You are on page 1of 12

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/261063471

Lane, H. B., & Pullen, P. C. (2004). Phonological awareness assessment and


instruction: A sound beginning. Boston: Allyn & Bacon.

Book · January 2004

CITATIONS READS

12 1,311

1 author:

Paige C. Pullen
University of Florida
68 PUBLICATIONS   1,391 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

IES Tier 2 Project IVI View project

Kauffman, J. M., Hallahan, D. P., and Pullen, P. C. (Eds.) Handbook of Special Education, Second Edition, New York: Routledge View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Paige C. Pullen on 01 January 2019.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Abstract. The article addresses translating
phonological awareness research for class-
room reading instruction. It presents a practi-
cal overview of phonological awareness devel-
opment and its relationship to beginning
reading, including a synopsis of findings of
recent research and an explanation of the
development of phonological skills. It presents
methods for formal and informal assessment
of children’s phonological awareness and
describes strategies for classroom-based
instruction in phonological skills with emer-

Preventing Reading gent readers.

Failure: Phonological R eading is a foundation skill for school


learning and life learning—the ability
to read is critical for success in modern
Awareness Assessment society. Learning to read is one of the most
important events in a child’s school career

and Instruction (Anderson, Hiebert, Scott, & Wilkinson,


1985; Lyon, 1999; National Reading
Panel, 2000). Unfortunately, many chil-
dren experience difficulties in the early
stages of learning to read that become bar-
riers to later reading and learning. A pri-
HOLLY B. LANE, PAIGE C. PULLEN, MARY R. EISELE, mary focus of recent research in educa-
and LuANN JORDAN tion, therefore, has been the prevention of
early reading problems (Adams, 1990;
Snow, Burns, & Griffin, 1998; Torgesen,
1998). One area of beginning reading
research that has received enormous atten-
tion in the professional literature is phono-
logical awareness. This research has been
called “a scientific success story”
(Stanovich, 1987) because phonological
awareness has been shown to be both a
reliable predictor of reading achievement
and a key to beginning reading acquisition
(Smith, Simmons, & Kame’enui, 1995).

What Is Phonological Awareness?


Phonological awareness can be defined
as conscious sensitivity to the sound
structure of language. Children with
strong phonological awareness can detect,
match, blend, segment, and manipulate
speech sounds. Such facility with the
sounds of spoken language enables chil-
dren to learn more readily how to apply
these skills to decode print. An under-
standing of phonemes, the smallest
Holly B. Lane is an assistant professor in the Department of Special Education detectable unit of sound in spoken lan-
at the University of Florida, Gainesville. Paige C. Pullen is an assistant pro- guage, is essential to the understanding of
fessor in the Department of Curriculum, Instruction, and Special Education at grapheme-phoneme (letter-sound) rela-
the University of Virginia, Charlottesville. Mary R. Eisele is a freelance edu- tionships. Numerous studies have demon-
cation consultant in Naples, Florida. LuAnn Jordan is an assistant professor strated the importance of phonological
in the Department of Counseling and Special Education at the University of awareness, particularly at the phoneme
North Carolina-Charlotte. level, as the foundation for skilled decod-
Vol. 46, No. 3 PREVENTING SCHOOL FAILURE 101
ing and, therefore, for fluent reading 1995; National Reading Panel, 2000) have
(Blachman, Tangel, Ball, Black, & yielded several important generalizations:
McGraw, 1999; Cornwall, 1992; Lenchn-
1. Phonological awareness is directly
er, Gerber, & Routh, 1990; Liberman &
related to reading ability.
Shankweiler, 1985; Pratt & Brady, 1988;
2. Although the relationship is recipro-
Wagner & Torgesen, 1987).
cal, phonological awareness precedes
Phonological awareness tasks have
skilled decoding.
been shown to be excellent predictors of
3. Phonological awareness is a reliable
reading ability or reading disability. That
predictor of later reading ability.
is, children who perform well on tasks of
4. Deficits in phonological awareness
phonological awareness typically are or
are usually associated with deficits in
will become good readers, but children
reading.
who perform poorly on them are or will
5. Early language experiences play an
become poor readers (Blachman, 1991;
important role in the development of
Catts, 1991; Perfetti, 1991; Perfetti, Beck,
phonological awareness.
Bell, & Hughes, 1987; Snow et al., 1998;
6. Early intervention can promote the
Stanovich, 1986, 1992; Torgesen, Wagner,
development of phonological awareness.
& Rashotte, 1997). To benefit from
7. Improvements in phonological
instruction in decoding and spelling, a FIGURE 1. Phonological awareness can
awareness can and usually do result in be illustrated as an umbrella term that
child must have a fundamental level of
improvements in reading ability. comprises four levels. Phonemic
phonological awareness (Chard & Dick-
awareness is an understanding of the
son, 1999; National Reading Panel, 2000). Phonological Skills and Developmental sound structure of language at the
Many educators confuse the terminol- Levels phoneme level.
ogy related to this research. In particular,
the terms phonological awareness, Several skills that are commonly asso-
phonemic awareness, and phonics are ciated with beginning reading instruction
is typically achieved at a very young age.
sometimes used interchangeably. As pre- help children develop phonological
The linguistic play of young children,
awareness. Typically, the first phonologi-
viously stated, phonological awareness is including rhyming and the generation of
cal skill that children master is the ability
a child’s sensitivity to the sound structure nonsense words, provides evidence of
to rhyme. Very young children may also
of language. Phonemic awareness refers this early level of phonological awareness
master skills such as phoneme detection
to a child’s ability to manipulate individ- (Bradley, 1988). When a child utters a
and sound matching with little instruc-
ual sounds (phonemes) within words. single word that he has only heard in
tion. More advanced phonological skills
Phonics is an instructional approach used combination with other words, he is
such as phoneme deletion, blending, and
to help children make sense of the con- demonstrating the word level of phono-
segmentation pose problems for many
nection between sounds and letters. Each logical awareness.
emergent readers. Most instruction
is important to early reading instruction.
designed to increase phonological aware- Syllable level. Syllables are the most eas-
Phonological Awareness Research ness emphasizes these difficult skills ily distinguishable units within words.
(activities designed to enhance these Most children acquire the ability to seg-
Phonological awareness has gained
skills will be described in detail in a sub- ment words into syllables with minimal
considerable attention in educational
sequent section). instruction (Liberman, Shankweiler, &
research during the last 15 years. The pri-
The levels of phonological awareness Liberman, 1989; Lundberg, 1988).
mary attraction to this relatively new area
development are associated with the dif- Activities such as clapping, tapping, and
of reading research is the repeated positive
ferent phonological components of spo- marching are often used to develop sylla-
results in studies of phonological aware-
ken language, including words, syllables, ble awareness. This level of phonological
ness interventions. Among the numerous
onsets and rimes, and phonemes (Blach- awareness is useful for initial instruction
reliable predictors of later reading perfor-
man, 1991; Smith, 1995). In Figure 1, we in detection, segmentation, blending, and
mance (e.g., socioeconomic status, moth-
depict phonological awareness as an manipulation of phonological compo-
er’s education) that educational
umbrella term that includes awareness at nents of language. The ability to detect,
researchers have identified phonological
each level of spoken language. Effective segment, and count syllables is more
awareness is one of the few that educators
assessment and instruction should important to reading acquisition than the
are able to influence significantly.
address the various levels of phonological ability to manipulate and transpose them
Numerous studies of phonological
awareness development. In the following (Adams, 1990).
awareness have contributed to the knowl-
sections, we describe each of these four
edge base in this area. These studies can Onset and rime level. The onset-rime or
levels of phonological awareness.
and should inform future educational intrasyllabic level of phonological aware-
research and practice. Syntheses of this Word level. The awareness that the speech ness is an intermediate and instructionally
research (see, for example, Smith et al., flow is a compilation of individual words useful level of analysis between the sylla-

102 PREVENTING SCHOOL FAILURE Spring 2002


ble and the phoneme (Adams, 1990). The blended in such a way that one phoneme’s Yopp (1988) investigated some of the most
onset is the part of the syllable that pre- production is influenced by the surround- commonly used measures of phonological
cedes the vowel (e.g., the /k/ in cat, the /br/ ing phonemes. For example, the /k/ is awareness and determined that the relia-
in brown). The rime is the rest of the sylla- formed in slightly different ways in the bility and validity of measurement tasks
ble (e.g., the /og/ in dog, the /ack/ in black). words cat and cot due to the influence of were greatest when a combination of mea-
Because a syllable must contain a vowel, the vowel that follows it. Because phone- sures was employed.
all syllables must have a rime, but not all mic analysis requires the reader to detect,
syllables have an onset (e.g., and, out, or). segment, and manipulate individual Group Assessment
Treiman (1985) found that children phonemes, it is a much more sophisticat- Several methods have been developed
make more errors with consonants at the ed task and, consequently, a much more for group screening and assessment of
end of words or with consonant blends difficult task than either syllabic or intra- phonological skills. Tests of invented
than with initial or medial consonants. syllabic analysis (Treiman, 1991, 1992). spelling can be administered in a group
This finding suggests that children natu- setting, and the analysis of children’s
rally segment words at neither the sylla- Assessing Phonological Awareness attempts to spell words provides the
ble nor the phoneme level, but at the intra- Educators face the formidable challenge teacher with information about their abil-
syllabic level. In addition, most children of determining which children have weak- ity to segment phonemes, their knowl-
spell rimes more accurately than individ- nesses in phonological awareness and, edge of the alphabet, and their under-
ual vowel sounds, which illustrates the therefore, which children are likely to standing of letter-sound correspondences
level at which they are attending. Onset- develop reading problems. Several ways to (Invernizzi, Meier, Swank, & Juel, 1998;
rime segmentation skill is an essential assess phonological awareness have been Moats, 2000; Snow et al., 1998). Mann,
component of phonological awareness developed. Which method to use should be Tobin, and Wilson (1987) have advocated
(Adams, 1990; Goswami & Mead, 1992). determined based on the number of chil- the use of children’s invented spellings as
Instruction at the onset-rime level is an dren to be assessed, the amount of existing a tool for screening phonological aware-
important step for many childen information about the children, and the ness. They developed a system for scor-
(Treiman,1985, 1991, 1992). Because amount of time available. The most reli- ing an invented spelling that can help
tasks that require onset and rime analysis able and informative method of assessing determine if additional assessment is war-
require the segmentation of syllables, they phonological awareness is through, indi- ranted. In this and other similar scoring
are more sophisticated than syllable-level vidual testing. Other methods have been methods, points are awarded on the basis
tasks. Yet these same tasks are easier than developed, however, that are quick and of a spelling’s phonological accuracy. For
phoneme-level tasks because they do not easy to administer and that have reliability example, in an attempt to spell an unfa-
require discrimination between individual adequate for most classroom purposes. miliar word, a child may produce a scrib-
phonemes. Onset-rime tasks could, there-
fore, be considered an intermediate step in
the development of phonological aware-
ness. The difficulty that many children
experience when progressing from syllab-
ic analysis to phonemic analysis may arise
because the intermediate step, the intra-
syllabic unit, is often omitted from early
reading instruction. Providing experience
working with onsets and rimes may alle-
viate this difficulty.
Phoneme level. The most sophisticated
level of phonological awareness is the
phoneme level, most commonly referred to
as phonemic awareness. Children with
strong phonemic awareness are able to
manipulate individual phonemes, the
smallest sound units of spoken language.
Phonemic awareness skills include the abil-
ity to detect, segment, and blend phonemes
and to manipulate their position in words
(Adams, 1990; Lenchner et al., 1990).
Because humans coarticulate or overlap
sounds in speech, phonemes are impossi- FIGURE 2 These example spellings for the word pretend begin with a series of scrib-
ble to segment in a pure sense. In the bles and move to a spelling that is phonologically accurate.
speech flow, phonemes are formed and
Vol. 46, No. 3 PREVENTING SCHOOL FAILURE 103
may be individualized, small-group, or
TABLE 1
Method for Scoring Invented Spelling
whole-class. For students who have signif-
icant weaknesses in phonological aware-
ness, 10–20 minutes of individual or
Characteristics of spelling Example spellings Score
small-group instruction each day may be
necessary to promote adequate growth. Of
One phoneme accurately represented other than t 1/2
initial phoneme course, student needs should dictate the
form and amount of instruction provided.
Initial phoneme accurately represented f 1
fesry Commerically Available Instructional
Initial phoneme and one other phoneme accurately ft 2 Programs
represented
As educators have become aware of the
Initial phoneme and two or more other phonemes fot 3
accurately represented flt
research on phonological awareness, pub-
lishers have recognized the demand for
Word is phonologically accurate flot 4 classroom materials. Numerous commer-
flote
cial programs are currently available.
Several of these programs have under-
gone field testing and evaluation. We pro-
ble, a random letter string, one or two cor- tion and production, sound matching, vide a list and brief description of
rectly represented phonemes, or even a phonemic oddity detection, deletion, research-based materials for teachers in
phonologically accurate spelling (Figure segmentation, and blending. Sample Table 4. In addition to these classroom
2). Each of these attempts represents sig- questions for informal assessment of a materials, several programs for computer-
nificant information about that child’s child’s phonological awareness appear assisted instruction have become avail-
phonological knowledge. See Table 1 for in Table 2. Several formal measures of able. A listing of these software programs
a summary of one method for scoring phonological awareness have been is provided in Table 5.
invented spellings. developed, as well. Descriptions of
The Screening Test of Phonological some of the most widely used appear in Informal Methods of Instruction
Awareness (STOPA) has been demon- Table 3. Most activities in commercial pro-
strated to be an effective group assess- grams such as those described can be
ment tool (Torgesen, Wagner, Bryant, & Developing Phonological Awareness
incorporated informally within existing
Pearson, 1992). The STOPA, designed for Just as assessing phonological aware- reading instruction. The following tasks
use with kindergarten students, requires ness is best accomplished by observing are useful for practicing and developing
students to identify sounds that are the students as they perform tasks that phonological skills. The tasks are
same and different. The test is simple to demonstrate phonological skills, devel- sequenced in an order that approximates
administer in a group setting, yet sophis- oping phonological awareness requires the developmental sequence. However,
ticated enough to detect individual differ- practicing phonological skills. Most skill the sequence and rate of skill develop-
ences in phonological awareness. instruction can be easily embedded ment vary from child to child, and skills
within the context of meaningful reading overlap during development. We also
Individualized Assessment or writing (Wadlington, 2000; Yopp, wish to stress that these activities are
To collect the kind of information 1992). Some children, however, require auditory and interactive in nature; chil-
about a child’s phonological knowledge more extensive practice with skills. Stu- dren do not develop phonological skills
that is necessary to design effective dents who have very low levels of read- by doing independent written work.
instructional interventions, individual ing ability benefit most from explicit
assessments are particularly useful. The instruction in phonological skills paired Tapping words. Children can be taught to
assessment method can be informal, cri- with explicit instruction in how to apply tap with a rhythm stick or finger for each
terion-referenced, or norm-referenced. those skills in a meaningful context word in a sentence or phrase. Most chil-
Clearly, the most direct informal (Cunningham, 1990; Lane, 1994). As dren acquire this skill with minimal
method of measuring a child’s phonolog- these children develop sensitivity to the instruction. Teacher modeling and guid-
ical awareness is repeated observation by sound structure of language, instruction ance are useful for those children who
a knowledgeable teacher of the child’s in phoneme segmentation and blending have difficulty with this task. Children
ability to perform tasks that require the should be coupled with instruction using who struggle with this activity typically
use of phonological skills. Such observa- letters (Pullen, 2000). confuse words and syllables. Care should
tion provides a teacher with an authentic Instruction in phonological skills can be taken to make this distinction explicit.
measure of what a child can or cannot do be conducted as formal, structured Counting and tallying words. Tallying the
(Valencia, 1997). A variety of skill areas lessons, as an integrated part of ongoing number of words in a sentence requires a
may be examined to measure phonologi- reading instruction, or as fun activities greater degree of cognitive engagement
cal awareness; these include rhyme detec- throughout the school day. Instruction than tapping words and is considered a
104 PREVENTING SCHOOL FAILURE Spring 2002
more sophisticated task. Another method ual syllables can begin in kindergarten. explicit instruction about what a rhyme is
of word counting involves moving a This process can be made into a game in (i.e., words rhyme when they sound the
marker to indicate the number of words in which children separate their names or the same in the middle and at the end). This
a sentence. names of familiar objects into syllables. instruction should be accompanied by
Instruction may begin with segmentation numerous examples and nonexamples of
Tapping syllables. Children may be of compound words (e.g., football, out- rhymes. Rhyme recognition simply
taught to tap out the number of syllables side, sidewalk). Children may also be requires the student to indicate whether or
in a word. This task requires auditory taught to count the number of syllables in not a pair of spoken words rhymes.
attention. The teacher should provide other long but familiar words. These tasks Instead of simply providing a pair of
extensive modeling and guided practice require auditory attention and memory. words, to promote rhyme recognition, the
to help children understand the concept of teacher might say, “Cat and sat both have
syllable. Starting with long but familiar Rhyme recognition. Children can be
an at. Does hat have an at?”
multisyllable words (e.g., refrigerator, taught to determine if two one-syllable
motorcycle) makes the acquisition of this words rhyme. Some children have an Rhyme generation. Generating a word or
skill easy for most children. inherent understanding of rhymes based list of words that rhyme with a given word
on extensive experiences with language is more difficult than determining if two
Segmenting syllables. Teaching students to and print. Other children who have not given words rhyme. The additional cog-
segment multisyllable words into individ- developed this understanding may need nitive and language requirements of

TABLE 2
Sample Assessment Questions

Assessment Directions Sample

Word level

Tapping words Teacher reads sentence aloud. Child taps for each word The little frog is jumping.
in the sentence.
Deleting words Teacher reads a compound word, the child deletes one word. Teacher says, “Say cowboy.” Child
repeats. “Now say cowboy, with-
out saying cow.”
Syllable level

Blending syllables Teacher reads word one syllable at a time. The child listens, What word do these sounds make?
then blends the sounds together to make the whole word. tea-cher.

Tapping syllables Teacher reads word aloud. Child taps for each syllable in the alligator
word.
Deleting syllables Teacher reads child a multisyllable word and the child deletes Teacher says, “Say wonder.” Child
a specific syllable. repeats. “Now say wonder with-
out saying der.
Onset-rime level

Matching rhymes Teacher gives child a word pair, the child decides whether Do these two words rhyme?
or not the pair rhymes. sack/black
Do these two words rhyme?
beat/bean
Blending onsets and rimes The teacher segments the word orally between the onset and What word do these sounds make?
rime. The child listens, then blends the sounds together n-ote.
to make the whole word.
Generating rhymes The teacher gives child a target word and the child must Tell me a word that rhymes with sat.
provide a word that rhymes with the target word.

Phoneme level

Blending phonemes Teacher segments a word into phonemes and the child is What word do these sounds make?
asked to blend the sounds to make the whole word. b-o-th.
Segmenting phonemes Teacher reads child a whole word and the child is asked to I will say a word, then you say it
produce the word sound-by-sound. sound by sound. mat.

Vol. 46, No. 3 PREVENTING SCHOOL FAILURE 105


rhyme generation make it quite challeng- begin or end with that sound. This activi- from a given word. For example, the
ing for some children. The ability to gen- ty can be used during story or passage teacher may ask, “Which of these words
erate rhymes, however, is an excellent reading, as well. While reading connect- does not have the same ending sound as
indicator of a child’s ability to apply ed text, students find all words in the cat: mutt, lift, cake, or bite?” Again, work
phonological knowledge. Many children selection that include the target phoneme. with medial sounds should start when stu-
engage in spontaneous word games that As students become comfortable with dents become skilled with beginning and
employ rhyming skills. This fun way to beginning and ending sounds, activities ending sounds.
practice skills should be encouraged. that include detection of a target medial
Rhyme and sound matching using pic-
Using nonsense words in such games sound should be added.
tures. For these activities, children are
reinforces the child’s attention to sounds.
Sound matching. To match sounds, stu- asked to look at pictures and generate the
Rhyme oddity detection. This task dents must determine which in a selection sounds themselves by naming the word
requires children to indicate which in a of words begins or ends with the same the picture represents. Students match
list of three or four words does not rhyme sound as a given word. For example, the pictures illustrating words that share a
with the other words in the list. The famil- teacher may ask students, “Which word common rime or a common initial, medi-
iar song from the television show Sesame begins with the same sound as dog: can, al, or final phoneme. This activity is
Street does this well: “Which of these must, or dish? somewhat more advanced than activities
words is not like the others? Which of As students master this skill, the activ- that begin with the teacher generating the
these words just doesn’t belong?” ity should be modified to request ending sounds, because some students find it
Rhyme matching. Given a list of three or or middle sound-matching, and more more difficult to detect individual
four words, students indicate which one words may be added to the list. phonemes when they do not hear some-
from the list rhymes with a target word. one else say the word.
Sound oddity detection. The procedures
For example, the teacher might say, for this activity are very similar to those Oddity detection using pictures. As in
“Which word rhymes with stamp: map, for rhyme oddity detection. The differ- the previous activity, the student is
lip, or lamp?” ence, however, is that students are asked expected to generate the name of the
Sound detection. Given a target phoneme, to determine which in a list of words word the pictures represents. The student
students determine which words on a list begins or ends with a sound different then determines which in a set of pic-

TABLE 3
Assessments of Phonological Awareness

Assessment Author/publisher Description

Lindamood Auditory Lindamood and Lindamood/ The LAC is a comprehensive, individually administered
Conceptualization Test Pro-Ed assessment for both children and adults. It is effective for a
(LAC) wide range of ages, however, it is difficult for very young
children (kindergarten).

Comprehensive Test of Wagner, Toregesen and The CTOPP is an individually-administered assessment that
Phonological Processing Rashotte/Pro-Ed measures (a) phonological awareness (e.g., sound matching,
(CTOPP) blending, elision); (b) phonological memory (memory of dig-
its, nonword repetition); and (c) rapid serial naming (rapid
naming of objects, numbers, and letters).

Test of Phonological Toregesen and Bryant/ TOPA is a measure of young children’s ability to isolate indi-
Awareness (TOPA) Pro-Ed vidual phonemes in spoken words. It can be administered to
groups of children and is available in Kindergarten and Early
Elementary versions.

Phonological Awareness Invernizzi, Meier, Swank PALS measures the child’s rhyming abilities and sound aware-
Literacy Screening and Juel/University of ness. In addition to these phonological skills, alphabet knowl-
(PALS) Virginia edge, letter sound knowledge, concept of word, and word
recognition are also assessed.

The Developmental Spelling Ganske/Guilford Press This assessment includes a screening inventory for determining
Analysis in Word Journey a child’s stage of spelling development and two parallel feature
inventories for high lighting strengths and weaknesses in a
child’s knowledge of specific spelling features.

106 PREVENTING SCHOOL FAILURE Spring 2002


tures illustrating words does not share a the skill orally. The teacher demonstrates Blending and segmenting. Blending and
common rime or a common initial, using fingers to count phonemes, raising segmenting phonemes are the most sophis-
medial, or final phoneme. one finger as each phoneme is pro- ticated skills associated with phonological
Counting phonemes. Elkonin (1963) nounced. With teacher guidance, the stu- awareness and the most important for
introduced a method of developing dent should be able to learn how to count application to decoding. Blending and seg-
phonemic segmentation skills that has phonemes independently. menting may be taught in a variety of
become quite popular in recent years. Phoneme deletion. Phoneme deletion ways. One of the most useful methods for
This method involves the use of Elkonin activities require students to detect and helping young children to understand the
boxes—picture cards with boxes under manipulate sounds in a word. Students concepts of phonemic blending and seg-
each picture representing the number of are asked to delete a specified sound from mentation is teaching them to “converse”
phonemes in the word (see Figure 3). a target word. For example, the teacher with a puppet or toy robot in a secret lan-
While saying the word slowly, sound by may say, “Say seat. Now say seat without guage. Torgesen and Bryant (1994) used
sound, the student moves a marker into saying the /t/ sound.” Again, practice with this approach in Phonological Awareness
each box to represent each sound in the this activity should begin with initial Training for Reading, but the method is
word. This activity may be modified to sounds and progress to final and, eventu- easy to adapt to informal instruction. In
allow the teacher and student to practice ally, medial sounds. this approach, the puppet or robot can only

TABLE 4
Commercially Available Instructional Programs

Program Author/publisher Description

Lindamood Phoneme Lindamood/ProEd LiPS is designed to provide intensive remedial instruction for
Sequencing Program students struggling to develop phonological awareness.
for Reading, Spelling, Sounds are represented with objects and descriptive names to
and Speech (LiPS) develop students’ concrete understanding of distinct sounds
by drawing a child’s attention to the motoric features of
phoneme articulation through individual or small group
instruction. Students learn how to position their mouths to
produce sounds and how to distinguish among various types
of sounds.

Road to the Code: A Program Blachman, Ball, Black, and Road to the Code includes activities to move students from
of Early Literacy Activities Tangel/Brookes phonological awareness to letter knowledge. The program
to Develop Phonological gradually moves into activities that encourage the application
Awareness of these skills in writing and spelling.

Phonological Awareness Torgesen and Bryant/ProEd This program includes games and activities to help children
Training for Reading develop sound segmenting and blending, reading and
spelling. An audiotape guide for sound pronunciation is
included.

Phonemic Awareness in Adams, Foorman, Lundberg, This curriculum provides a basis for assessment and instruc-
Young Children and Beeler/Brookes tion in phonological awareness. The book includes a variety
of language games, listening games, rhyming activities, and
activities for developing students’ understanding of sounds in
words. Several instruments for assessing phonological aware-
ness are also included.

Ladders to Literacy O’Connor, Notari-Syverson, These preschool and kindergarten activity books provide
and Vadasy/Brookes early literacy activities in phonological awareness, vocabulary
development, and letter names and sounds. The program is
designed to emphasize the provision of appropriate levels of
instructional support for developing students.

Sounds Abound Catts and Vartiainen/ Sounds Abound is for children in grades PreK–3. It targets
LinguiSystems listening and rhyming skills as students work toward match-
ing sounds with letters. Five sections include fun activities in
(1) speech sound awareness, (2) rhyming, (3) beginning and
ending sounds, (4) segmenting and blending sounds, and (5)
putting sounds together with words.

Vol. 46, No. 3 PREVENTING SCHOOL FAILURE 107


say words and can only understand words should be very careful to model correct this strategy may help them blend stop
when they are said one sound at a time. letter sound pronunciation. Many teach- consonants more readily.
Young children seem to accept and under- ers, in an effort to make short or “stop” Another way to alleviate this problem
stand this explanation quite readily and are consonant sounds more audible, add a is to begin instruction in blending and
eager to try communicating in this unusu- vowel sound to the consonant. This addi- segmenting using only words with con-
al fashion. tional sound, usually a schwa or short u tinuous consonant sounds (e.g., /s/, /v/,
When teaching or assessing blending sound, distorts the consonant sound, mak- /z/, /f/, /m/, /n/) at the beginning posi-
and segmentation skills, the teacher ing it very difficult to blend with other tion. These sounds are much easier to
should be careful to completely segment phonemes. For example, a b may be blend than the stop consonants, and their
phonemes before blending them. Many incorrectly pronounced “buh,” and a t use in early instruction makes the skill of
teachers have the tendency simply to say may be incorrectly pronounced “tuh.” blending more accessible for children.
a word slowly, drawing out the Blending the letters b, a, and t then pro- As children are introduced to blending
phonemes. The teacher should be certain duces “buh-a-tuh,” and many children skills, stop consonants may be used at
to include a brief but discernible pause have serious difficulty identifying the the end of words. In the final position,
between segmented sounds. When chil- word. It is important to pronounce these stop consonants are easier to pronounce
dren learn to decode, it is necessary for stop consonants as quickly as possible, quickly and with little distortion. When
them to identify the sounds of separate without the confusing “uh.” Because it is students become competent with pro-
letters and then to blend those letter impossible to pronounce a voiced stop nunciation, the introduction of stops at
sounds together. Previous oral blending consonant such as b or d in isolation with the initial position becomes less trouble-
practice is helpful for students when they no vowel sound attached, the teacher some.
are ready to become more fluent with should model saying the sound with an
Including Phonological Awareness
decoding skills. extremely brief short i sound following it.
Instruction in an Existing Reading
Another important caution for teachers The place in the vocal anatomy where the
Program
is that individual phonemes must be pro- short i is produced is closer to the loca-
nounced in a manner that will make them tion of more of the other vowel sounds Many of the activities we have
blendable. In other words, teachers than the short u. Teaching children to use described can be integrated into any read-

TABLE 5
Instructional Software Programs

Program Author/publisher Description

Earobics Wasowicz/Cognitive Concepts Earobics is a software program designed to develop phonemic


awareness and letter-sound knowledge through engaging
games.

Daisy Quest and Daisy’s Erikson, Foster, Foster, and Daisy Quest and Daisy’s Castle are computer programs for the
Castle Torgesen/ProEd Macintosh. The games are motivating for children while pro-
viding opportunities to develop phonological awareness at each
level. Research studies have found these software programs to
be an effective way to stimulate phonological awareness in
young children.

The Waterford Early The Waterford Institute/ The Waterford Early Reading Program is a comprehensive
Reading Program Electronic Education computer program for Kindergarten. It provides activities in
phonological awareness, concepts of print, letter names, and
letter sounds.

Read, Write, and Type! Herron and Grimm/ This computer software program merges the teaching of phon-
Learning System Talking Fingers ics skills with an introduction to typing. Children move
through a progressively challenging sequence games that
begins with single letters, advances to whole words, and con-
cludes with complete sentences, including capitalization and
punctuation.

Fast ForWord Merzenich, Tallal, Jenkins, Fast ForWord is an interactive program that stimulates child-
and Miller/Scientific ren’s phonological skills using acoustically modified speech
Learning sounds. This acoustic training is provided in five 20-minute
sessions each day. Requires teacher training.

108 PREVENTING SCHOOL FAILURE Spring 2002


at the end”). Teachers can challenge stu- REFERENCES
dents to think of words that have a partic- Adams, M. J. (1990). Beginning to read: Thinking
ular number of syllables or phonemes. and learning about print. Cambridge: MIT.
Adams, M. J., Foorman, B. R., Lundberg, I., & Beel-
Finding many fun and innovative ways to er, T. (1998). Phonemic awareness in young chil-
include such sound play in the school day dren: A classroom curriculum. Baltimore, MD:
will address the instructional needs of Brookes.
Anderson, R. C., Hiebert, E. H., Scott, J. A., &
many students. Additional practice in Wilkinson, I. A. (1985). Becoming a nation of
specific skills will certainly be required readers: The report of the commission on read-
for some students who have difficulty ing. Washington, DC: National Institute of Edu-
cation.
acquiring phonological skills, but such Blachman, B. A. (1991). Early interventions for
informal opportunities to practice children’s reading problems: Clinical applica-
throughout the day will help these stu- tions of the research in phonological awareness.
Topics in Language Disorders, 12(1), 51–65.
dents, as well. Blachman, B. A., Ball, E. W., Black, R., & Tangel,
Combining informal sound play and D. M. (2000). Road to the code: A phonological
formal phonological awareness instruc- awareness program for young children. Balti-
more, MD: Brookes.
tion during typical reading and writing Blachman, B. A., Tangel, D. M., Ball, E. W., Black,
activities for all students with explicit R., & McGraw, C. K. (1999). Developing phono-
skill instruction for students who need logical awareness and word recognition skills: A
two-year intervention with low-income, inner-
additional practice should address the city children. Reading and Writing: An Interdis-
diverse needs in most elementary class- ciplinary Journal, 11, 239–273.
FIGURE 3. The teacher models Bradley, L. (1988). Rhyme recognition and reading
moving one chip into a square for
rooms. The most important thing for
and spelling in young children. In R. L. Masland
each sound in fish. The student then teachers to do is to make the sound struc- & M. W. Masland (Eds.), Preschool prevention of
moves the chips into the squares with ture of language conspicuous to students reading failure (pp. 143-162). Parkton, MD:
help from the teacher, until the stu- who do not develop phonological aware- York.
dent is able to move the chips into the Catts, H. W. (1991). Early identification of reading
ness independently. disabilities. Topics in Language Disorders, 12(1),
squares independently. Eventually,
the student learns to write letters in
The activities presented here are 1–16.
designed to develop phonological aware- Chard, D. J., & Dickson, S. V. (1999). Phonological
the boxes. awareness: Instructional and assessment guide-
ness. Applying these auditory skills to lines. Intervention in School and Clinic, 34, 261-
reading requires students to have a work- 270.
ing knowledge of sound-symbol relation- Cornwall, A. (1992). The relationship of phonolog-
ical awareness, rapid naming, and verbal memo-
ing activity or used as games during ships, which is typically acquired through ry to severe reading and spelling disability. Jour-
instruction or during noninstructional phonics instruction. Despite popular mis- nal of Learning Disabilities, 25, 532–538.
time. For example, singing and play conceptions, phonological awareness Cunningham, A. E. (1990). Explicit versus implicit
instruction in phonemic awareness. Journal of
activities offer many opportunities for instruction is not the same as phonics Experimental Child Psychology, 50, 429–444.
kindergarten teachers to incorporate instruction; instead, phonological aware- Elkonin, D. B. (1963). The psychology of mastering
phonological skill development. Stories ness instruction prepares students to be the elements of reading. In B. Simon & J. Simon
(Eds.), Educational psychology in the U.S.S.R. (pp.
or poems that include rhyming words able to benefit from instruction in phonics. 165–179). Stanford, CA: Stanford University.
may be used as a tool to introduce and Erikson, G. C., Foster, K. C., Foster, D. F. & Torge-
develop concepts and skills in rhyming. A Conclusion sen, J. K. (1992). Daisy Quest. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.
Erikson, G. C., Foster, K. C., Foster, D. F. & Torge-
teacher of older students could ask them Reading research has clearly demon- sen, J. K. (1993). Daisy’s Castle. Austin, TX:
to count the number of syllables or strated the significance of phonological Pro-Ed.
phonemes in the names of story charac- awareness in the development of early Ganske, K. (2000). Word journeys: Assessment-
guided phonics, spelling, and vocabulary instruc-
ters or in new vocabulary words. If a reading skills, and a variety of effective tion. New York: Guildford.
teacher encourages the use of carefully methods for assessment and instruction of Goswami, U., & Mead, F. (1992). Onset and rime
invented spellings, students learn to seg- phonological skills has been developed. awareness and analogies in reading. Reading
ment words and represent the correct Research Quarterly, 27, 152–162.
Teachers in remedial and special educa- Herron, J., & Grimm, L. (2001). Read, Write, &
number of phonemes. Modeling how to tion programs now have another tool for Type! Learning System. San Raphael, CA: Talk-
sound out a word to invent a spelling can addressing students’ reading problems. ing Fingers.
help students develop these skills. As stu- Invernizzi, M., Meier, J. D., Swank, L., & Juel, C.
Teachers of young children must recog- (1998). Phonological awareness literacy screen-
dents learn about decoding, numerous nize the importance of incorporating ing. Charlottesville, VA: University of Virginia.
other opportunities for instruction in phonological awareness into programs Lane, H. B. (1994). The effects of explicit instruc-
blending and segmentation arise. designed to promote emergent literacy, tion in contextual application of phonological
awareness on the reading skills of first-grade stu-
The teacher could make a simple because these teachers now have a tool dents. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. Univer-
whole-class game out of rhyme or for preventing reading problems. sity of Florida, Gainesville.
phoneme matching (e.g., “Line up for Lenchner, O., Gerber, M. M., & Routh, D. K.
Key words: phonological awareness, phone- (1990). Phonological awareness tasks as predic-
lunch if your name rhymes with mic awareness, phonics, beginning reading, tors of decoding ability: Beyond segmentation.
___________,” or “if your name has a /t/ early intervention Journal of Learning Disabilities, 23, 240-247.

Vol. 46, No. 3 PREVENTING SCHOOL FAILURE 109


Liberman, I. Y., Shankweiler, D., & Liberman, A. M. read: Basic research and its implications (pp. diation of phonologically based reading disabili-
(1989). The alphabetic principle and learning to 33–44). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Asso- ties. In B. Blachman (Ed.), Foundations of read-
read. In D. Shankweiler & I. Y. Liberman (Eds.), ciates. ing acquisition and dyslexia: Implications for
Phonology and reading disability: Solving the Perfetti, C. A., Beck, I., Bell, L. C., & Hughes, C. early intervention (pp. 287-304). Mahwah, NJ:
reading puzzle (pp. 1-33). Ann Arbor: The Uni- (1987). Phonemic knowledge and learning to read Erlbaum.
versity of Michigan Press. are reciprocal: A longitudinal study of first grade Torgesen, J. K., Wagner, R. K., Bryant, B. R., &
Liberman, I., & Shankweiler, D. (1985). Phonology children. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 33, 283–319. Pearson, N. (1992). Toward development of a
and the problems of learning to read and write. Pratt, A. C., & Brady, S. (1988). Relation of phono- kindergarten group test for phonological aware-
Remedial and Special Education, 6, 8–17. logical awareness to reading disability in children ness. Journal of Research and Development in
Lindamood, C., & Lindamood, P. (1972). Lin- and adults. Journal of Educational Psychology, Education, 25, 113–120.
damood auditory conceptualization test. Austin, 80, 319–323. Treiman, R. (1985). Onsets and rimes as units of
TX: Pro-Ed. Pullen, P. C. (2000). The effects of alphabetic word spoken syllables: Evidence from children. Jour-
Lindamood, C., & Lindamood, P. (1998). Lin- work with manipulative letters on the reading nal of Experimental Child Psychology, 39,
damood phoneme sequencing program for read- acquisition of struggling first-grade students. 161–181.
ing, spelling, and speech. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. University of Treiman, R. (1991). The role of intrasyllabic units
Lundberg, I. (1988). Preschool prevention of read- Florida, Gainesville. in learning to read. In L. Rieben & C. A. Perfetti
ing failure: Does training in phonological aware- Smith, C. R. (1995, March). Assessment and reme- (Eds.), Learning to read: Basic research and its
ness work? In R. L. Masland & M. W. Masland diation of phonological segmentation difficulties. implications (pp. 149–160). Hillsdale, NJ:
(Eds.), Preschool prevention of reading failure Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Lawrence Erlbaum.
(pp. 163–176). Parkton, MD: York. Learning Disabilities Association of America, Treiman, R. (1992). The role of intrasyllabic units
Lyon, G. R. (1999). Education research: Is what we Orlando, FL. in learning to read and spell. In P. B. Gough, L.
don’t know hurting our children? Statement of the Smith, S. B., Simmons, D. C., & Kame’enui, E. J. C. Ehri, & R. Treiman (Eds.), Reading acquisi-
Chief of the Child Development and Behavior (1995). Synthesis of research on phonological tion (pp. 65–106). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erl-
Branch of the National Institute of Child Health awareness: Principles and implications for read- baum.
and Human Development, National Institutes of ing acquisition. Technical Report No. 21, Nation- Valencia, S. W. (1997). Authentic classroom assess-
Health, to the House Science Committee. Wash- al Center to Improve the Tools of Educators, Uni- ment of early reading: Alternatives to standard-
ington, DC. versity of Oregon. ized tests. Preventing School Failure, 41, 63–70.
Mann, V. A., Tobin, P., & Wilson, R. (1987). Mea- Snow, C. E., Burns, M. S., & Griffin, P. (Eds.). Wadlington, E. (2000). Effective language arts
suring phonological awareness through the (1998). Preventing reading difficulties in young instruction for students with dyslexia. Preventing
invented spellings of kindergarten children. Mer- children. Washington, D C: National Academy. School Failure, 44, 61-65.
rill-Palmer Quarterly, 33, 365–391. Stanovich, K. E. (1986). Matthew effects in reading: Wagner, R. K., & Torgesen, J. K. (1987). The nature
Merzenich, M., Tallal, P., Jenkins, W., & Miller, S. Some consequences of individual differences in of phonological processing and its causal role in
(1996). Fast ForWord. Oakland, CA: Scientific the acquisition of literacy. Reading Research the acquisition of reading skills. Psychological
Learning. Quarterly, 21, 360–406. Bulletin, 101, 192–212.
Moats, L. (2000). Speech to print: Language essen- Stanovich, K. E. (1987). Introduction to special Wagner, R., Torgesen, J., & Rashotte, C. (1999).
tials for teachers. Baltimore, MD: Brookes. issue. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 33. Comprehensive test of phonological processing
National Reading Panel. (2000). Teaching children Stanovich, K. E. (1992). Speculations on the causes (CTOPP). Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.
to read: An evidence-based assessment of the sci- and consequences of individual differences in Wasowicz, J. (1997). Earobics 1. Evanston, IL:
entific research literature on reading and its early reading acquisition. In P. B. Gough, L. C. Cognitive Concepts.
implications for reading instruction. Washington, Ehri, & R. Treiman (Eds.), Reading acquisition Wasowicz, J. (1999). Earobics 2. Evanston, IL:
DC: National Institute of Child Health and (pp. 307–342). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Cognitive Concepts.
Human Development, National Institutes of Torgesen, J. K. (1998). Catch them before they fall: Waterford Institute. (1993). Waterford early learn-
Health. Identification and assessment to prevent reading ing program. Sunnyvale, CA: Electronic Educa-
Notari-Syverson, A., O’Connor, R. E., & Vadasy, P. failure in young children. American Educator, 22, tion.
F. (1998). Ladders to Literacy: A Preschool Activ- 32–39. Yopp, H. K. (1988). The validity and reliability of
ity Book. Baltimore, MD: Brookes. Torgesen, J. K., & Bryant, B. R. (1994). Phonolog- phonemic awareness tests. Reading Research
O’Connor, R. E., Notari-Syverson, A., & Vadasy, P. ical awareness training for reading. Austin, TX: Quarterly, 23, 159–177.
F. (1998). Ladders to Literacy: A Kindergarten Pro-ed. Yopp, H. K. (1992). Developing phonemic aware-
Activity Book. Baltimore, MD: Brookes. Torgesen, J. K., & Bryant, B. R. (1997). Test of ness in young children. The Reading Teacher, 45,
Perfetti, C. A. (1991). Representations and aware- phonological awareness. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed. 696–703.
ness in the acquisition of reading competence. In Torgesen, J. K., Wagner, R. K., & Rashotte, C. A.
L. Rieben & C. A. Perfetti (Eds.), Learning to (1997). Approaches to the prevention and reme-

110 PREVENTING SCHOOL FAILURE Spring 2002


View publication stats

You might also like