You are on page 1of 8

Applied Neuropsychology: Adult

ISSN: (Print) (Online) Journal homepage: https://www.tandfonline.com/loi/hapn21

Toulouse-Piéron Cancellation Test: Normative


scores for the portuguese population

Marisa Lima, Élia Baeta, Diana Duro, Miguel Tábuas-Pereira, Daniela Valério,
Sandra Freitas, Mário R. Simões & Isabel Santana

To cite this article: Marisa Lima, Élia Baeta, Diana Duro, Miguel Tábuas-Pereira, Daniela
Valério, Sandra Freitas, Mário R. Simões & Isabel Santana (2023) Toulouse-Piéron Cancellation
Test: Normative scores for the portuguese population, Applied Neuropsychology: Adult, 30:2,
169-175, DOI: 10.1080/23279095.2021.1918694

To link to this article: https://doi.org/10.1080/23279095.2021.1918694

Published online: 13 May 2021.

Submit your article to this journal

Article views: 343

View related articles

View Crossmark data

Citing articles: 5 View citing articles

Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at


https://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=hapn21
APPLIED NEUROPSYCHOLOGY: ADULT
2023, VOL. 30, NO. 2, 169–175
https://doi.org/10.1080/23279095.2021.1918694

ron Cancellation Test: Normative scores for the


Toulouse-Pie
portuguese population
Marisa Limaa,b,c , Elia Baetad, Diana Duroa,b,c , Miguel Tabuas-Pereirab,c, Daniela Valerioa ,
Sandra Freitasa,e , Mario R. Simo~esa,e , and Isabel Santanab,c,f
a
Center for Research in Neuropsychology and Cognitive Behavioral Intervention (CINEICC), Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences,
University of Coimbra, Coimbra, Portugal; bNeurology Department, Centro Hospitalar e Universitario de Coimbra (CHUC), Coimbra, Portugal;
c
Center for Innovative Biomedicine and Biotechnology (CIBB), University of Coimbra, Portugal; dNeurology Department, Hospital Garcia de
Orta, Almada, Portugal; eFaculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences, University of Coimbra, Coimbra, Portugal; fFaculty of Medicine,
University of Coimbra, Coimbra, Portugal

ABSTRACT KEYWORDS
The Toulouse-Pieron Cancelation Test (TP) is a classic psychometric tool for the assessment of Healthy adults; normative
selective/sustained attention, processing speed and visuo-perceptual abilities. It is commonly used scores; processing speed;
in neurological disorders such as epilepsy, multiple sclerosis or Alzheimer’s disease. It encompasses psychometric tests;
selective/sustained
two main indexes: Work-Efficiency (WE) and Dispersion-Index (DI). The aim of this study is to pro- attention
vide normative scores for the TP in a sample of Portuguese healthy adults. The TP was adminis-
tered to a convenience sample of 357 cognitively-dwelling subjects aged between [45 and 86]
years old, following a standard assessment protocol. The normative scores were adjusted for age
and education. Education was the main predictor of TP-WE (R2 ¼ .310), whereas the influence of
age on this score was lower (R2 ¼ .191). These two variables explained 50.1% of the variance of
the results. Regarding TP-DI, education was also the main predictor of the results (R2 ¼ .039),
whereas age was responsible for R2 ¼ .011 and together, they explained 5% of the variance of TP-
DI. TP performances are strongly influenced by age and education. This is the first study focused
on the establishment of normative data after the age of 45 in the Portuguese population, allowing
a reliable assessment in both clinical and research contexts.

Introduction 
1904 by Edouard Toulouse for the evaluation of perception
and both selective and sustained attention is a very popular
Attention is one of the basic and more relevant domains of
psychometric test, largely dependent on the frontal lobe (do
neurocognitive functioning. The presence of attentional defi-
Amaral, 1967; see also Sim~ oes et al., 2016). The first studies
cits has a personal and social impact in daily living, hence
in Portugal were conducted by do Amaral in 1967, for the
the need to develop precise and accurate attention assess-
evaluation of attentional components and followed by Baeta
ment tools (Alberto, 2003; Muller et al., 2016). Attentional
deficits impair information processing at several levels, being (2002), when the test was included in a neuropsychological
involved in the more relevant deficits associated with normal battery for the assessment of adults with epilepsy. According
aging. An accurate characterization of possible attentional to a recent Portuguese study (Almeida, Sim~ oes, Almiro &
deficits is crucial for its diagnosis and understanding Santos, 2018), the TP is also currently the third most used
(Chauvin et al., 2016; Cohen, 2014). Despite several defini- neuropsychological instrument after the Rey Complex Figure
tions and subdivisions of attention capacity, this cognitive Test (Espırito-Santo, Lemos, Ventura, et al., 2015; Rey, 1959,
domain is usually divided in sub-domains regarding the type 1998) and the D2 Test of Attention (Brickenkamp, 1981;
of processing that is mainly involved: alertness/arousal, div- Brickenkamp & Schmidt-Atzert, 2019), with studies in
ided, selective and sustained attention (Strauss et al., 2006). healthy adults (Alchieri et al., 2002; Ara ujo, 2011; Montiel
There is also controversy about the accurate meaning of et al., 2006) or even in children between 10 and 15 years old
each one of these terms and some of them end up overlap- (Sim~ oes et al., 2016). Its clinical utility has been shown in
ping. As a consequence, the accuracy of attention-tools to several studies, in a wide range of neurological and psychi-
singly evaluate a specific domain is questionable and many atric disorders such as multiple sclerosis (MS) (Cunha,
of them involve other cognitive abilities such as motor and 2013), epilepsy (Baeta, 2002), depression (Kennedy, 2015;
information processing speed (Strauss et al., 2006). The Rock et al., 2014) and neurodegenerative and vascular disor-
Toulouse-Pieron Cancelation Test (TP), first developed in ders (McGuinness et al., 2010). It was also investigated in

CONTACT Marisa Lima marisalima5@hotmail.com Serviço de Neurologia – Hospitais da Universidade de Coimbra, Centro Hospitalar e Universitario de
Coimbra, Praceta Mota Pinto, Coimbra 3000-075, Portugal.
ß 2021 Taylor & Francis Group, LLC
170 M. LIMA ET AL.

the spectrum of Alzheimer’s disease (AD), namely mild cog- Materials


nitive impairment (MCI) (Lima et al., 2019). In this context,
 A sociodemographic questionnaire and a medical history
the rating of TP is based on the comparison of patient’s
and habits’ inventory.
scores with standardized data or percentiles, leading to
 The Mini Mental State Examination (MMSE; Folstein
results that are higher or below the average of the corres- et al., 1975; Guerreiro et al., 1994; Freitas et al., 2015)—
pondent normative cluster (Lezak et al., 2012). This high- Portuguese normative values were used for sample inclu-
lights the need to provide normative scores since in our sion. Subjects were excluded if their scores were
country robust normative values are still missing after the below 1SD.
age of 45. The main goals of this study are to demonstrate  The Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA; Nasreddine
the validity of this test, and further establish normative et al., 2005; Sim~oes et al., 2008; Freitas et al., 2011), was
scores. For this, we explored its psychometric properties used applying the normative scores proposed for the
and investigated the influence of sociodemographic variables Portuguese population according to age and education.
on the TP scores, using such results as criteria for stratifica- Subjects were excluded if their scores were below 1SD.
tion and establishment of normative values for the  The depressive symptoms were measured through the
Portuguese population. Geriatric Depression Scale—30 items, with a score of  11
points being indicative of the presence of depressive symp-
tomatology and consequently as study exclusion criteria
(GDS-30; Yesavage et al., 1982, Sim~ oes et al., 2013; Sim~
oes
Materials and methods et al., 2017).
Participants and procedures  Neuropsychological measures:
 The Trail Making Test (part A and B) (Cavaco et al.,
A community-based convenience sample of volunteers aged 2008, 2013), a measure of global cognitive dysfunction.
45 years and older was recruited from aging support groups Form A provides information about attention, visual
and senior universities. Caregivers and relatives of ambula- search, eye-hand coordination, processing speed,
tory patients with dementia followed at the Memory Clinic sequencing capability and cognitive flexibility. Form B
of a Portuguese tertiary hospital also contributed to our additionally evaluates executive functions, namely the
study. Several demographic and clinical inclusion criteria ability to switch between sequences. For this test we
were considered in the initial subject selection: aged 45 years recorded the total time (in seconds) to complete both
and older; Portuguese as their native language and formal part A and B, according to the Portuguese normative
schooling in Portugal; absence of significant motor, visual, study of Cavaco et al. (2013).
or auditory deficits that could influence performance on  The Stroop Color Test (conditions C and CW) (Espırito-
tests; autonomy in daily living activities; no history of alco- Santo, Lemos, Fernandes, et al., 2015; Fernandes, 2013;
holism or substance abuse; absence of neurological or psy- Trenerry et al., 1995). Condition C allows the assessment
chiatric diseases, as well as of chronic unstable systemic of selective attention and processing speed, whereas con-
disorders with impact on cognition; absence of significant dition CW adds the evaluation of inhibitory and fluid
depressive complaints and medication with possible impact abilities as well as cognitive flexibility, and it is related
on cognition. In order to implement and ensure these gen- with the semantic system. For this test we recorded the
eral criteria, the recruited subjects were interviewed by a total number of words correctly read (condition C), and
neuropsychologist using a standard clinical interview. This the total number of stimuli correctly referred (condition
interview included a sociodemographic questionnaire and CW), according to the Portuguese normative data of
data collection on habits, medical history and current medi- Espırito-Santo et al. (2015).
The TP can be administered individually or in group and
cation intake. For further inclusion in the study, all subjects
the completion time is exactly 10 minutes. The test consists
were required to display normal performance on other tools
of a blank sheet of paper with twenty-five lines and forty
of the cognitive assessment battery specifically composed for
small squares per line. The squares are distinguished from
this study and with accessible Portuguese-validated data (see
each other through the orientation of the rows on the outer
section “Materials”). To avoid circularity, the TP was never surface: in each square the stroke is oriented in eight pos-
used as a criterion for selection or classification. Informed sible directions, similar to the wind rose. The subject is
consent was obtained from all subjects after the aims and required to cross out three targets presented in the header.
procedures of the investigation and confidentiality require- For each line the evaluator must register the total number of
ments were fully explained. In order to evaluate test-retest hits (H), i.e., the number of targets correctly crossed out by
reliability a sub-sample (n ¼ 70) was assessed at six-month the subject, errors (E), i.e., when irrelevant targets are
follow-up, randomly selected from the caregivers/relatives of crossed out in violation of the instructions (false-positives),
out-patients with dementia followed at our center. The pre- and omissions (O), i.e., when the targets are not crossed out
sent research complied with the ethical guidelines for human (false-negatives). The test presents two main outcomes:
experimentation stated in the Declaration of Helsinki and by Work Efficiency (WE), which sets up a measure of both the
the Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences of attentional and perceptual abilities of the subject with a
University of Coimbra Scientific Committee. maximum score of 375 points calculated by the following
APPLIED NEUROPSYCHOLOGY: ADULT 171

method: [WE ¼ H–(E þ O)]; and Dispersion Index (DI), cor- Table 1. Sociodemographic description (n ¼ 357).
responding to the percentage of mistakes committed during Total sample 1–4 years 5–9 years 10
(n ¼ 357) (n ¼ 112) (n ¼ 99) (n ¼ 146)
the test, calculated by the following formula: [DI ¼ (E þ O)/
H  100]. This index allows a more precise and specific n % n % n % n %
interpretation of the scores obtained on TP-WE, namely if Gender
Male 152 42.6 49 43.8 48 48.5 62 40.2
they were primarily influenced by a pattern of global Female 205 57.4 63 56.2 51 51.5 86 59.8
response slowing or by selection mistakes, i. e., impulsivity Years old
(errors) or distractibility (omissions). Usually, lower per- 45–64 153 42.8 28 25.0 38 38.4 87 59.6
65–74 103 22.8 39 34.8 28 28.3 36 24.7
formances in the TP-WE are related with inattention and 75 101 22.4 45 40.2 33 33.3 23 15.7
global response slowing and high scores in TP-DI are Source
explained by distractibility or impulsivity patterns. When Senior universities 282 79.0 – – –
Aging support groups 49 13.7 – – –
there is a disturbance in flexibility, impairment in informa- Caregivers of ambulatory patients 26 7.3 – – –
tion processing or motor integration, the two indexes are
typically affected (Baeta, 2002). So, better performances in
the TP are a combination of high scores in TP-WE with Portuguese cut-off points. The final sample is composed by
lower scores in TP-DI. 357 cognitively healthy volunteers and the sociodemographic
characterization is presented in Table 1. One of the essential
challenges of this study was to obtain the best representa-
Statistical analysis tiveness of the real distribution of the Portuguese population
throughout the selected variables. Participants were stratified
Statistical analyses were performed using the Statistical
according to age and education. The sample was then div-
Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS), version 22.0 for
ided in three age groups: [45–64] (mean age ¼ 52.86 ± 5.44),
Windows. Normal distribution was assumed (n > 30), and
[65–74] (mean age ¼ 69.71 ± 3.60), and 75 (mean age ¼
multicollinearity was analyzed and showed to be adequate for
78.29 ± 3.21) and three education groups: [1–4] (mean edu-
all statistical procedures (Pallant & Manual, 2007). Descriptive
cation ¼ 3.96 ± 0.2) (primary school), [5–9] (mean education
statistics were used for the sample’s characterization.
¼ 8.02 ± 1.50) (middle school), and 10 (mean education ¼
Differences between subgroups according to sociodemographic
14.81 ± 2.89) (high school/university), since there were no
variables were examined using one-way between-groups ana- differences between groups on the TP performances after
lysis of variance (ANOVA) and Pearson’s v2 test, for numer- 10 years or more of formal education. The cognitive charac-
ical and nominal data, respectively. All tests were 2-tailed and terization of the sample can be found in Tables 2 and 3.
a p value < .05 was assumed to be statistically significant. Post The TP showed good convergent validity properties,
hoc Bonferroni and Tukey SD corrections were used to adjust obtained by Pearson’s correlation coefficient with MMSE,
for multiple pairwise comparisons (Cohen, 1988). Convergent MoCA, TMT and Stroop Test (Table 4).
validity was assessed by Pearson (r) correlation coefficients Test-retest reliability was assessed with Pearson’s correl-
between the TP total scores and MMSE, MoCA, TMT (part A ation coefficient between the baseline and six-month follow-
and B) and Stroop test conditions (C and CW) total scores. up data of a sub-sample (n ¼ 70) of our study population.
Test-retest reliability was also assessed with Pearson’s correl- The obtained values were r ¼ .865 (TP-DI) and r ¼ .908
ation coefficient between the baseline and the six-month fol- (TP-WE), both p < .01.
low-up data. Multiple linear regression (MLR) analysis (enter The analysis of differences between groups regarding
method) was performed to examine the influence of age (in sociodemographic variables showed that there were statis-
years) and education (years of formal schooling successfully tical significant differences between the three age groups in
completed). The coefficient of determination (R2) was consid- both TP-WE (F(2, 355) ¼ 23.57, p < .001) and TP-DI (F(2,
ered in the analysis of effect size in the regressions (Cohen,
355) ¼ 18.67, p < .001). Bonferroni and Tukey SD post hoc
1988). Finally, normative values of TP were stratified and tests revealed that the younger group [45–64 years old],
determined according to the sociodemographic variables most performed significantly better than the other two groups
significantly associated with TP scores showed by MLR ana- (p < .001), although the older groups did not differ from
lysis. The normative values are presented as means ± standard each other (p > .05). Statistical significant differences were
deviations (SD), and the distributions of means below (TP- also found for education in both TP indexes: TP-WE
WE) and above (TP-DI) 1 SD, 1.5 SD, and 2 SD. (F(2, 355) ¼ 77.96, p < .001) and TP-DI (F(2, 355) ¼ 49.57,
p < .001). Post hoc tests showed that the three groups sig-
Results nificantly differed from each other, with the most schooled
group ( 10 years), performing significantly better than the
From the initial community-based sample of 407 volunteers, others (p < .001). No statistically significant differences
3% (12 participants) were excluded following the interview between groups were found for gender (TP-WE: (v2(1) ¼
(the most frequent reasons were history of psychiatric or 4.500, p ¼ .051) and TP-DI: (v2(1) ¼ 9.500, p ¼ .060).
neurological disorder), and 9% (38 participants) were We found significant correlations between the total scores
excluded due to their performance on the assessment bat- of TP indexes (WE and DI), age and education. Statistically
tery, suggesting the presence of possible cognitive impair- significant correlations were observed between the TP-WE
ment or depressive symptomatology according to the and age (r ¼ –.598) and education (r ¼ .626), both p < .01.
172 M. LIMA ET AL.

Table 2. Cognitive characterization of the study sample.


MMSE MoCA TMT A TMT B
Final sample
(M ± SD) (29.43 ± 0.74) (25.96 ± 2.19) 55.81 ± 26.46 138.11 ± 84.86
[Min–Max] [28–30] [23–30] [21–174] [48–400]
Note. MMSE ¼ Mini Mental State Examination (Maximum score ¼ 30); MoCA ¼ Montreal Cognitive Assessment (Maximum score
¼ 30); TMT ¼ Trail Making Test.

Table 3. Cognitive characterization of the study sample. Table 5. Multiple Linear Regression (MLR) analysis for age and education.
Stroop C Stroop CW GDS Variable B SEB b p value
Final sample WE
(M ± SD) (61.89 ± 15.87) (181.84 ± 63.36) (6.86 ± 3.46) Education 5.038 .683 .456 < .001
[Min–Max] [48–80] [130–297] [1–11] Age –2.048 .285 –.311 < .001
Note. Stroop Color (C); Stroop Color-Word (CW); GDS ¼ Geriatric Depression DI †
Scale (Maximum score ¼ 30). Education –2.311 .928 –.156 < .001
Age 1.470 .805 .115 .021
Note. R2 ¼ .501; F(2,355) ¼ 99.314, p < .01; †R2 ¼ .050; F(2,355) ¼ 7.300, p <
Table 4. Summary of significant Pearson correlations obtained between the
.01. WE ¼ Work Efficiency; DI ¼ Dispersion Index; SEB ¼ Standard Error of B.
TP indexes and the remaining cognitive tests.
Neuropsychological measures TP-WE TP-DI
MMSE r ¼ .717 r ¼ –.617 Table 6. Normative scores of the TP-WE according to age and education.
MoCA r ¼ .520 r ¼ –.620
Work-efficiency (WE) Education
TMT A r ¼ –.565 r ¼ .486
Primary middle High
TMT B r ¼ –.524 r ¼ .422
Age (1–4) (5–9) (10) All education
Stroop C r ¼ –.551 r ¼ .451
Stroop CW r ¼ –.440 r ¼ .326 [45–64]
n 32 37 43 112
Note. p < .01; MMSE ¼ Mini Mental State Examination; MoCA ¼ Montreal
M ± SD 140 ± 47 167 ± 46 191 ± 46 170 ± 50
Cognitive Assessment; TMT ¼ Trail Making Test.
SD 93, 70, 46 121, 98, 75 145, 122, 99 120, 95, 70
[65–74]
The opposite pattern was found for TP-DI: r ¼ .123, with n 36 24 39 99
M ± SD 92 ± 23 106 ± 24 147 ± 44 115 ± 30
age and r ¼ –.360, with education, both p < .01. No statis- SD 69, 52, 36 82, 70, 58 103, 79, 56 85, 69, 54
cally significant correlations were found between gender and 75
the total scores of TP indexes (WE and DI), all p > .5. We n 85 42 19 146
M ± SD 80 ± 20 95 ± 25 125 ± 41 100 ± 29
then proceeded with a MLR analysis (enter method) in SD 60, 50, 40 70, 57, 45 84, 63, 43 71, 56, 41
order to examine the contribution of the significant sociode- All age
n 153 103 101 357
mographic variables (age and educational level), to the M ± SD 104 ± 30 123 ± 32 154 ± 44 128 ± 36
indexes of the TP and to assess the added contributions of SD 74, 59, 44 91, 74, 58 110, 88, 66 92, 74, 56
their interactions. The intercorrelations of the predictive var- Note. WE values are presented below 1 SD, 1.5 SD, and 2 SD, respectively.
iables can be found on Table 5. For TP-WE, age and educa-
tion are responsible for 50.1% of the results variance
Table 7. Normative scores of the TP-DI according to age and education.
(education: 31% and age: 19.1%). For TP-DI, these two vari-
Dispersion Index (DI) (%) Education
ables together explain 5% of the results variance (education: Primary Middle High
3.9% and age: 1.1%). Age (1–4) (5–9) (10) All education
In line with the obtained results, we considered age and [45–65]
n 32 37 43 112
education for the development of normative scores for the M ± SD 21 ± 9 15 ± 12 12 ± 9 16 ± 10
TP in the Portuguese population. Normative scores were SD 30, 35, 39 27, 33, 39 21, 26, 30 26, 31, 36
stratified according to the distributional properties of each [65–75]
n 36 24 39 99
variable. We considered the referred three educational M ± SD 26 ± 10 25 ± 8 21 ± 10 24 ± 9
groups (1–4 years, 5–9 years and 10 years) and three age SD 36, 41, 46 33, 37, 41 31, 37, 41 33, 38, 42
groups: 45–64, 65–74, and 75 years old. The TP scores are  75
n 85 42 19 146
expressed as the M ± SD); values below (TP-WE) and above M ± SD 28 ± 16 25 ± 14 25 ± 10 26 ± 13
(TP-DI) 1 SD, 1.5 SD and 2 SD can be indicated as cutoff SD 44, 52, 60 34, 41, 53 35, 40, 45 39, 46, 52
points for possible cognitive impairment (Tables 6 and 7). All age
n 153 103 101 357
Finally, we established the same norms to all education lev- M ± SD 25 ± 12 22 ± 11 19 ± 10 22 ± 11
els and all age to enable its use in cases where sociodemo- SD 37, 43, 49 33, 39, 44 29, 34, 39 33, 39, 44
graphic information is missing or uncertain. Note. DI values are presented above 1 SD, 1.5 SD, and 2 SD, respectively.

Discussion
reflect a community-based sample of cognitively healthy vol-
In this study we analyzed the influence of sociodemographic unteers that were stratified according to the most significant
variables on TP scores and were able to determine the nor- sociodemographic variables. Besides, this study highlights the
mative values for the Portuguese population. The results importance of having both validity and normative studies of
APPLIED NEUROPSYCHOLOGY: ADULT 173

more than a single neuropsychological test that allows the physical education found no differences for sex or age in
assessment of the same cognitive domain, namely attention. any of two evaluated indexes (TP-WE and TP-DI), corrobo-
Despite the worldwide use of the TP as a primary meas- rating the previous study of Maureira et al. (2014) with a
ure of attentional, perceptual and processing speed abilities, small sample of 15 students, where the same pattern was
few studies have analyzed the wide variety of sociodemo- primarily described. In 2011, Ara ujo conducted a normative
graphic and health variables that might influence its results. study with 701 participants and observed that the ANOVA
Our study allows to expand the knowledge about the test, analysis based on age, gender and education showed statis-
leading to a more accurate and reliable use of this psycho- tically significant differences in TP-WE on basis of educa-
metric tool. In order to demonstrate the clinical value of the tion only. Also, Montiel et al. (2006) found no differences
instrument, we firstly explored its psychometric properties. between subjects according to age on the TP-DI, and the
As expected, we observed significant positive correlations differences found for TP-WE were not corroborated by
between TP-WE total scores and both MMSE and MoCA Tukey SD post hoc analysis. Alchieri et al. (2002), stated that
total scores, and between TP-DI and both TMT-A and B there were no differences between the age groups on TP
and Stroop C and CW total scores. Moreover, significant total scores. We believe that the reason why no influence of
negative correlations were found between TP-WE and TMT age was found in the above-described studies is the very low
A and B and Stroop C and CW total scores as well as percentage of older adults on the samples, since two of them
between TP-DI and MMSE and MoCA total scores. All (Maureira et al., 2014, 2019) only included young students
together, these results support the convergent validity of this in their twenties and the remaining included a small number
test (Cohen, 1988). Regarding common measures of reliabil- of participants aged 50 and older. As previously described
ity, we explored test-retest reliability through Pearson’s cor- (e.g., Fortenbaugh et al., 2015), in the process of normal
relation coefficient between the baseline and six-month aging there is a performance peak in the early 40’s, followed
follow-up data of 70 individuals, and obtained high values by a gradual decrease in older adults, particularly related to
for both TP-WE and TP-DI indexes. Interrater reliability execution time/processing speed. Focusing on gender, it was
was not assessed in this study since TP has a single standard not a significant predictor of the TP total scores, which is
instruction before the beginning of the test and no further consistent with prior studies (Baeta, 2002; Rebollo &
prompting is required throughout the 10 minutes. Montiel, 2006), where this demographic variable did not sig-
Similar to previous studies conducted with the TP in our nificantly contribute to data distribution. Despite the fact
country (Baeta, 2002; do Amaral, 1967) TP-WE total scores that the MLR analysis results showed a minor influence of
increased with higher education and the opposite occurs age on the total scores of the TP, and once we followed a
with the TP-DI. Furthermore, we found that the educational regression-based approach to calculate normative data, we
level was a better predictor of the TP scores than age (31% decided to consider both sociodemographic variables (age
vs. 19.1% for TP-WE and 3.9% vs 1.1% for TP-DI). The and education) for the establishment of normative values for
higher influence of education compared to age was an the Portuguese population. Together, these variables contrib-
expected result considering our previous experience with ute significantly to the prediction of the total scores,
other screening tests in our country, such as the MMSE and explaining 50.1% of the results variance on TP-WE and 5%
MoCA, where education has been the strongest predictor, on TP-DI. We further determined the means and SD for
followed by age (Freitas et al., 2011, 2015). For this reason, each sub-group, crossing the three education and
in Portugal, we need to develop normative data considering age groups.
age and education groups instead of international standard This study has some limitations. Considering the low rep-
cut-off points for cognitive decline. Considering the greater resentativeness of illiterates in the Portuguese population
contribution of education to the total scores of the neuro- nowadays we chose not include those participants, although
psychological scoring in our country (Baeta, 2002; Cavaco the absence of a verbal component in the TP represents an
et al., 2013; Faustino et al., 2020), it is known that the advantage for its use in this population. Besides, it is
Portuguese society a few decades ago was critically influ- important to point out that, although the TP is one of the
enced by the achieved educational level which was determin- most used psychometric tests to assess attentional, percep-
ant of the attained socio-economic status, job access, culture tual and processing speed abilities, no clinical diagnosis
and long-life learning. More recently, the educational setting should be made based solely on its scores. This test should
has quickly changed as result of the rearrangement of the be integrated in a comprehensive neuropsychological assess-
school system and the definition of higher mandatory edu- ment battery at a well-defined timepoint application,
cational levels for all. Those changes are already reflected in respecting the conditions of the present study in order to
the youngest groups of our study sample. However, the guarantee that normative data are still applicable.
older group remains characterized by lower educational lev- This study has an important set of strengths. Our results
els. Our sample distribution could not fully remove this dis- were obtained in a community-sample of cognitively healthy
similarity in terms of number of participants per group, volunteers that were stratified according to the most signifi-
however, the achieved partition is quite close to the real cant sociodemographic variables and addressed the import-
one. Considering age, other international studies reported ance of considering the influence of age and education on
no influence of this variable on the subject’s performance: test performances, reflected on the establishment of norma-
Maureira et al. (2019) in a sample of 125 students of tive data for the Portuguese population according to the
174 M. LIMA ET AL.

combination of these two variables. These normative scores Araujo, R. S. (2011). Teste de atenç~ao concentrada Toulouse-Pieron:
will be useful in both clinical and research contexts, where Atualizaç~ao dos estudos de padronizaç~ao, validade e precis~ao
[Toulouse-Pieron attention test: Updating of standardization, validity
the TP has been increasingly used not only in normal aging and reliability studies [Master Thesis]. Universidade de S~ao Paulo,
but also in neurological conditions such as MCI, AD, MS S~ao Paulo, Brasil.
and epilepsy (Baeta, 2002; Cunha, 2013; Lima et al., 2019). Baeta, E. (2002). Bateria para avaliaç~ao neuropsicologica de adultos
It has also been widely used as part of the psychological com epilepsia [Battery for neuropsychological assessment of adults
assessment batteries for the renewal of driving licenses with epilepsy]. PSICOLOGIA, 16(1), 79–96. https://doi.org/10.17575/
rpsicol.v16i1.470
(Dora et al., 2018; Sandu, 2014), and also as a useful indica- Brickenkamp, R. (1981). Test d2 Aufmerksamkeits-Belastungs-Test (7th
tive tool of subject’s performances in recruitment and selec- ed.). Hogrefe.
tion contexts (Alchieri et al., 2002). In future studies, it Brickenkamp, R., & Schmidt-Atzert, L. (2019). d2-R Teste de Atenç~ao –
would be important to conduct specific validation studies Ediç~ao Revista [d2-R Test of Attention - Revised]. Hogrefe.
for neurological conditions with a known impact on atten- Cavaco, S., Gonçalves, A., Pinto, C., Almeida, E., Gomes, F., Moreira,
I., Fernandes, J., & Teixeira-Pinto, A. (2013). Trail Making Test:
tion such as MS, vascular dementia or depression, promot- Regression-based norms for the Portuguese population. Archives of
ing a more accurate use of this instrument in Clinical Neuropsychology, 28(2), 189–198. https://doi.org.10.1093/
clinical practice. arclin/acs115
Cavaco, S., Pinto, C., Gonçalves, A., Gomes, F., Pereira, A., &
Malaquias, C. (2008). Trail Making Test: Dados normativos dos 21
Conclusion aos 65 anos [Trail Making Test: Normative data from 21 to 65
years]. Psychologica, 49, 221–238.
This is the first study focused on the establishment of nor- Chauvin, J., Gillebert, C., Rohenkohl, G., Humphreys, W., & Nobre, C.
mative data for the TP in the Portuguese population after (2016). Temporal orienting of attention can be preserved in normal
aging. Psychology and Aging, 31(5), 442–455. https://doi.org.10.
the age of 45. The results emphasize the association of both
1037/pag0000105
education and age in TP performances. The higher influence Cohen, R. A. (2014). Historical and philosophical antecedents. In R. A.
of education compared to age was an expected result consid- Cohen (Ed.), The neuropsychology of attention (pp. 11–17). Springer.
ering our previous experience with other neuropsychological Cohen, R. J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences
instruments in our country, such as MMSE and MoCA, (2nd ed.). Erlbaum.
Cunha, C. (2013). Defice de Atenç~ao na Esclerose M ultipla [Attentional
where education has been the strongest predictor. Besides,
deficit in Multiple Sclerosis]. (Master Thesis). Faculty of Medicine,
across the normal aging process there is a gradual perform- University of Coimbra, Coimbra, Portugal.
ance decrease related to execution time/processing speed do Amaral, J. R. (1967). O teste da barragem de Toulouse e Pieron:
abilities which may explain the effect of age on TP perform- Elementos de aferiç~ao para a populaç~ao portuguesa [The cancellation
ance in older groups. test of the Toulouse and Pieron: Standardization elements for the
Portuguese population]. Fundaç~ao Calouste Gulbenkian.
Dora, H. M., Emese, S. H., & Eszter, C. (2018). Examination of the
attention distracting effects of different visual stimuli during driving
Disclosure statement
simulation task. Magyar Pszichologiai Szemle, 73(3), 357–373.
No potential conflict of interest was reported by the author(s). Espırito-Santo, H., Lemos, L., Fernandes, D., Cardoso, D., Neves, C. S.,
Caldas, L., & Daniel, F. (2015). Teste de Stroop [Stroop test]. In
Mario R. Sim~ oes, Isabel Santana, & Grupo de Estudos de
ORCID Envelhecimento Cerebral e Dem^encia (GEECD) (Coords.), Escalas e
Testes na Dem^encia [Scales and Tests in Dementia]. (3a ed., pp.
Marisa Lima http://orcid.org/0000-0003-4200-9284 110–115). Novartis.
Diana Duro http://orcid.org/0000-0002-9818-9862 Espırito-Santo, H., Lemos, L., Ventura, L., Moitinho, S., Pinto, A. L.,
Daniela Valerio http://orcid.org/0000-0001-8062-5817 Rodrigues, F., & Daniel, F. B. (2015). Teste da Figura Complexa de
Sandra Freitas http://orcid.org/0000-0002-3022-5353 Rey-Osterrieth [Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test]. In Mario R.
Mario R. Sim~oes http://orcid.org/0000-0002-1311-1338 Sim~ oes, Isabel Santana, & Grupo de Estudos de Envelhecimento
Isabel Santana http://orcid.org/0000-0002-8114-9434 Cerebral e Dem^encia (GEECD) (Coords.). Escalas e Testes na
Dem^encia [Scales and Tests in Dementia]. (3a ed.; pp. 100–107).
Novartis.
References Faustino, B., Oliveira, J., & Lopes, P. (2020). Normative scores of the
Wisconsin Card Sorting Test in a sample of the adult Portuguese
Alberto, I. (2003). Atenç~ao, por favor (!) a avaliaç~ao da atenç~ao population. Applied Neuropsychology, 2020, 1–8. https://doi.org/10.
[Attention, please (!) to the assessment of the attention]. 1080/23279095.2020.1810040
Psychologica, 34, 231–243. Fernandes, S. (2013). STROOP: Teste de Cores e Palavras: Manual
Alchieri, J. C., Lunkes, C. L. K., & Zimmer, D. (2002). Toulouse- [Stroop test: Manual]. CEGOC-TEA.
Pieron: Atualizaç~oes de resultados para o estado do Rio Grande do Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E., & McHugh, P. R. (1975). Mini-mental
Sul [Toulouse-Pieron: Results updated for the state of Rio Grande state”: A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients
do Sul]. Avaliaç~ao Psicologica, 1(2), 111–118. for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 12(3), 189–198.
Almeida, L. S., Sim~ oes, M. R., Almiro, P., & Santos, M. J. S. (2018). https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6
Praticas de avaliaç~ao psicologica em Portugal: Resultados gerais de Fortenbaugh, F. C., DeGutis, J., Germine, L., Wilmer, J. B., Grosso, M.,
inquerito internacional realizado sobre a utilizaç~ao de testes e outros Russo, K., & Esterman, M. (2015). Sustained attention across the life
instrumentos [Psychological assessment practices in Portugal: span in a sample of 10,000: Dissociating ability and strategy.
General results of an international survey carried out on the use of Psychological Science, 26(9), 1497–1510. https://doi.org.10.1177/
tests and other instruments]. Confer^encia plenaria apresentada no 0956797615594896
10 Congresso da Associaç~ao Ibero-Americana de Diagnostico e Freitas, S., Sim~ oes, M. R., Alves, L., & Santana, I. (2011). Montreal
Avaliaç~ao Psicologica (AIDAP/AIDEP). Coimbra, Portugal. Cognitive Assessment (MoCA): Normative study for the Portuguese
APPLIED NEUROPSYCHOLOGY: ADULT 175

population. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, Rebollo, M. A., & Montiel, S. (2006). Attention and the executive func-
33(9), 989–996. https://doi.org.10.1080/13803395.2011.589374 tions. Revista de Neurologia, 42, S3–S7.
Freitas, S., Sim~oes, M. R., Alves, L., & Santana, I. (2015). Mini Mental Rey, A. (1959). Manuel: Test de Copie dune Figure Complexe. Centre de
State Examination (MMSE): Normative study for the Portuguese Psychologie Appliquee.
population in a community stratified sample. Applied Rey, A. (1998). Teste de Copia de Figuras Complexas [Complex Figure
Neuropsychology: Adult, 22(4), 311–319. https://doi.org.10.1080/ Test] (adaptaç~ao portuguesa: A. Menezes Rocha, M. Helena Coelho).
23279095.2014.926455 Cegoc.
Guerreiro, M., Silva, A. P., Botelho, M., Leit~ao, O., Castro-Caldas, A., Rock, P. L., Roiser, J. P., Riedel, W. J., & Blackwell, A. D. (2014).
& Garcia, C. (1994). Adaptaç~ao a populaç~ao portuguesa da traduç~ao Cognitive impairment in depression: A systematic review and meta-
do Mini Mental State Examination [MMSE: Adaptation to the analysis. Psychological Medicine, 44(10), 2029–2040. https://doi.org/
Portuguese population]. Revista Portuguesa de Neurologia, 1, 9. 10.1017/S0033291713002535
Kennedy, G. J. (2015). The problem of depression in late life. In G. J. Sandu, M. C. (2014). Validating attention tests on external criterion on
Kennedy (Ed.), Geriatric depression: A clinical guide (pp. 2–18). driving school students – Pilot study. Procedia – Social and
Guilford.
Behavioral Sciences, 127, 913–917. https://doi.org.10.1016/j.sbspro.
Lezak, M. D., Howieson, D. B., Bigler, E. D., & Tranel, D. (2012).
2014.03.379
Neuropsychological assessment (5th ed.). Oxford University Press.
Sim~oes, M. R., Albuquerque, C. P., Pinho, M. S., Vilar, M., Pereira, M.,
Lima, M., Duro, D., Freitas, S., Sim~ oes, M. R., & Santana, I. (2019).
Lopes, A. F., Seabra-Santos, M. J., Alberto, I., Lopes, C., Martins, C.,
Validation study of the Toulouse-Pieron Cancellation Test for
Portuguese Patients with Mild Cognitive Impairment and & Moura, O. (2016). Bateria de Avaliaç~ao Neuropsicologica de
Alzheimer’s Disease. Sinapse, 19(1–2), 26–35. Coimbra (BANC): Manual de administraç~ao e cotaç~ao [Coimbra
Maureira, F., Bobadilla, A., Ramırez, M. A., & Fuentealba, D. (2019). Neuropsychological Assessment Battery (BANC): Administration
Valores normativos del test de Toulouse-Pieron de atenci on selectiva and scoring manual]. Cegoc.
y sostenida en una muestra de estudiantes de educaci on fısica de Sim~oes, M. R., Albuquerque, C. P., Pinho, M. S., Vilar, M., Pereira, M.,
Santiago de Chile. Revista Ciencias de la Actividad Fısica, 20(1), 1–7. Lopes, A. F., Seabra-Santos, M. J., Alberto, I., Lopes, C., Martins, C.,
https://doi.org.10.29035/rcaf.20.1.2 & Moura, O. (2016). Bateria de Avaliaç~ao Neuropsicologica de
Maureira, F., Trujillo, H., & Flores, E. (2014). Propiedades psicometri- Coimbra (BANC): Manual Tecnico [Coimbra Neuropsychological
cas y datos normativos del test de atenci on Toulouse-Pieron y del Assessment Battery: Technical manual]. Cegoc.
test de memoria visual de Benton forma D en estudiantes de educa- Sim~oes, M. R., Freitas, S., Santana, I., Firmino, H., Martins, C.,
on fısica de Chile [Psychometric properties and normative data of
ci Nasreddine, Z., & Vilar, M. (2008). Montreal Cognitive Assessment
the Toulouse-Pieron Attention Test and the Benton Form D Visual (MoCA). Serviço de Avaliaç~ao Psicol
ogica, Faculdade de Psicologia e
Memory Test in physical education students in Chile]. Gaceta de de Ci^encias da Educaç~ao da Universidade de Coimbra.
Psiquiatrıa Universitaria, 10(2), 238–245. Sim~oes, M. R., Prieto, G., Pinho, M. S., Sobral, M., Firmino, H., &
McGuinness, B., Barrett, S. L., Craig, D., Lawson, J., & Passmore, A. P. Grupo de Estudos de Envelhecimento Cerebral e Dem^encia. (2013).
(2010). Attention deficits in Alzheimer’s disease and vascular Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS-30). Laborat orio de Avaliaç~ao
dementia. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry, 81(2), Psicol
ogica e Psicometria, Faculdade de Psicologia e de Ci^encias da
157–159. https://doi.org/10.1136/jnnp.2008.164483 Educaç~ao da Universidade de Coimbra.
Montiel, J., Figueiredo, E., Lustosa, D., & Dias, N. (2006). Evid^encia de Sim~oes, M. R., Sousa, L. B., Pinho, M. S., Vilar, M., Prieto, G., &
validade para o teste de atenç~ao concentrada Toulouse-Pieron no
Firmino, H. (2017). Escala de Depress~ao Geriatrica (GDS) [Geriatric
contexto de tr^ansito. Psicologia: Pesquisa & Tr^ansito, 2(1), 19–27.
Depression Scale]. In Miguel M. Gonçalves, Mario R. Sim~ oes,
Muller, S., Rothermund, K., & Wentura, D. (2016). Relevance drives
Leandro S. Almeida (Coords.), Psicologia Clınica e da Sa ude:
attention: Attentional bias for gain-and loss-related stimuli is driven
by delayed disengagement. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Instrumentos de Avaliaç~ao [Clinical and Health Psychology:
Psychology, 69(4), 752–763. https://doi.org.10.1080/17470218.2015. Assessment Tools] (pp. 219–233). Pactor.
1049624 Strauss, E., Sherman, E., & Spreen, O. (2006). A compendium of neuro-
Nasreddine, Z. S., Phillips, N. A., Bedirian, V., Charbonneau, S., psychological tests. Oxford University Press.
Whitehead, V., Collin, I., Cummings, J. L., & Chertkow, H. (2005). Trenerry, M. R., Crosson, B., DeBoe, J., & Leber, W. R. (1995). Stroop
The montreal cognitive assessment, MoCA: A brief screening tool neuropsychological screening test: Manual. PsychologicaAssessment
for mild cognitive impairment. Journal of the American Geriatrics Resources.
Society, 53(4), 695–699. https://doi.org.10.1111/j.1532-5415.2005. Yesavage, J. A., Brink, T. L., Rose, T. L., Lum, O., Huang, V., Adey,
53221.x M., & Leirer, V. O. (1982). Development and validation of a geriat-
Pallant, J., & Manual, S. S. (2007). A step by step guide to data analysis ric depression screening scale: A preliminary report. Journal of
using SPSS for windows. SPSS Survival Manual. Open University Psychiatric Research, 17(1), 37–49. https://doi.org.10.1016/
Press. 00223956(82)90033-4

You might also like