcissism assessed bring, & Steinmeyer, 2001; Pukrop et al., 2009). Moreover, the
with the NPI is not necessarily advantageous (for a review see DAPP–BQ is a valuable indicator of symptom severity in pa-
Cain et al., 2008). tients with personality disorders: Pukrop et al. (2009) provided
In the past decade, several studies used the NPI as a tool evidence that patients with a variety of personality disorders
for identifying narcissism within nonclinical samples (Acker- scored higher on the narcissism subscale than nonclinical con-
man et al., 2011; Maxwell, Donnellan, Hopwood, & Ackerman, trols. The DAPP–BQ might thus be a valid tool for clinical
2011; Miller & Campbell, 2008; Pryor, Miller, & Gaughan, research with patients with NPD.
2009; Samuel & Widiger, 2008) or clinical samples including
patients with a variety of disorders (Fossati et al., 2009; Pin-
cus et al., 2009; Prifitera & Ryan, 1984; Svindseth, Nøttestad,
AIM OF THIS STUDY
Wallin, Roaldset, & Dahl, 2008). For example, Fossati et al. Our first goal was to examine the utility of the NPI for assess-
(2009) found that the NPI total score was a positive predictor ing pathological narcissism in NPD patients. In doing so, we
of NPD according to DSM–IV–TR criteria within a sample of use an extreme-groups approach by comparing the narcissism
people with various personality disorders. However, no single scores of patients with NPD with those of nonclinical controls.
study has used the NPI in a sample of patients exclusively di- As mentioned earlier, several researchers argue that the NPI pri-
agnosed with NPD. We assume that studying clinical patients marily assesses normal narcissism. However, the NPI predicted
with NPD will shed light on the discussion of whether the NPI NPD in a sample of clinical patients with various personal-
is a useful tool for assessing pathological narcissism. ity disorders (Fossati et al., 2009). Given that the literature is
When measuring narcissism with the NPI, researchers equivocal about whether the NPI can validly assess pathologi-
should consider the scale’s positive correlation with measures cal narcissism in patients with NPD, we compared NPI scores
of self-esteem. This characteristic might not only obfuscate among patients with NPD to those among nonclinical controls.
the NPI’s relation with other measures, but also suppress the As mentioned, there is evidence that self-esteem suppresses
differences between nonclinical and clinical samples. Several associations between NPI and other variables (Paulhus et al.,
studies show that the statistical removal of self-esteem increases 2004; Sedikides et al., 2004) and that patients with NPD report
the magnitude of associations between NPI narcissism and var- lower self-esteem in comparison to nonclinical controls (Vater
ious outcome variables (antisocial behavior: Paulhus, Robins, et al., 2012). It is thus possible that the NPI can only distin-
Trzesniewski, & Tracy, 2004; health: Sedikides, Rudich, Gregg, guish NPD patients from healthy controls after controlling for
Kumashiro, & Rusbult, 2004). Rosenthal and Hooley (2010) the suppressing effect of self-esteem. To provide evidence that
further showed that several NPI items are confounded with self-esteem, but not general psychological impairment, func-
self-esteem and are therefore unsuitable to differentiate between tions as a suppressor variable, we also included the Symptom
narcissists and nonnarcissists. As noted earlier, the DSM–IV–TR Checklist–90–Revised (SCL–90–R) and the Beck Depression
includes in its definition of NPD a tendency toward vulnerable Inventory (BDI).
self-esteem that is easily threatened by criticism or defeat. A second goal of this study was to assess the congruence of the
Moreover, recent research shows that patients with NPD have NPI with a measure of narcissism from clinical psychology and
lower self-esteem than healthy controls (Vater et al., 2012). psychiatry, the DAPP–BQ. As the DAPP–BQ is a useful mea-
Thus, when judging the utility of the NPI for clinical research, sure for assessing narcissism in clinical samples with personality
researchers should consider that self-esteem might act as a disorders (Pukrop et al., 2009), we hypothesized that patients
potential suppressor variable. That is, the relatively low self- with NPD, in comparison to nonclinical controls, would obtain
esteem of NPD patients could deflate their scores on the NPI. higher scores on the narcissism subscale of the DAPP–BQ.
Clinical psychology and psychiatry provide alternative mea- A third goal was to explore the overlap between narcissism
sures for assessing narcissism in clinical samples. One of the self-report measures (the NPI and the DAPP–BQ) and expert
most frequently used measures is the Dimensional Assessment ratings (Structured Clinical Interview for DSM–IV Personality
of Personality Pathology–Basic Questionnaire (DAPP–BQ). Disorders [SCID–II] scores). Renneberg, Chambless, Dowdall,
The DAPP–BQ was constructed to capture underlying dimen- Fauerbach, and Gracely (1992) found little overlap between
sions of personality pathology using a two-stage procedure the SCID–II interview and a self-report questionnaire for psy-
(Livesley & Jackson, 2002; Livesley, Jackson, & Schroeder, chopathology (the Millon Clinical Multiaxial Inventory–II; Mil-
1989; Livesley, Jang, & Vernon, 1998). First, the authors gen- lon, 1992). Therefore, we expected self-report measures and ex-
erated a pool of items from several sources including the de- pert ratings to be only weakly correlated. We also examined the
scription of NPD in the DSM–III, the literature on pathological correlation between the NPI and the DAPP–BQ. Recent stud-
personality traits, and input from practicing clinicians. In a sec- ies reported low or medium correlations between the NPI and
ond stage, these items were summarized to yield several factors, other measures of narcissism (Maxwell et al., 2011; Miller &
resulting in the final scale consisting of 18 subscales (e.g., in- Campbell, 2008; Samuel & Widiger, 2008), but no study has yet
secure attachment, identity problems). Of these subscales, one investigated the correlation between the NPI and the DAPP–BQ.
assesses pathological narcissism. Individuals who score high As both questionnaires assess the same underlying construct, we
on the DAPP–BQ narcissism subscale exaggerate their achieve- expected them to show a moderate association.
ments and abilities, crave admiration, are preoccupied with fan-
tasies, possess feelings of entitlement, and exhibit a strong need METHODS
for acceptance and approval (cf. Livesley & Jackson, 2002).
Recent studies provide strong support for its validity and relia- Participants
bility (e.g., Bagge & Trull, 2003; Gutiérrez-Zotes et al., 2008; We recruited 51 participants with a diagnosis of NPD ac-
Kushner, Quilty, Tackett, & Bagby, 2011; Pukrop, Gentil, Stein- cording to the DSM–IV–TR (American Psychiatric Association,
ASSESSING PATHOLOGICAL NARCISSISM 303

2000; German version: Saß, Wittchen, & Zaudig, 2003) and 44 Measures
nonclinical controls. To establish individual diagnoses in pa- Narcissistic Personality Inventory. The NPI consists of 40
tients, trained psychologists administered the German versions items in a forced choice format (Raskin & Terry, 1988; German
of the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM–IV Axis I Psy- version: Schütz, Marcus, & Sellin, 2004). According to our data,
chiatric Disorders (SCID–I; First, Spitzer, Gibbon, & Williams, the internal consistency (Cronbach’s alpha) of the scale was α
1996; German version: Wittchen, Zaudig, & Fydrich, 1997) = .83. The German version of the NPI consists of six subscales
and the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM–IV Personality (see Schütz et al., 2004). In the data set used here, internal
Disorders (SCID–II; First, Gibbon, Spitzer, Williams, & Ben- consistencies of all NPI subscales were low (average α = .58;
jamin, 1997; German version, Fydrich, Renneberg, Schmitz, & lowest α = .49; highest α = .74), and internal consistencies of
Wittchen, 1997). All diagnoses of NPD were verified with the other subscale solutions (four-factor solution by Emmons, 1984;
patients’ therapists (psychiatrist or psychologist) and the ther- two- and three-factor solutions by Kubarych, Deary, & Austin,
apists’ supervisor (last author, senior psychiatrist). Interrater 2004) were also lower than .50. We therefore only calculated
reliability of SCID–II personality disorder diagnoses was as- the overall score of the NPI.
sessed with eight patients with a pairwise diagnostic interview
design and three interviewers were blind to personality disorder
diagnoses. Kappa was acceptable, κ = 0.797.
The Dimensional Assessment of Personality Pathology.
The DAPP–BQ (Livesley & Jackson, 2002; German version:
NPD patients were excluded from participation if they had
Pukrop et al., 2001) is a reliable and valid dimensional measure
a history of psychotic disorder, current mania or hypomania,
of personality pathology (e.g., Pukrop et al., 2001). Due to the
current substance-induced disorder, or if they were nonnative
overall length of the questionnaire, we only used the narcissism
speakers. Furthermore, we controlled for mental retardation us-
subscale, which consists of 16 items. The 5-point response scale
ing a cognitive screening score for fluid intelligence (IQ > 80
ranges from 1 (very unlike me) to 5 (very like me). In this study,
as measured by subscale four of the German intelligence test
the internal consistency (Cronbach’s alpha) of the narcissism
Leistungsprüfsystem; Horn, 1983). Comorbid Axis I diagnoses
scale was α = .92.
of NPD patients included major depression (n = 20, 31.2%),
dysthymic disorder (n = 17, 33.3%), panic disorder (n = 2,
3.9%), agoraphobia (n = 2, 3.9%), generalized anxiety disor- Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale. Self-esteem was measured
der (n = 3, 5.9%), posttraumatic stress disorder (n = 7, 13.7%), using the total score of the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale (RSE;
obsessive–compulsive disorder (n = 2, 3.9%), adjustment disor- Rosenberg, 1965; German version: Collani & Herzberg, 2003).
der (n = 10, 19.6%), somatoform disorder (n = 3, 5.9%), alcohol This is a 10-item scale that measures people’s feelings of global
abuse (n = 11, 21.6%), drug abuse (n = 5, 9.8%), anorexia ner- self-worth. Responses were made on 4-point scales with end-
vosa (n = 4, 7.8%) and bulimia (n = 7, 13.7%). Comorbid points at 1 (strongly disagree) and 4 (strongly agree). The inter-
Axis II diagnoses of NPD patients included avoidant (n = 15, nal consistency (Cronbach’s alpha) of the scale was α = .92.
29.4%), dependent (n = 4, 7.8%), obsessive–compulsive (n =
15, 29.4%), paranoid (n = 19, 37.3%), schizoid (n = 3, 5.9%), Symptom Checklist–90–Revised. The SCL–90–R (Dero-
histrionic (n = 7, 13.7%), borderline (n = 27, 52.9%), antisocial gatis, 1997; German version: Franke, 2002) was employed to
(n = 23, 45.1%), and negativistic (n = 21, 41.2%) personality assess general psychopathological and physical impairments
disorder. Twenty-nine of the clinical patients were on various during the last week. Responses were made on 5-point scales
medications (antipsychotics n = 6, 11.8%; and antidepressants, with endpoints at 0 (not at all) and 4 (very much). The Global
n = 23, 45.1%). Severity Index (GSI) sum score of the SCL–90–R indicates
Participants in the nonclinical control group were screened for psychopathological impairment in general. The internal
current or lifetime diagnoses for any Axis I or Axis II disorder consistency (Cronbach’s alpha) of the scale was α = .97.
with the SCID–I (First et al., 1996; German version: Wittchen
et al., 1997) and SCID–II (First et al., 1997; German version: Beck Depression Inventory. The BDI–I (Beck, Ward,
Fydrich et al., 1997) questionnaires. We excluded individuals Mendelson, Mock, & Erbaugh, 1961; German version:
who met criteria of any present or past Axis I disorder or more Hautzinger, Bailer, Worall, & Keller, 1995) was employed to
than two criteria for any personality disorders assessed by the assess severity of depression. Participants responded to 21 items
SCID–II. on 4-point scales that were represented by four different state-
ments. The BDI is a widely used and well-validated self-report
Procedure measure for depression and reflects the individual’s experience
of specific symptoms over the past week. The internal consis-
All NPD patients were enrolled in a broad multicen- tency (Cronbach’s alpha) of the scale was α = .94.
ter clinical study on NPD at the Department of Psychiatry,
Charité—Universitätsmedizin Berlin, or at cooperating hospi-
tals and outpatient settings in Germany. During their inpatient RESULTS
treatment, patients with NPD were consecutively recruited as Descriptive Statistics and Group Differences
participants for this study. Nonclinical participants were re- Table 1 presents the means and standard deviations of all
cruited through newspaper advertisements. The groups were measures as well as the t-test results. Compared to the control
matched with respect to years of education, age, and gen- group, individuals with NPD had significantly higher scores
der. The study was approved by the ethics committee of the on general psychological impairment (indicated by the GSI
Charité—Universitätsmedizin Berlin. Informed consent to par- of the SCL–90–R), higher depression (BDI), and lower self-
ticipate was obtained from each participant. esteem (RSE). Moreover, NPD patients had significantly higher
304 VATER ET AL.

TABLE 1.—Descriptive statistics and t tests of all variables. gression analyses were conducted and indirect effects were ana-
lyzed. Suppression is present when statistically controlling for a
NPD CG t Test
variable (here, self-esteem) increases the magnitude of the rela-
Scale M SD M SD t df p d
tionship between the independent variable (here, group member-
RSE 2.79 0.90 3.87 0.90 –5.99 93 .000 1.20 ship) and the dependent variable (here, narcissism; MacKinnon
SCL–90–R 1.50 0.68 .39 0.51 9.04 91.38 .000 1.84 & Dwyer, 1993; MacKinnon, Krull, & Lockwood, 2000). We
BDI 24.22 11.96 4.97 8.51 9.18 88.28 .000 1.85 entered group membership as the independent variable (with
DAPP–BQ 3.07 0.64 2.46 0.62 3.93 92 .000 0.97 NPD = 0, nonclinical controls = 1), NPI narcissism as the
Narcissism
NPI 13.24 6.67 12.95 6.27 0.21 92 .832 0.04
dependent variable, and self-esteem as a control variable. We
SCID–II 5.70 1.20 0.50 0.82 23.42 89 .000 5.06 present the unstandardized coefficients in Figure 1, as this is
recommended in causal modeling (Preacher & Hayes, 2008).
Note. NPD = narcissistic personality disorder; CG = control group; RSE = Rosen- Bootstrapping with 1,000 bootstrap samples (see Preacher &
berg Self-Esteem Scale; SCL–90–R = Symptom Checklist–90–Revised; BDI = Beck
Depression Inventory; DAPP–BQ = Dimensional Assessment of Personality Pathology; Hayes) was used to assess the indirect effects. Bootstrapping is
NPI = Narcissistic Personality Inventory; SCID–II = Structured Clinical Interview for a nonparametric sampling procedure that generates an empirical
DSM–IV–TR Personality Disorders; d = effect size, Cohen’s d. approximation of the sampling distribution of a statistic from
the available data. Point estimates and 95% confidence intervals
DAPP–BQ scores in comparison to nonclinical control partic- are estimated for the indirect effects. When a confidence interval
ipants. Scores on the NPI did not differ significantly between does not contain zero, the point estimate for that indirect effect
groups (Cohen’s d = .04). is considered significant.
As shown in Figure 1, group membership did not predict
Correlations narcissism as measured with the NPI (c = –.286, p = .832).
We therefore explored whether self-esteem influenced the asso-
Table 2 presents correlations among all variables. We calcu- ciation of group membership and NPI narcissism. First, group
lated correlations separately for both groups to prevent over- or membership predicted self-esteem (a = 1.119, p < .001). Sec-
underestimation of correlations due to group effects. As shown ond, self-esteem predicted NPI narcissism (b = 2.624, p <
in Table 2, the NPI and DAPP–BQ were moderately related .001). Third and most important, when self-esteem was entered
in both groups. Furthermore, both the NPI and the DAPP–BQ into the model, group membership significantly predicted NPI
showed low correlations with SCID–II expert ratings in both narcissism (c’ = –3.220, p = .036), indicating that self-esteem
groups. Importantly, NPI narcissism was associated with higher suppressed the effect of group membership on NPI narcissism.
self-esteem, lower depression, and lower general psychologi- Furthermore, the confidence interval for the indirect pathway
cal impairment, whereas DAPP–BQ narcissism correlated with via self-esteem did not include zero, indicating a significant
higher depression and higher symptom severity. suppression pathway. In contrast, general symptom severity
Using Fisher r-to-z transformation, we investigated whether and depression did not function as suppressors between group
the strength of these correlation coefficients significantly dif- membership and NPI narcissism (see Figure 1). Moreover, self-
fered between NPD patients and nonclinical controls. A signifi- esteem did not influence the association of group membership
cant effect emerged for self-esteem and DAPP–BQ narcissism: and DAPP–BQ narcissism.
The narcissism subscale of the DAPP–BQ was unrelated to
self-esteem within the group of NPD patients, but it was nega-
tively correlated with self-esteem among nonclinical individuals DISCUSSION
(p = .020, z = 2.33). No other correlation coefficients differed
between the two groups (ps > .10). The study reported here is the first to evaluate the utility of the
NPI for research with NPD patients. In summary, patients with a
Suppression Analysis diagnosis of NPD did not score higher on the NPI than nonclin-
ical controls. This occurred because of the suppression effect of
To examine whether self-esteem functions as a suppressor of self-esteem. Given that other variables (i.e., general psychologi-
group membership when predicting NPI narcissism, multiple re- cal impairment and depression) did not influence the association
between group membership and the NPI, the suppression effect
TABLE 2.—Correlations of all variables. we found here is specific to self-esteem. Unlike the NPI, the
DAPP–BQ differentiated between NPD patients and nonclin-
SCL– DAPP–BQ SCID– ical individuals and might thus represent a more appropriate
RSE 90–R BDI Narcissism NPI II
instrument for identifying NPD. Moreover, the DAPP–BQ and
RSE –.459∗∗∗ –.579∗∗∗ –.044 .381∗∗ –.222
the NPI were moderately correlated. However, both of these
SCL–90–R –.732∗∗∗ .616∗∗∗ –.100 –.177 –.002 self-report inventories showed little overlap with expert ratings
BDI –.688∗∗∗ .905∗∗∗ –.082 –.163 –.086 (SCID–II interview). In the following sections, we discuss our
DAPP–BQ –.493∗ .285 .257 .506∗∗∗ .090 results in detail.
Narcissism
NPI .302∗ –.205 –.319∗ .341 .089
SCID–II –.036 .216 .134 .342 .120 Suppression Analysis
Note. Narcissistic personality disorder above diagonal, control group below diagonal.
The finding that low self-esteem suppresses scores on the NPI
RSE = Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale; SCL–90–R = Symptom Checklist–90–Revised; in patients with NPD has a variety of theoretical and practical
BDI = Beck Depression Inventory; DAPP–BQ = Dimensional Assessment of Personality implications. Of particular importance is the analysis wherein
Pathology; NPI = Narcissistic Personality Inventory; SCID–II = Structured Clinical Inter-
view for DSM–IV–TR Personality Disorders.
NPD patients, in comparison to nonclinical controls, scored

p < .05. ∗∗ p < .01. ∗ ∗ ∗ p < .001. higher on the NPI after controlling for self-esteem. Conversely,
ASSESSING PATHOLOGICAL NARCISSISM 305

c= –.286 (se = 1.340), p = .832

RSE: c’= –3.220 (se = 1.516), p = .036


GSI: c’= 2.715 (se = 1.801), p = .135
BDI: c’= –2.918 (se = 1.341), p = .109

c/c’
Group Narcissism
Membership (NPI)

Self-Esteem (RSE)
a b
Psychological Impairment (GSI)
Beck Depression Inventory (BDI)

RSE: a= 1.119 (se = .177), p < .001 RSE: b = 2.624 (se = .747), p < .001
GSI: a = –101.120 (se = 11.352), p < .000 GSI: b = –.024 (se = .012), p = .050
BDI: a= –.320 (se = –19.490), p < .000 BDI: b = –.223 (se = –1.135), p < .000

FIGURE 1.—Effects of group membership on Narcissistic Personality Inventory (NPI) narcissism with self-esteem (RSE), psychological impairment (GSI), and
depression (BDI) as suppressor variables. Note. Unstandardized coefficients presented. The total effects (weight c) is composed of a direct effect (weight c’) and
the indirect effect (sum of all a an b weights); se = standard deviation; group membership: Narcissistic Personality Disorder = 0, control group = 1.

the DAPP–BQ’s ability to identify NPD was unaffected by the feelings of vulnerability are a central component of patholog-
self-esteem of NPD patients. The fact that the NPI is contami- ical narcissism (e.g., Ackerman et al., 2011; Cain et al., 2008;
nated with self-esteem implies that this measure is not optimal Maxwell et al., 2011; Miller & Campbell; Miller et al., 2012;
for detecting NPD. Accordingly, researchers should consider Pincus et al., 2009). Although we can only conjecture at this
removing the influence of self-esteem when investigating rela- point, it might be possible that the narcissism subscale of the
tions of NPI narcissism with other variables. Doing so might DAPP–BQ captures both grandiose aspects of narcissism (e.g.,
enhance the validity of the NPI as a predictor of narcissism in “I am destined for greatness,” “I dream of being looked up to and
clinical samples with NPD. admired”) and the vulnerability to self-esteem threats described
Significant suppressor effects have often been overlooked in the DSM–IV–TR (e.g., “I get very anxious if I think some-
in psychological research, although suppressor analyses might one does not like me,” “I am only really satisfied when people
be fruitful when establishing theoretical models of narcissism acknowledge how good I am”). Future research should investi-
(Paulhus et al., 2004). Paulhus et al. (2004) suggested that there gate the shared and distinct components of different narcissism
might be a legitimate personality component captured by the measures with respect to grandiosity and vulnerability.
shared variance of narcissism and self-esteem. For instance, it is One finding that merits further analysis was the low correla-
plausible that individuals high in NPI narcissism and individu- tion between self-report inventories (NPI and DAPP–BQ) and
als high in self-esteem share positive self-evaluations. However, expert ratings (SCID–II) of narcissism. This finding is consistent
Paulhus et al. argued that narcissism and self-esteem are distinct: with another work showing low overlap between the SCID–II
Whereas self-esteem reflects realistic self-favorability, NPI nar- interview and a self-report questionnaire for psychopathology
cissism instead reflects unrealistically positive self-evaluations. (Renneberg et al., 1992). Differences in the conceptualization of
Neither one is likely perfectly realistic, though, as both variables narcissism might account for the low associations between the
include some discrepancies with reality (Paulhus, Harms, Bruce, SCID–II and both self-report inventories used here. For instance,
& Lysy, 2003). It thus remains unclear whether the overlap be- the SCID–II interview assesses diagnostic criteria that are not
tween narcissism and self-esteem can be isolated or remains an captured by the NPI or the DAPP–BQ. Consider the finding that
artifact. a lack of empathy is characteristic of patients with NPD (Ritter
et al., 2011). Yet, neither the NPI nor the DAPP–BQ assesses
Correlations Between Self-Report Inventories and a lack of empathy. Second, the threshold for receiving a diag-
Expert Ratings nosis of NPD is quite high in the diagnostic interview, whereas
The NPI was moderately correlated with the DAPP–BQ. This it might be considerably easier to obtain a high score on the
finding is consistent with studies demonstrating overlap be- self-report inventories. Whereas the DAPP–BQ and NPI narcis-
tween the NPI and other narcissism measures (Maxwell et al., sism scales only assess whether or not grandiose fantasies occur
2011; Miller & Campbell, 2008; Samuel & Widiger, 2008). at all, a patient with NPD only meets criteria for “fantasies” if
In essence, this strengthens the assumption that the NPI and his or her fantasies reflect an enduring, inflexible pattern across
the DAPP–BQ capture associated but independent facets of the a broad range of situations that leads to psychological distress
same underlying construct. Several authors have argued that the (e.g., restricted work efficiency). Moreover, the diagnostic cri-
NPI assesses grandiose facets of normal narcissism (Miller & teria must be traced back to early adolescence or the beginning
Campbell; Miller, Price, & Campbell, 2012; Samuel & Widiger). of adulthood, information that is not assessed in the narcissism
This conceptualization of narcissism seems to be too narrow, as self-report measures used here.
306 VATER ET AL.

Limitations for detecting NPD. However, the pattern of correlations of the


Several limitations of this study merit attention. A first crit- NPI with other measures was similar in the NPD group and the
ical issue pertains to the selection of patients with NPD, who healthy control group. This indicates that results from studies
exhibited high comorbidity rates in our study. Nevertheless, of healthy individuals using the NPI might provide a better un-
the observed comorbidity rates in our study are comparable derstanding of NPD, at least when it comes to grandiose facets.
to those reported in other studies (Russ, Shedler, Bradley, & Moreover, we suggest that researchers be explicit about how
Westen, 2008). We therefore assume that our clinical sample is they define narcissism (i.e., state whether normal or pathological
representative of patients who seek treatment. narcissism is their focus). This might help facilitate a common
Another potential limitation was our concentration on self- understanding of what normal and pathological narcissism are,
esteem as a suppressor variable. Other mediators might also be increase empirical knowledge of the narcissism construct, and
important, such as the influence of dominance. For instance, provide a rationale for choice of measurement tools (also see
Brown and Zeigler-Hill (2004) found that the association be- Miller, Maples, & Campbell, 2011).
tween self-esteem and narcissism depends on the overlap of each
measure with dominance as a personality trait. The greater the ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
overlap of the self-esteem measure with dominance, the higher This research was supported by a doctoral fellowship from
its association with narcissism. Controlling for dominance led to the Cluster of Excellence Languages of Emotion, FU Berlin
a diminished association of self-esteem and the NPI. We there- (Aline Vater), from Charité—Universitätsmedizin Berlin (to
fore believe that future studies should focus on untangling the Kathrin Ritter), and the foundation Sonnenfeld-Stiftung, Berlin
links between normal and pathological narcissism, self-esteem, (Kathrin Ritter). We are grateful to the cooperating Departments
and other personality traits such as dominance. of Psychiatry of the following hospitals: Theodor-Wenzel-Werk,
Another issue that merits critical debate is the low internal Berlin; Asklepios Clinik North, Hamburg; and the Institute for
consistencies of all six NPI subscales of the NPI. Due to these Behavioral Therapy (IVB GmbH), Berlin; for their assistance
low internal consistencies, we only used the overall sum score in with patient recruitment. We would also like to thank two un-
analyses. Low internal consistencies (e.g., del Rosario & White, known reviewers and the editor for their valuable comments on
2005) and an unstable factor structure were described in other the article.
studies of the NPI, and a confirmatory analysis (Kubarych et al.,
2004) suggested three rather than seven stable factors (power,
exhibitionism, being a special person). Furthermore, Emmons REFERENCES
(1984) found four NPI factors, and in the German version of
Ackerman, R. A., Witt, E. A., Donnellan, M. B., Trzesniewski, K. H., Robins,
the NPI, six factors were found (Schütz et al., 2004). Given this
R. W., & Kashy, D. A. (2011). What does the Narcissistic Personality Inven-
inconsistency across studies, the standard practice is to use the tory really measure? Assessment, 18, 67–87. doi:10.1177/1073191110382845
more valid and reliable overall sum score, which is, unfortu- American Psychiatric Association. (1980). Diagnostic and statistical manual of
nately, uneconomical and reduces the complexity of the narcis- mental disorders (3rd ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
sism construct (Cain et al., 2008). Analyzing specific NPI scales American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of
might have been profitable for this study. Using a large sample mental disorders (4th ed., text rev.). Washington, DC: Author.
of nonclinical individuals, Maxwell et al. (2011) reported that Bagge, C. L., & Trull, T. J. (2003). DAPP–BQ: Factor structure and relations to
the links between self-esteem and narcissism fluctuated across personality disorder symptoms in a nonclinical sample. Journal of Personality
NPI subscales. Future studies with bigger samples might thus Disorders, 17, 19–32.
want to analyze the subscales separately. Beck, A. T., Ward, C. H., Mendelson, M., Mock, J., & Erbaugh, J. (1961).
An inventory for measuring depression. Archives of General Psychiatry, 4,
Finally, we included only two self-report measures of narcis-
561–571. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.1961.01710120031004
sism. One promising new measure is the Pathological Narcis- Brown, R. P., & Zeigler-Hill, V. (2004). Narcissism and the non-equivalence
sism Inventory (Pincus et al., 2009). We could not include this of self-esteem measures: A matter of dominance? Journal of Research in
measure, however, as there is currently no German translation Personality, 38, 585–592. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2003.11.002
for it. The findings reported here should therefore be replicated Cain, N. M., Pincus, A. L., & Ansell, E. B. (2008). Narcissism at the crossroads:
with additional narcissism measures. Phenotypic description of pathological narcissism across clinical theory,
social/personality psychology, and psychiatric diagnosis. Clinical Psychology
Review, 28, 638–656. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2007.09.006
Implications for Future Research Collani, G. V., & Herzberg, P. Y. (2003). Eine revidierte Fassung der
This study has several implications for future research on nar- deutschsprachigen Skala zum Selbstwertgefühl von Rosenberg [A revised
cissism. We propose that researchers should be more cautious version of the German adaptation of Rosenberg’s Self-Esteem Scale].
with planning studies, especially when choosing assessment Zeitschrift für Differentielle und Diagnostische Psychologie, 24, 3–7.
devices. Measures should be chosen according to the research Cramer, P. (2011). Young adult narcissism: A 20 year longitudinal study
question at hand. If researchers aim to investigate the grandiose of the contribution of parenting styles, preschool precursors of nar-
cissism, and denial. Journal of Research in Personality, 45, 19–28.
aspects of narcissism, the NPI seems to be an appropriate as-
doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2010.11.004
sessment tool. If researchers wish to investigate vulnerability, del Rosario, P. M., & White, R. M. (2005). The Narcissistic Personality Inven-
however, other questionnaires (e.g., the narcissism subscale of tory: Test–retest stability and internal consistency. Personality and Individual
the DAPP–BQ or the Pathological Narcissism Inventory; Pin- Differences, 39, 1075–1081. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2005.08.001
cus et al., 2009) might be indicated. As the NPI is contaminated Derogatis, L. R. (1997). SCL–90–R, administration, scoring & procedures man-
by self-esteem, and self-esteem is impaired in most patients ual for the R(evised) version. Baltimore, MD: John Hopkins University School
with clinical disorders including NPD, the NPI is not useful of Medicine.
ASSESSING PATHOLOGICAL NARCISSISM 307

Ellis, H. (1898). Autoeroticism: A psychological study. Alienist and Neurologist, Miller, J. D., & Campbell, W. K. (2008). Comparing clinical and social-
19, 260–299. personality conceptualizations of narcissism. Journal of Personality, 76,
Emmons, R. A. (1984). Factor analysis and construct validity of the Narcissis- 449–476. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.2008.00492.x
tic Personality Inventory. Journal of Personality Assessment, 48, 291–300. Miller, J. D., Maples, J., & Campbell, W. K. (2011). Comparing the construct
doi:10.1207/s15327752jpa4803 11 validity of scales derived from the Narcissistic Personality Inventory: A reply
First, M. B., Gibbon, M., Spitzer, R. L., Williams, J. B. W., & Benjamin, to Rosenthal and Hooley (2010). Journal of Research in Personality, 45,
L. (1997). Structured Clinical Interview for DSM–IV Personality Disorders 401–407. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2010.12.004
(SCID–II). Washington DC: American Psychiatric Press. Miller, J. D., Price, J., & Campbell, W. K. (2012). Is the Narcissistic
First, M. B., Spitzer, R. L., Gibbon, M., & Williams, J. B. W. (1996). Structured Personality Inventory still relevant? A test of independent grandiosity
Clinical Interview for DSM–IV Axis I Disorders Research Version (SCID–I). and entitlement scales in the assessment of narcissism. Assessment, 19,
New York, NY: New York State Psychiatric Institute, Biometrics Research. 8–13.
Fossati, A., Borroni, S., Grazioli, F., Dornetti, L., Marcassoli, I., Maffei, C., Millon, T. (1992). Millon Clinical Multiaxial Inventory: I and II. Jour-
& Cheek, J. (2009). Tracking the hypersensitive dimension in narcissism: nal of Counseling & Development, 70, 421–426. doi:10.1002/j.1556-
Reliability and validity of the Hypersensitive Narcissism Scale. Personality 6676.1992.tb01627.x
and Mental Health, 3, 235–247. doi:10.1002/pmh.92 Paulhus, D. L., Harms, P. D., Bruce, M. N., & Lysy, D. C. (2003).
Foster, J. D., & Campbell, W. K. (2007). Are there such things as “Narcis- The over-claiming technique: Measuring self-enhancement independent
sists” in social psychology? A taxometric analysis of the Narcissistic Per- of ability. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 681–693.
sonality Inventory. Personality and Individual Differences, 43, 1321–1332. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.84.4.890
doi:10.1016/j.paid.2007.04.003 Paulhus, D. L., Robins, R. W., Trzesniewski, K. H., & Tracy, J. L. (2004).
Franke, G. H. (2002). SCL–90–R: Die Symptom-Checkliste von Derogatis, Two replicable suppressor situations in personality research. Multivariate
Deutsche Version. [SCL–90–R Symptom Checklist by Derogatis, German Behavioral Research, 39, 301–326. doi:10.1207/s15327906mbr3902 7
version]. Göttingen, Germany: Testzentrale. Paulhus, D. L., & Williams, K. (2002). The dark triad of personality: Narcissism,
Freud, S. (1957). On narcissism: An introduction. In Standard edition (Vol. Machiavellianism, and psychopathy. Journal of Research in Personality, 36,
14, pp. 73, 102). London, England: Hogarth Press. (Original work published 556–568. doi:10.1016/S0092-6566(02)00505-6
1914) Pincus, A. L., Ansell, E. B., Pimentel, C. A., Cain, N. M., Wright,
Fydrich, T., Renneberg, B., Schmitz, B., & Wittchen, H.-U. (1997). A. G. C., & Levy, K. N. (2009). Initial construction and validation of the
SKID–II—Strukturiertes Klinisches Interview für DSM–IV Achse II: Pathological Narcissism Inventory. Psychological Assessment, 21, 365–379.
Persönlichkeitsstörungen [SCID II, Structured clinical interview for DSM–IV doi:10.1037/a0016530
Axis II: Personality disorders]. Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe. Pincus, A. L., & Lukowitsky, M. R. (2010). Pathological narcissism and narcis-
Gutiérrez-Zotes, J. A., Gutiérrez, F., Valero, J., Gallego, E., Bailles, E., Torres, sistic personality disorder. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 6, 421–446.
X., . . . Livesley, W. J. (2008). Structure of personality pathology in normal and doi:10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.121208.131215
clinical samples: Spanish validation of the DAPPBQ. Journal of Personality Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2008). Asymptotic and resampling strategies
Disorders, 22, 389–404. doi:10.1521/pedi.2008.22.4.389 for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models.
Hautzinger, M., Bailer, M., Worall, H., & Keller, F. (1995). Beck-Depressions- Behavior Research Methods, 40, 879–891. doi:10.3758/BRM.40.3.879
Inventar (BDI) Testhandbuch [Beck Depression Inventory manual]. Bern, Prifitera, A., & Ryan, J. (1984). Validity of the narcissistic person-
Switzerland: Huber. ality inventory in a psychiatric sample. Joumal of Clinical Psy-
Horn, W. (1983). L-P-S Leistungsprüfsystem [Mental Abilities Test]. Göttingen, chology, 44, 140–142. doi:10.1002/1097-4679(198401)40:1<140::AID-
Germany: Hogrefe. JCLP2270400127>3.0.CO;2-E
Kernberg, O. (1975). Borderline conditions and pathological narcissism. New Pryor, L. R., Miller, J. D., & Gaughan, E. T. (2009). Testing two alternative
York, NY: Aronson. pathological personality measures in the assessment of psychopathy: An
Kohut, H. (1966). Forms and transformations of narcissism. Journal of the examination of the SNAP and DAPP–BQ. Journal of Personality Disorders,
American Psychoanalytical Association, 14, 243–272. 23, 85–100. doi:10.1521/pedi.2009.23.1.85
Kubarych, T. S., Deary, I. J., & Austin, E. J. (2004). The Narcissistic Personality Pukrop, R., Gentil, I., Steinbring, I., & Steinmeyer, E. (2001). Factorial struc-
Inventory: Factor structure in a nonclinical sample. Personality and Individual ture of the German version of the Dimensional Assessment of Person-
Differences, 36, 857–872. doi:10.1016/S0191-8869(03)00158-2 ality Pathology–Basic Questionnaire in clinical and nonclinical samples.
Kushner, S., Quilty, L. C., Tackett, J. L., & Bagby, R. M. (2011). Journal of Personality Disorders, 15, 450–456. doi:10.1521/pedi.15.5.450.
The hierarchical structure of the Dimensional Assessment of Personality 19195
Pathology (DAPP–BQ). Journal of Personality Disorders, 25, 504–516. Pukrop, R., Steinbring, I., Gentil, I., Schulte, C., Larstone, R., & Livesley,
doi:10.1521/pedi.2011.25.4.504 J. W. (2009). Clinical validity of the Dimensional Assessment of Person-
Livesley, W. J., & Jackson, D. N. (2002). Manual for the dimensional assessment ality Pathology (DAPP) for psychiatric patients with and without a per-
of personality pathology. Port Huron, MI: Sigma Press. sonality disorder diagnosis. Journal of Personality Disorders, 23, 572–586.
Livesley, W. J., Jackson, D. N., & Schroeder, M. L. (1989). A study of the doi:10.1521/pedi.2009.23.6.572
factorial structure of personality pathology. Journal of Personality Disorders, Raskin, R. N., & Hall, C. S. (1979). A narcissistic personality inventory. Psy-
3, 292–306. doi:10.1521/pedi.1989.3.4.292 chological Reports, 45, 590. doi:10.2466/pr0.1979.45.2.590
Livesley, W. J., Jang, K. L., & Vernon, P. A. (1998). Phenotypic and ge- Raskin, R., & Terry, H. (1988). A principal component analysis of the Narcissis-
netic structure of traits. Archives of General Psychiatry, 55, 941–948. tic Personality Inventory and further evidence of its construct validity. Jour-
doi:10.1001/archpsyc.55.10.941 nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 890–902. doi:10.1037//0022-
MacKinnon, D. P., & Dwyer, J. H. (1993). Estimating medi- 3514.54.5.890
ated effects in prevention studies. Evaluation Review, 17, 144–158. Renneberg, B., Chambless, D. L., Dowdall, D. J., Fauerbach, J. A., & Gracely,
doi:10.1177/0193841X9301700202 E. J. (1992). The Structured Clinical Interview for DSM–III–R, Axis II and
MacKinnon, D. P., Krull, J. L., & Lockwood, C. M. (2000). Equivalence of the Millon Clinical Multiaxial Inventory: A concurrent validity study of per-
the mediation, confounding, and suppression effect. Prevention Science, 1, sonality disorders among agoraphobic outpatients. Journal of Personality
173–181. doi:10.1023/A:1026595011371 Disorders, 6, 117–124. doi:10.1521/pedi.1992.6.2.117
Maxwell, K., Donnellan, M. B., Hopwood, C. J., & Ackerman, R. A. (2011). Ritter, K., Dziobek, I., Preißler, S., Rüter, A., Vater, A., Fydrich, T., . . . Roepke,
The two faces of Narcissus? An empirical comparison of the Narcissistic S. (2011). Lack of empathy in patients with Narcissistic Personality Dis-
Personality Inventory and the Pathological Narcissism Inventory. Personality order. Psychiatry Research, 187, 241–247. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2010.09.
and Individual Differences, 50, 577–582. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2010.11.031 013
308 VATER ET AL.

Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the adolescent self-image. Princeton, NJ: Sedikides, C., Rudich, E. A., Gregg, A. P., Kumashiro, M., & Rusbult, C. (2004).
Princeton University Press. Are normal narcissists psychologically healthy? Self-esteem matters. Jour-
Rosenthal, S., & Hooley, J. M. (2010). Narcissism assessment in social- nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87, 400–416. doi:10.1037/0022-
personality research: Does the association between narcissism and psycho- 3514.87.3.400
logical health result from a confound with self-esteem? Journal of Research Svindseth, M. F., Nøttestad, J. A., Wallin, J., Roaldset, J., & Dahl A. A. (2008).
in Personality, 44, 453–465. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2010.05.008 Narcissism in patients admitted to psychiatric acute wards: Its relation to
Russ, E., Shedler, J., Bradley, R., & Westen, D. (2008). Refining the con- violence, suicidality and other psychopathology. BMC Psychiatry, 8, 13.
struct of narcissistic personality disorder: Diagnostic criteria and sub- doi:10.1186/1471-244X-8-13
types. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 165, 1473–1481. doi:10.1176/ Vater, A., Ritter, K., Schröder-Abé, M., Schütz, A., Lammers, C. H., Bosson, J.,
appi.ajp.2008.07030376 & Roepke, S. (2012). When grandiosity and vulnerability collide: Implicit and
Samuel, D. B., & Widiger, T. A. (2008). Convergence of narcissism mea- explicit self-esteem in patients with narcissistic personality disorder. Journal
sures from the perspective of general personality functioning. Assessment, of Behavioral Therapy and Experimental Psychology, 44(1), 37–47.
15, 364–374. doi:10.1177/1073191108314278 Wallace, H. M., & Baumeister, R. F. (2002). The performance of narcissists
Saß, H., Wittchen, H.-U., & Zaudig, M. (2003). Diagnostisches und Statistisches rises and falls with perceived opportunity for glory. Journal of Personality
Manual Psychischer Störungen—Textrevision (DSM–IV–TR) [The diagnostic and Social Psychology, 82, 819–834. doi:10.1037//0022-3514.82.5.819
and statistical manual of mental disorders]. Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe. Wittchen, H.-U., Zaudig, M., & Fydrich, T. (1997). Strukturiertes Klinisches
Schütz, A., Marcus, B., & Sellin, I. (2004). Die Messung von Narzissmus Interview für DSM–IV–TR - Achse I [Structured Clinical Interview for
als Persönlichkeitskonstrukt: Psychometrische Eigenschaften einer Lang- DSM–IV–TR - Axis I]. Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.
und einer Kurzform des Deutschen NPI [Measuring narcissism as a per- Zeigler-Hill, V., Green, B. A., Arnau, R. C., Sisemore, T. B., & Myers,
sonality construct: Psychometric properties of a long and a short version E. M. (2011). Trouble ahead, trouble behind: Narcissism and early maladap-
of the German Narcissistic Personality Inventory]. Diagnostica, 4, 202– tive schemas. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 42,
218. 96–103. doi:10.1016/j.jbtep.2010.07.004

You might also like