You are on page 1of 12

Review Article

Learning Empathy Through Simulation


A Systematic Literature Review

Margaret Bearman, PhD; Summary Statement: Simulation is increasingly used as an educational methodology for
teaching empathy to preservice health professional students. This systematic review
Claire Palermo, PhD; aimed to determine if and how simulation, including games, simulated patients, and role-
play, might develop empathy and empathetic behaviors in learners. Eleven databases or
Downloaded from https://journals.lww.com/simulationinhealthcare by BhDMf5ePHKav1zEoum1tQfN4a+kJLhEZgbsIHo4XMi0hCywCX1AWnYQp/IlQrHD3wX04VDhDA65JyIrQxIAmw8OudmFC7vF5gSn6ug+Sx+k= on 02/01/2020

Louise M. Allen, B NutrDiet (hons); clearing houses including MEDLINE, EMBASE, CINAHL, PsychInfo, and ERIC were
searched for all articles published from any date until May 2014, using terms relating to
(i) preservice health professional students, (ii) simulation, and (iii) empathy. Twenty-
Brett Williams, PhD seven studies met the inclusion criteria, including 9 randomized controlled trials. A
narrative synthesis suggests that simulation may be an appropriate method to teach
empathy to preservice health professional students and identifies the value of the learner
taking the role of the patient.
(Sim Healthcare 10:308Y319, 2015)

Key Words: Simulation, Empathy, Role-play, Communication skills, Simulated patients.

S imulation-based education, where students engage in an


experience to learn, is frequently used to develop empathy
by evidence, such as the better outcome of diabetic patients
associated with more empathetic doctors10,11 and the reduc-
and empathetic behaviors in medical,1 nursing,2 and allied tion of seclusion and restraint of psychiatric patients associ-
health3 students. Simulation techniques in health profes- ated with more empathetic nurses.12 However, the value of
sional education includes a variety of different approaches health professional education in teaching empathy is not so
such as simulated or standardized patient (SP) methodology, clear. On the one hand, the 2013 systematic review conducted
mannequin-based methodologies, role-play, games, and by Batt-Rawden et al13 of 18 educational interventions to teach
virtual reality.4 This approach is not universally accepted. empathy to medical students from 2003 to 2012 concluded
Wear and Varley5 caution that simulated empathy lacks that the interventions were mostly effective in promoting
authenticity as students learn to act ‘‘empathetic’’ for pur- empathy despite methodological flaws. This review identified
poses of performance rather than establishing a genuine the success of some approaches to teaching empathy, which
connection with real people, a notion described as learning might be considered simulation, such as ‘‘experiential learn-
to play the ‘‘simulation game.’’6 ing’’ and ‘‘drama.’’ Other studies14,15 also indicate the value of
The value of empathy to health care practice is not con- empathy education for practitioners. On the other hand, a
troversial. Hojat7 distinguishes empathy, with its predomi- recent systematic review found that empathy seems to decline
nantly cognitive and altruistic orientation, from sympathy, during medical education and residency.16 Similar findings
which is denoted as a predominantly emotional and self- can be found in other disciplines.17 Neumann et al16 believe
serving orientation. This distinction, which has its critics,7 is that this may be a consequence of entering into the clinical
maintained in the associated definition of empathy in health environment, which increases feelings of vulnerability; they
care as a cognitive response of understanding ‘‘the experiences, also note the role of idealistic belief in the role of doctor and
concerns, and perspectives’’7 of the patient, including the ca- some students’ generally heightened levels of distress (eg,
pacity to communicate this understanding. This emphasis on burnout, depression).
the behavioral manifestations of empathy is notable as the This article builds on previous reviews by specifically fo-
latter is easier to objectively measure. Some measures of em- cusing on simulation as the learning strategy. It aimed to deter-
pathy do emphasize the internal experience of empathy,8 and mine if simulation-based education of preservice health care
others again require the perspective of a patient.9 professionals, in comparison with an alternative or no inter-
This intuitive notion that practitioners’ empathetic be- vention, is associated with improved empathetic behaviors. The
haviors improve the delivery of care is increasingly supported secondary aim was to explore the key learning and teaching ap-
proaches, if any, that are associated with the improved outcomes.
From the HealthPEER (M.L.B.), Department of Nutrition and Dietetics (C.P., L.M.A.),
and Department of Community Emergency Health and Paramedic Practice (B.W.),
METHODS
Monash University, Victoria, Australia. Literature Search and Study Selection
Reprints: Margaret Bearman, PhD, HealthPEER, G08 13c Monash University, Clayton Eleven databases or clearing houses (MEDLINE, EMBASE,
Vic 3800, Australia (e<mail: argaret.bearman@monash.edu). CINAHL, PsychINFO, ERIC, Web of Science, Scopus, Informit,
This work should be attributed to HealthPEER, Monash University.
Copyright * 2015 Society for Simulation in Healthcare Campbell Collaboration, BEME, and Cochrane) were searched
DOI: 10.1097/SIH.0000000000000113 for all records from any date up to May 2, 2014. Search terms

308 Learning Empathy Through Simulation Simulation in Healthcare


Copyright © 2015 by the Society for Simulation in Healthcare. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
were grouped into 3 conceptual categories as follows: (i) against the Medical Education Research Study Quality In-
health professional students, (ii) simulation, and (iii) empa- strument (MERSQI).18 The MERSQI was designed specifi-
thy. Search terms regarding health professional education cally for quantitative observational, quasi-experimental, and
students included variants of 18 professions combined with experimental studies in medical education and has been
variants of education, learner, student, and teaching. Search tested with respect to item and rater reliability, principal
terms regarding simulation included variants on clinical skill, components, and criterion validity.18 Two researchers (M.B.
interactive computer, fish bowl, haptic, manikin/mannequin, and C.P.) independently assessed the quality of all articles
role-play, simulated/standardized patient, virtual environment, (range, 5Y18/18). Differences in interpretation were resolved
and simulation. Search terms regarding empathy included through consensus.
compassion, communication skills, doctor-patient relations,
emotions, patient-centred communication, and student-pa- Synthesis
tient communication. A specialist librarian oversaw the de- A narrative, descriptive approach was taken across the
velopment and implementation of the search strategies. The studies, drawing from the principles of realist review19 by
initial yield of studies for review contained studies that focusing on ‘‘demiregularities’’ to elucidate potential mech-
fulfilled all 3 search concepts, that is, articles that contained anisms whereby health professional students learn empathy
search terms for health professional students AND simula- from simulation-based education. Because of limitations of
tion AND empathy. The following limitations were applied: pre-post studies,20 randomized comparative studies were
English language, peer reviewed, and availability of the full- considered most closely, and effect sizes were displayed on a
text article. forest plot. Effect sizes were not pooled in a meta-analysis
Duplicates and studies that were unrelated to the review because this is not recommended when there is a diversity
question were excluded on title. Ten articles were selected of comparators21 or outcomes.22
from the yield to pilot the inclusion/exclusion criteria. All
authors rated all 10 articles and finalized criteria through RESULTS
consensus. The final criteria for inclusion of a publication Literature Search and Study Inclusion
were as follows: first, the population of the study was From the initial search of 11 databases, a total of 14,748
preservice (prelicensure) health professional students; sec- articles were retrieved, dated up to May 2, 2014. After du-
ond, the simulations could be any modality including role- plicates and title review, 836 abstracts were admitted to the
play, simulated patients, virtual, or mannequin based; third, next phase. After the review based on the inclusion/exclusion
that there was some comparison to assess improvement in criteria, 765 were excluded. Full publications for the re-
empathetic behaviors; and finally, that there be some kind of maining 71 articles were retrieved, and 44 were excluded
qualitative or quantitative assessment of empathy. In the last after review. An overview of the study inclusion process is
case, we included studies that contained subscales or even shown in Figure 1.
single items, if the intention was to assess empathy. Veteri-
nary students, cases where there was no enactment of a Data Extraction and Quality Assessment
situation (eg, part-task trainers, paper cases), commentaries Table 1 summarizes the 27 included studies. Fourteen
or similar, studies that investigated communication without studies were published from 2000 onward, 8 from 2012
specific mention of empathy, or experiences that were not onward. Professional groups were as follows: medicine (18),
educational were excluded. Systematic reviews that matched nursing (4), pharmacy (2), social work (1), dental hygiene
the inclusion criteria were acceptable. All remaining abstracts (1), and nutrition/dietetics (1). There were 14 pre-post de-
were then independently examined by 2 of the authors signs, 9 randomized controlled trials (RCTs) or randomized
against the inclusion/exclusion criteria for progression to trials, of which 1 contained a 3-way comparison and 5 had
full-text articles. quasi-experimental designs. This includes 1 study that
All remaining studies were read in full text by 2 of the reported both an RCT and pre-post design. Three studies
authors and independently examined against the inclusion/ reported supplementary qualitative methods.
exclusion criteria. Agreement was reached through discus- Of the 9 RCT studies, 4 (44%) reported significant
sion and negotiation. improvements in learners’ empathy or empathetic behaviors
between those who learned via simulation and those who
Data Extraction
were given an n = 2 or no n = 2 alternative (quality as-
Data were extracted from all eligible articles, including
sessment range, 11Y15.5). Of the 9 RCTs, 3 (33%) reported
the location of study, student sample, study design, simu-
no significant change between those who learned via simu-
lation modality, comparator, outcomes, and results. Data
lation and those who were given an n = 1 or no n = 2 al-
were extracted by 2 of the authors, and agreement was
ternative (quality assessment range, 9.5Y12.5). Of the 9
reached by negotiation. Two of the authors (M.B. and either
RCTs, 3 (33%) reported significant differences between
C.P. or L.M.A.) finalized terms, gaps, and discordances be-
different approaches to simulation-based education (quality
tween reviewers. Where effect sizes were not reported, Cohen
assessment range, 12.5Y15.5). Of the 14 pre-post designs,
d was calculated from available data.
12 (86%) reported a significant improvement in learners’
Quality Assessment measures of empathy (quality assessment range, 7.5Y12.15).
The included studies presented either entirely or pre- One pre-post study (7%) reported a decrease in empathy
dominantly quantitative data and so were assessed for quality (quality assessment score, 10.5), and 1 (7%) did not conduct

Vol. 10, Number 5, October 2015 * 2015 Society for Simulation in Healthcare 309
Copyright © 2015 by the Society for Simulation in Healthcare. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
FIGURE 1. Study inclusion process.

statistical analysis (quality assessment score, 8). Of the quasi- Synthesis


experimental designs, 4 (80%) of the 5 reported significant When the simulation designs of the studies were con-
improvement in learners’ empathy levels between those who sidered as a whole, with a particular focus on mechanisms
learned via simulation and those who were given an n = 1 or that might promote learning, 2 themes were noted as follows:
no n = 3 alternative (quality assessment range, 8.5Y12.5). Of 1. Being a health professional
5 quasi-experimental studies, 1 (20%) reported no change These designs required the learner to simulate a health
between those who learned via simulation and those who professional. These were generally designed for stu-
were given no alternative (quality assessment score, 8.5) dents to develop skills in communication. The most
(Table 1). common designs were interacting with an SP or as
One article reported a pre-post and an RCT study of part of a peer role-play.
the same intervention in different years. Cahan et al21 de- 2. Being a patient
scribe that the pre-post indicated significant learning for a These designs required the learner to simulate the
cohort experiencing an intervention when comparative trials patient. The most common designs were role-play,
showed that there were no significant differences in empathy auditory hallucination simulations, and games about
between simulation group and those who did not have an negotiating being elderly.
alternative. When the studies are categorized according to these
The outcome measures were diverse, and many were themes, of 27 simulation designs, 10 (37%) required the
subdomains of other scales. Of the 27 studies, 17 (63%) used learner to only simulate the patient, 7 (26%) required the
self-report measures; 81Y3,22,26,29,40,41 used well-studied at- learner to only simulate the health professional, and 10
titudinal scales such as the Jefferson Scale of Empathy36; (37%) (most commonly role-play) required learners to act as
325,28,30 used the literature-based but not rigorously studied both or either patient and health professional. Other
scale, the ‘‘Maxwell and Sullivan’’ questionnaire; and 634,39,42Y45 groupings, such as type of measure, type of comparator,
used self-designed questionnaires. Of the 27 studies, 10 (37%) length of intervention, or content of simulation, did not
used raters’ assessment of behaviors with simulated patients, present as coherent themes.
using previously developed scales or Objective Structured As noted earlier, one study showed different results be-
Clinical Examination (OSCE) scores. Raters included sim- tween a pre-post study and an RCT. Norman20 suggests that
ulated patients, trained laypeople, examiners, researchers, experimental designs are best for investigating efficacy. He
and, in one instance, patients. Many studies had a focus on underlines the limitations of pre-post 1-group design, which
measuring changes in empathetic behaviors through self- cannot distinguish improvements that are due to the inter-
ratings or observational ratings; others considered more vention from ‘‘natural’’ student progression.20 To reduce this
closely the learner’s affect through attitudinal questionnaires. risk of bias and because there were sufficient experimental
Some studies focused on attitudes to specific patient expe- designs within the included articles, the synthesis process
riences (obesity, aged care, mental illness). One RCT dem- focused on studies where the participants were randomized.
onstrated improved empathy after intervention using one Arms of RCT interventions were categorized according
different scale, the Arizona Clinical Interview Rating Scale to the 2 themesVbeing a patient and being a health profes-
(ACIRS) but not another, the Roter Interactional Analysis sional. Refer to Table 2 for details. As mentioned, there were
System (RIAS).46 3 RCTs3,21,23 that did not show significant differences in

310 Learning Empathy Through Simulation Simulation in Healthcare


Copyright © 2015 by the Society for Simulation in Healthcare. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
TABLE 1. Profile of Included Studies
Comparator Outcome Measure/s Psychometric MERSQI18
Simulation Modality (Experimental (Empathy Only) Properties of (Range,
Reference Location Sample Size Study Design and Topic Designs) and Rater Instrument Results 5Y18)
Bath et al,23 Un 34 final-year Pre-post 24-h care of a V Author devised questionnaire: Not reported 3% less empathy 8
2000 medical students computerized doll questions regarding 47% unchanged attitudes
(at posttest) simulating 6-wk-old empathy toward parents 35% a little more empathy
infant of young children 15% a lot more empathy
Self-rated Insufficient data to
calculate ES
Bayne et al,24 United States 22 third-year Pre-post SP and RP, part V Consultation and Relational CARE referenced in the Improved empathy 11.5

Vol. 10, Number 5, October 2015


2001 medical students of facilitated Empathy (CARE) Scale literature; previous (P G 0.01, d = 0.75)
discussions regarding Rated by SPs. psychometric properties
empathy (2 d) reported as > = 0.93 and
concurrent validity with
other measures,
r = 0.84/r = 0.63.
Bosse et al,25 Germany 103 fifth-year RCT, 3 group using SP or RP communication Established course Calgary-Cambridge subscale Calgary-Cambridge subscale Improved understanding of 15.5
2012 medical students pre-post measures with and counselling without RP or SP Rated by trained psychologist referenced in the literature. parents’ perspective for RP
of parents of sick compared with within an OSCE > reported as 0.705. and SP (P G 0.001, d = 1.48)
children (9 cases) both SP and RP and P G 0.006, d = 0.63)
compared
with CG.
Higher scores for the RP group
than for the SP group
(P G 0.001, d = 0.71)
Bunn et al,26 United States 150 medical students RCT, 2 group using 40-min simulated No intervention Jefferson Scale of Physician JSPE-S referenced in Improved empathy (P G 0.0001) 11
2009 pre-post measures auditory hallucination Empathy Student Version the literature. for intervention compared
(JSPE-S) with control
Self-rated ES calculated as d = 0.60
Cahan et al,21 United States 147 third-year Two study designs: SP following Study 1: no Author devised assessment Interrater reliability: 288 Study 1. RCT: no significant Study 1, 14;
2010 medical students 2-group RCT and communication skills intervention rubric grading written responses by 2 raters; > improvement in empathy study 2,
pre-post training, workshop Study 2: no response to vignettes reported as 0.808 (P G 0.94) compared with 12.5
part of a human comparator Vignettes rated by 1 or control. ES calculated
factors curriculum 2 authors as d = 0.02
Study 2. Pre-post: improved
empathy (P G 0.001) ES
calculated as d = 1.95
Chaffin and United States 67 senior Pre-post content 30- to 40-min V Author devised rating: Likert None reported Significantly improved empathy 8.5
Adams nursing students analysis of written simulated auditory scale (1Y5) regarding (P G 0.001) ES calculated
201327 reflections hallucination self-rated level as d = 3.22
of empathy
Self-rated
Chunharas Thailand 89 fifth-year Two groups Students practice Students practice Author devised scale (1Y4) None reported Intervention groupVsignificantly 9
et al,28 2013 medical students nonrandomized injection skill injection skill on rating feeling of empathy more students retrospectively rated
with postmeasure manikins and manikins toward children their empathy as ‘‘high’’ after
on themselves Self-rated retrospectively injecting children as those
assessing empathy at times who retrospectively rated their
during the experience empathy as high (P G 0.01).
There was a significant difference
between the intervention and
control line at the baseline
measure of retrospectively rating
empathy before injecting children.

Copyright © 2015 by the Society for Simulation in Healthcare. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
Insufficient data to calculate ES.

* 2015 Society for Simulation in Healthcare


311
(continued on next page)
TABLE 1. (Continued)

312
Comparator Outcome Measure/s Psychometric MERSQI18
Simulation Modality (Experimental (Empathy Only) Properties of (Range,
Reference Location Sample Size Study Design and Topic Designs) and Rater Instrument Results 5Y18)
Daeppen et al,29 Switzerland 91 fifth-year RCT Motivational No motivational Motivational Interviewing MITI psychometric properties Empathy subscale higher 14.5
2012 medical students interviewing after interviewing Treatment Integrity (MITI) referenced in the literature. in the intervention
training 2  4 h workshops scoring tool empathy subscale. ICC = 0.51 reported for groupV(P G 0.001),
workshops, with Videos with SPs assessed by empathy subscale ratings. ES: 5 = 0.73
significant RP trained nonmedical raters (nonparametric)
component
Dearing et al,2 United States 94 nursing students 1. Two-group 45-min No intervention 1. Medical Condition Regard MCRS referenced in 1. There was a significant 12.5
2006 nonrandomized simulated auditory Scale (MCRS) the literature. difference in the posttest
comparison hallucination Self-rated MCRS scores of the 2 groups
2. Focus groups 2. Themes from focus group (P = 0.001). Significant
transcripts analyzed using improvement in 6 items.
schema analysis Analysis was item by item,
no ES calculated.
2. Qualitative differences
noted between 2 groups
in the ability to grasp

Learning Empathy Through Simulation


patients’ perspective.
Deladisma United States 84 medical students Two-group randomized Virtual patient SP Consensus devised score sheet > for empathy Higher empathy in the SP group 14
et al,30 2007 trial, single measure case with including observed behavior measureVreported (P G 0.05) compared with
of response to abdominal pain and specific empathy item. as 0.92 virtual patient. ES calculated
intervention Clinician raters assessed as d = 0.70.
videos for nonverbal Empathy positively correlated
communication. with eye contact, body lean,
head nod, and level
of immersion.
Dikici et al,31 Turkey 146 medical students, Pre-post, with SP within a breaking V Empathetic behavior domain None reported Improved empathetic 11
2009 level not stated 6 mo postrepeated bad news sessions in OSCE score on breaking behaviors immediately
(4 cases) bad news. after (P G 0.001) and 6 mo
Rated as part of OSCE. later (P G 0.001). ES calculated
pre-post as 14.70.
Evans et al,32 United States 101 first-year Pre-post Geriatric Medication V Self-devised 8-item None reported Responses to 6 of 8 items 7.5
2005 pharmacy students Game (1Y1.5 h) questionnaire, regarding significantly improved after
attitudes toward intervention (individual
the elderly. items were compared,
Self-rated. significant P ranged from
0.001 to 0.47) Analysis
was item by item, no
ES calculated.
Galletly et al,33 Australia* 87 final-year Pre-post Simulated auditory V Attitudes to Mental Illness AMIQ referenced in Improved attitudes (P G 0.001) 8.5
2011 medical students hallucinations Questionnaire (AMIQ) the literature. overall; lowest 2 and lowest 3
(45 min) as part Self-rated improved significantly but
of session on highest 2 and highest 3
mental illness did not. ES calculated as
d = 2.25.
Gleber et al,3 United States 24 dental RCT, 2-group with SP as part of No intervention Mehrabian and Epstein Mehrabian and Epstein No significant improvement in 9.5
1995 hygiene students pre-post and 12-mo other modalities Measure of Emotional Measure of Emotional empathy after test (P G 0.73)
follow-up measures within 20 h on Empathy Empathy referenced in the compared with control group
interpersonal Self-rated. literature, cited as subscale or at follow-up. ES calculated
skills training intercorrelation r 9 0.30 as d = 0.11.
and significant at 0.01 level
of confidence;

Copyright © 2015 by the Society for Simulation in Healthcare. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
> cited as 0.84.

Simulation in Healthcare
Gockel and United States 81 first-year social Pre-post, with RP counselling V Interpersonal Reactivity IRI referenced in the Total empathy score overall 10.5
Burton work students 3 mo postrepeated scenarios, central Index (IRI) (4 subscales : literature. Reliability reduced from pre (week 1)
201434 to a foundational perspective taking, between subscale referenced and follow-up (3 mo after
interviewing empathetic concern, fantasy as > = 0.61Y0.81, completing task) (P G 0.05).
skills course scale, personal distress). and interitem Subscale of personal distress
Self-rated reliabilities = 0.65Y0.81. reduced from pre to post to
follow-up (P G 0.01)
Pre-to-post empathy decline,
ES calculated as d = 0.09.
Grice et al,35 United States 158 (2009 cohort), Pre-post RP teaching how to V Author devised rubric, None reported Significant improvement in 12.5
2012 plus 126 (2010 (both cohorts) use medical devices subscale of Empathy empathy aspects across
cohort) third-year Habit from Four both cohorts (P G 0.05)
pharmacy students Habits Model. except 2009 missed one

Vol. 10, Number 5, October 2015


Rated by faculty with live item being measured.
video feed of performance Insufficient data to
with SP. calculate ES.
Henry et al,36 United States 127 nursing (73) 1. RCT 2 groups with The Aging Game, Class discussion 1. Four items adapted from Maxwell and Sullivan 1. Significant improvement 11
2011 and nutrition pre-post measures (75 min including (75 min) Maxwell and Sullivan referenced in the literature pre-post in intervention
(54) students 2. Qualitative data debriefing) questionnaire (but not psychometric group in 1 of 4 items
2. Reflective writing properties). (I understand what it feels
like to have problems
growing older, P G 0.05).
Intervention group
significantly more empathetic
than control (P G 0.01).
ES calculated as d = j0.24.
Self-rated 2. Thematic analysis indicating
cognitive and emotive aspects
of empathy.
Hueberger United States 93 dietetic students Pre-post RP as patient V Jefferson Scale of JSE adapted for dietetics Improved empathy shown 8.5
et al,37 2010 with gestational Empathy (JSE) students from JSPE. JSPE in 6/20 questions (range,
diabetes (2 wk) Self-rated referenced in the literature. P G 0.001 to P G 0.05).
Analysis was item by item,
no ES calculated.
Kushner United States 127 first-year Pre-post plus SP session V 16-item author devised Reliability of empathy Improved empathy shown in 10.5
et al,38 2014 medical students 1-yr postrepeated regarding obesity survey constructed from subscale, reported as subscale, from baseline to post
other instruments. > = 0.63. (P G .0001) and baseline to
Self-rated long-term follow-up
(P = 0.001). ES calculated
as d = 0.34.
Lim et al,39 New Zealand 149 fifth-year One year no RP workshop No intervention Jefferson Scale of Physician JSPE referenced in the literature, Improved empathy (P G 0.001). No 13
2011 medical students, intervention compared (5 training scenarios) Empathy Self-rated. referenced > = 0.87Y0.89. significant differences in baseline.
across 2 y with next year with as part of ES calculated as d = 0.57.
intervention using a psychological
baseline and medicine module
postmeasures
Mawson,47 Australia 59 second-year Pre-post 5 min role-play with V Author devised 11-item None reported Increase in compassion toward 8.5
2014 nursing students simulated auditory survey for increased those who experience auditory
hallucinations knowledge, empathy, hallucinations (P G 0.001).
and changed attitudes Analysis was item by item,
toward people with no ES calculated.
auditory hallucinations
Self-rated
Pacala et al,48 United States 55 fourth-year Volunteers (39) Modified Aging Game. No intervention 11 items adapted from Maxwell and Sullivan No difference in change in 8.5
1995 medical students compared with 3-h workshop Maxwell and Sullivan questionnaire referenced empathy between groups
others (16), measured questionnaire. as unvalidated. (P = 0.15). ES calculated

Copyright © 2015 by the Society for Simulation in Healthcare. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
with pre-post Self-rated. as d = 0.41 Improved
empathy by 23% (P G 0.001)
in participants. ES calculated

* 2015 Society for Simulation in Healthcare


as d = 0.47.

313
(continued on next page)
TABLE 1. (Continued)

314
Comparator Outcome Measure/s Psychometric MERSQI18
Simulation Modality (Experimental (Empathy Only) Properties of (Range,
Reference Location Sample Size Study Design and Topic Designs) and Rater Instrument Results 5Y18)
Pacoe et al,49 United States* 20 first-year Volunteers (13) RP workshop of No intervention 1. The Wells Empathic WECT and IFD psychometric Improved scores for intervention 11.5
1976 medical students compared with counselling session Communication Test testing referenced in the group with both measures
others (7), measured alternating with (WECT) literature. (P G 0.001 for both)
with pre-post group discussions 2. The Index of Facilitative ES calculated as 5 = 2.96
across 16 wk Discrimination (IFD) (nonparametric)
(recognition of empathy)
Raters assessed written
responses to 10 videos.
Sanson-Fisher Australia* 40 second-year RCT, 2-group cross-over SP, history taking with Real patients Truax empathy scale Truax empathy scale referenced No significant improvement in
et al,40 1980 medical students study, single measure patients with neurotic Trained assessors of in the literature Concordance empathy between simulated
of response to disorders random 3  2-min between 20% randomly and real patients in total or in
intervention selection of audio selected segments either arm of crossover. ES
recording, average and original rating, 93.8% real-simulated calculated as
ratings used. d = 0.25 (favors real) ES
simulated-real calculated
as :d = 0.25 (favors real)

Learning Empathy Through Simulation


Schweller Brazil 124 fourth-year Pre-post (fourth-year Each learner experiences V Jefferson Scale of Physician JSPE and IRI referenced in Fourth-year students demonstrated 9
et al,1 2014 medical students students administered an SP encounter, plus Empathy (JSPE), IRI. the literature. a significant increase in JSPE
n = 123 fifth-year posttest after 3 mo); observation of others Self-rated (P G 0.001, ES = 0.61) and IRI
medical students) sixth-year students and debriefing; emphasis (P = .003, ES = 0.19). Sixth-year
administered posttest on patient feelings students demonstrated a
after 1 mo significant increase in JSPE
(P G 0.001, ES = 0.64) and
IRI (P G 0.001, ES = j0.20).
Vannatta United States 154 first-year RCT, 3 groups with SPs with feedback from SPs with feedback Empathy skills from (1) RIAS and ACIR referenced in SPs with feedback from 12.5
et al,41 1996 medical students pre-post and SPs teaching medical from faculty and modified RIASVpre-post the literature. SPsVACIR significantly
postmeasures interviewing RP with feedback measures and improved relative to feedback
from faculty (2) Arizona Clinical to faculty alone (P = 0.2,
Interviewing Rating Scale calculated d = 0.60). RIAS no
(ACIRS)Vpost only. significant difference (calculated
Trained assessors d = 0.024 (favors faculty
rated video-recorded feedback). SPs with SP feedback
SP encounters. and RP with faculty feedback.
ACIR, no significant difference
[calculated d = 0.0822 (favors SP
feedback)] RIAS no significant
difference [calculated d = 0.3791
(favors SP feedback)]. SP with
faculty feedback and RP with
faculty feedback. ACIR, no
significant difference [calculated
d = 0.5153 (favors RP)]; RIAS,
no significant difference
d = 0.403 (calculated d favors
faculty feedback).
Varkey United States* 84 first-year Pre-post Modified Aging Game V 11 items from the Maxwell and Sullivan Improved attitudinal change 9.5
et al,50 2006 medical students modified Maxwell and questionnaire referenced in 6/8 questions (range,
Sullivan questionnaire. as unvalidated. P = 0.0001 to P = 0.049);
Self-rated increased empathy in 2 of 3
questions (P G 0.0001 and
P G 0.0002) Analysis was item

Copyright © 2015 by the Society for Simulation in Healthcare. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
by item, no ES calculated.
*Location, if not specified, is the location of the primary author’s affiliation.
Calculated, statistic compiled by review authors.
d, Cohen d; ES, effect size; RP, role-play methodology; SP, simulated patient methodology; >, Cronbach >.

Simulation in Healthcare
TABLE 2. Details of Randomized Controlled Trials
Learner Role Effect Size favors
Outcome Measure/s in Simulation Simulation, Unless
Study Details RCT Design Comparisons (Empathy Only) Rater Significance (BP or BHP) Noted Otherwise
Bosse et al,25 2012 Three groups using 1. RP with Calgary-Cambridge Observational rating 1. RP significantly improves 1. BP and BHP 1. d = 1.47
Germany pre-post measures alternative course subscaleVpreviously within OSCE empathy compared 2. BHP only 2. d = 0.46
103 fifth-year 2. SP with validated communication with alternative. 3. Comparing 3. d = 1.12 (favors RP)
medical students alternative course skills scale 2. SP significantly improves 1 and 2
SP or RP communication 3. RP with SP External behavioral rating. empathy compared
with and counselling with alternative.

Vol. 10, Number 5, October 2015


of parents of sick 3. RP significantly improves
children (9 cases) empathy compared
with SP.
Bunn et al,26 2009 Two-group RCT using Simulation with JSPE-SVpreviously Self-rated Simulation significantly BP d = 0.60
United States pre-post measures no intervention validated empathy improves empathy
150 medical students specific scale
40-min simulated
auditory hallucination
Cahan et al,21 2010 Two-group RCT Simulation with Author devised assessment Rating of No significant improvement BHP d = 0.19
(study 1) using postmeasures no intervention rubric grading written written responses in empathy compared
SP following response to vignettes. with control
communication skills > reported as 0.808
training, workshop
part of a human
factors curriculum
Daeppen et al,29 2012 Two-group RCT Motivational interviewing Motivational Interviewing Videos of SPs by Empathy subscale BP/BHP d = 0.92
Switzerland using postmeasures workshops with Treatment Integrity (MITI) trained raters significantly higher in
Fifth-year no motivational scoring tool empathy intervention group
medical students interviewing workshops subscale Y previously
Motivational interviewing validated.
after training 2  4-h
workshops, with
significant RP component
Deladisma et al,30 2007 Two-group randomized Virtual patient with Consensus devised score Videos rated SP significant improves BHP d = 0.70 (favors SP)
United States trial, single measure simulated patient sheet including observed by clinicians empathy relative to
84 medical students of response to behavior and specific for nonverbal virtual patient.
Virtual patient case with intervention empathy item. > r reported communication
abdominal pain as 0.92.
Gleber et al,3 1995 Two-group RCT with Interpersonal skills Mehrabian and Epstein Self-rated No significant improvement BHP d = 0.11
United States pre-post and 12-mo training with no Measure of Emotional in empathy in intervention
24 dental hygiene students follow-up measures interpersonal skills Empathy (previously compared with control
SP as part of other training validated) group or at follow-up.
modalities within
20 h on interpersonal
skills training
Henry et al,36 2011 Two-group RCT with Simulation with class 4 items adapted from Self-rated Intervention group BP d = 0.24
United States pre-post measures discussion (75 min) Maxwell and Sullivan significantly more
127, nursing (73) and (referenced but empathetic than control.
nutrition (54) students not validated).

Copyright © 2015 by the Society for Simulation in Healthcare. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
The Aging Game, (75 min
including debriefing)

* 2015 Society for Simulation in Healthcare


315
(continued on next page)
(favors SP/SP feedback),
student empathy development between simulation inter-

with BHP and BP 2. ACIR d = 0.08 (favors

RIAS d = 0.34 (favors


RIAS d = 0.02 (favors

SP/faculty feedback)
Simulation, Unless

(favors real patient)


vention and a control. In all of these, the intervention re-

SP/faculty feedback)
Effect Size favors

Noted Otherwise

(favors RP/faculty
SP/SP feedback),

feedback), RIAS
SP/SP feedback)

d = 0.40 (favors
quired the learners to only ‘‘be a health professional’’ and

3. ACIR d = 0.52
1. ACIR d = 0.60
never a patient. All 4 RCTs that contained interventions
d = 0.30 where the learner assumed the role of the patient all or some
of the time within the simulation22,24,27,28 showed significant
improvements relative to a control. These are represented in
the forest plot in Figure 2. The study of Sanson-Fisher et al,23

with BHP and BP


that compared SP and real patient encounters, is separated in

2. BHP compared

3. BHP compared
in Simulation
Learner Role

(BP or BHP)

1. BHP only
the forest plot, as the comparator is very different. RCTs that
compare different aspects of simulation are not included in
this figure.
BHP

There was a range of educational design features sur-


rounding the simulated experience. There were 4 (15%) of 27
feedback to faculty alone;
improvement in empathy

in either arm of crossover.

comparative studies that compared different types and fea-


SPsVACIR significantly
real patients in total or
between simulated and

SPs with feedback from

tures of simulation in promoting empathy; 3 of these were


improved relative to

no other significant
differences between
Significance

randomized (Table 2). Two of these indicated that role-play


encounters improved learner’s empathy more than SP en-
No significant

categories.

counters27 or using a mannequin.43 One study indicated that


SP encounters improved learner’s empathy more than virtual
patients,31 and another study had mixed results regarding the
efficacy of feedback from SPs compared with feedback from
6 min of audio, from

real patients and SPs

faculty.46
Videos of SPs assessed
assessment on both
a combination of

by trained raters
Trained raters on

DISCUSSION
Rater

The findings of this review suggest that simulation may


be an appropriate educational methodology for developing
empathy and/or empathetic behaviors in preservice health
professional students. This finding was not universal, and it
seems dependent on the type and educational features of the
Interviewing Rating Scale
(ACIRS), both previously
Outcome Measure/s

simulations as well as the definition of empathy and asso-


(previously validated).

validated in literature.
(Empathy Only)

RIASVpsychometric

ciated measures. The variety and complexity of the reported


2. Arizona Clinical
1. Real patients with SPs Truax empathy scale

from: 1. modified

educational designs are exciting from a teaching perspective


Empathy skills

but make definite conclusions challenging from a research


perspective. The challenges with measures are brought into
focus by 1 study,29 which indicated a decline in empathy after
the intervention. The authors attributed this to a decline in
the ‘‘personal distress’’ subscale of the Interpersonal Reac-
2. SPs with real patients

tivity Index, which measures ‘‘one’s own feelings of discom-


Three-group RCT with 1. SPs with feedback

2. SPs with feedback

3. SPs with feedback


from SPs and SPs

RP with feedback
from SPs and RP
Comparisons

from faculty and

fort in the face of emotionally challenging interpersonal


with feedback

with feedback

BHP, Being a health professional; BP, Being a patient; d, Cohen d; RP, role-play.

situations.’’29 This inconsistency highlights broader debates


from faculty

from faculty

from faculty

about whether empathy should be measured and taught as


an internal process or as observable communications.32
Collectively, the randomized controlled studies suggest
that the simulation approach that seems most beneficial is
one that asks the learners to literally stand in patients’ shoes.
single measure of
crossover study,
RCT Design

These results are also supported by an RCT, which


Sanson-Fisher et al,40 1980 Two-group RCT

postmeasures
pre-post and
intervention
response to

specifically examined the issue of ‘‘playing the patient.’’ In


this study, Bosse et al27 compared communication skills
development in learners role-playing (that is, acting as both
learner and health professional) with those working with a
simulated patient (that is, acting as a health professional)
and with a control (no simulation experience). Final-year
SPs with feedback from
SPs teaching medical
TABLE 2. (Continued)

Vannatta et al,41 1996

medical students (n = 103) were assigned to 3 groups re-


neurotic disorders
History taking with
medical students

medical students
SP/patients with

ceiving education and training in counseling caregivers of


40 second-year

interviewing
Study Details

sick children. Two groups received counseling and com-


United States
154 first-year

munication training using either role-plays or simulated


Australia

patient encounters, whereas the control group received


the only standard coursework learning material. After the

316 Learning Empathy Through Simulation Simulation in Healthcare


Copyright © 2015 by the Society for Simulation in Healthcare. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
FIGURE 2. Forest plot of RCTs that compare simulation to no or some alternative, with empathy as an outcome. Effect sizes with 95%
confidence interval are shown in 3 themes, represented by diamonds, squares, and a triangle. Effect size of RCTs where the
comparison is between a ‘‘being the patient’’ (including role-plays) simulation and some or no alternative. Effect size of RCTs where
the comparison is between a ‘‘being health professional’’ only (excluding role plays) and some or no alternative (excluding real
patients). Effect size of RCT where the comparison is between a ‘‘being a health professional’’ simulation and real patients. Across
these themes, the study author and date are in bold when the comparator is ‘‘no alternative’’; other studies have nonsimulation
comparators.

interventions, 6 OSCE stations were undertaken, each with empathy and empathetic behaviors. There is also a caveat
1 SP. Final OSCE scores were rated using the Calgary- to using role-play methodology. Without creating an ap-
Cambridge Referenced Observation Guide (CCROG); anal- propriately safe learning environment,33 acting as a surrogate
ysis indicated that the role-play group significantly improved for a patient might also provoke other emotions such as
compared with the SP group with respect to ‘‘understanding vulnerability or anxiety.35 The role of debriefing may
the patient’s perspective’’ (t = 5.11, and P G 0.001, d = 1.12), be particularly important in assisting students to translate
and notably, this was the only domain where this significant their experiences as role-play patients to general commu-
difference was found. This study was rated 15.5/18 on the nication skills.
MERSQI. It is also worth noting that of the 17 studies where the
The theoretical constructs of empathy support this idea. learners were ‘‘being a health professional,’’ only 1 focussed
Assuming the role of the patient introduces the under- solely on empathy development. Empathy development was
standing and shared feelings of the patient’s perspective, but interwoven with learning other skills such as interviewing,31
then, the learner must remove themselves from the simu- motivational interviewing,24,41 effective patient/client com-
lation and through debriefing or feedback processes and munication skills,3,21,27,29 interpersonal skills,3 and psycho-
must translate this experience into the empathetic behaviors motor skills.43 In many instances, these associated skills
we expect from health care practitioners. Simulation edu- developed alongside empathy; sometimes, these skills were
cation might combat the decline of empathy noted in later used as markers or indicator of empathy. For example,
years of study,16 as it permits students to manage both a less Deladisma et al31 reported the development of nonverbal
idealistic view of health care practice and an appropriate level communication skills and learning to ask clear questions as a
of identification with patients. measure for increased empathy.
Role-play may be particularly valuable. Role-play per- This review builds on and adds to the work of other
mits rotating roles of patient and health care provider and related systematic reviews, particularly those of Stepien and
therefore provides both the experience of the patient and the Baernstein37 and Batt-Rawden et al.13 It contains 23 addi-
experience of working with patients. This role reversal may tional studies not reported within these previous reviews,
be an important mechanism in developing empathy and which explore empathy in medical education, and specifi-
deserves further exploration. Role-play is sometimes seen cally draws the link between empathy and simulation.
as a less desirable but a cheaper alternative for SP encoun- This review highlights the value of taking the role of pa-
ters, but perhaps, it could become first choice for learning tient, either through role-play or as part of specific ‘‘patient

Vol. 10, Number 5, October 2015 * 2015 Society for Simulation in Healthcare 317
Copyright © 2015 by the Society for Simulation in Healthcare. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
experience’’ simulation design, which was not noted in ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
previous studies, although it is congruent with their findings. The authors thank Anne Young, expert librarian, for her
The limitations to the findings of this review are de- guidance and assistance in searching the literature, and the
rived from the included studies and from the review process anonymous reviewers of this article.
itself. With respect to the included studies, although some
had high-quality experimental designs, these were in the
minority. Reporting standards were variable; effect sizes REFERENCES
were rarely reported, and sometimes, even a lack of means 1. Schweller M, Costa FO, Antônio MARGM, Amaral EM, De
Carvalho-Filho MA. The impact of simulated medical consultations on
and SDs made calculating these statistics challenging. There
the empathy levels of students at one medical school. Acad Med
were inconsistencies with the outcome measures, as noted 2014;89(4):632Y637.
earlier in the discussion. There was notably a lack of ex- 2. Dearing KS, Steadman S. Challenging stereotyping and bias: a voice
perimental data examining groups over periods, which simulation study. J Nurs Educ 2008;47(2):59Y65.
might give information about how empathy decays after 3. Gleber JM. Interpersonal communications skills for dental hygiene
interventions. The nature of the comparator was not well students: a pilot training program. J Dent Hyg 1995;69(1):19Y30.
considered. As has been noted previously,38 studies that aim 4. Ker J, Bradley P. Simulation in m edical e ducation. In: Swanick T, ed.
to compare a new medium to some or no alternatives have Understanding Medical Education: Evidence, Theory and Practice.
limitations because of the difficulty in forming valid Chicester, England: Wiley-Blackwell, 2010:164Y180.
comparison groups; this type of the design formed sizeable 5. Wear D, Varley JD. Rituals of verification: the role of simulation in
majority of the included studies. More consideration could developing and evaluating empathic communication. Patient Educ
Couns 2008;71(2):153Y156.
be given as to when and why simulation is the optimal way
to teach empathy, building on the body of work reported in 6. de la Croix A, Skelton J. The simulation game: an analysis of interactions
between students and simulated patients. Med Educ 2013;47(1):49Y58.
this review.
Future work might include further experimental studies 7. Hojat M. Definitions and conceptualization. In: Empathy in Patient
Care: Antecedents, Development, Measurement, and Outcomes. New
to replicate the results of the study of Bosse et al,27 which York, NY: Springer; 2007:3Y15.
compared the effects of empathy on SP encounters relative to
8. Davis MH. Measuring individual differences in empathy: evidence for a
role-play encounters and would be particularly useful, as well multidimensional approach. J Pers Soc Psychol 1983;44(1):113.
as in-depth qualitative comparative investigations of the
9. Mercer SW, McConnachie A, Maxwell M, Heaney D, Watt GC.
learner experience ‘‘being a patient.’’ It would be interesting Relevance and practical use of the Consultation and Relational Empathy
to see if this type of empathy education might be effective for (CARE) Measure in general practice. Fam Pract 2005;22(3):328Y334.
practicing health professionals. The role of feedback is 10. Hojat M, Louis DZ, Markham FW, Wender R, Rabinowitz C, Gonnella
worthy of further exploration. In addition, with the excep- JS. Physicians’ empathy and clinical outcomes for diabetic patients.
tion of 1 study comparing student behavior with simulated Acad Med 2011;86(3):359Y364.
to real patients, all other behavioral ratings were not assessed 11. Del Canale S, Louis DZ, Maio V, et al. The relationship between
with real patient encounters, with most tested in simulation. physician empathy and disease complications: an empirical study of
primary care physicians and their diabetic patients in Parma, Italy. Acad
Practitioners have had improvements in empathy assessed by Med 2012;87(9):1243Y1249.
real patients,14,15 and we suggest this type of assessment
12. Yang C-PP, Hargreaves WA, Bostrom A. Association of empathy of
could be extended to preservice health practitioners. nursing staff with reduction of seclusion and restraint in psychiatric
The review process itself also had limitations. The inpatient care. Psychiatr Serv 2014;65(2):251Y254.
synthesis was based on a simple categorization of studies. 13. Batt-Rawden SA, Chisolm MS, Anton B, Flickinger TE. Teaching
The review strategy was less likely to include qualitative empathy to medical students: an updated, systematic review. Acad Med
studies, as few of these are framed as comparative studies. 2013;88(8):1171Y1177.
There was no systematic hand-searching for additional ar- 14. Riess H, Kelley J, Bailey R, Dunn E, Phillips M. Empathy training for
ticles. Balanced against this is the comprehensive search resident physicians: a randomized controlled trial of a
neuroscience-informed curriculum. J Gen Intern Med
strategy that incorporated 11 databases, independent review
2012;27(10):1280Y1286.
for inclusion of each full text by 2 authors, use of an identical
15. Bonvicini KA, Perlin MJ, Bylund CL, Carroll G, Rouse RA, Goldstein
data extraction table for all studies, independent duplicate MG. Impact of communication training on physician expression of
quality assessment, and a synthesis approach that provides empathy in patient encounters. Patient Educ Couns 2009;75(1):3Y10.
an insight into possible learning mechanisms. 16. Neumann M, Edelhauser F, Tauschel D, et al. Empathy decline and its
In conclusion, the findings of this review suggest that reasons: a systematic review of studies with medical students and
simulation may be a useful educational methodology for residents. Acad Med 2011;86(8):996Y1009.
developing empathetic behaviors in preservice health pro- 17. Nunes P, Williams S, Sa B, Stevenson K. A study of empathy decline in
fessional students. The most interesting inference is the students from five health disciplines during their first year of training.
notion that simulations that ask the learner to act in the role Int J Med Educ 2011;2: 12Y17.
of patient may be more effective in developing empathy. This 18. Reed DA, Beckman TJ, Wright SM, Levine RB, Kern DE, Cook DA.
Predictive validity evidence for medical education research study quality
may have significant implications for educational design of
instrument scores: quality of submissions to JGIM’s Medical Education
simulations in preservice health professional curricula. Special Issue. J Gen Intern Med 2008;23(7):903Y907.
Further research is needed to confirm this result and also to 19. Wong G, Greenhalgh T, Pawson R. Internet-based medical education: a
investigate other features of simulation, which promote or realist review of what works, for whom and in what circumstances. BMC
inhibit learning empathy. Med Educ 2010;10(1):12.

318 Learning Empathy Through Simulation Simulation in Healthcare


Copyright © 2015 by the Society for Simulation in Healthcare. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
20. Norman G. Data dredging, salami-slicing, and other successful 34. Bath LE, Cunningham S, McIntosh N. Medical students’ attitudes to
strategies to ensure rejection: twelve tips on how to not get your paper caring for a young infantVcan parenting a doll influence these beliefs?
published. Adv Health Sci Educ Theory Pract 2014;19(1):1Y5. Arch Dis Child 2000;83(6):521Y523.
21. Cahan MA, Larkin AC, Starr S, et al. A human factors curriculum for 35. Nestel D, Tierney T. Role-play for medical students learning about
surgical clerkship students. Arch Surg 2010;145(12):1151Y1157. communication: guidelines for maximising benefits. BMC Med Educ
22. Bunn W, Terpstra J. Cultivating empathy for the mentally ill using 2007;7: 3.
simulated auditory hallucinations. Acad Psychiatry 2009;33(6):457Y460. 36. Fields SK, Mahan P, Tillman P, Harris J, Maxwell K, Hojat M. Measuring
23. Sanson-Fisher RW, Poole AD. Simulated patients and the assessment of empathy in healthcare profession students using the Jefferson Scale of
medical students’ interpersonal skills. Med Educ 1980;14(4):249Y253. Physician Empathy: health providerVstudent version. J Interprof Care
24. Daeppen JB, Fortini C, Bertholet N, et al. Training medical students to 2011;25(4):287Y293.
conduct motivational interviewing: a randomized controlled trial. 37. Stepien KA, Baernstein A. Educating for empathy. A review. J Gen Intern
Patient Educ Couns 2012;87(3):313Y318. Med 2006;21(5):524Y530.
25. Pacala JT, Boult C, Bland C, O’Brien J. Aging game improves medical 38. Cook DA. The research we still are not doing: an agenda for the study of
students’ attitudes toward caring for elders. Gerontol Geriatr Educ computer-based learning. Acad Med 2005;80(6):541Y548.
1995;15(4):45Y57.
39. Kushner RF, Zeiss DM, Feinglass JM, Yelen M. An obesity educational
26. Galletly CA, Burton CA. The effects of experiencing simulated auditory
intervention for medical students addressing weight bias and
hallucinations on attitudes to schizophrenia in final year medical
students. Schizophr Res 2010;117(2Y403):402Y3. communication skills using standardized patients. BMC Med Educ
2014;14: 53.
27. Bosse HM, Schultz JH, Nickel M, et al. The effect of using standardized
patients or peer role play on ratings of undergraduate communication 40. Heuberger R. Increasing empathy, knowledge, and skills regarding
training: a randomized controlled trial. Patient Educ Couns gestational diabetes. Topics in Clinical Nutrition 2010;25(3):
2012;87(3):300Y306. 250Y255.
28. Henry BW, Ozier AD, Johnson A. Empathetic responses and attitudes 41. Lim BT, Moriarty H, Huthwaite M. ‘‘Being-in-role’’: a teaching
about older adults: how experience with the aging game measures up. innovation to enhance empathic communication skills in medical
Educ Gerontol 2011;37(10):924Y941. students. Med Teach 2011;33(12):e663Ye669.
29. Gockel A, Burton DL. An evaluation of prepracticum helping skills 42. Chaffin AJ, Adams C. Creating empathy through use of a hearing voices
training for graduate social work students. J Soc Work Educ simulation. Arch Dis Child 2013;9(8):e293Ye304.
2014;50(1):101Y119.
43. Chunharas A, Hetrakul P, Boonyobol R, Udomkitti T, Tassanapitikul T,
30. Varkey P, Chutka DS, Lesnick TG. The aging game: improving medical Wattanasirichaigoon D. Medical students themselves as surrogate
students’ attitudes toward caring for the elderly. J Am Med Dir Assoc
patients increased satisfaction, confidence, and performance in
2006;7(4):224Y229.
practicing injection skill. Med Teach 2013;35(4):308Y313.
31. Deladisma AM, Cohen M, Stevens A, et al. Do medical students respond
empathetically to a virtual patient? Am J Surg 2007;193(6):756Y760. 44. Evans S, Lombardo M, Belgeri M, Fontane P. The geriatric medication
game in pharmacy education. Am J Pharm Educ 2005;69(3):304Y310.
32. Greason PB, Cashwell CS. Mindfulness and counseling self-efficacy: the
mediating role of attention and empathy. Counselor Education and 45. Mawson K. Use of media technology to enhance the learning of student
Supervision 2009;49(1):2Y19. nurses in regards to auditory hallucinations. Int J Ment Health Nurs
2014;23(2):135Y144.
33. Baile WF, Blatner A. Teaching communication skills: using action
methods to enhance role-play in problem-based learning. Simul Healthc 46. Vannatta JB, et al. Comparison of standardized patients and faculty in
2014;9(4):220Y227. teaching medical interviewing. Acad Med 1996;71(12):1360Y1362.

Vol. 10, Number 5, October 2015 * 2015 Society for Simulation in Healthcare 319
Copyright © 2015 by the Society for Simulation in Healthcare. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.

You might also like