You are on page 1of 10

The Clinical Neuropsychologist KD Psychology Press

2004, Vol. 18, No. 1, pp. 4 1 ^ 9 ^ T^i.F™nci.c™,p

The Pattern of Neuropsychological Deficits


in Vascular Cognitive Impairment-No
Dementia (Vascular CIND)
David L. Nyenhuis', Philip B. Gorelick', Emily J. Geenen^, Clifford A. Smith",
Eugenia Gencheva', Sally Ereels^, and Leyla deToledo-Morrell'*'^
'Department of Neurology and Rehabilitation, University of Illinois at Chicago, IL, USA,
Psychology Department, Wayne State University, USA, 'School of Public Health,
University of Illinois at Chicago, IL, USA, and Rush University Medical Center, Chicago, IL, USA,
•"Psychology Department and 'Department of Neurological Sciences

ABSTRACT

We examined the pattern of neuropsychological deficits in Vascular Cognitive Impairment-No Dementia


(Vascular CIND) by comparing the cognitive and behavioral performance of 41 post-stroke Vascular CIND
patients to that of 62 post-stroke patients with no cognitive impairment (NCI). Neuropsychological test
scores were grouped into seven cognitive and four behavioral domains, then converted to standardized,
weighted principle component scores (PCS) for each domain. Multivariate logistic regression models built
on cognitive domains found the immediate recall and psychomotor domains to best predict diagnostic group
membership. In a separate model limited to behavioral data, the depressed mood domain best predicted
group membership. The combination of immediate memory deficits, psychomotor slowness and depression
have also been found in Vascular Dementia (VaD), suggesting that the pattern of deficits in Vascular CIND
and VaD neuropsychological deficits are similar. This cognitive and behavioral pattern similarity supports
the hypothesis that Vascular CIND lies on a continuum between NCI and VaD.

Vascular Dementia (VaD) may be the second lead- VCI with other dementing illness, most often
ing cause of dementia in the United States and Alzheimer's disease (Rockwood et al., 1999).
Western Europe (Gorelick, Roman, & Mangone, This study is the first that we are aware of to
1994). However, VaD is only the most severe examine the neuropsychological impairment pat-
subgroup of cerebrovascular-related cognitive tern of Vascular CIND.
impairment (Babikian, Wolfe, Linn, Knoefel, & Vascular CIND impairments vary widely and
Albert, 1990; Rockwood, Howard, MacKnight, & can include deficits in language, praxis, orienta-
Darvesh, 1999), and it's exclusive use may focus tion, memory, executive functioning and visuo-
treatment on persons too impaired to benefit spatial functioning. The critical factors that
(Hachinski, 1994; Rockwood et al., 2000). separate Vascular CIND from VaD are whether
Vascular Cognitive Impairment (VCI) is a more there are enough deficient cognitive domains (all
inclusive construct than VaD. It is subdivided into dementia nosologies require at least two impaired
three categories: Vascular Cognitive Impairment, domains) and whether the cognitive deficits are
No Dementia (Vascular CIND), VaD, and Mixed linked to functional (activities of daily living)

Address correspondence to: David L. Nyenhuis, Ph.D., University of Illinois Center for Stroke Research, 1645 W.
Jackson Blvd, Suite 400, Chicago, IL 60612, USA. E-mail: nyenhuis@uic.edu
Accepted for publication: January 29, 2004.

1385-4046/04/1801-041$!6.00 © Taylor & Francis Ltd.


42 DAVID L. NYENHUIS ET AL.

impairment. A sufficient number of impaired and Garron (2002) found no cognitive decline in
domains and a link between cognitive and func- their sample of African American ischemic stroke
tional impairments lead to a VaD diagnosis. patients who were not demented at baseline and
The prevalence and incidence of Vascular followed for up to 7 years. Differences in the two
CIND depend on which VaD criteria are used. study samples such as in the type of vascular
VaD criteria that are more stringent will result in pathology and differences in inclusion criteria
fewer patients diagnosed with dementia and a (e.g., Nyenhuis et al. included all non-demented
correspondingly larger number of patients diag- patients, such as those with no cognitive impair-
nosed with Vascular CIND. The National Institute ment) may explain the differences in results.
of Neurological Disorders and Stroke and the Neuropsychological research in VaD has at-
Association Internationale pour la Recherche et tempted to define the pattern of cognitive impair-
l'Enseignement en Neurosciences (NINDS- ments in VaD and compare it with the pattern in
AIREN) require memory plus two other impaired other dementia syndromes, most often Alzhei-
cognitive domains (Roman et al., 1993). The State mer's disease (AD) (Benthem, Jones, & Hodges,
of California Alzheimer's Disease Diagnostic and 1997; Crossley, D'Arcy, & Rawson, 1997;
Treatment Centers (ADDTC) criteria for Fuld, 1984; Kontiola, Laaksonen, Sulkava, &
Ischemic Vascular Dementia (IVD) are relatively Erkinjuntii, 1990; Mendez & Ashla-Mendez,
less stringent. ADDTC criteria do not require 1991; Starkstein, Sabe, & Vazquez, 1996;
memory impairment, only that the cognitive Villardita, 1993). A guiding hypothesis in this
impairment is not isolated to a single narrow research is that, relative to AD, VaD presents with
category of intellectual performance (Chui et al., more deficits in attention, concentration and
1992). A comparison of NINDS-AIREN and executive function and less delayed memory
ADDTC dementia criteria in our sample of post- impairment (Kertesz & Clydesdale, 1994;
stroke patients showed VaD to be diagnosed in McPherson & Cummings, 1996). Libon et al.
13.2% by ADDTC and 7.7% by NINDS-AIREN and others (Bogdanoff, Gitlin, & Norman, 1997;
criteria (Geenen, Nyenhuis, Gencheva, & Lamar, Podell, & Carew, 1997; Libon, Bogdanoff,
Gorelick, 2003). Thus, use of NINDS-AIREN & Bonavita, 1997; Libon, Bogdanoff, & Cloud
criteria may lead to more Vascular CIND cases 1998; Libon, Mattson, & Glosser, 1996) have
than when ADDTC criteria are used. completed a number of projects that compare
Few studies have examined neuropathology deficits in Ischemic Vascular Dementia (IVD)
associated with Vascular CIND. As with VaD, patients to those of patients with AD or Parkin-
focal and diffuse white matter lesions are thought son's disease. They conclude that neuropsycholog-
to play a key role (Desmond, 2002; Englund, ical assessment can successfully differentiate
2002). Global atrophic changes, and vascular AD from IVD but that IVD is similar to other
lesion location, size and number, have been impli- subcortical dementing illnesses in the pattern of
cated in VaD (Nyenhuis & Gorelick, 1998). These neuropsychological results. Looi and Sachdev
same variables may be related to Vascular CIND. (1999), in their review, conclude that VaD patients
Similarly, stroke risk factors, such as hyperten- showed better performances in verbal long-term
sion, diabetes, obesity and smoking (Gorelick, memory and poorer performance in frontal execu-
1995, 1997) are likely to be associated with tive functioning than AD patients.
Vascular CIND. Rockwood, Ebly, Hachinski, The present study examines the neuropsycho-
and Hogan (1997) found that 57% of their Vas- logical pattern of Vascular CIND patients by
cular CIND group had a history of stroke, 47.6% comparing them with a group of patients without
had hypertension, 36.3% had cardiac symptoms cognitive impairment. Patients from both groups
and 26.8% had a history of heavy smoking. had a history of ischemic stroke 3-6 months
Few longitudinal studies have been completed prior to neuropsychological examination. Our
with Vascular CIND patients. Wentzel et al. hypothesis is that Vascular CIND patients will
(2001) found that 46% of Vascular CIND patients show a similar pattern of neuropsychological
went on to develop dementia during a 5-year impairment to that of patients with VaD when
period. By contrast, Nyenhuis, Gorelick, Freels, compared with the NCI group.
NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL DEHCITS IN VASCULAR CIND 43

METHODS Mental Control (Wechsler, 1945); Symbol Digit (Oral


Version; Smith, 1991); portions of the Behavioral
Subjects Dyscontrol Scale (Grisby & Kaye, 1996); Mini-Mental
State Examination (MMSE; Folstein, Folstein, &
Subjects were consecutive ischemic stroke patients who
McHugh, 1975); Figural Recognition Test (Scherr
presented to the Stroke and Critical Care Service at
et al., 1988); CERAD Boston Naming Test and Verbal
Rush-Presbyterian-St. Luke's Medical Center. Inclu-
Fluency (Morris et aL, 1989); Brief version of the
sion criteria were age 50 or over; sufficient English
Ravens Progressive Matrices (Raven, 1995); Grooved
language skills for psychological tests; availability of a
Pegboard Test (Russell & Starkey, 1993); and the
friend or relative knowledgeable about the subject's
Chicago Multiscale Depression Inventory (Nyenhuis
recent and past medical history who has known the
et al., 1998). The subjects were also administered the
subject at least 10 years and sees them at least three
Blessed Information, Memory and Concentration Test
times per week; high likelihood of being available for
(Blessed IMC; Thai, Grundman, & Golden, 1986) by a
follow-up; verbal and written consent; and ischemic
study physician as part of the neurological examination
stroke within the last 6 months. All ischemic strokes
and the patient's designated informant completed the
met criteria for diagnosis of cerebral infarction by the
Frontal Systems Behavior Scale (Grace, Stout, &
National Institute of Neurological and Communicative
Malloy, 1999).
Disorders and Stroke (NINDS) Stroke Data Bank
While the study subject was completing the neuro-
(Foulkes, Wolf, Price, Mohr, & Hier, 1988) and have
psychological examination, the informant, usually the
at least one focal symptom and/or sign of stroke at the
patient's spouse or child, completed an epidemiologic
time of stroke. Exclusion criteria included patients with
interview. A brief, 12-question background information
aphasia who score less than 50% on the Boston
and mental status test was completed and passed by the
Diagnostic Aphasia Examination Commands Subtest
informant prior to the interview. The epidemiologic
or who otherwise could not complete psychological
interview consisted of pretested questionnaires about
testing because of a language disorder (two potential
background, demographic, medical history and risk
subjects were excluded for this reason); chronic or
factor information. The information contained in the
degenerative disease prior to stroke (e.g., Alzheimer's
questionnaires was adapted primarily from cardiovas-
disease) affecting the central nervous system; any
cular disease risk factor questionnaires used previously
metabolic, traumatic, endocrine, infectious, prion-
by the study team (Gorelick, Brody, & Cohen, 1993)
related, nutritional or toxic disorder that could affect
and from the East Boston Senior Health Project (Evans
the central nervous system; substance abuse disorders
et al., 1989). The epidemiologic interviewer, under the
as defined by DSM-IV; intracranial hemorrhage;
supervision of the study neurologist, also completed an
difficult to control epilepsy that could present with
unstructured neurologic interview to determine the
cognitive impairment; or temporal lobe epilepsy.
patient's history of, number of, timing, symptoms and
Potential subjects were contacted by the study team
recovery from stroke; a history of memory loss and
and informed of the purpose and methods of the study.
other symptoms of dementia; and a structured neuro-
Those who consented to participate in the study were
logic interview to determine the history of dementia
scheduled for the baseline epidemiological, neurological,
onset and symptoms of cognitive dysfunction.
neuropsychological and MRI examination. The Institu-
tional Review Board (IRB) at Rush-Presbyterian-St.
Luke's Medical Center approved all study procedures. Diagnostic Procedures
The study neuropsychologist classified cognitive tests
into the following domains based on test content: basic
Study Iustrumeuts and Examination attention, working memory/executive function, lan-
Procedures guage, visuospatial skills, psychomotor skills, immediate
All study subjects completed formal neuropsycho- memory, delayed memory and recognition memory.
logical assessment, administered by a trained neuro- Behavioral tests were classified into tests of depression,
psychological technician under the supervision of apathy, dysexecutive function, and disinhibition. Table 1
the study neuropsychologist. The examination took lists the neuropsychological tests by domain. To confirm
approximately 90-120 min to administer and included the relationship of intra-domain tests, pairwise correla-
the following tests: Boston Diagnostic Aphasia Exami- tional analyses were completed with all cognitive tests,
nation, Commands subtest (Goodglass & Kaplan, using the entire subject sample. With the exception of
1989); Controlled Learning and Enhanced Recall, the delayed recognition memory domain (r=—.113,
Immediate and Delayed (Buschke & Grober, 1986); p> .10) all tests correlated significantly (p< .01) with
Self-Ordered Pointing Task (Shimamura & Jurica, each other test in its assigned domain. The average
1994); Wechsler Memory Scale, Third Edition: "Anna intra-domain correlation was .443 {SD = .092) while the
Thompson" paragraph and Digit Span subtests average inter-domain correlation was .364 (5D = .148).
(Wechsler, 1997); Wechsler Memory Scale, Form I The intra-domain correlations for each domain were as
44 DAVID L. NYENHUIS ET AL.

Table 1. Neuropsychological Tests Listed by Domain. tion Memory domain score was removed from subse-
quent analyses.
Basic attention Cognitive impairment was rated in the following
Digits Forward domains for each subject: basic attention, working
WMS Mental Control memory, language, spatial skills, psychomotor skills,
Working memory orientation and memory. This rating was a clinical
Self-Ordered Pointing Test Total Score decision made by an experienced, board-certified
Digit Span Backward neuropsychologist (ABPP/ABCN) and based on nor-
Behavioral Dyscontrol Scale mative data and subject demographic characteristics.
When normative data were applicable, test performance
Language was judged as impaired if it was below the 5th
BDAE Commands percentile for persons of similar age and educational
Boston Naming Test background. Occasionally, a subject differed signifi-
Animal Naming cantly from a test's standardization sample (e.g., there
Spatial was a large deviation between the subject's reported
Figural Recognition Test education and that of the normative sample or English
Ravens Matrices [while fluently spoken] was a second language for the
subject). When this occurred, a correction of up to one-
Psychomotor half of a standard deviation (based on the test's
Grooved Pegboard (pegs/sec) standardization sample) was applied to the score when
Symbol Digit Oral making the impairment decision. The overall perfor-
mance rating in a domain was based on the preponder-
Memory: Immediate
ance of scores within the domain, taking into account
Controlled Learning and Enhanced Recall Total
the degree of impairment severity on each test and the
WMS-III Paragraph I
number of impaired tests in the domain.
Memory: Delayed Because the impairment decision was, in part, based
Controlled Learning and Enhanced Recall on clinical judgment and was not entirely based on preset
Delayed Recall cut scores, we examined the inter-rater reliability and
WMS-III Paragraph I Delayed Recall the validity of the impairment decision. To assess the
reliability, a second neuropsychologist (CS) was trained
Memory: Recognition
in study methods and independently rated 25 subjects
Controlled Learning and Enhanced Recall
chosen at random from the study sample. He there-
Delayed Recognition
fore made 175 impairment/non-impairment ratings
WMS-III Paragraph I Delayed Recognition
(25 subjects x 7 cognitive domains). There was indepen-
Depression dent agreement on 166 of 175 ratings, yielding a Kappa
Chicago Multiscale Depression Inventory - Mood score of .79, suggestive of "substantial agreement"
(Landis & Koch, 1977) between raters and reliable
Apathy
placement of subjects into impairment categories.
Frontal Systems Behavior Scale
To examine the validity of the method, we compared
Dysexecutive the Vascular CIND group's (a group largely defined by
Frontal Systems Behavior Scale the impairment rating decision) performance to the NCI
group on two tests that were not considered when
Disinhibition making impairment decisions: the MMSE and the
Frontal Systems Behavior Scale Blessed IMC Test. The Vascular CIND group was more
Note. WMS = Wechsler Memory Scale. impaired than the NCI group on both tests, as well as on
BDAE = Boston Diagnostic Aphasia Exami- two measures of functional ability (Table 2).
nation. Diagnoses and Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR) were
completed at a diagnostic consensus case conference,
attended by the neurologist, neuropsychologist and the
follows: Basic attention: .405; Working memory: .388; epidemiologic interviewer. Based on cognitive, func-
Language: .401; Visuospatial: .403; Psychomotor: .653: tional (activities of daily living scale scores) and other
Itnmediate Memory: .497; Delayed Recall Memory: clinical data, patients were diagnosed with vascular de-
.546. These results demonstrate the inter-relationship of mentia (VaD), vascular cognitive impairment-no demen-
all cognitive tests and a higher degree of relationship tia (Vascular CIND), or no cognitive impairment (NCI).
within than across cognitive domains. Because of the VaD was diagnosed according to NINDS-AIREN Criteria
lack of intra-domain correlation, the Delayed Recogni- (Roman et al, 1993). Vascular CIND was diagnosed
NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL DEHCrrS IN VASCULAR CIND 45

Table 2. Demographic and Summary Cognitive and above (Table 1). To allow for inter-domain comparison, a
Functional Scores by Diagnosis. principal component score was calculated for each
domain, based on all subjects' test scores. All domain
Vascular NCI scores were standardized to have a mean of zero and a
CIND in = 62) standard deviation of one. Estimated effects of domain
scores are then comparable as the effect of one standard
deviation increase. For domains with a single variable,
Male (%) 54 52 the score was simply that variable standardized. For
Age (years) 65.6 (9.7) 63.8 (8.4) domains with two variables, both variables were first
Years of education*** 12.71 (3.1) 15.1 (3.7) standardized and then weighted equally to form a mean
CDR*** 0.54 (0.52) 0.10 (0.22) value with a mean of one and a standard deviation of zero.
MMSE*** 26.5 (2.3) 28.5 (1.3) For domains with more than two variables, the first
Blessed** 3.3 (2.7) 1.7 (1.9) principle component score provided optimal weights
Barthel ADL* 95.2 (15.6) 99.8 (1.3) to produce a score which captured the most variation
Total IADL* 12.3 (3.3) 13.6(1.1) possible.
r-tests and Chi-square analyses were completed to
Note. ADL = Activities of Daily Living. compare groups on demographic variables. Correla-
CDR = Clinical Dementia Rating. tional analyses were used to test a priori assumptions
IADL = Instrumental Activities of Daily Living. about intra-domain tests. Stepwise logistic regression
MMSE = Mini-Mental State Examination. analyses were completed to test models of neuropsy-
*/7<.05;**/7<.01:***p<.001. chological patterns.

according to the method of Rockwood and his colleagues


in the Canadian Study of Health and Aging (Ebly, Hogan, RESULTS
& Parhad, 1995), which requires that patients not meet
dementia criteria, but do present with cognitive impair-
ment in at least one domain. NCI was diagnosed for The Vascular CIND and NCI groups did not differ
patients who were not impaired in any cognitive domain. in age or gender distribution (Table 2). The NCI
group completed more years of education than the
Statistical Analyses Vascular CIND group. As expected, the Vascular
Neuropsychological tests were grouped a priori into CIND group showed greater cognitive and
cognitive domains, based on test content as is explained functional deficits than the NCI group, as

Table 3. Principle Component Scores by Domain and Diagnosis.

Vascular CIND (n = 41) NCI (n = 62) Difference

Cognitive Domains
Basic attention -0.26 (1.42) 0.17 (0.53) 0.43
Working memory -0.36 (1.20) 0.24 (0.77) 0.60
Language -0.53 (1.16) 0.35 (0.70) 0.88
Visuospatial -0.42 (1.38) 0.28 (0.47) 0.70
Psychomotor" -0.58(1.02) 0.40 (0.77) 0.98
Immediate memory -0.66 (0.86) 0.44 (0.83) 1.10
Delayed recall memory -0.66 (0.89) 0.43 (0.82) 1.09
Behavioral Domains
Depression 0.25 (1.11) 0.16 (0.89) 0.41
Apathy'' 0.02 (0.98) 0.02 (1.02) 0.04
Executive*" 0.07 (1.03) 0.05 (0.97) 0.12
Disinhibition** 0.16 (1.27) 0.11 (0.77) 0.27
Note, "n = 35 for Vascular CIND, n = 51 for NCI.
"n = 40 for Vascular CIND, n = 61 for NCI.
CIND = cognitive impairment-no dementia.
NCI = no cognitive impairment.
46 DAVID L. NYENHUIS ET AL.

measured by more impaired CDR, MMSE, sion analyses were completed to examine the
Blessed, Barthel and IADL scores. ability of each individual principal component
Principal component scores are listed by score to predict group membership, controlling
domain in Table 3. Visual inspection shows the for education (Table 4). All cognitive domains
greatest cognitive group differences to be in successfully predicted group membership with
immediate memory, delayed recall memory and the exceptions of basic attention and working
psychomotor speed, while the greatest behavioral memory. Of the behavioral variables, depressed
difference is in depressed mood. Logistic regres- mood showed a trend (p = .056) toward predicting
group membership.
Two step-wise multiple logistic regression mod-
Table 4. Effects of Principle Components Predicting els were tested to see which variables indepen-
Vascular CIND (n = 41) Versus NCI (n = 62) dently explained the greatest group membership
Adjusted for Years of Education.
variance. In the first, cognitive domain variables
OR Confidence p value were eligible for inclusion after years of education
interval was forced into the equation. Two domains,
immediate memory and psychomotor speed, were
Cognitive Domains
selected to the model (Table 5). A second model
Basic attention 0.54 (0.22-2.13) .208
Working memory 0.52 (0.35-1.03) .062 tested only the behavioral domains. After years of
Language 0.38 (0.21-0.67) .001 education was forced into the equation, only the
Spatial 0.37 (0.17-0.78) .009 depressed mood domain was selected.
Psychomotor^ 0.32 (0.17-0.61) .001
Immediate memory 0.24 (0.13-0.47) .000
Delayed recall memory 0.23 (0.12-0.45) .000 DISCUSSION
Behavioral Domains
Depressed mood 1.55 (0.98-2.43) .056 In this study. Vascular CIND patients were most
Apathy'' 0.85 (0.55-1.31) .458 impaired on tests of immediate memory and
Disinhihition"' 1.20 (0.77-1.85) .422 psychomotor speed when compared to post-stroke
Executive*" 0.99 (0.65-1.53) .970
patients without cognitive impairment. The two
Note. "^ = 35 for Vascular CIND, n = 51 for NCI. groups also differed on tests of language function-
"n = 40 for Vascular CIND, n = 61 for NCI. ing, spatial functioning and delayed memory, but
CIND = cognitive impairment-no dementia. these domains did not add additional explained
NCI = no cognitive impairment.
variance in our multivariate model. Tests of basic
attention and working memory did not differenti-
Table 5. Multiple Logistic Regression Models ate the two diagnostic groups. A separate model,
Predicting Vascular CIND Versus NCI. limited to behavioral symptoms, found depressed
mood, as measured by the Mood subscale of the
OR Confidence p value CMDI, to successfully differentiate the two
interval groups, with Vascular CIND subjects reporting
Cognitive Domains^ more depressed mood than NCI subjects.
Years of education 0.946 (0.80-1.12) .509 Decreased immediate recall memory, psycho-
Psychomotor 0.367 (0.19-0.72) .004 motor slowing and depressed mood have also
Immediate memory 0.284 (0.14-0.59) .001 been noted in patients with VaD (e.g., Looi &
Behavioral Domains"' Sachdev, 1999). In this way, the present pattern of
Years of education 0.775 (0.66-0.91) .002 results in our non-demented post-stroke patients
Depression 1.618 (1.02-2.56) .040 is similar to what has been seen in cerebrovascular
Note. ''n = 35 for Vascular CIND, n = 51 for NCI.
patients with more severe cognitive impairment.
"n = 40 for Vascular CIND, n = 61 for NCI. The lack of predictive value of working memory
CIND = cognitive impairment-no dementia. tests in our study was unexpected, as tests of
NCI = no cognitive impairment. working memory and executive function have
NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL DEHCITS IN VASCULAR CIND 47

been shown to be sensitive to VaD. This may be research should focus on a comparison of diag-
due to the specific tests of working memory that nostic criteria with an effort to reach consensus on
we chose to include in the study. Perhaps other this ongoing diagnostic problem.
tests of executive function, such as the Wisconsin We will continue to follow this cohort of post-
Card Sorting Test, the Stroop, or the Trailmaking ischemic stroke patients to examine the rate of
Test would have been more sensitive to deficits in cognitive and functional decline, risk factors for
this domain. Previous research has shown that decline, and the rate of "turnover" from Vascular
some Vascular CIND patients develop dementia CIND to VaD. We will also examine whether the
(Wentzel et al., 2001). These data, combined with pattern of neuropsychological impairment will
the present findings, suggest that Vascular CIND remain constant in patients who progress from
may be on a continuum between normal cognition Vascular CIND to VaD.
and VaD in post-ischemic stroke patients. Vascu-
lar CIND patients may therefore be well suited for
treatment studies designed to arrest the progres- ACKNOWLEDGMENT
sion of cognitive and functional decline in
patients with cerebrovascular disease. This work was supported by NIA ROl AG17934 (PI:
PBG).
Limitations of this study include the fact that
the same neuropsychological tests used to classify
patients into the Vascular CIND and NCI groups REFERENCES
were also used to examine the neuropsychological
deficit pattern. However, the classification proce- Babikian, V, Wolfe, N., Linn, R., Knoefel, J., & Albert,
dure was independent of the pattern analysis. It M. (1990). Cognitive changes in patients with
did not matter which cognitive domain(s) was multiple cerebral infarcts. Stroke, 21, 1013-1018.
impaired for diagnostic classification purposes. Benthem, PW., Jones, S., & Hodges, J.R. (1997). A
comparison of semantic memory in vascular
Therefore, classification results did not bias pat- dementia and dementia of the Alzheimer type.
tern analysis. Secondly, our patient sample con- International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 12,
sisted entirely of ischemic stroke patients. It may 575-580.
be that other vascular patient groups, such as Bogdanoff, B., Gitlin, H., & Norman, L. (1997). Neuro-
patients with extensive subcortical leukoencepha- psychological comparisons between subjects with
lopathy in the absence of stroke, may present with ischemic vascular dementia, Parkinson's disease, and
a different deficit pattern. Indeed, one of the Alzheimer's disease. Neurology, 48, A231-A232.
Buschke, H., & Grober, E. (1986). Genuine memory
challenges of vascular cognitive impairment is
deficits in age-associated memory impairment.
the heterogeneity of patients' presenting clinical Developmental Neuropsychology, 2, 287-307.
pictures. Thirdly, the study classified cognitive Chui, H., Victoroff, J., Margolin, D., Jagust, W.,
tests a priori into separate cognitive domains. The Shankle, R., & Katzman, R. (1992). Criteria for
bivariate correlational analyses supported the the diagnosis of ischemic vascular dementia pro-
classification. While a more comprehensive (and posed by the State of Califomia Alzheimer's Dis-
satisfying) method would be to complete explora- ease Diagnostic and Treatment Centers (ADDTC).
Neurology, 42, 473^80.
tory or confirmatory factor analysis, the study
Crossley, M., D'Arcy, C , & Rawson, N.S.B. (1997).
lacked a sufficient number of subjects to complete Letter and category fiuency in community-dwelling
such analyses. Canadian seniors: A comparison of normal partici-
The present study defined dementia using the pants to those with dementia of the Alzheimer or
NINDS-AIREN criteria. This affected who was vascular type. Journal of Clinical and Experimental
diagnosed with Vascular CIND. It is likely that a Neuropsychology, 19, 52-62.
greater percentage of patients would have been Desmond, D.W. (2002). Cognition and white matter
lesions. Cerebrovascular Diseases, 13, 53-57.
diagnosed with VaD using ADDTC criteria,
Ebly, E.M., Hogan, D.B., & Parhad, I.M. (1995).
resulting in fewer Vascular CIND patients. It is Cognitive impairment in the nondemented elderly:
not known how this might affect the neuropsy- Results from the Canadian Study of Health and
chological deficit pattern. Future Vascular CIND Aging. Archives of Neurology, 52, 612-619.
48 DAVID L. NYENHUIS ET AL.

Englund, E. (2002). Neuropathology of white matter Lamar, M., Podell, K., & Carew, T.G. (1997).
lesions in vascular cognitive impairment. Cerebro- Perseverative behavior in Alzheimer's disease and
vascular Diseases, 13, 11-15. subcortical ischemic vascular dementia. Neuro-
Evans, D.A., Funkenstein, H.H., Albert, M.S., Scherr, psychology, 11, 1-12.
P.A., Cook, N.R., Chown, M.J., Herbert, L.E., Landis, J.R., & Koch, G.G. (1977). The measurement
Hennekens, C.H., & Taylor, J.O. (1989). Prevalence of observer agreement for categorical data. Bio-
of Alzheimer's disease in a community population metrics, 33, 159-174.
higher than previously reported. Journal of the Libon, D.J., Bogdanoff, B., & Bonavita, J. (1997).
American Medical Association, 262, 2251-2256. Dementia associated with periventricular and deep
Folstein, M.F., Folstein, S., & McHugh, P (1975). white matter alterations: A subtype of subcortical
Mini-Mental state: A practical method for grading dementia. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology,
the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. 12, 239-250.
Journal of Psychiatric Research, 12, 189-198. Libon, D.J., Bogdanoif, B., & Cloud, B.S. (1998).
Foulkes, M.A., Wolf, RA., Price, T.R., Mohr, J.R, & Declarative and procedural learning, quantitative
Hier, D.B. (1988). The stroke data bank: Design, measures of the hippocampus, and subcortical white
methods, and characteristics. Stoke, 19, 547-554. matter alterations in Alzheimer's disease and
Fuld, PA. (1984). Test profile of cholinergic dysfunc- ischemic vascular dementia. Journal of Clinical
tion and Alzheimer-type dementia. Journal of and Experimental Neuropsychology, 20, 30-41.
Clinical Neuropsychology, 6, 380-392. Libon, D.J., Mattson, R.E., & Glosser, G. (1996). A
Geenen, E.J., Nyenhuis, D.L., Gencheva, E., & nine word dementia version of the California
Gorelick, PB. (2003). Comparison of VaD fre- Verbal Learning Test. Clinical Neuropsychology,
quency using NINDS-AIREN and ADDTC diag- 10, 237-244.
nostic criteria. Journal of the International Looi, J., & Sachdev, P (1999). Differentiation of
Neuropsychological Society, 9, 166. vascular dementia from AD on neuropsychological
Goodglass, H., & Kaplan, E. (1989). The assessment tests. Neurology, 53, 670-^78.
of aphasia and related disorders (2nd ed.). McPherson, S.E., & Cummings, J.L. (1996). Neuro-
Philadelphia: Lea and Fehiger. psychological aspects of vascular dementia. Brain
Gorelick, PB. (1995). Stroke prevention. Archives of and Cognition, 31, 269-282.
Neurology, 52, 347-354. Mendez, M.F., & Ashla-Mendez, M.A. (1991). Differ-
Gorelick, PB. (1997). Status of risk factors for ences between multi-infarct dementia and Alzhei-
dementia associated with stroke. Stroke, 28, mer's disease on unstructured neuropsychological
459-463. tasks. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neu-
Gorelick, PB., Brody, J., & Cohen, D. (1993). Risk wpsychology, 13, 923-932.
factors for dementia associated with multiple Morris, J.C., Heyman, A., Mohrs, R.C., Hughes, J.P,
cerebral infarcts: A case-control analysis in pre- van Belle, G., Fillenbaum, G., Mellits, E.D., Clark,
dominantly African-American hospital-based C , & the CERAD Investigators. (1989). The
patients. Archives of Neurology, 50, 714-720. Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer's
Gorelick, PB., Roman, G., & Mangone, C.A. (1994). Disease (CERAD). Part 1. Clinical and neuropsy-
Vascular dementia. In PB. Gorelick & M.A. chological assessment of Alzheimer's disease.
Alter (Eds.), Handbook of neuroepidemiology Neurology, 39, 1159-1165.
(pp. 197-214). New York: Marcel Dekker. Nyenhuis, D.L., & Gorelick, PB. (1998). Vascular
Grace, J., Stout, J., & Malloy, PF. (1999). Assessing dementia: A contemporary review of epidemiology,
frontal lobe behavioral syndromes with the Frontal diagnosis, prevention, and treatment. Journal of the
Lobe Personality Scale. Assessment, 6, 269-284. American Geriatric Society, 46, 1437-1448.
Grisby, J., & Kaye, K. (1996). Behavioral Dyscontrol Nyenhuis, D.L., Gorelick, PB., Freels, S., & Garron,
Scale: Manual. Denver, CO: Author. D. (2002). Cognitive and functional decline in
Hachinski, V.C. (1994). Vascular dementia: A radical African-Americans with VaD, AD, and stroke
redefinition. Dementia, 5, 130-132. without dementia. Neurology, 58, 56-61.
Kertesz, A., & Clydesdale, S. (1994). Neuropsychologi- Nyenhuis, D.L., Luchetta, T, Yamamoto, C , Terrien,
cal deficits in vascular dementia versus Alzheimer's A., Bemardin, L., Rao, S.M., & Garron, D.C.
disease: Frontal lobe deficits prominent in vascular (1998). The development, standardization and
dementia. Archives of Neurology, 51, 1226-1231. initial validation of the Chicago Multiscale Depres-
Kontiola, P., Laaksonen, R., Sulkava, R., & Erkinjuntii, sion Inventory. Journal of Personality Assessment,
T. (1990). Pattern of language impairment is 70,386-401.
different in Alzheimer's disease and multi-infarct Raven, J.C. (1995). Colored Progressive Matrices Sets
dementia. Brain and Language, 38, 364-383. A, Ab, B. Oxford, England: University Press Ltd.
NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL DEHCITS IN VASCULAR CIND 49

Rockwood, K., Ebly, E., Hachinski, V.C., & Hogan, D. Smith, A. (1991). Symbol Digit Modalities Test. Los
(1997). Presence and treatment of vascular risk Angeles: Westem Psychological Services.
factors in patients with vascular cognitive impair- Starkstein, S.E., Sabe, L., & Vazquez, S. (1996).
ment. Archives of Neurology, 54, 33-39. Neuropsychological, psychiatric, and cerebral blood
Rockwood, K., Howard, K., MacKnight, C , & Darvesh, flow findings in vascular dementia and Alzheimer's
S. (1999). Spectrum of disease in vascular cognitive disease. Stroke, 27, 408^14.
impairment. Neuroepidemiology, 18, 248-254. Thai, L.J., Grundman, M., & Golden, R. (1986).
Rockwood, K., Wentzel, C , Hachinski, V.C, Hogan, Alzheimer's disease: A correlational analysis of the
D., MacKnight, C , & McDowell, I. (2000). Pre- Information-Memory-Concentration Test and the
valence and outcomes of vascular cognitive impair- Mini-Mental State Exam. Neurology, 36, 262-264.
ment. Neurology, 54, 447-451. Villardita, C. (1993). Alzheimer's disease compared
Roman, G.C., Tatemichi, T.K., Erkinjuntti, T., with cerebrovascular dementia: Neuropsychological
Cummings, J., Masdeu, J., Garcia, J., et al. (1993). similarities and differences. Acta Psychiatrica
Vascular dementia: Diagnostic criteria for research Scandinavica, 87, 299-308.
studies. Neurology, 43, 250-260. Wechsler, D. (1945). A standardized memory scale
Russell, E., & Starkey, R. (1993). Halstead-Russell for clinical use. Journal of Psychology, 19,
Neuropsychological Evaluation System (HRNES). 87-95.
Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services. Wechsler, D. (1997). WMS-III administration and
Scherr, PA., Albert, M.S., Funkenstein, H.H., Cook, scoring manual. San Antonio, TX: The Psycholog-
N.R., Hennekens, C.H., Branch, L.G., White, L.R., ical Corporation, Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.
Taylor, J.O., & Evans, D.A. (1988). Correlates of cog- Wentzel, C , Rockwood, K., MacKnight, C , Hachinski,
nitive function in an elderly community population. V.C, Hogan, D.B., Feldman, H., Ostbye, T,
American Journal of Epidemiology, 128, 1084—1101. Wolfson, C , Gauthier, H., Verreau, H.R., &
Shimamura, A., & Jurica, P. (1994). Memory inter- McDowell, I. (2001). Progression of impairment in
ference effects and aging: Finds from a test of patients with vascular cognitive impairment without
frontal lobe function. Neuropsychology, 1,408-412. dementia. Neurology, 57, 714-716.

You might also like