You are on page 1of 10

Self-Recognition in Primates

A Comparative Approach to the


Bidirectional Properties of Consciousness

GORDON G. GALLUP, JR. State University of New York at Albany

ABSTRACT: A technique is described in which organ- tional properties of consciousness translate into
isms are provided with extended exposure to mirrors consciousness and self-consciousness.
and then given an explicit test of self-recognition (ac- To be able to think about oneself presupposes a
complished through the unobtrusive application of sense of identity, and for some time man has been
marks to facial features visually inaccessible without a held unique in his capacity to form a self-concept
mirror). Use of this procedure with chimpanzees and
(e.g., Ardrey, 1961; Black, 1968; Buss, 1973; Kin-
orangutans turns up striking evidence of self-recogni-
get, 197S; Lorenz, 1971). By being able to con-
tion, with patterns of self-directed behavior emerging
after only 2 or 3 days. In support of the widely held template his own existence, man is in the seemingly
view that the self-concept may develop out of social unique and certainly precarious position of being
interaction with others, the capacity for self-recognition able, at least in principle, to take steps to modify
in chimpanzees appears to be influenced by early social that existence. In fact, one widely respected evolu-
experience. To date, however, attempts to demonstrate tionary biologist (Slobodkin, in press) sees the de-
self-recognition in all other species except man have velopment of self-awareness as having emancipated
failed. The phyletic limits of this capacity may have man from some of the otherwise deterministic and
important implications for claims concerning the evolu- unrelenting forces of evolution.
tionary continuity of mental experience. The history of science, however, can be viewed
in part as having brought about gradual changes in
man's conception of man, and with such changes
Consciousness has always been an elusive topic in man may eventually have to relinquish, or at least
psychology. As a working hypothesis, however, it temper, his claim to special status (e.g., Gallup,
seems reasonable to suppose that there can be at Boren, Gagliardi, & Wallnau, in press). Primate
least two dimensions to conscious experience. The research poses one of the greatest contemporary
basic distinction is between having an experience threats to traditional notions about man.
and being aware of having an experience. In this
sense, human consciousness is typically bidirec-
tional. In effect, most people can direct their at- Men and Great Apes
tention outward or inward. Not only can I be
In a recent article based on a comprehensive review
consciously aware of events in the world around
of biochemical and immunological evidence, King
me, but I can become the object of my own atten-
and Wilson (197S) conclude that the "average
tion. I can contemplate my own death. My brain
human polypeptide is more than 99 percent identi-
can think about my brain and even speculate about
cal to its chimpanzee counterpart" and that the
the mechanisms of its own functioning. This re-
genetic distance between humans and chimpanzees
flective dimension of consciousness is isomorphic
is so small that we are at least as similar to them
with self-awareness. In other words, the bidirec-
as sibling species of other animals are to one an-
other. King and Wilson note that the major ana-
The author would like to thank R. E. Hicks, J. M. Suls, tomical and behavioral differences between chim-
and L. Tornatore for their comments on an earlier draft of panzees and man seem paradoxical in light of these
this paper. data. While anatomical differences obviously do
Requests for reprints should be sent to G. G. Gallup, Jr.,
Department of Psychology, State University of New York exist, growing evidence casts doubt on man's be-
at Albany, Albany, New York 12222. havioral uniqueness. In fact, after reviewing the

AMERICAN PSYCHOLOGIST • MAY 1977 • 329


psychological literature on primates, Mason (1976) (1973) calls "the apparent evolutionary indepen-
concludes that "the essential terms of our unique- dence of representational ability and verbal lan-
ness have yet to be denned." * guage." There is increasing evidence that thinking-
Many cherished notions of human uniqueness like processes in the great apes antedate language.
have become increasingly suspect, especially as a Recent examples include the work of Davenport
consequence of what we now know about the men- and Rogers (1970) with chimpanzees and orangu-
tality of the great apes, which include chimpanzees, tans on cross-modal perception (or the ability to
orangutans, and gorillas. For example, contrary integrate and equate sensory input across several
to preconceived anthropological notions about man modalities) and the ingenious work of Menzel
as a tool user or even tool maker, it is now known (1975) on the possible existence of a natural lan-
that chimpanzees living in the wild use and fabri- guage among chimpanzees.
cate tools in ingenious and strikingly hominoid One of the last substantive holdouts for human
ways (see Beck, 197S). Also, unlike many other uniqueness is self-awareness. In fact, there is a
primates, not only do chimpanzees eat meat, they deep-seated feeling among some psychologists that
are more than mere opportunistic predators. consciousness and self-awareness are simply not
Teleki's (1973) work shows that male chimpanzees amenable to objective study in other organisms.
engage in cooperative hunting, and meat is one of According to Kluver (1933), the content of ex-
the few foods chimpanzees are willing to share. perience or of consciousness itself is not a reason-
Male-oriented, cooperative hunting patterns that able object of scientific study, since "scientifically
terminate in sharing have been held to be a homi- they do not and cannot exist." Similarly, Gardiner
noid hallmark and an evolutionary precursor to the (1974) claims that
development of modern man.
Unfortunately, there is no way to interview animals to
Chimpanzees also seem capable of learning to use discover the exact point on the evolutionary scale at which
various nonvocal forms of language in ways that consciousness emerges. Neither is there any way to deter-
parallel language development in both deaf and mine when "self" becomes an element within the subjective
map. (p. 207)
speaking children (Fouts, 1974; Gardner & Gard-
ner, 1969; Linden, 1974; Premack, 1971; Rum-
baugh, Gill, & von Glasersfeld, 1973). Since chim- Mirror-Image Stimulation
panzees can learn to use the functional properties of A few years ago, however, I did a study with
language, this puts us in a peculiar predicament. chimpanzees and mirrors that reflects on this prob-
To view language as the distinguishing feature of lem. My interest in mirrors can best be described
man means that now we either have to redefine in terms of what I have called mirror-image stimu-
language or redefine man, just as Leakey once said lation, which refers specifically to a situation in
of tools. Implicitly faced with this choice, many which an organism is confronted with its own re-
people seem to be opting for the former rather than flection in a mirror. Many organisms react to such
the latter alternative (e.g., Mounin, 1976). Al- stimulation as if they were seeing another animal,
though the controversy that has arisen over the and they engage in species-typical social responses
existence or nonexistence of language in chimpan- directed toward the reflection (Gallup, 1968).
zees may eventually improve our understanding of However, this is no ordinary social encounter. If
language, it is not clear that it will have the same you think about it, when an animal first sees him-
effect on our thinking about man. self in a mirror, he will be seeing an animal with
One of the most interesting by-products of lan- facial features he has never seen before. In other
guage training with chimpanzees is that, contrary words, a mirror ought to simulate the presence of
to the linguistic relativity hypothesis, language does a stranger, and indeed the initial reaction often
not seem to provide the animals with any radically consists of responses that would be made to un-
new concepts. Language simply gives them a familiar conspecifics. Mirror-image stimulation also
means of expressing what they already know has incentive properties for many species (Gallup
(Premack & Premack, 1972) or what Menzel & McClure, 1971; Thompson, 1964), and animals
will learn to make instrumental responses in order
to gain brief visual access to their own reflection.
1
Although man clearly has the cortical balance of power, Some animals even appear to show a peculiar pref-
Yeni-Komshian and Benson (1976) report temporal lobe
asymmetries in chimpanzees, but not rhesus monkeys, that erence for viewing mirrors instead of other members
are similar to those found in the human brain. of their own species (e.g., Gallup, 197S).

330 • MAY 1977 • AMERICAN PSYCHOLOGIST


Another point about mirrors, and probably the
most obvious for humans, is that they provide a
potential source of information about the self. Mir-
rors enable visually capable organisms to see them-
selves as they are seen by others. In front of a
mirror an animal is literally an audience to its own 5-
to
behavior. Since the observer's behavior dictates UJ
the behavior of the reflection, there is always a po- 10-
tential basis for predicting and controlling what
the reflection will do. The image never initiates a o
social encounter. It never reciprocates. It only o
CO
5.
^^
mimics the observer. Yet, for some curious reason,
most animals seem incapable of recognizing the
dualism inherent in such stimulation, and even 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 TEST
after prolonged exposure to mirrors they fail to dis-
DAYS
cover the relationship between their own behavior
and the reflection of that behavior. This inability Figure 1. Number of time-sampled social re-
bears on a distinction between self-sensation and sponses directed to the mirror by chimpanzees over
self-perception. Mirror-image stimulation for an days. (From "Chimpanzees: Self-Recognition" by
animal constitutes self-sensation. Although osten- G. G. Gallup, Jr., Science, 1970,167, 86-87. Copy-
sibly stimulated by themselves, most organisms per- right 1969 by the American Association for the Ad-
sist in showing other-directed rather than self- vancement of Science. Reprinted by permission.)
directed behavior in response to the reflection.
When looking at themselves in mirrors, animals in man (Abely, 1930; Delmas, 1929; Ostancow,
react as though they were seeing other animals. 1934). In fact, some schizophrenics appear to have
Humans, however, are capable of self-perception in lost their capacity for self-recognition (Faure,
the sense of using mirrors for grooming and pur- 19S6; Traub & Orbach, 1964; Wittreich, 1959)
poses of self-inspection. Whether self-sensation and react as if in the presence of another person.
translates into self-perception depends on selj- Aside from the fact that most of us are probably
recognition. given explicit verbal instructions by our parents
about the identity of the reflection, the ability to
Self-Recognition recognize one's own image would seem to be partly
a function of prolonged confrontation with mirrors.
As far as humans are concerned, the available data After all, mirror surfaces, at least in this culture,
show that self-recognition is learned. For example, are an everyday part of the human experience.
people born with visual defects, who later undergo Maybe if animals were given the same opportunity
operations that provide normal sight, initially re- for extended self-inspection, they might also come
spond to mirrors as if confronted with another to recognize the dualism implicit in such surfaces.
person and react to mirror space as though it were To test this conjecture, I gave a number of wild-
real (von Senden, 1960). Infants often show born, preadolescent chimpanzees individual expo-
other-directed behaviors as well and are notorious sure to a full-length mirror for 10 days (Gallup,
for responding to mirrors as playmates. Although 1970). During that period, each subject was kept
imperfect, recent studies indicate that children do in a small cage in an otherwise empty room to in-
not begin to show signs of self-recognition until sure enforced self-confrontation. Figure 1 depicts
they approach 2 years of age (Amsterdam, 1972; the average incidence of time-sampled social re-
Schulman & Kaplowitz, in press). On the other sponses or other-directed behaviors made in re-
hand, some profoundly retarded children, adoles- sponse to the mirror. The only behaviors scored as
cents, and even comparably handicapped adults other-directed were those that would be typically
seem totally incapable of recognizing their reflec- made in the presence of another chimpanzee (e.g.,
tions (Boulanger-Balleyguier, 1964; Harris, 1977; bobbing, vocalizing, threatening, etc.). Although
Pechacek, Bell, Cleland, Baum, & Boyle, 1973). It this was the predominant behavior initially, the
is also curious to note that prolonged mirror gazing tendency to treat the reflection as a companion
has been associated with the onset of schizophrenia tended to wane over days. Other-directed responses

AMERICAN PSYCHOLOGIST • MAY 1977 • 331


w ing, alcohol-soluble dye. The animal was then
Ul
placed back into its cage and allowed to recover in
g 18
the absence of the mirror. The significance of this
£ 16 procedure is threefold. First, the chimpanzees had
Ul 14 no way of knowing they had been marked, since
the procedure was accomplished under deep anes-
5 10 thesia. Second, the dye (Rhodamine B-base) was
carefully selected because of its complete lack of
6 8 tactile and olfactory properties, as determined by
UJ
* 6 applying it to my own skin several days prior to
Q
i 4 testing. Finally, the marks were strategically
u_ placed at predetermined points where it would be
impossible for the animals to see them without a
mirror.
I 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 TEST Following recovery, the subjects were directly
DAYS observed in the absence of the mirror to determine
Figure 2. Number of time-sampled self-directed the number of times any marked portion of the
responses made while watching the mirror over days. skin was touched "spontaneously." The mirror
(From "Chimpanzees: Self-Recognition" by G. G. was then reintroduced as an explicit test of self-
Gallup, Jr., Science, 1970, 167, 86-87. Copyright recognition. The results are shown in Figure 4. As
1969 by the American Association for the Advance- evidence for self-recognition, the number of mark-
ment of Science. Reprinted by permission.) directed responses, or attempts to touch a marked
area on themselves through visually guided mirror
were replaced by a self-directed orientation (see feedback, increased by a factor of over 25 times in
Figure 2). Rather than respond to the mirror as the presence of the mirror. In terms of the chim-
such, after the second or third day the chimpanzees panzees' perceptions of their own facial features,
began to use the mirror to respond to themselves. over twice as many attempts were made to touch
Under conditions of self-directed responding, they the marked eyebrows as compared to the ears. Not
used the reflection to gain visual access to and to only did the incidence of mark-directed behaviors
experiment with otherwise inaccessible information increase upon exposure to the mirror, but so did
about themselves (e.g., grooming parts of the body
that could not be seen directly, picking bits of food
600-
from between their teeth, blowing bubbles, making
faces at the mirror, etc.). Figure 3 shows the aver-
age amount of time spent viewing their own reflec-
tions during 30-minute recording sessions each day.
Prior to the development of self-directed behavior,
all the animals showed considerable visual attention
to the mirror, but with the emergence of a self-
directed orientation, this interest subsided.
These patterns of self-directed behavior implied
that the subjects had correctly identified the source 120-
of the reflection. However, I was concerned that
others might not be terribly convinced or enamored
with my subjective interpretations. In an attempt I 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 TEST
to clarify and objectify these impressions, an un- DAYS
obtrusive and more rigorous test of self-recognition Figure 3. Average amount of time, during two
was instituted. Following the last day of mirror IS-minute sessions, that chimpanzees spent view-
exposure, each chimpanzee was anesthetized and the ing themselves in the mirror over days. (From
mirror was removed. When the animal was un- "Chimpanzees: Self-Recognition" by G. G. Gallup,
conscious, I proceeded to paint the uppermost por- Jr., Science, 1970, 167, 86-87. Copyright 1969 by
tion of an eyebrow ridge and the top half of the the American Association for the Advancement of
opposite ear with a bright red, odorless, nonirritat- Science. Reprinted by permission.)

332 • MAY 1977 • AMERICAN PSYCHOLOGIST


viewing time (see Figure 3). The presence of red
marks on the chimpanzees' faces greatly enhanced
their visual attention to the reflection. In addition
to mark-directed responses, there were also a num-
ber of noteworthy attempts to visually examine and
smell the fingers that had been used to touch
marked areas of the skin, even though the dye had
long since dried and was indelible.
In an attempt to eliminate any doubt about the
source of these reactions, several comparable chim-
panzees that had never seen themselves in mirrors
were also anesthetized and marked. When 'they
were given access to the mirror for the first time,
there were no mark-directed behaviors (see Figure
4), patterns of self-directed responses were com- PRETEST TEST CONTROL
pletely absent, and the dye was ostensibly ignored. Figure 4. Number of mark-directed responses
Throughout the test, their orientation to the mirror made by experimental animals before being exposed
was as though they were seeing another chimpan- to the mirror, and by experimental and control ani-
zee, which implies that self-recognition was learned mals during the test of self-recognition. (From
by the other animals sometime during the prior 10 "Chimpanzees: Self-Recognition" by G. G. Gallup,
days of mirror exposure. Jr., Science, 1970, 167, 86-87. Copyright 1969 by
To the extent that self-recognition implies a the American Association for the Advancement of
rudimentary concept of self, these data show that Science. Reprinted by permission.)
contrary to popular opinion and preconceived ideas,
man may not have a monopoly on the self-concept.
likewise tested gibbons and macaques, respectively,
Man may not be evolution's only experiment in self-
but failed to find any indication that these animals
awareness. To date, these findings have been repli-
were capable of realizing that their behavior was
cated a number of times by several investigators the source of the behavior depicted in the mirror.
(e.g., Gallup, McClure, Hill, & Bundy, 1971; Hill, In a more recent and exceptionally thorough
Bundy, Gallup, & McClure, 1970; Lethmate &
study, Benhar, Carlton, and Samuel (197S) at-
Diicker, 1973) and extended by Lethmate and
tempted to demonstrate self-recognition in olive
Diicker to include orangutans as well as chimpan- baboons. In a series of four experiments they
zees.2 found that while a mirror had potent reinforcing
properties, none of the baboons, after as long as
Self-Recognition in Other Primates 250 hours of mirror exposure, showed even the
faintest signs of self-recognition. In desperation
In the initial study of self-recognition (Gallup,
Benhar et al. (1975) even went so far as to try
1970), I also tested feral rhesus monkeys, Java
to teach a baboon to recognize its reflection with
monkeys, and stumptail macaques, but found no
raisins as a reward, but to no avail. They con-
evidence for self-directed behavior even after as
cluded that "this ability to distinguish between
long as 21 consecutive days of mirror exposure.
'self and 'others' seems to be a quality specific to
Moreover, after the application of red dye, the
man and (great) apes." 8
monkeys appeared insensitive to the source and
Since it is unusual to find substantial qualitative
significance of the marks reflected in the mirror.
differences between monkeys and great apes in
While reporting striking success with chimpanzees learning ability, or for that matter in most other
and orangutans, Lethmate and Diicker (1973) were
basic psychological processes (e.g., Cowey & Weis-
also unable to demonstrate selfrrecognition in spider
kranz, 1975; Mason, 1976), the apparent inability
monkeys, capuchins, macaques, mandrill and hama-
dryas baboons, and two species of gibbons. K.
Pribram (Note 1) and M. Bertrand (Note 2) have
s I used to feel the same way, but porpoises, because of
their unusually large and complex brains, represent an in-
triguing alternative. The problem, however, would be to
2
As yet there have been ho systematic attempts to assess contrive an objective test for an animal with only flippers
self-recognition in gorillas. instead of arms and fingers.

AMERICAN PSYCHOLOGIST • MAY 1977 • 333


of monkeys to discern the identity of the reflection 1975, in press) that the answer may relate to a
is peculiar.4 However, even though gibbons, which cognitive deficit rather than a more mechanistic
are classified as apes, and monkeys have been one. Monkeys appear to lack a cognitive category
given seemingly ample opportunity to recognize for processing mirrored information about them-
their reflections, maybe with more time they might selves. The capacity for self-recognition, although
learn to decipher the significance of the "stranger" influenced by learning, is predicated on a sense of
in the mirror. In the most recent attempt to sal- identity.7 The unique feature of mirror-image
vage the conceptual integrity of monkeys (Gallup, stimulation is that the identity of the observer and
in press), a wild-born, preadolescent, crab-eating his reflection in a mirror are necessarily one and
macaque was given over 2,400 hours of mirror ex- the same. The capacity to correctly infer the
posure for a period extending over 5 months, but it identity of the reflection must, therefore, presuppose
still failed to show any convincing evidence of self- an already existent identity on the part of the or-
recognition. By comparison, chimpanzees begin ganism making this inference. Without an identity
showing signs of self-recognition after only 2 or 3 of your own it would be impossible to recognize
days of mirror confrontation. yourself. And therein may lie the basic difference
Why the striking discontinuity between great between monkeys and great apes. The monkey's
apes and other primate species? Why does a mon- inability to recognize himself may be due to the
key seem incapable of learning that his behavior absence of a sufficiently well-integrated self-concept.
constitutes the source of the behavior depicted in a While many organisms are ostensibly conscious of
mirror? 6 On practically all other psychological different features of themselves as a result of visual,
tasks, including concept formation and problem- tactile, chemical, and proprioceptive feedback, in
solving ability, the modal finding is a high degree principle this is quite different from self-conscious-
of continuity among most primates. Although ness. Without an identity, such input may not be
monkeys can learn to use mirrors to manipulate fundamentally different from a simple awareness of
objects (Brown, McDowell, & Robinson, 1965), other objects and/or events in the environment.
they appear incapable of learning to sufficiently in- With the exception of man and the great apes,
tegrate features of their own reflections to use mir- eventually we may have to entertain the possibility
rors to respond to themselves. Again, it is not that that primate consciousness may be unidirectional.
they are unable to learn to respond to mirrored We are a long way away from being able to
cues. When looking at the reflection of a human specify a neurological basis for the sense of identity.
or a bit of food, they can detect the inherent dual- However, self-recognition may represent an emer-
ism as it pertains to objects other than themselves, gent phenomenon that only occurs once a species
and after adequate experience they do respond ap- acquires a certain number of cortical'neurons with
propriately by turning away from the mirror to gain sufficiently complex interconnections. Alternatively,
more direct access to the object of the reflection one might view these data from the standpoint of
(Tinklepaugh, 1928). Still, for some strange rea- a threshold model. Different organisms may very
son they fail to correctly interpret their own re- well have differing degrees of self-awareness, but
flections. only with an explicit sense of identity does self-
How are we to account for such an apparent recognition become possible. The threshold for
psychological void between great apes and other self-recognition may be quite high compared to
primates? 6 I have suggested elsewhere (Gallup, other forms of self-conception. It is possible that
as yet undefined tests of the self-concept, which
require a lower threshold for recognition, might
4
For some notable exceptions to the foregoing, see Rum- yield positive evidence for monkeys.8 For the
baugh (1971) and Rurabaugh and Gill (1973). meantime, however, the differences among primates
6
Given several taxonomic categories (e.g., prosimians,
New World monkeys, Old World monkeys, apes, and great
apes), for the sake of simplicity I am using the word
7
monkey to refer to all groups except the great apes. Identity can be defined as a sense of continuity over
6
It is important to acknowledge that the apparent lack time and space. Memory represents one form of such con-
of continuity in this instance may be a consequence of a tinuity, but memory by itself neither presupposes nor neces-
failure to tap an underlying continuum. The techniques sarily provides for such a sense.
8
described here were designed simply to assess the existence Self-recognition also presupposes a high degree of inter-
of self-recognition, not to quantify the capacity. Neverthe- modal equivalence, and until recently (Cowey & Weiskranz,
less, the absence of self-recognition in monkeys still requires 197S; Weiskranz & Cowey, 1975), cross-modal perception in
explanation. monkeys was difficult to demonstrate (e.g., Ettlinger, 1967).

334 • MAY 1977 • AMERICAN PSYCHOLOGIST


in self-recognition raise serious questions about re- 3001
cent claims (e.g., Griffin, 1976) concerning the
evolutionary continuity of mental experience.
As far as chimpanzees and orangutans are con-
cerned, I do not think their sense of identity 'or 225-
self-concept in any way emerges out of experience
with a mirror. A mirror simply represents a means .1
of mapping what the chimpanzee already knows,
and it provides him with a new and more explicit 150-
dimension of knowing about himself, in the sense

I
that he now has an opportunity to see himself as
he is seen by other chimpanzees. In other words,
chimpanzees may already have a self-concept, and
75-
a mirror may merely represent a means of objecti-
fying its existence. This would imply that manipu- • Wild born
lations designed to retard or prevent the develop- •Isolate
ment of a self-concept ought to affect the capacity
—i—i—i 1—i \ 1 1—i 1
for self-recognition. Psychologists, however, know I 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 Test
very little about the early development of self-
concepts. Since it is introspectively obvious that Trials
most humans do have a sense of self-awareness, this Figure 5. Average time during ten 30-minute
has fostered an empirical and conceptual laziness sessions that wild-born and isolate chimpanzees
about operationalizing the phenomenon. One has spent viewing their reflections in the mirror.
only to look at the literature on self-recognition in (From "Capacity for Self-recognition in Differenti-
infants to realize how imprecise our methodology ally Reared Chimpanzees" by G. G. Gallup, Jr.,
has been (Gallup, 1975). M. K. McClure, S. D. Hill, and R. A. Bundy, The
Psychological Record, 1971, 21, 69-74. Copyright
Social Influences and Self-Conception 1971 by The Psychological Record. Reprinted by
permission.)
An early approach to the ontogeny of self-concep-
tion involved what was called the "looking glass" Clure, Hill, & Bundy, 1971). As shown in Figure
theory of self (Cooley, 1912). Cooley proposed S, the wild-born animals exhibited a high degree of
that the human concept of self was an interpersonal visual curiosity in the reflection, but as in the initial
entity and as such was dependent upon social inter- study, visual attention waned over days. The iso-
action with others. Similarly, according to George lates, however, showed continuing and exaggerated
Herbert Mead (1934), in order for the self to attention to the mirror for the entire period. The
emerge as an object of conscious inspection, the increased viewing time demonstrated by the isolates
opportunity to examine one's self from another's is curiously reminiscent of the fact that socially de-
point of view is required. Figuratively speaking, in prived rhesus monkeys prefer to view mirrors
order to conceive of yourself you may need to see rather than other rhesus monkeys (Gallup & Mc-
yourself as you are seen by others. Clure, 1971) and of the reports of prolonged mirror
In a preliminary attempt to evaluate the appli- gazing by schizophrenic humans.
cability of the Cooley-Mead model, I decided to On the 10th day, all the animals were anesthe-
examine the effects of social isolation on self-recog- tized and marked just as before. When tested for
nition in chimpanzees. Certainly a chimpanzee self-recognition, the feral chimpanzees evidenced
reared in isolation would never have had the benefit 13.S times more mark-directed responses than the
of another chimpanzee's experience or the oppor- isolates (see Figure 6). Moreover, the number of
tunity to examine himself from another chimpan- mark-directed responses shown by isolates on the
zee's point of view. To see if this would make a test of self-recognition was equivalent to the num-
difference, both feral chimpanzees maintained in ber shown by ferals on the pretest. It is also inter-
group cages and laboratory-born, isolation-reared esting to note in Figure 5 that feral animals showed
chimpanzees were provided with 9 days of indi- a dramatic increase in viewing time when they first
vidual self-confrontation in a mirror (Gallup, Mc- saw themselves in the marked condition, and this

AMERICAN PSYCHOLOGIST • MAY 1977 • 335


3(h or lack thereof, it is important to acknowledge that
there are several other and perhaps more parsimoni-
ous interpretations. Since isolates have never seen

r
§20H
other chimpanzees, maybe they fail to speculate
about the curious and atypical behavior of the
"other" animal in the mirror who only responds
when responded to. However, to the extent that
viewing time can be used as a measure of the
mirror's attractive potential, isolate chimpanzees
have certainly not lost interest in the mirror. They
show intense and unabated attention to the reflec-
tion for the entire 10-day period. On the other
I \0-\ hand, it could be argued that early isolation some-
how impairs or retards learning ability. Apparent
learning deficits engendered by social isolation in
5- primates, however, can be complicated by enhanced
emotionality in novel situations (Harlow, Schlitz, &
Harlow, 1968).
Pretest Test Pretest Test
Wild Born Isolate Self-Concept Distortion
Figure 6. Number of times any marked portion If explicit self-awareness is predicated on the oppor-
of the skin was touched during a pretest with- tunity to examine oneself from another's point of
out the mirror and on the test by wild-born and view, some intriguing possibilities are raised. Jung
isolate chimpanzees. (From "Capacity for Self- (19S8) argued that a truly objective view of man
recognition in Differentially Reared Chimpanzees" would only be possible if we were able to see our-
by G. G. Gallup, Jr., M. K. McClure, S. D. Hill, selves from another species' perspective. However,
and R. A. Bundy, The Psychological Record, 1971, if the present analysis is correct and the opportu-
21, 69-74. Copyright 1971 by The Psychological nity to examine oneself was restricted to another
Record. Reprinted by permission.) species' point of view, this ought to profoundly dis-
tort one's concept of self. Although incomplete, the
reinstatement of visual attention replicates the available data are not at odds with this proposition.
earlier findings (see Figure 3). But isolate-reared According to Linden (1974), when Washoe, who
chimpanzees, as further evidence that they remain was reared with humans, was first confronted with
completely oblivious to the source of the reflection, other chimpanzees, she referred to them in sign
showed virtually no change in viewing time on the language as "black bugs." As a further illustration,
test of self-recognition. Isolates, unlike normal it is well known that being reared in social isolation
chimpanzees, also failed to show any signs of self- can have devastating effects on primate sexual be-
directed behavior during the entire 10-day period. havior (e.g., Mason, 1960). Human-reared chim-
As an extension of these findings, we (Hill, panzees, however, are actually more impaired sexu-
Bundy, Gallup, & McClure, 1970) obtained three ally than those reared in complete and abject social
additional isolation-reared chimpanzees, none of isolation (Rogers & Davenport, 1969). Why?
which showed any evidence of self-recognition. Maybe it is because, just like Washoe, they think
Prior to retesting each of the animals, two of the they are human.
three were given 3 months of remedial social ex- Also related to the Cooley-Mead view of self-
perience by housing them together in the same cage. conception is an informal experiment conducted
In support of the Cooley-Mead model, after the with another home-reared chimpanzee named Vicki.
opportunity to view themselves from the other's Among other things, Vicki was taught to sort stacks
point of view, both animals showed preliminary of snapshots into a human and an animal pile. One
signs of self-recognition, but the third chimpanzee, day, unbeknown to Vicki, her own photograph was
who remained in isolation, did not. placed in the stack, and when she came to her pic-
While I would like to interpret these data as ture she placed it on the human pile (Hayes &
being related to the development of a self-concept, Nissen, 1971). There are two ways to interpret

336 • MAY 1977 • AMERICAN PSYCHOLOGIST


this. First, man may not be the only one to appre- Benhar, E. E., Carlton, P. L., & Samuel, D. A search for
mirror-image reinforcement and self-recognition in the
ciate the similarities between chimpanzees and men.
baboon. In S. Kendo, M. Kawai, & A. Ehara (Eds.),
The other is that maybe Vicki thought she was Contemporary primatology; Proceedings of the Fifth In-
human. The imprinting literature makes it clear ternational Congress of Primatology. Basel, Switzerland:
Karger, 197S.
that species identity, at least as defined by affili-
Black, M. The labyrinth of language. New York: Praeger,
ation tendencies, is often tied to early social ex- 1968.
periences. For you and I and the rest of the great Boulanger-Balleyguier, G. Premieres reactions devant le
apes, individual identity may also be subject to miroir. Enfance, 1964,1, 51-67.
Brown, W. L., McDowell, A. A., & Robinson, E. M. Dis-
social influences.9 crimination learning of mirrored cues by rhesus monkeys.
Journal of Genetic Psychology, 1965, 106, 123-128.
Buss, A. Psychology—Man in perspective. New York:
Conclusion Wiley, 1973.
Epstein (1973) has recently argued that the self- Cooley, C. H. Human nature and the social order. New
York: Scribner's, 1912.
concept as traditionally conceived is intellectually Cowey, A., & Weiskranz, L. Demonstration of cross-modal
bankrupt, since practically all definitions lack mean- matching in rhesus monkeys, Macaca mulatto. Neuro-
ingful referents or tend to be circular. On the psychologia, 1975, 13, 117-120.
Davenport, R. K., & Rogers, C. M. Intermodal equivalence
other hand, self-recognition represents a technique of stimuli in apes. Science, 1970, 168, 279-280.
for providing empirical and operational substance Delmas, F. A. Le signe du miroir dans la demence precoce.
to the existence of self-awareness. Moreover, con- Annales Medico-Psychologiques, 1929, 87, 227-233.
Epstein, S. The self-concept revisited, or a theory of a
trary to the claims of some (e.g., Rose, 1976), theory. American Psychologist, 1973, 28, 404-416,
these data demonstrate that the distinction between Ettlinger, G. Analysis of cross-modal effects and their rela-
consciousness and self-consciousness may not be a tionship to language. In F. L. Darley & C. H. Millikan
(Eds.), Brain mechanisms underlying speech and lan-
pseudoproblem. guage. New York: Grune & Stratton, 1967.
For better or for worse, there is as yet no way Faure, H. L'investissement delirant de l'image de soi.
that I can experience your experience, or for that Evolution Psychiatrique, ;956, 3, 545-577.
Fouts, R. S. Language: Origins, definitions and chimpan-
matter, the experience of any organism other than zees. Journal of Human Evolution, 1974, 3, 475-482.
myself. However, some of the most intriguing Gallup, G. G., Jr. Mirror-image stimulation. Psychologi-
problems in psychology involve the discovery of cal Bulletin, 1968, 70, 782-793.
Gallup, G. G., Jr. Chimpanzees: Self-recognition. Science,
ways to infer and map experience in other organ- 1970,167, 86-87.
isms. As far as the self-concept is concerned, it Gallup, G. G., Jr. Towards an operational definition of
would appear that on the morning before God cre- self-awareness. In R. H. Tuttle (Ed.), Socio-ecoiogy and
psychology of primates. The Hague, Netherlands: Mou-
ated the great apes, maybe he became distracted ton, 1975.
by his own reflection in the mirror and forgot to Gallup, G. G., Jr. Absence of self-recognition in a monkey
shave with Occam's razor. (Macaca fascicularis) following prolonged exposure to a
mirror. Developmental Psychobiology, in press.
Gallup, G. G., Jr., Boren, J. L., Gagliardi, G. J., & Wallnau,
9
L. B. A mirror for the mind of man, or will the chim-
It is intriguing to note that while the content may, the panzee create an identity crisis for Homo sapiens? Jour-
existence of self-awareness in chimpanzees does not appear nal of Human Evolution, in press.
to be predicated on a particular species identity. Many Gallup, G. G., Jr., & McClure, M. K. Preference for
home-reared chimpanzees show signs of self-recognition in mirror-image stimulation in differentially reared rhesus
response to mirrors but an active avoidance of, or even monkeys. Journal of Comparative and Physiological
disdain for, members of their own species (e.g., Temerlin, Psychology, 1971, 75, 403-407.
197S). Gallup, G. G., Jr., McClure, M. K., Hill, S. D., & Bundy,
R. A. Capacity for self-recognition in differentially reared
REFERENCE NOTES chimpanzees. The Psychological Record, 1971, 21, 69-74.
Gardiner, W. L. Psychology: A story of a search. Bel-
1. Pribram, K. Personal communication, June 20, 1972. mont, Calif.: Wadsworth, 1974.
2. Bertrand, M. Personal communication, June 19, 1972. Gardner, R. A., & Gardner, B. T. Teaching sign language
to a chimpanzee. Science, 1969, 165, 664-672.
REFERENCES Griffin, D. R. The question of animal awareness. New
York: Rockefeller University Press, 1976.
Abely. Le signe du miroir dans les psychoses et plus sp6- Harlow, H. F., Schlitz, K. A., & Harlow, M. K. Effects of
cialement dans la demence precoce. Annales Midico- social isolation on the learning performance of rhesus
Psychologiques, 1930, 88, 28-36. monkeys. Proceedings of the 2nd International Congress
Amsterdam, B. Mirror self-image reactions before age two. of Primatology. Basel, Switzerland: Karger, 1968.
Developmental Psychobiology, 1972 5, 297-30S. Harris, L. P. Self-recognition among institutionalized pro-
Ardrey, R. African genesis. New York: Dell, 1961, foundly retarded males: A replication. Bulletin of the
Beck, B. B. Primate tool behavior. In R. H. Tuttle (Ed.), Psychonomic Society, 1977, 9, 43-44.
Socio-ecology and psychology of primates. The Hague, Hayes, K. J., & Nissen, C. H. Higher mental functions in
Netherlands: Mouton, 1975. a home-raised chimpanzee. In A. M, Schrier & F. Stoll-

AMERICAN PSYCHOLOGIST • MAY 1977 « 337


nitz (Eds.), Behavior of nonhuman primates (Vol. 3). Premack, D. Language in chimpanzee? Science, 1971,
New York: Academic Press, 1971. 772, 808-822.
Hill, S. D., Bundy, R. A., Gallup, G. G., Jr., & McClure, Rogers, C. M., & Davenport, R. K. Effects of restricted
M. K. Responsiveness of young nursery-reared chim- rearing on sexual behavior of chimpanzees. Develop-
panzees to mirrors. Proceedings of the Louisiana Acad- mental Psychology, 1969, 1, 200-204.
emy of Sciences, 1970, 33, 77-82. Rose, S. The conscious brain. New York: Vintage Books,
Jung, C. G. The undiscovered self. Boston: Little, Brown, 1976.
1958. Rumbaugh, D. M. Evidence for qualitative differences in
King, M.-C., & Wilson, A. C. Evolution at two levels in learning processes among primates. Journal of Compara-
humans and chimpanzees. Science, 1975, 188, 107-116. tive and Physiological Psychology, 1971, 76, 250-255.
Kinget, G. M. On being human. New York: Harcourt Rumbaugh, D. M., & Gill, T. V. The learning skills of great
Brace Jovanovich, 1975. apes. Journal of Human Evolution, 1973, 2, 171-179.
Kliiver, H. Behavior mechanisms in monkeys. Chicago: Rumbaugh, D. M., Gill, T. V., & von Glasersfeld, E. C.
University of Chicago Press, 1933. Reading and sentence completion by a chimpanzee (Pan).
Lethmate, J., & Diicker, G. Untersuchungen zum selbster- Science, 1973, 182, 731-733.
kennen im Spiegel bei orang-utans und einigen anderen Schulman, A. H., & Kaplowitz, C. Mirror-image responses
affenarten. Zeitschrijt fur Tierpsychologie, 1973, 33, during the first two years of life. Developmental Psy-
248-269. chobiology, in press.
Linden, E. Apes, men, and language. New York: Penguin, Slobodkin, L. B. Evolution is no help. World Archaeology,
1974. in press.
Lorenz, K. Studies in animal behavior (Vol. 2). Cam- Teleki, G. The predatory behavior of wild chimpanzees.
bridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press, 1971. Lewisburg, Pa.: Bucknell University Press, 1973.
Mason, W. A. The effects of social restriction on the be- Temerlin, M. K. Lucy: Growing up human. Palo Alto,
havior of rhesus monkeys: I. Free social behavior. Calif.: Science & Behavior Books, 1975.
Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, Thompson, T. Visual reinforcement in fighting cocks.
1960, 53, 582-589. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1964,
Mason, W. A. Environmental models and mental modes: 7, 45-49.
Representational processes in the great apes and man. Tinklepaugh, O. L. An experimental study of representative
American Psychologist, 1976, 31, 284-294. factors in monkeys. Journal of Comparative Psychology,
Mead, G. H. Mind, self and society: From the standpoint 1928, 8, 197-236.
of a social behaviorist. Chicago: University of Chicago Traub, A. C., & Orbach, J. Psychophysical studies of body-
Press, 1934. image: I. The adjustable body-distorting mirror. Ar-
Menzel, E. W., Jr. Chimpanzee spatial memory organiza- chives of General Psychiatry, 1964, 11, 53-66.
tion. Science, 1973, 182, 943-945. von Senden, M. Space and sight: The perception of space
Menzel, E. W., Jr. Natural language of young chimpanzees. and shape in the congenitally blind before and after oper-
New Scientist, January 16, 1975, pp. 127-130. ation. Glencoe, 111.: Free Press, 1960.
Mounin, G. Language, communication, chimpanzees. Cur- Weiskranz, L., & Cowey, A. Cross-modal matching in the
rent Anthropology, 1976, 17, 1-21. rhesus monkey using a single pair of stimuli. Neuropsy-
Ostancow, P. Le signe du miroir dans la demence pr6coce. chologia, 1975, 13, 257-261.
Annales Medico-Psychologiques, 1934, 92, 787-790. Wittreich, W. Visual perception and personality. Scientific
Pechacek, T. F., Bell, K. F., Cleland, C. C., Baum, C., & American, 1959, 200, 56-60.
Boyle, M. Self-recognition in profoundly retarded males. Yeni-Komshian, G. H., & Benson, D. A. Anatomical study
Bulletin of the Psychonomic Satiety, 1973, 1, 328-330. of cerebral asymmetry in the temporal lobe of humans,
Premack, A. J., & Premack, D. Teaching language to an chimpanzees, and rhesus monkeys. Science, 1976, 192,
ape. Scientific American, 1972, 227, 92-99. 387-389.

338 • MAY 1977 • AMERICAN PSYCHOLOGIST

You might also like