You are on page 1of 24

EDITORIAL

Dream Bizarreness Reconsidered


Claudio Colace, Ph.D.

INTRODUCTION neurobiological conditions that are found


only in REM sleep (7-14). In addition, this
reams frequently show impossible approach emphasizes the role of PGO
D and/or improbable aspects compared
with common life experiences, which have
(ponto-geniculo-occipital) activity in
determining dream bizarreness, on which
been referred to as “dream bizarreness”. two other neurobiological models, the
Although the issue of dream bizarreness “reverse learning” theory (15) and
has already been discussed by the Seligman and Yellen’s model (16) are also
psychoanalytic dream theory, the first grounded.
systematic investigations on this aspect of This paper will review the literature on
dreaming only began in the Sixties, when a dream bizarreness in order to demonstrate
few authors started attempting to measure that the current neurobiological approach
dream bizarreness by objective indicators to dreaming, represented primarily by the
(1-3). “activation-synthesis” hypothesis, is
The “activation-synthesis” model (4) reductive and not consistent with various
later proposed a first systematic attempt to research data.
explain the causes of dream bizarreness The paper, after an attempt to define
after Freud’s initial hypothesis. This model the term “dream bizarreness”, is structured
was based on the neurobiological events of as follows: Theoretical Models (Part I),
REM sleep and opened a new perspective Measuring Dream Bizarreness (Part II),
to investigation. However, it automatically Empirical Data and Implications (Part III),
invalidated the psychoanalytic approach and Conclusions.
and therefore excluded any sort of analysis
of psychological or motivational DEFINING DREAM BIZARRENESS
determinants. At the same time the
cognitive-psychological approach to According to the Webster’s New
dreaming processes (5,6) was limited to Collegiate Dictionary, there are two
analysing dream bizarreness generation features that define the word “bizarreness”:
mechanisms (i.e., “how”) rather than its 1.Improbability (strikingly out of the
possible reasons and causes (i.e., “why?”). ordinary) and 2.Unusualness, oddness,
Therefore, investigations on the causes of extravagance. Several terms have been
dream bizarreness were restricted to the used in literature to describe bizarreness,
neurobiological level alone. for example, “distortion from reality”,
Still according to the more recently “metamorphosis”, “implausibility”, but
revised versions of the “activation- many authors agree that the concept of
synthesis” hypothesis, dream bizarreness bizarreness includes both: a) Impossibility,
can be fully explained by the particular and b) Improbability and/or oddness
Acknowledgments: I am grateful to Dr. Vincenzo Natale for compared to “common daily experiences”.
critical reading and constructive comments on the earlier The first dimension includes those
version of this manuscript.
situations that are impossible from a
Address reprint requests to: Dr. Claudio Colace, Via Luigi physical and/or logical point of view; the
Volpicelli, 8, 00133 Roma, Italy second dimension implies statistical
Phone: 3336148977
e-mail: claudio65@infinito.it improbability.

Sleep and Hypnosis, 5:3, 2003 105


Dream Bizarreness Reconsidered

PART I: THEORETICAL MODELS phase of sleep “dreaming is the cognitive


by-product of a physiological state, REM
Psychoanalytic theory sleep” (14, p. 202). The original theory
stated that bizarreness is the result of a
According to psychoanalytic theory, temporally random and non-cognitive
dream bizarreness is an expression of a input from the brainstem (PGO spikes)
motivated effort to disguise unconscious and that dreams are merely the product of
wishes that are unacceptable to the “the best of a poor job” that the forebrain
conscience. The Ego and Superego makes to give sense to this random
defensive mechanisms are responsible for bombardment (forebrain synthesis) (4).
this effort to disguise latent dream Later on, it was suggested that the
contents. For example, children do not aminergic demodulation found in REM
dream bizarre dreams because they have sleep (or the lack of the inhibiting
not yet developed the superego which influence of norepinephrine and
enables these defensive changes of latent serotonin) causes defects in cognitive
dream contents (i.e. psychical censorship functioning (e.g. attention, memory, etc.)
functions) (17-21). In adults, bizarreness during forebrain synthesis, which
may be present in direct relation to the contributes to dream bizarreness (7,14,23-
state of the individual’s superego at 24). More recently, Hobson and others
particular moment in time (19). The proposed an updated version of the
psychoanalytic theory even classifies “activation-synthesis” hypothesis,
dreams according to bizarreness features. incorporated in a general brain/mind
Dreams may be as follows: model called AIM (9-10,12-13). According
to the AIM model, sleep and waking
a. “Sensible, plausible and without thought vary in function of three
oddities” - these sort of dreams lack any kind parameters: Activation level (A), Input
of “censorship” and dream-work activity; sources (I) and information-processing
b. “Sensible and consistent in itself but Mode (M) (i.e., the aminergic to
odd compared to common life”; cholinergic neuromodulation rate). REM
c. “Senseless, inconsistent and bizarre”; sleep is hypo-aminergic (compared to
waking state) and hypercholinergic. As in
Types b and c show a moderate and high the previous version, this model claims
degree, respectively, of “censorship” activity that dream bizarreness is due to an
(17). In Freud’s view, dream bizarreness is alteration of cognitive functioning caused
not an invariant property of dreams, as by the shift from a high level of aminergic
there are dreams that are typically non- neuromodulation during waking state to a
bizarre (e.g., young children’s dreams, and low level of same during REM sleep.
adult dreams directly engendered by the In Hobson’s view, dream bizarreness is
frustration of vital needs) (19,22). Bizarre a constant formal property of all dreams.
elements are psychologically meaningful, Bizarreness in itself has no particular
and dreams do have a meaning. psychological significance. It is
Motivations play an important role in this motivationally neutral and its
model, while other models reviewed give interpretation is gratuitous and probably
little relevance to them. hasty. Dream is inherently meaningless–a
state of the mind similar to delirium or
Neurobiological approach insanity (7,12,14,25-27).

“Activation-synthesis” hypothesis “Reverse learning” theory

This model ascribes dream bizarreness According to the “reverse learning”


exclusively to the unique neurobiological theory, dreaming and bizarreness are
conditions of the brain during the REM merely the result of the effort to erase from

106 Sleep and Hypnosis, 5:3, 2003


C. Colace

memory redundant associations or differences in the frequency of


“parasitic” thoughts (i.e., information bizarre/realistic dreams.
causing an overload in the memory
system) during REM sleep. In other words, Cognitive approach
we dream to forget, or to reduce fantasies
in the waking state. Random PGO spikes The cognitive approach assumes that
impinge on the neocortex, resulting in the dreaming is probably distributed along all
erasure, or “unlearning”, of false sleep stages (particularly–but not limited
information. As in Hobson and McCarley’s to–the REM and 2-NREM stage) and that
theory, in this model too random there is a common generative system for
subcortical PGO activity plays an dreaming and waking fantasies. This
important role for bizarreness. Dream is approach is principally interested in
inherently meaningless (15,28). studying dream production processes.

Seligman and Yellen’s theory Foulkes’s cognitive model

This model is based on “activation- Foulkes (5-6) indicates three


synthesis” hypothesis and on previous components in dream production: the
studies that had found a relationship input, i.e. the activation of memory units,
between tonic/phasic activities in REM their processing, known as “planner”, and
sleep and some aspects of dream mentation the output, i.e. conscious organisation.
(29-31). Seligman and Yellen (16) The dream is an attempt to give a plausible
suggested that dreaming consists of three sense to input information. In Foulkes's
elements: (i) periodic, unrelated visual view this attempt is generally successful.
episodes (REM burst), (ii) emotional However this dream production
episodes, and (iii) the cognitive synthesis of mechanism may be disturbed by the
both of the episodes above during REM presence of memory units with a higher
quiescence. The bizarreness and and more impinging level of activation
discontinuity found in REM dreams are due which the planner cannot exclude from
to the “visual burst” (discharges of eye processing. This event translates into the
movements) that supposedly cause presence of thematic changes (i.e.,
intrusions of inconsistent and discontinuity) and bizarre elements.
discontinuous images in the dream plot. Therefore bizarreness is considered an
The authors suggest that there are two exception rather than the rule. Indeed
separate forms of visual information/ dream Foulkes, based on the results of previous
imagery. The first, generated by bursts, is studies, suggests that the content of
more vivid and disjointed from the representatively sampled dreams (REM
underlying plot while the second, generated dreams) of both adults and children are
by cognitive synthesis, is less bizarre. As in generally realistic and ordinary, rather than
Hobson and McCarley’s model, Seligman fantastic and bizarre (32-35).
and Yellen attributed bizarreness to random
PGO activity (PGO spikes are commonly Antrobus’s General Cortical Activation/
associated with visual bursts) and therefore Thresholds (GCAT) model
to the difficulty in finding a sense in visual
bursts. However, the authors argued that According to Antrobus and colleagues,
their model also accounts for the banality bizarre mentation is the product of two
and realism of certain dreams (32). In other factors: cortical/cognitive activation and
words, while bizarre dreams are due to the level of environmental stimulation (or
visual burst activity, realistic dreams are due auditory thresholds) (36-41, Klinger’s
to a successful cognitive integration. study as cited in 39,42). Reinsel et al. (39)
Furthermore, individual differences in found that bizarreness is maximal in
individual cognitive styles could explain the conditions of high to moderate

Sleep and Hypnosis, 5:3, 2003 107


108
Table 1. Current models for dream bizarreness: Questions about dream bizarreness

Psychological Neurobiological Approach Cognitive Approach


Dream Bizarreness Reconsidered

Psychonalytic Activation- Reverse - Seligman and Foulkes’s Antrobus’s Hunt’s


Questions about dream bizarreness synthesis learning Yellen’s theory cognitive (GCAT) model phenomenological
hypothesis theory model perspective

Bizarreness is an invariant property no yes yes no no no no


of dreams

Bizarreness is exclusive to dream mentation no yes yes yes no no no

A generative system of dreaming and waking yes no / no yes yes yes


imagery, including bizarreness, is common

Bizarreness is present even when PGO / no no no / yes /


activity is notably reduced

Bizarreness is explained exclusively by / yes yes yes no no no


neurobiological events of REM sleep

Bizarreness has a psychological meaning yes no no no no no yes

Influences of motivations on dream yes no no no no no yes


bizarreness production

Sleep and Hypnosis, 5:3, 2003


C. Colace

cortical/cognitive activation when cognitive confusion (e.g., deficiencies in


external stimulation is minimal (i.e., REM memory and reasoning). Vice versa,
sleep and relaxed waking). REM dreams dreams of Freud’s and Jung’s collections
are not specifically more bizarre than any were characteristically bizarre. This
other form of thought when the approach does not consider bizarreness an
environmental conditions and the length invariant property of dreaming. Bizarreness
of the dream report (i.e., rate of cognitive is of great importance as an evidence of
activation) are under control. For creative symbolic imagination (visual-
instance, the authors noticed that NREM spatial processes) and has a metaphorical
dreams happen to be more bizarre in significance. In Hunt's view, dream
individuals with high activation levels bizarreness should be interpreted from
during NREM sleep (42). In addition, several different levels of analysis and not
Reinsel et al. show that bizarreness has from a merely physiological standpoint.
many dimensions that can differentiate
the quality of bizarre mentation across PART II: MEASUREMENTS
sleep and waking conditions. Thus, the
types of bizarreness depend on the At the beginning of the 1960s a few
variations of the two parameters above: authors attempted to measure the
waking mentation bizarreness (i.e., frequency of bizarre elements in dream
discontinuity) is due to external stimuli contents by means of objective indicators.
disrupting ongoing mentation (in Reinsel Retrospectively, we can see now that the
et al.’s study (39) relaxed waking was terminology used by these scales of
interrupted at varying intervals); while contents was influenced by the then
bizarreness in REM dreams is associated prevailing theories. The measurement
with the more genuinely “strange” nature created in the 60’s and 70’s used terms like
of the images (i.e., improbable or “distortion”, “metamorphosis”, “primary
impossible identities). Improbable process thinking”, reflecting psychoanalytic
combinations (bizarre elements concepts (e.g., “censorship”, “dream-work”,
regardless of context) increase and are etc.). On the other hand, after the
equally prevailing in relaxed waking development of neurobiological and
(without stimuli) and in dreams. cognitive approaches, terms such as
“discontinuities”, or “improbable
Phenomenological perspective combinations”, was clearly influenced by
concepts like “random activity in the
Hunt (43-44) elaborated a memory system” or “cognitive deficits” etc.
“phenomenological classification-ratings
system for formal anomalies in the The various scales of bizarreness may
dreaming experience” (see Table 2) and be categorized as follows:
analyzed different samples of dream
reports. In particular, laboratory dreams, • general scales: these provide a
home dreams, the “most fantastic” and the qualitative ranking of general classes of
“most realistic” of home dreams, and bizarreness (e.g., discontinuity,
dreams collected by Freud and Jung. In incongruity, etc.) (see Table 2);
Hunt’s view, “normative dreams” (i.e., • analytical scales: these provide a
laboratory and home dreams), albeit largely qualitative ranking of more specific
realistic in content and plot (in the sense kinds of bizarreness previously defined
that they reproduce typical waking by the author (e.g., “changes in sex and
situations and capacities), may show identity”, “monsters”, etc.) (see Table 3);
aspects of bizarreness that can be • global scales: these rank dreams using
assimilated to a mild clinical delirium, ordinal scales (e.g., various types of
namely: visual intrusion (e.g., visual bizarreness are considered together
transformations of form and setting) and without distinction in order to assign a

Sleep and Hypnosis, 5:3, 2003 109


Dream Bizarreness Reconsidered

Table 2. Bizarreness - “General” content scales


Scale Name Authors Brief Description

Scoring Reactive Rychlak & Brams (1963) (3) This scale scores the unusual content compared to
Content common expectation. Five categories: location,
actors, action, mood terms, implements.

Salience Cohen ’s study (as quoted in Salience is defined by 8 scales. Bizarreness is one of
Winget & Kramer, 1979) (45) these. Dreamer scores bizarreness as follows: settings
(objects, space) and events (behavior, experiences)
may be, 0-realistic likely, 1-unlikely, unexpected, but
possible, 2-impossible, ridiculous.

Discontinuities. Reinsel et al. (1992) (39) Discontinuities: when a part of mentation is


Improbable inconsistent with other parts according to common
combinations. daily experiences; Improbable combinations (e.g.
Improbable winged men); Improbable identities (e.g.
identities impossible or changing identities).

Classification- Hunt (1982) (43) Dimensions/changes: a. Competence functions,


ratingsystem b. performance (e.g. reasoning,memory attention),
of formal c. State of consciousness, d. Interpersonal relation
anomalies and personality identity. These dimensions are
divided into three stages: 1. Hypersensitivity to
“ordinary” subjective aspects of experience. 2.
Changes in awareness (e.g. derealization). 3. Specific
anomalies in each dimension.

Dream Hobson et al. (1987) (27) Two-stage scoring system. Stage 1 identifies
bizarreness Hobson (1988) (7) items as bizarre if they are physically impossible
or improbable (e.g. plot, characters, thoughts of
dreamer, etc.); Stage 2 characterizes items as
showing discontinuity, inconsistency or uncertainty.

Bizarre contents Cipolli, Bolzani, Cornoldi, De Bizarreness is defined according to the following
Beni, and Fagioli (1993) (46) criteria: a. physical impossibility, b. physical
implausibility c. behavioral implausibility, d. functional
implausibility and e. incongruity of dialogue, thought
and feeling with respect to the situation.

Continuity and Sutton, Rittenhouse, Pace- This scale attempts to measure continuity and
discontinuity Schott, Stickgold, and Hobson discontinuity in visual attention using the graphs
(1994) (47) theory. The sequence and developments of narrative
reports are presented in hierarchy graphs. Discontinuities
in temporal order are quantified by imposing a
weighted value to each transition within the graph.

Discontinuities Rittenhouse, Stickgold, and This scale analyzes the "mode" of discontinuities
Hobson (1994) (48) in settings, character and object. For example,
we can observe the "Insertion" (sudden appearance)
or "Removal" (sudden disappearance) of an
object and character or "Shift" (initial) or
"Return" (subsequent) in setting and plot.

Content Revonsuo & Salmivalli Dreams are classified into a two-stage scoring:
Bizarreness (1995) (49) 1. Element identification (14 categories, e.g. self,
Scoring actions, emotions) and 2. Content bizarreness
scoring: A. Non-bizarre element (consistent with
waking reality), B. Incongruous element (e.g.
impossible in waking reality), C. Vague element,
D. Discontinuous element.

Bizarreness Colace & Natale (1997) (50) This scoring scale classifies bizarreness as follows: 1)
Bizarre Elements (4 types), a. Improbable or
impossible characters, b. metamorphoses, c.
improbable or impossible actions/inappropriate roles
d. improbable or impossible objects; 2) Script
bizarreness (4 types): a. improbable or impossible
(physical) plot, b. improbable or impossible (logical)
plot, c. plot discontinuity, d. improbable or
impossible settings.

Bizarreness Bosinelli (1999) (51) Bizarreness is classified as: a. improbability, b. oddity


or a+b. c. physical impossibility, d. logical impossibility
or c+d.

110 Sleep and Hypnosis, 5:3, 2003


C. Colace

Table 3. Bizarreness - “Analytical” content scales


Scale Name Authors Brief Description

Bizarre Elements Domhoff & Kamiya (1964) (1) This scale identifies 3 general classes of
bizarre elements: metamorphoses (4
categories), unusual acts (2 categories),
magical occurrences (4 categories).

Distortion from reality Sutcliffe, Perry, & Sheehan, This scale scores atypical aspects with
(1970) (52) respect to the common experience
occurring in real world. Samples of
distortion considered are: changes in sex
and identity, false belief, implausible
behavior of an agent, alteration of typical
appearances or dimension of an agent, etc.

Setting and Hall & Van de Castle (1966) Metamorphoses of characters: changes in
characters distortion (53) sex, identity, or age. Changes from human
into animal or vice-versa. Setting
distortion: familiar settings indicated by
the dreamer have an element of peculiarity
or incongruity insofar as they differ from
the way the dreamer knows the setting to
be in his waking life.

Dream Bizarreness McCarley & Hoffman Dream bizarreness is divided into


(1981) (54) three major groups: a. animate characters,
6 kinds (e.g. monsters, alien beings),
b. inanimate environment, 3 kinds (e.g.
violation of physical laws), c. dream
transformations, 4 kinds (e.g. scene shift).

global score) (see Table 4); PART III: EMPIRICAL DATA AND
IMPLICATIONS
These three types of scales measure the
A. Frequencies
quantity of dream bizarreness in different
ways: while general and analytical scales Bizarreness in REM dreams
score the frequency of bizarreness of
dream reports in single units or elements Several studies have attempted to report
(e.g., action or setting), global scales the frequency of bizarre REM dreams;
frequently score dream bizarreness by however, due to the different scales used,
considering the dream as a whole. the conclusions reached were conflicting.
A common problem associated with Most of these studies agree in that
many measurements is that some bizarre bizarre dreams are very frequent, about
elements may be estimated differently 74% of REM dream reports (46,50,54,
without the help of the dreamer’s own 60,62-67) (see Table 5 and 6).
judgments (e.g., improbability and Less frequently, certain authors have
implausibility in the light of their own suggested that there are notably lower
personal waking reality) (49, 60). For percentages of bizarre REM dreams;
example, Zepelin (60) compared the however, they generally used a different
dreamer’s and the judge’s bizarreness definition of bizarreness (see Table 5) (32-
ratings and concluded that the lack of 33). For example, in Snyder's study, an
knowledge about the dreamer’s waking element being "extremely unlikely from
experiences may lead the judge to the standpoint of waking reality" and yet
exaggerate his/her rating of bizarreness. conceivable is not evaluated as bizarre
Future research should specify whether (p.146). Dorus et al. (33), who found little
the bizarreness scale adopted includes the bizarreness in REM dreaming, used a
dreamer’s contribution or not1. content scale substantially different from
1
For the definitions and scales of bizarreness see also Hobson et al. (27) and, Bonato, Moffitt, Hoffmann, Cuddy & Wimmer (61).

Sleep and Hypnosis, 5:3, 2003 111


Dream Bizarreness Reconsidered

Table 4. Bizarreness - “Global” content scales


Scale Name Authors Brief Description
Distortion Foulkes & Rechtschaffen’s Distortion is measured as follows: 1 extremely
study (as quoted in Winget distorted , 2 quite distorted, 3 fairly distorted,
& Kramer, 1979) (45) 4 somewhat distorted, 5 slightly distorted, 6 not
distorted at all.

Dream ratings Gooudenough, Lewis, Shapiro, A three-point scale of bizarreness/ rationality:


(bizarreness) and Sleser’s study (as cited in 1= “most bizarre” (e.g. strange objects or events),
Winget & Kramer 1979) (45) 1/ = unusual elements or confused combinations of
2
ordinary elements, 0 = dreams with ordinary elements.

Regressive dream Vogel, Foulkes, & Trosman’s This scale classifies dreams as follows:Nonregressive
content study (as quoted in Winget content dreams = plausible, realistic, consistent and
& Kramer, 1979) (45) undistorted dreams. Regressive content dreams =
1 or more bizarre categories such as, bizarre
sequence of images, inappropriate or distorted
images, magical omnipotent thinking, etc.

Child rating scales Foulkes, Pivik, Steadman, Dreams are classified into four levels: level 0 – no
/ distortion Spear, & Symonds (1967) distortion (realistic recreation or anticipation of
(55) upcoming event), 1- no distortion (plausible and
very probable event), 2- slight distortion (plausible
but not probable), 3- considerable distortion
(content is neither plausible nor probable but
contains certain elements of reality) 4- major
elements are neither plausible nor probable.

Scoring dream Koulack’s study (as quoted in Dream bizarreness: this scale refers to the extent of
dimensions Winget & Kramer, 1979) (45) “unreality”. 1. Dream which is entirely true to life.
2. Dream containing both real and unreal
elements. 3. Dream which is totally unreal.

Vivid fantasy Weisz and Foulkes (1970) (56) This scale measures the feeling of unreality (i.e.,
imagination and distortion) coupled with intensity
of experience (dramatization) (A 5 - point scale).

Primary process Auld, Goldenberg, & Weiss Starting from the psychoanalytic concept of
thinking (1968) (57) primary process, the authors focus on evaluating
the “mode of thinking”. A 7- point scale: 1) logical
vs. 7) bizarre and illogical thought.

Classification of Dorus, Dorus, & This scale measures the novelty of elements (e.g.
novelty in Rechtschaffen (1971) (33) setting, objects etc.) with respect to the experience of
dreams each dreamer. 1. the dream element is an exact
replication of something previously experienced vs.
6. the dream element was not previously experienced
and it is extremely unlikely that such an element
could occur in the dreamer’s experience

Chicago sleep Rechtschaffen, Watson, The experimenter asks: How unfamiliar, strange, or
mentation scales Wincor, and Molinari’s study distorted was the very last experience in terms
(as quoted in Winget & of your waking experience? 1. almost exactly like
Kramer, 1979) (45) my waking experience vs 6. new and unfamiliar
and very unlikely to occur in my waking life.

Implausibility Breger, Hunter, & This scale classifies dreams as follows: 1- quite
Lane’s study (as quoted in plausible (something that could well happen to
Winget & Kramer, 1979) (45) the dreamer) vs. 5-bizarre (something that is so
extremely unreal or fantastic that would be
unusual even in a dream report).

Dream distortion Zepelin’s study (as quoted This scale measures the strangeness of dream
in Winget & Kramer, contents compared to waking experience. Level 0 =
1979) (45) the event of dream closely resembles recent waking
experience vs level 5 = major aspects of the dream
are impossible (i.e. combination of illogical or
improbable elements).

Distortion/ Colace, Violani & Solano The authors attempt to formalize the original
Bizarreness (1993) (58) classification of dream bizarreness in Freud’s view.
Colace & Tuci (1996) (59) Dreams are classified as follows: 1 = “sensible,
plausible and without strange elements”, 2 =
“sensible, consistent in themselves but odd
compared to common life”, 3 = “senseless,
inconsistent and bizarre”.

112 Sleep and Hypnosis, 5:3, 2003


C. Colace

Table 5. Percentages of bizarre dreams in REM sleep


No of Bizarre
Studies Dreams REM Dreams Measurement

McCarley & Hoffman (1981) (54) 104 67 % reports containing at least one
element of bizarreness (e.g. monsters,
scene shift)1

Zepelin (1989) (60) 322 91 % reports containing at least a number of


elements not true to life (scores:
“perfectly true to life” vs. “not at all
true” (ratings 1-6)

Zito, Cicogna & Cavallero 183 63 % reports containing at least one bizarre
(1992) (67) element (physical and/or logical
impossibility, or improbability
(according to dreamer’s experience)

Cicogna, Cavallero & 27 67 % reports were scored as “plausible”


Bosinelli (1991) (63) or “implausible” according to waking
standards.

Cavallero et al. (1992) (62) 50 66% reports were scored as “plausible” or


“implausible” according to waking
standards.

Cipolli et al. (1993) (46) 110 79 % reports containing bizarre elements


(e.g. physical impossibility, behavioral
implausibility, etc.)

Colace & Natale (1997) (50) 50 82 % reports containing at least one bizarre
feature (e.g. improbable or impossible
characters, actions, roles, etc.)

Cicogna et al (1998) (64) 1442 84 % reports were scored as “plausible” or


“implausible”

Cicogna et al (2000) (65) 20 75 % reports containing one or more


impossible or improbable elements
according to the subject’s waking
experience

Natale & Esposito (2001) (66) 342 70 %3 reports containing one or more
impossible or improbable elements
according to the subject’s waking
experience

Snyder (1970) (32) 635 L: 20-35% very improbable elements with respect
M: 5-15% to waking reality but yet conceivable
H: 2-7%4 are not necessarily evaluated as bizarre

Dorus et al. (1971) (33) 119 16 % dreams with elements that are a
replication of something previously
experienced, but with major changes
from the original (level 3 of 6)5
1
See table 3 for detail
2
So, St 2, REM.
3
Average score REM 1, 2, 3, 4, cicle
4
L = Low, M = medium and H = high bizarreness
5
See table 4 for detail

more common bizarreness scales (see dream bizarreness is not an invariant


Tables 2-4). formal property of all dreams. About 25%
From a theoretical point of view, these of REM dreams among adults are not at all
data are not consistent with the hypothesis bizarre. In addition, non-bizarre dreams
that REM dreams are generally realistic occur very frequently in young children
compared to dreams derived from a (see below). The non-invariant nature of
psychoanalytic setting or from a home bizarreness in REM dreams cannot be
setting (70). Actually, as stated above, REM easily explained through approaches that
dreaming often reveals bizarre features. On regard bizarreness as intrinsic to the
the other hand, it may be observed that neurobiology of REM sleep (7,15).

Sleep and Hypnosis, 5:3, 2003 113


Dream Bizarreness Reconsidered

Table 6. Percentages of bizarre dreams across sleep stages


Sleep Stages Bizarre Dreams / Range Average STUDIES

Sleep onset min 33% - 43% max 38 % Cicogna, Cavallero & Bosinelli (1991) (63);
Cicogna et al (1998) (64); Zito, Cicogna &
Cavallero (1992) (67); Cicogna et al.
(1996) (68).

NREM, stage 2 47% - 79 % 61 % Cicogna, Cavallero & Bosinelli (1991) (63);


Cicogna et al (1998) (64); Natale &
Esposito (2001) (66); M.Bosinelli (personal
communication).

NREM, stages 3-4 and 4 48% - 54% 51 % Cavallero et al. (1992) (62); Cicogna et al.
(2000) (65); Colace & Natale (1997) (50);
M. Bosinelli (personal communication);
Natale (2000) (69).

REM 63% - 91% 74 % Cipolli et al. (1993) (46); Colace & Natale
(1997) (50); McCarley & Hoffman, (1981)
(54); Zepelin, (1989) (60); Zito et al. (1992)
(67); Cicogna, Cavallero & Bosinelli (1991)
(63); Cavallero et al. (1992) (62); Cicogna
et al (1998) (64); Cicogna et al. (2000)
(65); Natale & Esposito (2001) (66).

Comparing bizarreness in REM and support the hypothesis that the


NREM dreams underlying cognitive processes of dream
production mechanisms could be the
Empirical evidence suggests that the same through all sleep stages. However
original “REM sleep=dreaming” hypothesis this dream-generation system may be
and the rigid REM/NREM dichotomy associated to the quantity of mnestic
needs to be completely revised. In activation that is different between REM
particular, researchers have found that2: and NREM sleep (i.e., different levels of
system engagement) (85);
• REM sleep, in itself is not a sufficient
condition for dreaming. Foulkes has • In agreement with the above,
demonstrated that in children aged 3 to 5 Solms’neuropsychological studies
REM dreaming is relatively absent; i.e. the suggest that REM sleep and dreaming
presence of REM sleep is not a guarantee are dissociable states, controlled by
of concomitant dream activity (34-35,73); different brain mechanisms (86-88).
Thus, dreaming is preserved even when
• REM sleep is not a necessary condition a major damage to pontine brainstem
for dream production. Several studies eliminate REM sleep. Therefore, dream
have shown that dreams can occur mentation can occur without REM
during all the sleep stages sleep. Furthermore, forebrain damage
(6,62,63,67,70,74-83). In particular, (dopamine circuit) would stop
the work of the research group of the dreaming but would not affect REM
Sleep and Dream Laboratory of the sleep. Consequently, dreaming can be
Bologna University Department of initiated by the forebrain mechanisms
Psychology has shown that dream-like regardless of REM state.
mental activity is present also in slow
wave sleep (SWS) (from a physiological These results open the field to
point of view this sleep phase differs investigations on bizarreness in NREM
greatly from the REM phase) dreaming as well. As a further analysis of
(65,66,69,72,84). These studies dream bizarreness the following two
2
On the REM//NREM current debate see Nielsen (71) and Cavallero (72)

114 Sleep and Hypnosis, 5:3, 2003


C. Colace

questions should be answered to: measurement used for bizarreness. For


example, by applying “global” (ordinal)
1. Is bizarreness an exclusive feature of bizarreness scales the REM dreams
REM dreaming? observed are more bizarre than NREM
2. Are REM dreams characteristically dreams, while “general” (qualitative)
(qualitatively and quantitatively) scales applied to the same dream reports
more bizarre than NREM dreams? do not lead to the same results (39,92).

The data found in literature demonstrate • Correlation between length and


that bizarreness is not exclusive to REM bizarreness of dream reports. The well-
dreaming, as bizarre dreams are frequently known correlation between the length
present in NREM sleep. In particular, the of dream reports and their bizarreness
percentage of bizarre dreams is about 38% (36,54,57,93) may make the
in sleep onset, about 61% in NREM stage 2, differences between REM and NREM
and about 51% in stages 3 and 4 (SWS) (see dreams bizarreness scores less clear
Table 6). These results suggests that bizarre (REM dreams are frequently longer).
mentation is not state-specific of REM sleep, Thus, the difference between REM and
and its distinctive neurobiological events NREM stage 2 dream reports vary
(PGO activity and aminergic demodulation), according to whether dream length is
are not a necessary condition for bizarre controlled or not (39).
dreaming.
It could be hypothesized that there are In general, authors suggesting that
other causes of bizarreness, different from REM–NREM mentation differ only in
the ones assumed in REM sleep. Thus, the quantitative terms but not qualitatively (i.e.,
models that base their explanation of the mechanisms of dream production are the
bizarreness on REM neurobiological events same across sleep stages) usually compare
do not account exhaustively for the REM and NREM dreams after dream report
explanation of dream bizarreness in other length is controlled and find no difference
phases of sleep. (39,66,69). These authors suggest that the
The followings studies have compared heightened frequency of bizarreness in REM
directly the frequency of bizarre dreams in dreams is the result of the increased cortical
REM and NREM sleep and might activation (memory systems included) found
contribute to providing an answer to the in REM sleep (36,81). In other words, they
second question. suppose that REM dreams are longer and
REM vs. sleep onset. Several studies more bizarre because the underlying
underlined the low frequency of bizarre cognitive processes operate on higher levels
dreams in sleep onset stage compared to REM of engagement than in NREM stage 2 sleep
sleep (63,64,67,76,82,89-91). A few authors conditions.
have suggested that the contents of mentation On the other hand, authors claiming
at sleep onset are still strongly related to the that the correlation between bizarreness
waking state, which supposedly limits the and length does not justify the differences
presence of bizarreness (66,78). between in REM and NREM dream
REM vs. NREM, Stage 2. The studies bizarreness, (wrongly assumed only in
that have compared dream reports for quantitative terms), maintain that the
bizarreness in REM and NREM stage 2 are difference remains even when length is
controversial, and certain methodological controlled, hence REM dreams are always
aspects should be viewed in deeper detail more bizarre than NREM, stage 2,
before interpreting their results. (12,92,94). These authors have
interpreted these results in support of the
• Measurements. The differences found activation-synthesis model.
between REM and NREM Stage 2 The studies that analyzed REM vs.
dreams depend much on the NREM stage 2 differences in dream

Sleep and Hypnosis, 5:3, 2003 115


Dream Bizarreness Reconsidered

bizarreness are still numerically insufficient wakeful mentation may contribute to clarify
in order to draw any sort of general whether bizarreness is exclusive to
conclusions. The evidence that emerges dreaming or not, and whether REM dreams
from previous studies is insufficient to are typically more bizarre than waking
conclude that REM dreams are more mentation (i.e. relaxed wakefulness,
bizarreness than NREM stage 2 dreams. "simulated dreams," daydreaming).
REM vs. NREM, stages 3 and 4. Very few The findings in literature reveal that
studies have compared REM and Stage 3 bizarreness is also undoubtedly present in
and 4 sleep mentation for bizarreness scores. relaxed wakeful mentation (97-99).
Cavallero et al. (62) used a measurement of Furthermore, other studies have found
Plausibility/Implausibility and found no that relaxed wakeful mentation and
significant differences in the frequency of "simulated dreams” are equally–or perhaps
“plausible” dreams between REM and SWS even more–bizarre than REM dream
sleep (slow wave sleep). Colace and Natale reports (39,100). For example, Reinsel et
(50) compared REM and SWS sleep al. (39) compared REM dreams, NREM
mentation (Dreams Data Bank, Bologna stage 2 dreams, and relaxed wakeful
University, Department of Psychology, 95) mentation (subjects were reclining in a
and found that REM dream reports were darkened room) and found that there is a
significantly more bizarre than SWS dream greater quantity of bizarreness in waking
reports for “script bizarreness”, but no thoughts. The authors suggested that these
significant differences were found when results were consistent with the GCAT
looking at “bizarre elements” (see Table 2 for model, according to which the higher
detail). It is interesting to note that when cortical activation level during waking
dream length was controlled, all REM/SWS state (compared to REM and NREM sleep)
differences disappeared. Recently, Cicogna might increase bizarre contents.
et al. (65) found no significant differences in The studies based on “home dreaming”
“implausibility” (one or more impossible or led to controversial results. Williams et al.
improbable elements) between REM and (99) compared “home dreaming” reports
SWS mentation. to waking fantasies and showed that home
Although the comparison between dreams were significantly more bizarre.
REM and Stage 3-4 sleep mentation The authors claimed that their results were
should be examined more closely, the data consistent with the “activation synthesis”
available seem to point to the conclusion hypothesis. Subsequently, Strauch &
that there are not significant differences in Lederborgen (101) obtained comparable
bizarreness between these two stages. results. On the other hand, Carswell and
In conclusion, previous researches have Webb (102) compared home dreams and
found no clear differences in bizarreness “artificial dream reports” (i.e., subject-
between REM and NREM dreaming (with developed summaries of a random
the exception of sleep onset dreaming). succession of photographs) and found no
There is not enough evidence to support difference in the rates of “implausibility”
the hypothesis that REM dreaming is and bizarreness used (“unusual acts” and
typically more bizarre than NREM “magical occurrence”). The only category
dreaming. The methodological issues of found to be more frequent in home
the uniformity of the measures used, and dreams was the “metamorphosis”.
of whether it is appropriate or not to Since studies have revealed that there
correct report length are still open in were at least no significant differences
comparative analyses between REM and between REM dream reports and waking
NREM dreaming (96). mentation, the great quantity of bizarreness
found in home dreams by certain authors
Bizarreness: dreams vs. waking mentation could be due to the effects of a better recall of
bizarre elements after a longer time span since
The studies on bizarreness in relaxed dream generation (46). It is clear, from the

116 Sleep and Hypnosis, 5:3, 2003


C. Colace

studies reviewed, that there is no distinctive data do not support this hypothesis:
physiological property of REM sleep that can
explain bizarreness alone, because • bizarreness is also present in NREM
bizarreness also occur in waking mentation dreams where PGO activity is notably
and because REM dreams do not seem to be reduced (39);
characteristically more bizarre than waking
• there are insufficient data to support the
fantasies. Vice versa, these results are more
concept of random and non-cognitive
consistent with those models that assume
nature of pontine brainstem activation.
qualitatively common processes during
In fact the beginning of eye movement
dreams and waking fantasies.
(EM), its associated PGO activity, and the
B. Neurophysiological and resulting dream imagery might not be
neurobiological factors totally independent from the prior
cortical/cognitive activity. In other words,
Phasic events the cortex may have an important role in
initiating and creating the visual imagery
Several authors have focused on the of dreaming. (38,112-116)3;
phasic events of REM and NREM sleep and
on the parallel presence of bizarre • Solms (88) suggested that dream imagery
elements in dream contents (103-105). is not generated by the brainstem’s chaotic
Bizarreness seems to be related to REMs activation of the forebrain–on the
(Rapid eye movements) (106), with PIPs contrary, it is apparently built through
(Periorbital Integrated Potentials, the complex cognitive processes;
equivalent of PGO in humans) in REM and
NREM sleep (107-109) and with MEMA • various studies have shown that dreams
(middle-ear muscle activity) in REM and are meaningful rather than random
NREM sleep (110). However these data events. In particular, dream contents
were not always confirmed (111). are affected by gender, age, social status
While there is no evidence supporting and psychopatology (118-119,45).
that PGO spikes are a necessary cause of
bizarreness (see below) the possibility • the data from studies on memory
remains that, phasic neural events consolidation during REM sleep clash
(intrusion), in general and not only at with the hypothesis of random
PGO level, during REM and NREM sleep processing in memory system. Pavlides
may provide one source (i.e., correlate) of and Winson (120-121) in an single-
bizarre mentation. unit experiment with rats found that
hippocampal neurons (CA1 “place
PGO activity cell”), that had fired preferably in their
place fields during waking state, in
The hypothesis that dream bizarreness order to encode spatial information and
may be attributed to the nature of pontine committing it to memory, fired
PGO activity suggests that dream preferably in subsequent sleep states
bizarreness imagery might be due to: a) (REM/SWS). This result was replicated
non-cognitive (subcortical) and random in another study with three rats (122).
nature of eye movements and their Similarly, Skaggs and McNaughton
associated PGO spikes, and b) from the (123) found that the pattern of rat
fact that dreaming and dream bizarreness hippocampal pyramidal cells during
could consist of associations and memory sleep reflects the order in which the
units elicited from the forebrain in cells fired during earlier spatial
response to random inputs from the exploration in a waking state. These
brainstem (PGO) (i.e., random processing data imply the existence of a sort of
in memory system). Unfortunately, several orderly processing of the memory
3
On this topic see also Mancia (117)

Sleep and Hypnosis, 5:3, 2003 117


Dream Bizarreness Reconsidered

rather than a random activity during dreams, it has been found that young
sleep. In particular, Winson suggest children’s dreams are not bizarre at all (see
that in REM sleep types of memory that below);
are important for survival information On grounds of the above, aminergic
during waking state are selectively and demodulation is neither a necessary nor a
preferentially reprocessed (122,124). sufficient condition for dream bizarreness.

This group of data is not consistent Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex inactivity


with the models according to which PGO
random activation and its supposed Studies on dreaming with Positron
consequences in the memory system are Emission Tomography (PET) show which
the cause of bizarreness. Specifically, these parts of the brain areas are active and
studies show that PGO activity is neither a which are inactive during REM sleep. With
necessary nor a sufficient condition of this method, some authors have
dream bizarreness. More generally, these hypothesized that the deactivation of the
data clash with the concept of dreams as a dorsolateral pre-frontal cortex during REM
chaotic and random product. sleep, which causes cognitive deficits (e.g.,
short-term memory, orientation, etc.),
Aminergic demodulation would explain why dreams are so bizarre
and illogical (12, 23,127,132-136).
In Hobson’s view, the aminergic These approaches to the explanation of
demodulation of the brain in REM sleep REM dream bizarreness are inclined to
could be a cause of cognitive deficits that considering bizarreness as a constant
contribute to dream bizarreness, formal property; however, we have seen
compared to waking state where aminergic that this is not true.
neuromodulation is high (12,14,125). For
example, aminergic demodulation might Cortical/cognitive activation
predict an alteration in the strength of
associative links in memory (i.e., The hypothesis of Antrobus et al. (38-
hyperassociative character) that would 39,112), i.e. bizarreness is due to an
explain the bizarre character of REM overall increase in cortical and cognitive
dreaming (126). Recently this view has activation, appears to be backed by the
been suggested also by Gottesman (127- following data:
128) who claimed that the aminergic
demodulation in REM sleep (with the a) Individuals with high cortical
exception of dopaminergic neurons) could activation in NREM sleep have shown a
be responsible for unusual cognitive greater amount of “dreaming” while
functioning and for dream bizarreness. individuals with slow cortical activation
These hypotheses seem scarcely showed NREM mentation defined as
plausible, as long as we have seen that: “thought-like” (42);
b) Dream reports of the second half of
a) bizarreness is also present in waking night sleep (where it is supposed to be a
mentation and in NREM sleep, where greater cortical/cognitive activation) are
aminergic modulation is supposed to be generally more bizarre than in the first part
efficient (see above); (66,70,94,114,137-139);
b) recent studies suggest that certain c) Waking and REM mentation (states
cognitive abilities, e.g. attentional with higher activation), if report length is
processes, are not impaired during not controlled, may appear more bizarre
dreaming compared to waking (129-131); than NREM stage 2 mentation (39).
c) while, according to the “aminergic Based on these data, the bizarreness of
demodulation hyphotesis”, bizarreness REM dreaming could reflect persistent
should be a constant formal property of all cortical activation rather than specific

118 Sleep and Hypnosis, 5:3, 2003


C. Colace

mechanism of REM sleep. quite surprisingly, no implications of this


finding were elaborated for their own
Frontal-limbic structures theory of dream bizarreness (12,145).
Finally, some preliminary systematic
Based on clinic/anatomical studies, observations in young children’s dreams
Solms (86,88) hypothesized that the show that they are frequently a clear and
frontal–limbic structures (anterior easily understandable wish-fulfillment
cingulate gyrus, anterior and dorsomedial (142,150). From this point of view, dreams
thalamus, basal forebrain nuclei, and cannot be characterized as meaningless,
medial frontal cortex) would be implicated the result of random nerve cell activity (7)
in Freud’s “censorship” function. In fact or as a mere activity aimed at reducing
this region seems essential for the control spurious associations, fantasy and
and regulation of emotions and impulses, obsession (15). Studies on formal aspect of
and for the reality monitoring system children dreams are fully consistent with
through the inhibition of the motor system the psychoanalytic approach.
during sleep. When this region is
functioning normally, arousal stimuli of Personality development
dream processes are deflected towards the
perceptive system (i.e., dream-work, The psychoanalytic model predicts that
symbolic operation etc.); conversely, when children dreams are not bizarre because they
this region is damaged excessively have not yet developed the superego
frequent and intense dreaming is triggered. function that could permit the defensive
transformation of latent dream contents
C. Individual factors (17,19-22). Certain studies have found
indications consistent with this hypothesis.
Age For example, Foulkes (34) has shown that
dream “distortion” is correlated to “social
Various studies have shown that dreams comprehension” scores (Wechsler Test) (i.e.,
of normal children of preschool age are often development of moral norms, adjustment to
simple rather than bizarre, even if collected reality). Colace et al. (58,141,142) suggest
with different methodologies (family/non- that the appearance of bizarreness in
family interviewer) and/or in different children’s dreams seems more related to
settings (dream laboratory, home, school) measures of the “capacity to experience guilt
(34,58,59,140-145). The frequency of non- feelings” (i.e., differentiation of the super- ego
bizarre dreams is about 70%. These data function, interiorization of moral norms)
confirm previous anecdotal and quantitative than to descriptive/linguistic abilities.
observation on the simplicity of children’s Furthermore, in agreement with Foulkes’
dreams (146-148). Bizarreness seems to findings, these authors found that in children
occur more frequently starting from 5 to 6 of 3-6 years of age “social comprehension” is
years of age compared to dreams of younger correlated to scores of the “capacity to
children (34,58,142,149). experience guilt feelings” and that both these
These data suggest that dream variables are positively correlated with
bizarreness should not be considered as an measures of dream bizarreness (142).
intrinsic regular property of the dream
process. Indeed, in the light of children’s Waking creativity
dreams, it’s difficult to ascribe dream
bizarreness to a unique neurobiological Several empirical studies have strongly
condition. In particular, why do random supported the hypothesis of a positive
PGO activity and aminergic demodulation relationship between dream bizarreness and
not cause bizarre features in children’s waking creativity (151-157). In a critical
dreams? Hobson’s colleagues also found review of these studies, Wood, Sebba and
that children’s dreams are not bizarre, but, Domino (93) suggested that dream

Sleep and Hypnosis, 5:3, 2003 119


Dream Bizarreness Reconsidered

bizarreness might be primarily related to bizarreness was associated with well-


vocabulary knowledge than to waking adjusted/maladjusted (MMPI) scores.
creativity. According to Wood, Sebba and Carrington concluded that a high rate of
Domino, the dream reports of verbally dream bizarreness might, on the basis of
intelligent people obtain higher bizarreness further investigation, turn out to be at least
ratings only because they are longer, and an important index of the degree of
not because they contain a higher “density” maladjustment in the dreamer. It is also
of bizarre events. A similar result was found worth noting that dreams are more
by Livingston and Levin (158). unrealistic among children suffering from
However, at least three facts should be emotional disturbances (55). These results
mentioned in opposition to these are consistent with the psychoanalytic
statements: a) people with higher verbal view according to which the dreams of
creativity do not always have longer dreams maladjusted persons are generally more
(159), b) several studies have found that bizarre (quantitatively) than those of
non-verbal measures of imaginative ability normal persons (18).
rather than verbal ability are excellent
predictors of dream bizarreness (160-164), The results of studies on waking
c) certain authors suggest and maintain creativity and psychopathology converge
that, in observing the relationship between together towards the following statements:
bizarreness and creativity, to separate dream
length from bizarreness may be a • NREM PIPs activity (correlate of
questionable practice (43,96,164). bizarreness), that vary from one
In accordance with these data, individual to another, is more frequent
Hartmann’s group found that individuals in psychiatric patients and in
with “thin boundaries” property (i.e., individuals who show a greater
flexible, imaginative and creative imagination (correlates with dream
individuals) report more bizarre dreams bizarreness) (181-184);
than people with “thick boundaries”
property (i.e., solid, rigid and reliable • NREM PIPs activity seems more frequent
individuals) (165-169). in persons who have less control of over-
anxiety, fluid ego boundaries, less solid
Psychopathology indices sense of self , and with more imagination
(i.e., “thin boundaries” structure) rather
Several studies have given good than in persons with better control of
support to the relationship between dream anxiety, rigid ego boundaries and less
bizarreness ratings and the degree of imagination (i.e., “thick boundaries”
psychological disturbance of the dreamer structure) (182). We have seen that
measured with the MMPI test (total score “thin” persons are exactly those who
and/or scale of “hysteria” and Sc) dream a greater number of bizarre
(2,3,171). Other studies have observed dreams (see above).
that the dreams of schizophrenic patients
generally appear more bizarre (oddity, The data on the relationship between
implausibility) than those of normal dream bizarreness and waking creativity,
individuals (170,172-174). In addition, psychopathology degree, and types of ego
the dreams of the schizophrenic are boundaries structure, suggest that dream
frequently characterised as unrealistic bizarreness is substantially and not negligibly
(175-180). Carrington (170) compared affected by individual differences. All those
the dream reports of schizophrenic and models that ascribe dream bizarreness solely
nonschizophrenic individuals and showed to neurobiological events cannot explain the
that the dreams of the former were more effects of individual differences (Hobson,
bizarre than those of control subjects. Crick and Mitchison) or at least do not
Furthermore, in the control group, dream explain how can these variables reconcile

120 Sleep and Hypnosis, 5:3, 2003


C. Colace

with a neurobiological explanation. rat sleep suggest that during REM sleep the
processes in the memory system are orderly,
CONCLUSIONS rather than random.
Most of these data, conflicting with the
The activation-synthesis hypothesis “activation-synthesis” hypothesis, also
and its updated versions, that attribute question the two other neurobiological
dream bizarreness solely to the distinctive explanations of bizarreness, which are
neurobiological conditions of REM sleep, largely based on the concept of PGO activity
can be essentially rejected on the basis of and on the REM=dreaming equation (i.e.,
the data found in literature. the “reverse learning” theory and Seligman
This approach suggests that dream and Yellen’s model).
bizarreness is a constant formal property of all Apart from the evidence to the contrary at
dreams because it is intrinsic to REM sleep neurobiological level, there is another major
neurobiology, namely PGO random activity reason for reconsidering the neurobiological
and aminergic brain demodulation. approach to dream bizarreness: the data on
Moreover, as these neurobiological events are the relationship between dream bizarreness
not present in NREM dreaming and in and waking creativity, psychopathology and
waking mentation, bizarreness is supposed to types of ego boundaries structure, suggest
be exclusive and peculiar to REM dreaming. that dream bizarreness is substantially
These assertions are not supported by influenced by individual variables.
data. Indeed there is evidence that dream From this viewpoint, the studies on
bizarreness is a non-invariant, non-exclusive children’s dreams have been quite useful in
and not peculiar feature of REM dreaming. reconsidering dream bizarreness. In
Particularly, although REM dreams are particular, they have shown that: (i)
frequently bizarre in adults, there are also bizarreness is not present in early forms of
REM dreams that are not at all bizarre, and dreaming, (ii) bizarreness seems to appear
non-bizarre dreams have been often at around 5 to 6 year of age, and (iii) it
observed in young children. In practice, seems to be related to the development of
the neurobiological events of REM sleep moral norms.
are not a sufficient condition for the While the literature data do not seem
occurrence of bizarreness. consistent with the activation-synthesis
The literature shows that bizarreness is hypothesis and other neurobiological
present also in NREM dreaming and in approaches, they appear to be at least in
some waking mentation (e.g., relaxed part consistent with other alternative
waking, fantasies, etc.). In addition, it theories on dream bizarreness.
cannot be clearly distinguished from REM The non-invariant nature of bizarreness
dreaming bizarreness. In other words, the seems more consistent with models that
neurobiological events of REM sleep (PGO do not regard bizarreness as intrinsic to
activity, aminergic demodulation) are not dreaming processes.
an essential requirement for bizarreness. The presence of bizarreness in dreams
Results from other researches have cast in all sleep stages and in some waking
doubts on the role of PGO brainstem mentation (e.g., fantasies) seems more
activation as a cause of REM dream consistent with the concept of a common
bizarreness. Thus, the assumption that REM dream generation system in the stages of
dream bizarreness may be attributed to the sleep and of waking fantasies that is typical
random and non-cognitive nature of PGO of cognitive approaches. On the other
activity is not supported by the following hand, Foulkes’s claim of an essential
data: in first place, dream imagery seems to realism of REM dream reports does not
be actively constructed through complex seems to be confirmed.
cognitive processes rather than generated The evidence of more bizarreness in
by brainstem chaotic activation of the dream reports of the second half of night
forebrain; secondly, experimental studies on sleep than in those of the first part is

Sleep and Hypnosis, 5:3, 2003 121


Dream Bizarreness Reconsidered

consistent with the GCAT model on the Briefly, the literature has highlighted a
role of cortical cognitive activation in the need to reconsider dream bizarreness in a
increase of dream bizarreness. more general way, including several levels of
While there is no evidence that PGO analysis (neurobiological, cognitive and
spikes are fundamental for bizarreness, there psychological-motivational), rather than
still is the possibility that, phasic neural taking only the neurobiological point of
events (intrusion), in general and not only view. Dream bizarreness cannot be
PGO, during REM and NREM sleep may be explained as a simple expression of
a source (i.e., correlate) of bizarre mentation. neurobiological facts, and, on the other
From this viewpoint, Watson has shown hand, individual determinants seem to play
that PIPs activity varies individually and is an important role in its theoretical
more frequent in psychiatric patients and in explanation. From this perspective,
persons with greater imaginative abilities; considering dream bizarreness as deprived
exactly those people who apparently have of any psychological significance and
more bizarre dreams. dreams themselves as a meaningless by-
The data on the relationship between product appears to be premature. The
dream bizarreness and psychopathology studies on dream bizarreness could
are consistent with Hunt’s finding that probably be supported by research work on
dream reports in a psychoanalytic setting those forms of dreaming which, for reason
are more bizarre than home and laboratory still unclear, do not show the presence of
dreams. Together, these data seem to bizarreness (e.g., children dreams, childish
confirm Freud‘s observation, i.e. that the adult dreams, etc.). On the other hand, the
dreams of neurotic patients were more clinical/anatomical method, which has
bizarre than those of normal people. In already proved to be useful somehow in the
addition, the studies on the formal aspect study of dreams, might also contribute to
of children’s dreams are totally consistent the research on the underlying factors of
with the psychoanalytic approach. dream bizarreness production.

REFERENCES
1. Domhoff B, Kamiya J. Problem in dream content 9. Hobson JA. Activation, input source, and
study with objective indicators. Arch Gen modulation:neurocognitive model of the state of the
Psychiatry 1964;11:519-532. brain-mind. In: Bootzin RR, Kihlstrom JF, Schacter
DL, eds. Sleep and Cognition. University of
2. Foulkes D, Rechtschaffen A. Presleep determinants Arizona: American Psychological Association,
of dreams content: the effects of two films. Percept 1990;25-40.
Mot Skills 1964;19:983-1005.
10. Hobson JA. A new model of brain-mind state:
3. Rychlak JF, Brams JM. Personality dimension in Activation level, Input source, and Mode of
recalled dream content. Journal of Projective processing (AIM). In: Antrobus JS, Bertini M, eds.
Techniques 1963;27:226-234. The neuropsychology of sleep and dreaming.
Hillsdale, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum
4. Hobson JA, McCarley RW. The brain as a dream-state Associates Publisher, 1992;227-247.
generator: Activation-Synthesis hypothesis of dream
process. Am J Psychiatry 1977;134:1335-1368. 11. Hobson JA. Sleep and Cognition (1999). Retrieved
from the Web 2/ 22/00.
5. Foulkes D. Cognitive-psychological model of REM http://www.websciences.org/worldsleep/cancun99/hobson99.html
dream production. Sleep 1982;5:169-187.
12. Hobson JA, Pace-Schott EF, Stickgold R. Dreaming
6. Foulkes D. Dreaming: A cognitive-psychological and the brain: toward a cognitive neuroscience of
approach, Hillsdale: Erlbaum, 1985. conscious states. Behav Brain Sci 2000;23:793-
842.
7. Hobson JA. The dreaming brain, New York: Basic
Books Inc., 1988. 13. Hobson JA, Stickgold R. Dreaming: a
neurocognitive approach. Conscious Cognition
8. Hobson JA. Sleep, San Francisco: Freeman, 1989. 1994;3:1-15.

122 Sleep and Hypnosis, 5:3, 2003


C. Colace

14. Mamelak A, Hobson JA. Dream bizarreness as the 29. Bosinelli M, Cicogna P, Molinari S. The Tonic-
cognitive correlate of altered neuronal behavior in phasic model and the feeling of Self-participation in
REM sleep. Journal Cogn Neurosci 1989;1:843-849. different stages of sleep. Giornale Italiano di
Psicologia 1974;1:35-65.
15. Crick F, Mitchison G. The function of REM sleep.
Nature 1983;304:111-114. 30. Foulkes D, Pope R. Primary visual experience and
secondary cognitive elaboration in stage REM: a
16. Seligman M, Yellen A. What is a Dreaming? Behav modest confirmation and extension. Percept Mot
Res Ther 1987;25:1-24. Skills 1973;37:107-118.

17. Freud S. The Interpretation of Dreams. In: 31. Molinari S, Foulkes D. Tonic and phasic events
Strachey J, ed. The complete psychological works of during sleep: psychological correlates and
Sigmund Freud, London: Hogarth Press, 1900, vol implications. Percept Mot Skills 1969;29:343-368.
4-5.
32. Snyder F. The phenomenology of dreaming. In: Madow
18. Freud S. On dreams. In: Strachey J, ed. The L, Snow LH, eds. The Psychodynamic implications of
complete psychological works of Sigmund Freud, the physiological studies on dreams. Springfield:
London: Hogarth Press, 1901, vol 5. Charles C. Thomas Publisher, 1970;124-151.

19. Freud S. Introductory Lectures on Psycho-Analysis. 33. Dorus E, Dorus W, Rechtschaffen A. The incidence
In: Strachey J, ed. The complete psychological of novelty in dreams. Arch Gen Psychiatry
works of Sigmund Freud, London: Hogarth Press, 1971;25:364.
1915-17, vol 15-16.
34. Foulkes D. Children's Dreams, longitudinal studies,
20. Freud S. The dissolution of the Oedipus Complex. New York: Wiley-Interscience Publication, 1982.
In: Strachey J, ed. The complete psychological
works of Sigmund Freud, London: Hogarth Press, 35. Foulkes D. Children’s dreaming and the
1924, vol 19. development of consciousness, Cambridge,
Massachusetts: Harvard University Press, 1999.
21. Freud S. Autobiography. In: Strachey J, ed. The
complete psychological works of Sigmund Freud, 36. Antrobus JS. REM and NREM Sleep Reports:
London: Hogarth Press, 1924, vol 20. Comparison of Word Frequencies by Cognitive
Classes. Psychophysiology 1983;20:562-568.
22. Freud S. Jokes and their relation to the
Unconscious. In: Strachey J, ed. The complete 37. Antrobus JS. Dreaming: Cortical activation and
psychological works of Sigmund Freud, London: Perceptual Thresholds. J Mind Behav 1986;7:193-
Hogarth Press, 1905, vol 8. 212.

23. Hobson JA, Stickgold R, Pace-Schott EF. The 38. Antrobus JS. Dreaming: Cognitive processes during
neuropsychology of REM sleep dreaming. cortical activation and high afferent thresholds.
Neuroreport 1998;9:1-14. Psychol Rev 1991;98:96-121.

24. Kahn D, Pace-Schott EF, Hobson JA. Consciousness 39. Reinsel R, Antrobus JS, Wollman M. Bizarreness in
in waking and dreaming: the roles of neuronal dreams and waking fantasy. In: Antrobus JS,
oscilation and neuromodulation in determining Bertini M, eds. The neuropsychology of sleep and
similarities and differences. Neuroscience dreaming. Hillsdale, New Jersey: Lawrence
1997;78:13-38. Erlbaum Associates Publisher, 1992;157-183

25. Hobson JA. How the brain goes out of its mind. 40. Reinsel R, Wollman M, Antrobus JS. Effects of
Endeavour 1996;20:86-89. enviromental contex and cortical activation on
thought. J Mind Behav 1986;7:259-275.
26. Hobson JA. Dreaming as delirium : a mental status
analysis of our nightly madness. Semin Neurol 41. Wollman M, Antrobus JS. Sleeping and waking
1997;17:121-128. thought: effects of external stimulation. Sleep
1986;9:438-448.
27. Hobson JA, Hoffman SA, Helfand R, Kostner D.
Dream bizarreness and the activation-synthesis 42. Zimmerman WB. Sleep mentation and auditory
hypothesis. Hum Neurobiol 1987;6:157-164. awakening thresholds. Psychophysiology
1970;6:540-549.
28. Crick F, Mitchison G. REM sleep and neural nets. J
Mind Behav 1986;7:229-249. 43. Hunt HT. Forms of dreaming. Percept Mot Skills
1982;54:559-633.

Sleep and Hypnosis, 5:3, 2003 123


Dream Bizarreness Reconsidered

44. Hunt HT. The multiplicity of dreams, New Haven: 59. Colace C, Tuci B, Ferendeles R. Bizarreness in early
Yale University Press, 1989. children’s dreams collected in the home setting:
preliminary data. Sleep Res 1997;26:241.
45. Winget C, Kramer M. Dimension of dream,
Gainesville: Presses of Florida, 1979. 60. Zepelin H. Bizarreness in REM dreams. Sleep Res
1989;18:161.
46. Cipolli C, Bolzani R, Cornoldi C, De Beni R, Fagioli
I. Bizarreness effects in dream recall. Sleep 61. Bonato RA, Moffit AR, Hoffmann RF, Cuddy MA,
1993;16:163- 170. Wimmer L. Bizarreness in dream and nightmares.
Dreaming 1991;1:53-61.
47. Sutton JP, Rittenhouse CD, Pace-Schott E, Stickgold
R, Hobson JA. A new approach to dream 62. Cavallero C, Cicogna P, Natale V, Occhionero M,
bizarreness: graphing continuity and discontinuity Zito A. Slow Wave Sleep Dreaming. Sleep
of visual attention in narrative reports. Conscious 1992;15:562-566.
Cogn 1994;3:61-88.
63. Cicogna P, Cavallero C, Bosinelli M. Cognitive
48. Rittenhouse C, Stickgold R, Hobson JA. Constraints aspects of mental activity during sleep. Am J
on the transformation of characters, objects, and Psychol 1991;104:413- 425.
settings in dream reports. Conscious Cogn
1994;3:100-113. 64. Cicogna P, Natale V, Occhionero M, Bosinelli M. A
comparison of Mental Activity During Sleep Onset
49. Revonsuo A, Salmivalli C. A content analysis of bizarre and Morning Awakenings. Sleep 1998;21:462-470.
elements in dreams. Dreaming 1995;5:169-187.
65. Cicogna P, Natale V, Occhionero M, Bosinelli M.
50. Colace C, Natale V. Bizarreness in REM and SWS Slow Wave and REM Sleep Mentation. Sleep
dreams. Sleep Res 1997;26:240. Research Online 2000;3:67-72.

51. Bosinelli M. The study of Meta-Cognition during 66. Natale V, Esposito MJ. Bizarreness across the first
Sleep Mental Experiences. Metodological Problems, four cycles of sleep. Sleep and Hypnosis 2001;3:18-
1999. Retriewed February 22, 2000, from: 24.
http://www.websciences.org/worldsleep/cancun99/bosinelli2.html
67. Zito A, Cicogna P, Cavallero C. Sogni REM e sogni
52. Sutcliffe JP, Perry CW, Sheehan PW. Relation of some di addormentamento:in che termini è ancora
aspects of imagery and fantasy to hypnotic legittimo parlare di differenze?. Ricerche di
susceptibility. J Abnorm Psychol 1970;76:279-28. Psicologia 1992;2:7-18.

53. Hall CS, Van de Castle RL. The content analysis of 68. Cicogna P, Natale V, Occhionero M, Bosinelli M.
dreams, New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, Comparing dreams at onset and end of sleep. J
1966. Sleep Res 1996;5(Suppl. 1):33.

54. McCarley RW, Hoffman E. REM Sleep Dreams an 69. Natale V. Slow Wave Sleep Mentation: A
the Activation-Synthesis Hypothesis. Am J Comparison Between the First and the Second
Psychiatry 1981;138:904-912. Sleep Cycle. Sleep and Hypnosis 2000;2:84-89.

55. Foulkes D, Pivik T, Steadman HS, Spear PS, 70. Foulkes D. Dream Research: 1953-1993. Sleep
Symonds JD. Dreams of the male child: an EEG 1996;19:609-624.
study. J Abn Psychol 1967;72:457-467.
71. Nielsen TA. A review of mentation in REM and
56. Weisz RA, Foulkes D. Home and laboratory NREM sleep: “Covert” REM sleep as a possible
dreams collected under uniform sampling reconciliation of two opposing models. Behav Brain
conditions. Psychophysiology 1970;16:155-156. Sci 2000;23: 851-866.

57. Auld F, Goldenberg GM, Weiss JV. Measurement of 72. Cavallero C. REM sleep=dreaming: The never-
Primary-Process thinking in dream report. J Pers ending story. Behav Brain Sci 2000;23:904-907.
Soc Psychol 1968;8:418-426.
73. Foulkes D. Dreaming and REM sleep. J Sleep Res
58. Colace C, Violani C, Solano L. La 1993;2:199-202.
deformazione/bizzarria onirica nella teoria
freudiana del sogno:indicazioni teoriche e verifica 74. Bosinelli M. Impressione di realtà e proprietà
di due ipotesi di ricerca in un campione di 50 sogni percettive nella fenomenologia onirica. In: Gerbino
di bambini. Archivio di Psicologia Neurologia e W, ed. Conoscenza e struttura. Bologna: Il Mulino,
Psichiatria 1993;54:380-401. 1985;107-118.

124 Sleep and Hypnosis, 5:3, 2003


C. Colace

75. Bosinelli M, Molinari S. Contributo alle interpretazioni 90. Rowley JT, Stickgold R, Hobson JA. Eyelid
psicodinamiche dell'addormentamento. Rivista di movements and mental activity at sleep onset.
Psicologia 1968;62:369-393. Conscious Cogn 1998;7:67-84.

76. Bosinelli M, Cavallero C, Cicogna P. Self 91. Vogel GW. Sleep onset mentation. In: Arkin AM,
representation in two different psychophysiological Antrobus JS, Ellman SJ, eds. The mind in sleep:
contidions: the analysis of dream experiences psychology and psychophysiology. Hillsdale, New
during sleep onset and REM sleep. Sleep Jersey: Lawrence Erlabaum Associates Publishers,
1982;5:290-299. 1978;113-140.

77. Brown J, Cartwright R. Locating NREM dreaming 92. Porte H, Hobson JA. Bizarreness in REM and
through instrumental responses. Psychophysiology NREM reports. Sleep Res 1986;15:81.
1978;15:35-39.
93. Wood JM, Sebba D, Domino G. Do creative people
78. Cicogna P, Bosinelli M, Occhionero M. (1992). have more bizarre dreams? A reconsideration.
Processi ed esperienze mentali in sonno a onde Imagination, Cognition and Personality 1989;9:3-
lente. In: Smirne SL, Ferini Strambi L, Zucconi M, 16.
eds. Il sonno in Italia. Milano: Poletto Edizioni,
1992;55-59. 94. Casagrande M, Violani C, Lucidi F, Buttinelli E,
Bertini M. Variation in sleep mentation as a
79. Foulkes D. Dreams report from different stages of function of time of night. International. J Neurosci
sleep. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology 1996;85:19-30.
1962;65:14-25.
95. Zito A, Cicogna P, Cavallero C. DDB: una banca
80. Foulkes D, Vogel G. Mental activity at sleep onset. dati per la ricerca sulla fenomenologia del sogno.
J Abnorm Psychol 1965;20:231-240. Giornale Italiano di Psicologia 1991;18:781-786.

81. Foulkes D, Schmidt M. Temporal sequences and 96. Hunt HT, Ruzycki-Hunt K, Pariak D, Belicki K.
unit composition in dream reports from different The relationship between dream bizarreness and
stages of sleep. Sleep 1983;6:265-280. imagination:artifact or essence? Dreaming
1993;3:179-199.
82. Foulkes D, Spear PS, Symonds JD. Individual
differences in mental activity at sleep onset. J 97. Foulkes D, Fleischer S. Mental activity in relaxed
Abnorm Psychol 1966;71:280-286. wakefulness. J Abnorm Psychol 1975;84:66-75.

83. Moffitt A. Dreaming: Functions and meanings. 98. Foulkes D, Scott E. An above zero baseline for the
Impuls 1995;3:18-31. incidence of momentarily hallucinatory mentation.
Sleep Res 1973;2:108.
84. Occhionero M, Cicogna P, Natale V, Esposito MJ,
Bosinelli M. A comparison of mental activity during 99. Williams J, Merritt J, Rittenhouse C, Hobson JA.
slow wave and REM sleep. J Sleep Res Bizarreness in dreams and fantasies:implication for
1998;7(suppl 2):190. the activation-synthesis hypothesis. Conscious Cogn
1992;1:172-185.
85. Bosinelli M, Cicogna P. REM and NREM mentation:
Nielsen’s model once again supports the supremacy 100. Cavallero C, Natale V. Was I dreaming or did it
of REM. Behav Brain Sci 2000;23:913-914. really happen? A comparison between real and
artificial dream reports. Imagination, Cognition
86. Solms M. The neuropsychology of dreams: A and Personality 1988-89;8:19-24.
clinico-anatomical study, Mahwah, New Jersey:
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers, 1997. 101. Strauch I, Lederbogen S. The home dreams and
waking fantasies of boys and girls between ages 9
87. Solms M. New findings on the neurological and 15: a longitudinal study. Dreaming
organization of dreaming: implications for 1999;9:153-161.
psychoanalysis. Psychoanal Q 1995;64:43-67.
102. Carswell CM, Webb WB. Real and artificial dream
88. Solms M. Dreaming and REM sleep are controlled episodes: Comparison ofreport structure. J Abnorm
by different brain mechanisms. Behav Brain Sci Psychol 1985;94:653-655.
2000;23:843-50.
103. Grosser GS, Siegal AW. Emergence of a tonic-
89. Bosinelli M. Recent research trends in sleep onset phasic model for sleep and dreaming. Psychol Bull
mentation. In: Ellman S, Antrobus JS, eds. The mind 1971;75:60-72.
in Sleep, 2nd ed. New York: Wiley, 1991;137-142.

Sleep and Hypnosis, 5:3, 2003 125


Dream Bizarreness Reconsidered

104. Pivik T. Tonic states and phasic events in relation to 117. Mancia M. Dream production is not chaotic. Behav
sleep mentation. In: Arkin AM, Antrobus JS, Brain Sci 2000;23:967-968.
Ellman SJ, eds. The mind in sleep: psychology and
psychophysiology, Hillsdale, New Jersey: Lawrence 118. Kramer M. Do Dreams have meaning? An empirical
Erlabaum Associates Publishers, 1978;245-271 inquiry. Am J Psychiatry 1976;133:778-781.

105. Rechtschaffen A. The Psychophysiology of mental 119. Kramer M. Dreaming has content and meaning not
activity during sleep. In: McGuigan FJ, Schoonover just a form. Behav Brain Sci 2000;23:959-961.
RA, eds. The psyschophysiology of thinking. New
York: Academic Press, 1973;153-205. 120. Pavlides C, Winson J. Influences on hippocampal
place cell firing in the awake state on the activity of
106. Goodenough DR, Lewis H, Shapiro A, Jaret L, these cells during subseguent sleep episodes. J
Sleser I. Dream reporting following abrupt and Neurosci 1989;9:2907-2918.
gradual awakenings from different types of sleep. J
Pers Soc Psychol 1965;2:170-179. 121. Winson J. The biology and function of rapid eye
movement sleep. Curr Opin Neurobiol 1993;3:243-
107. Bliwise D, Rechtschaffen A. Phasic EMG in human 248.
sleep: III. Periorbital potentials and NREM
mentation. Sleep Res 1978;27:58. 122. Wilson MA, McNaughton BL. Reactivation of
hippocampal ensemble memories during sleep.
108. Rechtschaffen A, Watson R, Wincor M, Molinari. S, Science 1994;265:676-679.
Barta S. The relationship of phasic and tonic
periorbital EMG activity to NREM mentation. 123. Skaggs W, McNaughton BL. Replay of neuronal
Sleep Res 1972;1:114. firing sequences in rat hippocampus during sleep
following spatial experience. Science
109. Watson, RK. Mental correlates of periorbital PIPs 1996;27:1870-1873.
during REM sleep. Sleep Res 1972;1:116.
124. Winson J. The meaning of Dreams. Sci Am 1990;
110. Ogilvie RD, Hunt HT, Sawicki C, Samanhalsky J. Nov,263:88-88,90-92,94-96.
Psychological correlates of spontaneous middle-ear
muscle activit during sleep. Sleep 1982;5:11-27. 125. Fosse R, Stickgold R, Hobson JA. Brain-mind states:
reciprocal variations in thoughts and
111. Watson RK, Bliwise DL, Friedman L, Wax D, hallucinations. Psychol Sci 2001;12:30-36.
Rechtschaffen A. Phasic EMG in human sleep:II.
Periorbital potentials and REM mentation. Sleep 126. Stickgold R, Scott L, Rittenhouse C, Hobson JA.
Res 1978;7:57. Sleep-induced Changes in Associative Memory. J
Cogn Neurosci 1999;11:182-193.
112. Antrobus JS. The neurocognition of sleep mentation:
rapid eye movements, visual imagery, and 127. Gottesman C. Hypothesis for the Neurophysiology
dreaming. In: Bootzin RR, Kihlstrom JF, Schacter of Dreaming. Sleep Research Online 2000;3:1-4.
DL, eds. Sleep and Cognition. Washington DC:
American Psychological Association, 1990;3-24. 128. Gottesman C. Each distinct type of mental state is
supported by specific brain functions. Behav Brain
113. Antrobus JS. How does the dreaming brain explain Sci 2000;23:941-943.
the dreaming mind? Behav Brain Sci 2000;23:904-
907. 129. LaBerge S, Kahan T, Levitan L. Cognition in
dreaming and waking. Sleep Res 1995;24A:239.
114. Antrobus JS, Kondo T, Reinsel R, Fein G. Dreaming
in the late morning: summation of REM and 130. Kahan TL, LaBerge S, Levitan L, Zimbardo P.
diurnal cortical activation. Conscious Cogn Similarities and differences between dreaming and
1995;4:275-299. waking cognition: an exploratory study. Conscious
and Cogn 1997;6;132-147.
115. Antrobus JS, Conroy D. Dissociated neurocognitive
processes in dreaming in sleep. Sleep and Hypnosis 131. Hartmann E. The waking-to-dreaming continuum
1999;2:105-111. and the effects of emotion. Behav Brain Sci
2000;23:947-950.
116. Herman JH. Transmutative and reproductive
properties of dreams: evidence for cortical 132. Braun AR, Balkin TJ, Wesenten NJ, Carson RE,
modulation of brain-stem generators. In: Antrobus Varga M, Baldwin P, Selbie S, Belenky G,
JS, Bertini M, eds. The neuropsychology of sleep Herscovitch P. Regional cerebral blood flow
and dreaming Hillsdale, New Jersey: Lawrence throughout the sleep-wake cycle. Brain
Erlbaum Associates, Publisher, 1992;251-262. 1997;120:1173-1197.

126 Sleep and Hypnosis, 5:3, 2003


C. Colace

133. Braun AR, Balkin, TJ, Wesensten NJ, Gwadry F, 146. Ablon SL, Mack JE. Children's dreams
Carson RE, Varga M, Baldwin P, Belenky G, reconsidered. Psychoanal Study Child
Herscovitch P. Dissociated pattern of activity in 1980;35:170- 217.
visual cortices and their projections during human
rapid eye movement sleep. Science 1998;279:91- 147. Kimmins CW. Children's Dreams, Longman's
95. Green, 1920.

134. Gottesman C. Neurophysiological support of 148. Despert JL. Dreams in children of preschool age.
consciousness during waking sleep. Prog Neurobiol Psychoanal Study Child 1949;3-4:141-180.
1999;59:469-508.
149. Colace C, Doricchi F, Di Loreto E, Violani C.
135. Hobson JA, Pace-Schott, EF, Stickgold R. Dream Developmental qualitative and quantitative aspects
science 2000: A response to commentaries on of bizarreness in dream reports of children. Sleep
Dreaming and the brain. Behav Brain Sci Res 1993;22:57.
2000;23:1019-1035.
150. Colace C. Wish-fulfillment in dream reports of
136. Maquet P, Peters J, Aerts J, Delfiore G, Degueldre young children. Sleep 1998;21 (Suppl. 3):286.
C, Luxen A, Franck G. Functional neuroanatomy
of human rapid-eye-movement sleep and 151. Adelson J. Creativity and the dream. Merril Palmer
dreaming. Nature 1996;383:163-166. Q 1960;6:92-97.

137. Cohen DB. Changes in REM dream content during 152. Caan DR, Donderi DC. Junghian personality
the night: implications for a hypothesis about typology and the recall of everyday and archetypal
changes in cerebral dominance across REM dreams. J Pers Soc Psychol 1986;50:1021-1030.
periods. Percept Mot Skills 1977;44:1267-1277.
153. Domino G. Primary process thinking in dream
138. Pivik T, Foulkes D. NREM Mentation: relation to report as related to creative achievement. J Consult
personality, orientation time, and time of night. J Clin Psychol 1976;44:929-932.
Consult Clin Psychol 1968;2:144:151.
154. Gaines R, Price-Williams D. Dream images as a
139. Rechtschaffen A. Stimulus Determinants of technique for the study of the creative process.
Dreaming, Mimeographed, 1962. Percept Mot Skills 1990;70:540-542.

140. Colace C, Tuci B. Early children’s dreams are not 155. Schechter N, Schmeidler GR, Staal M. Dream
bizarre. Sleep Res 1996;25:147. reports and creative tendencies in student of the
arts, sciences, and engineering. J Consult Psychol
141. Colace C, Violani C. La bizzarria del sogno 1965;29:415-421.
infantile come correlato della capacità di provare
sensi di colpa. Psichiatria dell'infanzia e 156. Sylvia WH, Clark PM, Monroe LJ. Dream reports
dell'adolescenza 1993;60:367-376. of subjects high and low in creative ability. J Gen
Psychol 1978;99:205-211.
142. Colace C. I sogni dei bambini nella teoria
psicodinamica: un contributo teorico e 157. Sladeczek P, Domino G. Creativing, Sleep and
sperimentale. Ph.D. dissertation, Department of Primary Process Thinking in Dreams. J Creat
Psychology, Università degli Studi di Bologna, Behav 1985;19:38-55.
1997.
158. Livingston G, Levin R. The effects of dream length
143. Colace C, Tuci B, Ferendeles R, Testa A, Celani G, on the relationship between primary process in
Gasparini S. Uno studio sul sognare in età dream and creativity. Dreaming 1991;1:301-310.
evolutiva attraverso un questionario compilato dai
genitori: dati preliminari. Psichiatria dell’Infanzia 159. Schredl M. Dream length and creativity: an opposite
e dell’adolescenza 2000;67:559-570. finding. Percept Mot Skills 1994;78:1297-1298.

144. Levi G, Pompili E. La narrazione del racconto del 160. Belicki K, Bowers P. The role of hypnotic ability in
sogno in bambini in età prescolare. Psichiatria dream recall. Sleep Res 1981;10:155.
dell'infanzia e dell'adolescenza 1991;58:517-525.
161. Belicki K, Belicki D. Predisposition for nightmares:
145. Resnick J, Stickgold R, Rittenhouse C, Hobson JA. A study of hypnotic ability, vividness of imagery
Self-representation and bizarreness in children's and absorption. J Clin Psychol 1986;42:714-718.
dream reports collected in the home setting.
Conscious and Cogn 1994;3:30-45. 162. Gackenbach J, Bonsveld J. Control your dreams,
New York: Harper and Row, 1989.

Sleep and Hypnosis, 5:3, 2003 127


Dream Bizarreness Reconsidered

163. Hunt HT, Popham C. Metaphor and states of 175. Boss M. Psychopatology of dreams in schizophrenia
consciousness: A preliminary correlational study of and organic psychoses. In: De Martino MF, ed.
presentational thinking. J Mind Behav 1987;11:83-100. Dreams and personality dynamics. Springfield, Ill.:
Charles C Tomas Publisher, 1959;156-175.
164. Spadafora A, Hunt H. The multiplicity of dreams:
Cognitive-Affective correlates of lucid, archepital, 176. Cappon, D. Morphology and Other Parameters of
and nightmare dreaming. Percept Mot Skills Fantasy in the Schizophrenics. Arch Gen
1990;71:627-644. Psychiatry 1959;1:17-34.

165. Kunzendorf R G, Hartmann E, Cohen R, Cutler J. 177. Doust-Lovett J. Studies in the physiology of
Bizarreness of the dreams and daydreams reported awareness: the incidence and content of dream
by individuals with thin and thick boundaries. patterns and their relationship to anoxia. In: De
Dreaming 1997;7:265-271. Martino MF, ed. Dreams and personality
dynamics. Springfield, Ill.: Charles C Tomas
166. Hartmann E. Outline for theory on the nature and Publisher, 1959.
functions of dreaming. Dreaming 1996;6;147-170.
178. Freedman N, Grand SE, Karacan I. An Approach
167. Hartmann E, Elkin R, Garg M. Personality and to the study of dreaming and changes in
dreaming: the dreams of the people with very thick or psychopathologic states. J Nerv Ment Dis
very thin boundaries. Dreaming 1991;1:311-324. 1966;143:399-405.

168. Schredl M, Schafer N, Hoffman F, Jacobs S. 179. Kant, O. Dreams of schizophrenic patients. J Nerv
Dream content and personality: thick vs. thin Ment Dis 1942;95:335-347.
boundaries. Dreaming 1999;9:257-263
180. Kramer M. Manifest Dream Content in Normal
169. Zborowski M, McNamara P, Hartmann E, Murphy and Psychopatological States. Arch Gen Psychiatry
M, Mattle L. Boundary structure related to sleep 1970;22:149-159.
measures and to dream content. Sleep
1998;21(Suppl):474D, 284. 181. Rechtschaffen A, Bliwise D, Litchman J. Phasic
EMG in human sleep: V. PIPs and MMPI scores in
170. Carrington, P. Dreams and Schizophrenia. Arch normals. Sleep Res 1978;7:60.
Gen Psychiatry 1972;26:343-350.
182. Watson RK. Phasic integrated potentials and ego
171. Cartwright RD, Ratzel R. Effects of dream loss on boundary deficits. In: Antrobus JS, Bertini M, eds.
Waking Behaviors. Arch Gen Psychiatry The neuropsychology of sleep and dreaming.
1972;27:277-280. Hillsdale, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum
Associates Publisher, 1992;247-250.
172. Fisher S, Greenberg RP. The scientific credibility of
Freud's theories and therapy, New York: Basic 183. Watson, RK, Liebmann K, Watson S. Comparison
Books, Inc., 1977. of NREM PIP frequency in schizophrenic and non-
schizophrenic patients. Sleep Res 1976;5:154.
173. Kramer M, Baldridge BJ, Whitman RN, Ornstein
PH, Smith PC. An exploration of the manifest 184. Watson RK, Buttler S, Liebmann K. Individual
dream in schizophrenic and depressed patients. Dis differences in the Rorschach M response and the
Nerv Syst, 1969;30(Suppl.):126-130. distribuition of phasic integrated potentials (PIPs)
during sleep. Percept Mot Skills 1983;57:507-514
174. Richardson G, Moore R. On the manifest dream in
schizophrenia. J Am Psychoanal Assoc
1963;11:281-302.

128 Sleep and Hypnosis, 5:3, 2003

View publication stats

You might also like