You are on page 1of 9

This article was downloaded by: [Sandra Freitas]

On: 04 July 2011, At: 03:23


Publisher: Psychology Press
Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer
House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK

Journal of Clinical and Experimental


Neuropsychology
Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information:
http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/ncen20

Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA): Normative


study for the Portuguese population
a a a b c
Sandra Freitas , Mário R. Simões , Lara Alves & Isabel Santana
a
Psychological Assessment Department, Faculty of Psychology and Educational
Sciences, University of Coimbra, Coimbra, Portugal
b
Faculty of Medicine, University of Coimbra, Coimbra, Portugal
c
Neurology Department, Coimbra University Hospital, Coimbra, Portugal

Available online: 1 July 2011

To cite this article: Sandra Freitas, Mário R. Simões, Lara Alves & Isabel Santana (2011): Montreal Cognitive
Assessment (MoCA): Normative study for the Portuguese population, Journal of Clinical and Experimental
Neuropsychology, DOI:10.1080/13803395.2011.589374

To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13803395.2011.589374

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Full terms and conditions of use: http://www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-and-conditions

This article may be used for research, teaching and private study purposes. Any substantial or
systematic reproduction, re-distribution, re-selling, loan, sub-licensing, systematic supply or distribution
in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden.
The publisher does not give any warranty express or implied or make any representation that the
contents will be complete or accurate or up to date. The accuracy of any instructions, formulae and drug
doses should be independently verified with primary sources. The publisher shall not be liable for any
loss, actions, claims, proceedings, demand or costs or damages whatsoever or howsoever caused arising
directly or indirectly in connection with or arising out of the use of this material.
JOURNAL OF CLINICAL AND EXPERIMENTAL NEUROPSYCHOLOGY
2011, iFirst, 1–8

Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA): Normative


study for the Portuguese population
Sandra Freitas1 , Mário R. Simões1 , Lara Alves1 , and Isabel Santana2
1
Psychological Assessment Department, Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences, University
of Coimbra, Coimbra, Portugal
2
Faculty of Medicine, University of Coimbra, Coimbra, Portugal; Neurology Department, Coimbra
University Hospital, Coimbra, Portugal

The Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) is a brief cognitive screening instrument with good psychomet-
ric features and an excellent sensitivity in the early detection of mild cognitive decline. The MoCA was applied
to a community-based sample of cognitively healthy adults (n = 650), stratified according to sociodemographic
Downloaded by [Sandra Freitas] at 03:23 04 July 2011

variables (age, gender, educational level, geographic region, geographic localization, and residence area), with a
distribution similar to that observed in the Portuguese population. The normative data were determined accord-
ing to age and education as these were the sociodemographic variables that most significantly contributed to the
prediction of the MoCA scores, explaining 49% of their variance.

Keywords: Norms; Cognitive screening; Mild cognitive impairment; Alzheimer’s disease.

INTRODUCTION Strauss, 2004). In this context, in which dementia has a


significant impact on worldwide public health (Comas-
One of the most significant demographic changes of Herrera et al., 2011; Federal Interagency Forum on
recent decades is the decline of birth rates and the Aging-Related Statistics, 2010; Langa et al., 2001; World
increase in life expectancy. Demographic aging is a real- Health Organization, 2003), early diagnosis and screen-
ity, and the proportion of elders has increased rapidly in ing for cognitive impairment are extremely important.
most countries (Federal Interagency Forum on Aging- The cognitive screening tests remain the best method
Related Statistics, 2000), including in Portugal (European for the early detection of dementia in population stud-
Commission, 2010; Instituto Nacional de Estatística, ies (Cullen, O’Neill, Evans, Coen, & Lawlor, 2007; Ismail
2004, 2009). Aging is a major risk factor for dementia & Shulman, 2006), allowing the identification of individ-
and its most common form, Alzheimer’s disease (AD; uals in preclinical stages (Fabrigoule, Barberger-Gateau,
Barranco-Quintana, Allam, Castillo, & Navajas, 2005; & Dartigues, 2006). However, the results of these evalu-
Chen, Lin, & Chen, 2009; Herrera-Rivero, Hernández- ations can be compromised by deficient translations or
Aguilar, Manzo, & Aranda-Abreu, 2010; Luck, Luppa, cultural background adaptations and by lack of psycho-
Briel, & Riedel-Heller, 2010; Seshadri et al., 1997). metric validation of the instruments used.
Epidemiological studies indicate that rates of AD inci- The Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA;
dence and prevalence increase with aging, almost dou- Nasreddine et al., 2005; Simões et al., 2008) was
bling every five years after the sixth decade of life specifically developed for the screening of mild cog-
(Ferri et al., 2005; Jorm, Korten, & Henderson, 1987; nitive impairment (MCI), an intermediate clinical
McDowell, 2001; Wimo, Winblad, Aguero-Torres, & von state between normal cognitive aging and dementia

We thank Professor Cristina Martins for the English revision of the manuscript. This work was supported by the Fundação para a
Ciência e Tecnologia (Portuguese Foundation for Science and Technology) through a PhD fellowship (SFRH/BD/38019/2007) and by
the Fundação Calouste Gulbenkian (Calouste Gulbenkian Foundation) through the project “Memória, avaliação funcional e qualidade
de vida: Estudos de validade e normas para a população portuguesa” (Memory, functional assessment and quality of life: Validation
studies and norms for the Portuguese population; Proc. 74569, SDH-22 Neurociências).
Address correspondence to Sandra Freitas, Psychological Assessment Department, Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences,
University of Coimbra, Rua do Colégio Novo, Apartado 6153, 3001–802 Coimbra, Portugal (E-mail: sandrafreitas0209@gmail.com).
© 2011 Psychology Press, an imprint of the Taylor & Francis Group, an Informa business
http://www.psypress.com/jcen DOI: 10.1080/13803395.2011.589374
2 FREITAS ET AL.

that often precedes and leads to dementia (Cargin, medication with possible impact in cognition (e.g., psy-
Maruff, Collie, & Masters, 2006; Gauthier et al., 2006; chotropic or psychoactive drugs). In order to implement
Petersen, 2000, 2003, 2004, 2007; Petersen et al., 2001; and confirm these general criteria, the recruited subjects
Winblad et al., 2004). The MoCA is a one-page test were interviewed by a psychologist with a standard ques-
with a maximum score of 30 points, and it assesses tionnaire including a complete sociodemographic ques-
eight cognitive domains: executive functions; visuospa- tionnaire, an inventory of current clinical health status,
tial abilities; short-term memory; language; attention, and past habits and medical history. In the case of older
concentration, and working memory; and temporal participants, this information was always also checked
and spatial orientation (Nasreddine et al., 2005). The with a general practitioner, community center directors,
results of several studies (see studies in the official Web and/or an informant, usually an individual in cohabi-
Page to MoCA’s Group, http://www.mocatest.org) show tation or a close relative. For further inclusion in the
that the MoCA has good psychometric features and study, all the subjects were required to display normal
excellent sensitivity, with better results than the widely performance on other tools of the assessment battery
used Mini Mental State Examination (MMSE; Folstein, especially composed for this study and with accessible
Folstein, & McHugh, 1975) in the early identification Portuguese-validated data (see “Materials and proce-
of cognitive decline in the course of the disease. The dure”). The study was approved by the Fundação para a
use of the MoCA is widespread, having been adapted Ciência e Tecnologia [Portuguese Foundation for Science
and validated in 34 countries for several pathologies (see and Technology] and by the Faculty of Psychology
also http://www.mocatest.org), and it has become one of and Educational Sciences Scientific Committee, and an
the preferred brief screening tests (Appels & Scherder, informed consent was obtained from all the participants
Downloaded by [Sandra Freitas] at 03:23 04 July 2011

2010; Ismail, Rajji, & Shulman, 2009; Jacova, Kertesz, after the aims and procedures of the investigation had
Blair, Fisk, & Feldman, 2007; Lonie, Tierney, & Ebmeier, been fully explained by a member of the study group.
2009). From the initial community-based sample of the 936
The MoCA was translated, adapted, and validated for volunteers, 194 (20.73%) were excluded after the inter-
Portuguese population by our group (Freitas, Simões, view (most frequent reasons were history of neurological
Martins, Vilar, & Santana, 2010) and has been widely or psychiatric disorder and history of alcohol abuse),
used in clinical practice and in research. Despite previ- and 92 (9.83%) were excluded because of their perfor-
ous results supporting the excellent psychometric features mance on the assessment battery, suggesting the presence
and discriminant validity of the MoCA Portuguese final of cognitive impairment or depressive symptoms accord-
version, its clinical value is restricted by the lack of nor- ing to Portuguese cutoff points. The final sample was
mative data. The main objective of this study was to composed of 650 cognitively healthy adults that met all
obtain the MoCA’s normative data for the Portuguese the inclusion criteria defined. The stratification according
population. Norms allow interpreting test performance to sociodemographic variables confirmed that this final
in comparison with a reference group. We also wanted sample was representative of the distribution observed in
to confirm the applicability of the MoCA in this cultural the Portuguese population.
context and to evaluate the influence of the sociodemo-
graphic characteristics on the MoCA’s performance. For Materials and procedure
this, we used a large Portuguese community-based sample
stratified according to sociodemographic variables. All participants were assessed by two psychologists with
expertise in neuropsychological assessment. Besides the
Portuguese final version of MoCA, the following instru-
METHOD ments were administered for a global assessment of
each participant: a complete sociodemographic ques-
Study population tionnaire; an inventory of current clinical health status,
and past habits and medical history; MMSE (Folstein
A community-based sample of volunteers aged 25 et al., 1975; Guerreiro, 1998); Clinical Dementia Rating
years and older, living in all geographic regions of scale (CDR; Garret et al., 2008; Hughes, Berg, Danziger,
the Portuguese continental territory, and representa- Coben, & Martin, 1982; only for participants above 49
tive of the Portuguese population, was recruited at years); Irregular Word Reading Test (Teste de Leitura de
national health and social security services. Several demo- Palavras Irregulares, TeLPI; Alves, Simões, & Martins,
graphic and clinical inclusion criteria were considered 2009) for premorbid intelligence estimation; Subjective
in the initial subject selection: age 25 years and older; Memory Complaints scale (SMC; Ginó et al., 2008;
Portuguese as their native language and schooling in Schmand, Jonker, Hooijer, & Lindeboom, 1996); and
Portugal; absence of significant motor, visual, or audi- Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS-30; Barreto, Leuschner,
tory deficits, all of which may influence performance Santos, & Sobral, 2008; Yesavage et al., 1983). As men-
on tests; and to ensure that participants were cogni- tioned, sociodemographic and clinical data were obtained
tively healthy adults: autonomy in daily living activities; directly from the cognitively healthy subjects and were
no history of alcoholism or substance abuse; absence corroborated by a third party in the case of the older
of neurological or psychiatric diseases, as well as of participants. The autonomy in daily life activities was
chronic unstable systemic disorders with impact in cog- assessed through the CDR and was supplemented with
nition; absence of significant depressive complaints and the interview with the subject and with information
MoCA NORMATIVE STUDY 3

obtained through a general practitioner, community cen- TABLE 1


ter directors, and/or an informant. The depressive com- Sociodemographic characterization of the population sample
plaints were measured through clinical interview and
Variables Levels Sample n (%) Portugal a n (%)
Geriatric Depression Scale, excluding subjects with a
score of 20 or more points. Age (years) 25–49 214 (33.0) —
50–64 218 (33.5) —
≥65 218 (33.5) —
Sociodemographic variables
Gender Female 408 (62.8) 3,946 (52.6)
Male 242 (37.2) 3,559 (47.4)
The sample of 650 subjects was stratified according
the following sociodemographic variables: age, gender, Educational Primary 256 (39.4) 2,426 (36.6)
educational level, geographic region, geographic local- level Middle 170 (26.2) 2,280 (34.4)
ization, and residence area. The age intervals considered High 112 (17.2) 960 (14.5)
were the following: 25–49 (mean age = 38.12 ± 8.086), University 112 (17.2) 956 (14.5)
50–64 (mean age = 57.12 ± 4.199), and 65 and over years Geographic North 251 (38.6) 2,722 (36)
of age (mean age = 71.96 ± 5.433). Four educational lev- region Center 174 (26.8) 1,794 (24)
els were considered, according to the number of school Lisbon 164 (25.2) 2,091 (28)
years successfully completed in the Portuguese education Alentejo 44 (6.8) 577 (8)
system: 1–4 (primary education), 5–9 (middle school), Algarve 17 (2.6) 321 (4)
10–12 (high school), and over 12 years of education Geographic Coast 546 (84) 6,379 (85)
Downloaded by [Sandra Freitas] at 03:23 04 July 2011

(university/college), categories that match the divisions localization Inland 104 (16) 1,126 (15)
in the Portuguese school system. The Portuguese con- Residence PUA 446 (68.6) 5,103 (68)
tinental territory is divided into five geographic regions area MUA 112 (17.2) 1,200 (16)
(Nomenclature of Territorial Units for Statistics, NUTS PRA 92 (14.2) 1,200 (16)
II, classification): North, Centre, Lisbon, Alentejo, and
Algarve; furthermore, two geographic localizations were Note. PUA = predominantly urban areas; MUA = moderately
considered: coast and inland (Instituto Nacional de urban areas; PRA = predominantly rural areas.
a The values (n) of the Portuguese population are expressed in
Estatística, 2010). The residence area, which was cate-
thousands and represent data of the resident population in con-
gorized according to the types of urban areas (Instituto
tinental Portugal aged over 24 years (Instituto Nacional de
Nacional de Estatística, 2010), was divided into pre- Estatística, 2010).
dominantly urban areas (PUA), moderately urban areas
(MUA), and predominantly rural areas (PRA). the means ± standard deviations (SDs), and those of the
distributions are given as means below 1 SD, 1.5 SDs, and
2 SDs.
Statistical analysis

Statistical analyses were performed using the Statistical RESULTS


Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS), Version 17.0.
First, differences in the MoCA scores among subgroups The final sample is composed of 650 participants (mean
stratified according to sociodemographic variables were age = 55.84 ± 15.12 years, age range = 25–91; mean edu-
examined using Student’s t test, analysis of variance cation = 8.16 ± 4.72 years, education range = 2–27).
(ANOVA), and Tukey post hoc test. The influence of geo- The sociodemographic characteristics of the study sub-
graphic regions and residence area on the MoCA scores, jects are provided in Table 1. We can observe that the
considering the influence of age and education, was distribution of the sample in several strata is compara-
addressed with the analysis of covariance (ANCOVA). ble to the distribution of the target Portuguese popula-
Eta squared (η2 ) was used as an estimate of the effect tion. The participants’ performance in other tools of the
size (Cohen, 1988). The correlation between the MoCA assessment battery are summarized in Table 2.
scores, age, and education was investigated with the The MoCA showed internal consistency measured by
Pearson correlation coefficient (r; Cohen, 1988). Multiple a Cronbach alpha of .775, in this community sample.
linear regression (MLR) analysis, using the enter method, Furthermore, the analyses of the relationships between
was performed to examine the significance of age (in the MoCA score1 and the sociodemographic variables
years), and education (years of schooling completed suc- showed that age, F(2, 647) = 95.130, p < .001, educa-
cessfully) as influencing factors for the MoCA. The tional level, F(3, 646) = 194.996, p < .001, geographic
multicollinearity was examined through tolerance and region, F(4, 645) = 8.765, p < .001, and residence
variance inflation factor (VIF) statistics (Meyers, Gamst, area, F(2, 647) = 4.175, p = .016, all had significant
& Guarino, 2006). The coefficient of determination (R2 ) effects on the distribution of MoCA scores. There are
was considered in the analysis of effect size in the regres-
sions (Cohen, 1988). Finally, the norms of the MoCA
were stratified and determined according to the sociode- 1 The total score of the MoCA refers to the real score with-
mographic variables most significantly associated with out one correction point for education effects, considered in the
the MoCA scores. The normative data are expressed as original study (Nasreddine et al., 2005).
4 FREITAS ET AL.

TABLE 2
Performance of the population sample in battery assessment

Age levels (years) CDR (M) MMSE (M ± SD) SMC (M ± SD) GDS-30 (M ± SD)

25–49 0 29.47 ± 0.902 4.37 ± 3.627 6.59 ± 5.495


50–64 0 28.81 ± 1.266 6.21 ± 3.419 8.03 ± 5.384
≥ 65 0 28.39 ± 1.506 6.37 ± 3.398 7.36 ± 5.158

Note. CDR = Clinical Dementia Rating. MMSE = Mini-Mental State Examination. SMC = Subjective Memory Complaints scale.
GDS-30 = Geriatric Depression Scale.

no statistically significant differences in gender (t = 1.80, TABLE 4


p = .072) or geographic localization (t = 1.546, p = .122). Multiple regression analysis summary for age and education
We found differences in mean-age and mean-education predicting the MoCA scores
levels among subjects living in different geographic Variable B SE B ß
regions—age, F(4, 645) = 6.507, p < .001; education,
F(4, 645) = 15.675, p < .001—and residence areas— Age –.07 .01 –.29∗∗
education, F(2, 647) = 8,450, p < .001. We then pro- Education .40 .02 .52∗∗
ceeded to the analysis of covariance, in order to examine
whether differences on the MoCA scores remained sig- Note. MoCA = Montreal Cognitive Assessment. R2 = .49; F(2,
Downloaded by [Sandra Freitas] at 03:23 04 July 2011

647) = 317.016, p < .001.


nificant after controlling the effect of covariates (age and ∗∗ p < .01.
education). The differences on the MoCA scores among
subjects living in different residence areas were not sus-
According to results of the multiple linear regression
tained, F(2, 646) = 0.122, p = .885, ηp 2 = .000, and those
analysis, age and education were considered in the devel-
among subjects living in different geographic regions
opment of the normative data of the MoCA for the
showed a medium–low effect size, F(4, 643) = 4.972,
Portuguese population. The normative data were deter-
p = .001, ηp 2 = .030.
mined and stratified according to the distributional prop-
Multiple linear regression, using the enter method, was
erties of each variable. The MoCA scores are expressed
conducted to compare the independent influences of age
as the means ± SDs, and those of the distributions that
and educational level on the MoCA scores and to exam-
are below 1 SD, 1.5 SDs, and 2 SDs can be consid-
ine the additional contributions of these significant vari-
ered as cutoff points for possible cognitive impairment
ables and their interactions. Preliminary analyses were
(Table 5).
done to ensure no violation of the assumptions of nor-
mality, linearity, multicollinearity, and homoscedasticity.
The means, standard deviations, and intercorrelations
can be found in Table 3. Both variables significantly con- DISCUSSION
tribute to the prediction of the MoCA scores, F(2, 647) =
317.016, p < .001. The beta weights, presented in Table 4, This study analyzes the influence of sociodemographic
suggest that educational level contributes most to predict- variables on MoCA performance and provides the norms
ing the MoCA scores and that age also contributes to of the MoCA according to age and educational level
this prediction. The adjusted R2 value is .49, which indi- for the Portuguese population. The use of a representa-
cates that 49% of the variance on the MoCA scores was tive sample stratified according to various levels of each
explained by the model. sociodemographic variable with a distribution very close
to that observed in the Portuguese population enhances
the equivalence with the target population and the con-
fidence of the conclusions drawn. Age and educational
level were the sociodemographic variables that more sig-
TABLE 3
nificantly contributed to the prediction of the MoCA
Means, standard deviations, and intercorrelations for MoCA
and predictor variables scores, explaining 49% of the variance. This is consid-
ered a large effect, according to Cohen (1988), and a
Intercorrelation respectable result, according to Pallant (2007).
As expected, our results suggest that the total score
Variable M SD Age Education on the MoCA consistently increases with the educa-
tional level and decreases as age progresses. According
MoCA 24.70 3.668 –.522∗∗ .652∗∗
to previous studies of cognitive screening tests (Bravo &
Age 55.84 15.120 — –.438∗∗ Hébert, 1997; Gallacher et al., 1999; Han et al., 2008;
Education 8.16 4.724 — Langa et al., 2009; Lieberman et al., 1999; Mathuranath
Note. n = 650. MoCA = Montreal Cognitive Assessment. Age, et al., 2007; Measso et al., 1993; Moraes, Pinto, Lopes,
education: predictor variables. Litvoc, & Bottino, 2010; Morgado, Rocha, Maruta,
∗∗ p < .01. Guerreiro, & Martins, 2010; Nguyen, Black, Ray, Espino,
& Markides, 2002), our results confirm that older age has
MoCA NORMATIVE STUDY 5

TABLE 5
Normative data of the MoCA scores according to age and educational level

Educational level (years)

Age Primary (1–4) Middle (5–9) High (10–12) University (>12) All education

n 29 66 59 60 214
25–49 23.55 ± 2.56 26.42 ± 2.18 27.39 ± 1.86 28.83 ± 1.38 26.98 ± 2.55
SDa 21, 20, 18 24, 23, 22 26, 25, 24 28, 27, 26 24, 23, 22
n 91 59 33 35 218
50–64 21.78 ± 2.86 25.58 ± 2.25 26.61 ± 2.28 27.51 ± 2.13 24.46 ± 3.43
SDa 19, 18, 16 23, 22, 21 24, 23, 22 25, 24, 23 21, 19, 18
n 136 45 20 17 218
≥ 65 21.27 ± 3.37 24.60 ± 2.87 25.11 ± 1.94 26.35 ± 1.87 22.71 ± 3.60
SDa 18, 16, 15 22, 20, 19 23, 22, 21 25, 24, 23 19, 17, 16
n 256 170 112 112 650
All age (n) 21.71 ± 3.18 25.65 ± 2.50 26.77 ± 2.15 28.04 ± 1.94 24.70 ± 3.67
SDa 19, 17, 15 23, 22, 21 25, 24, 23 26, 25, 24 21, 19, 17

Note. MoCA = Montreal Cognitive Assessment.


Downloaded by [Sandra Freitas] at 03:23 04 July 2011

a MoCA values below 1 SD, 1.5 SDs, and 2 SDs, respectively.

a significant effect on MoCA performance, contributing We are not aware of previous studies in Portugal
to lower test scores. The influence of educational level where the influence of geographical variables on cognitive
on brief cognitive screening test performance is also screening tests performance was evaluated. Our results
widely reported in the literature (Bravo & Hébert, 1997; indicate that there are no statistically significant differ-
Gallacher at al., 1999; Han et al., 2008; Langa et al., ences between subjects living in the coastal and inland
2009; Lieberman et al., 1999; Mathuranath et al., 2007; areas. Furthermore, the differences observed between
Measso et al., 1993; Moraes et al., 2010; Morgado, residents in predominantly urban or rural areas were
Rocha, Maruta, Guerreiro, & Martins, 2009; Morgado not significant after controlling for the effect of edu-
et al., 2010; Nguyen et al., 2002), with the worst per- cation. The observed differences between residents in
formance in lower education levels and ceiling effects in different geographic regions also showed a reduced mag-
highly educated individuals. The magnitude of this effect nitude after controlling for age and education. In fact,
is so strong that education is invariably considered a these regional subgroups were not completely matched
criterion for the establishment of normative data for cog- for age and education, which may explain the obtained
nitive tests (Bravo & Hébert, 1997; Guerreiro, 1998; Han results.
et al., 2008; Mathuranath et al., 2007; Measso et al., 1993; We determined the means and standard deviations
Morgado et al., 2009). Because of the verified effect of for each subgroup result from the crossing of the var-
education on the MoCA performance, Nasreddine and ious educational and age levels. In addition, cutoffs of
collaborators (2005) included a correction point for indi- 1 SD, 1.5 SDs, and 2 SDs were also used to define
viduals with 12 or fewer years of education. However, the norms. Several cutoff points were calculated because
for the Portuguese population, as it is characterized by different cutoffs are presented in the literature, partic-
a considerable lower education level than the original ularly in MCI studies. This study provides appropriate
study population (mean education of Portuguese pop- normative data for the MoCA, but we cannot corrobo-
ulation = 8.16 ± 4.72; mean education of Canadian rate these data in other cultural settings, as there are no
sample = 13.33 ± 3.40; Nasreddine et al., 2005), the equivalent international studies that analyze the influence
correction point is not integrally applicable because of of sociodemographic variables on MoCA performance.
these differences in education found between the two Furthermore, in our country there are no studies with
samples. This is an additional reason why in this study national representative samples using the MoCA or any
the norms for the Portuguese population were calcu- other similar screening instrument. Recently new MMSE
lated and stratified according to the different educational normative data were established for the Portuguese pop-
levels. ulation (Morgado et al., 2009) according to age and
The influence of gender in screening tests is more con- educational level, but the study’s sample is exclusively
troversial in the literature. Some studies (Bravo & Hébert, composed of metropolitan area residents.
1997; Han et al., 2008; Measso et al., 1993) suggest The main limitation of the present study was the exclu-
the importance of this variable, while in other studies, sion of the illiterate subjects. The MoCA is a tool with a
gender does not contribute significantly to data distribu- significant impact of literacy in the performance. Several
tion (Lieberman et al., 1999; Mathuranath et al., 2007; MoCA items are unsuitable for subjects without formal
Morgado et al., 2009). In our study, gender did not reveal education. The maintenance of those items with a scor-
a significant effect on MoCA results. ing of zero would impact on the global scoring with a
6 FREITAS ET AL.

strong floor effect and possible loss of sensibility to detect Psychiatry, 12, 1008–1018. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1099-
the cognitive impairment. Furthermore, the MoCA was 1166(199710)12:10<1008::AID-GPS676>3.0.CO;2-A
Cargin, J. W., Maruff, P., Collie, A., & Masters, C. (2006).
developed for screening subjects with formal education,
Mild cognitive impairment in healthy older adults is dis-
having not covered illiterate subjects in the original study tinct from normal aging. Brain Cognition, 60(2), 146–155.
(Nasreddine et al., 2005) or in any other international doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2005.10.004
study with the instrument. The adequate assessment of Chen, J., Lin, K., & Chen, Y. (2009). Risk factors for dementia.
the illiterate subjects requires significant adaptation of Journal of Formosan Medical Association, 108(10), 754–764.
doi:10.1016/S0929-6646(09)60402-2
the items for this context or tools specifically developed Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sci-
with this objective. Moreover, the impossibility to match ences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
all the age subgroups in terms of education mean consti- Comas-Herrera, A., Northey, S., Wittenberg, R., Knapp,
tutes another limitation. The younger group shows higher M., Bhattacharyya, S., & Burns, A. (2011). Future costs
of dementia-related long-term care: Exploring future
education levels than the older group, which is represen-
scenarios. International Psychogeriatrics, 23(1), 20–30.
tative of the demographic profile of our country, where, doi:10.1017/S1041610210000025
in recent decades, the educational scenery has rapidly Cullen, B., O’Neill, B., Evans, J. J., Coen, R. F., & Lawlor, B.
changed as a result of the reorganization of the school A. (2007). A review of screening tests for cognitive impair-
system and the imposition of higher obligatory educa- ment. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry, 78,
790–799. doi:10.1136/jnnp.2006.095414
tional plateaus. These changes are already reflected in European Commission. (2010). The social situation in the
the younger strata of the population studied; however, European Union 2009. Retrieved August 27, 2010,
the older group continues to be characterized by a very from European Commission Employment, Social
low education level. As an illustrative example, 30 years Affairs and Inclusion website: http://epp.eurostat.
Downloaded by [Sandra Freitas] at 03:23 04 July 2011

ec.europa.eu/cache/ITY_OFFPUB/KE-AG-10-001/EN/KE-
ago, according to the 1981 Census (Instituto Nacional
AG-10-001-EN.PDF
de Estatística, 2010), the percentage of analphabetism Fabrigoule, C., Barberger-Gateau, P., & Dartigues, J. F. (2006).
in Portugal was close to 26.4%, and only 1.6% of the The PAQUID study. In H. A. Tuokko & D. F. Hultsch (Eds.),
population held a university degree. Mild cognitive impairment: International perspectives (pp.
Our study stresses the importance of considering age 31–56). New York, NY: Taylor & Francis.
Federal Interagency Forum on Aging-Related Statistics.
and education influences on test performances and pro- (2000). Older Americans 2000: Key indicators of well-being.
vides robust normative data for the MoCA. These ref- Retrieved August 27, 2010, from AgingStats website:
erence values are useful in both clinical and research http://www.agingstats.gov/agingstatsdotnet/Main_Site/Data/
contexts, where the MoCA has been increasingly used 2000_Documents/entire_report.pdf
Federal Interagency Forum on Aging-Related Statistics.
to assess cognitive dysfunction, and are furthermore a
(2010). Older Americans 2010: Key indicators of well-being.
baseline reference for further normative studies in other Retrieved August 27, 2010, from AgingStats website:
countries. http://www.agingstats.gov/agingstatsdotnet/Main_Site/Data/
2010_Documents/Docs/OA_2010.pdf
Original manuscript received 27 February 2011 Ferri, C. P., Prince, M., Brayne, C., Brodaty, H., Fratiglioni, L.,
Ganguli, M., et al. (2005). Global prevalence of dementia:
Revised manuscript accepted 23 April 2011
A Delphi consensus study. Lancet, 366(9503), 2112–2117.
First published online day month year
doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(05)67889-0
Folstein, M., Folstein, S., & McHugh, P. (1975). Mini-Mental
State: A practical method for grading the cognitive state of
REFERENCES patients for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research,
12(3), 189–198.
Alves, L., Simões, M. R., & Martins, C. (2009). Teste de Leitura Freitas, S., Simões, M. R., Martins, C., Vilar, M., &
de Palavras Irregulares (TeLPI) [Irregular Word Reading Santana, I. (2010). Estudos de adaptação do Montreal
Test]. Coimbra, Portugal: Serviço de Avaliação Psicológica Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) para a população portuguesa
da Faculdade de Psicologia e de Ciências da Educação [Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA): Portuguese adap-
da Universidade de Coimbra [Psychological Assessment tation studies]. Avaliação Psicológica, 9(3), 345–357.
Department, Faculty of Psychology and Educational Gallacher, J. E., Elwood, P. C., Hopkinson, C., Rabbitt,
Sciences, University of Coimbra]. P. M., Stollery, B. T., Sweetnam, P. M., et al.
Appels, B. A., & Scherder, E. (2010). The diagnostic accuracy of (1999). Cognitive function in the Caerphilly study:
dementia-screening instruments with an administration time Associations with age, social class, education and
of 10 to 45 minutes for use in secondary care: A systematic mood. European Journal of Epidemiology, 15(2), 161–169.
review. American Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease and Other doi:10.1023/A:1007576324313
Dementias, 25(4), 301–316. doi:10.1177/1533317510367485 Garret, C., Santos, F., Tracana, I., Barreto, J., Sobral, M.,
Barranco-Quintana, J. L., Allam, M. F., Castillo, A. S., & & Fonseca, R. (2008). Avaliação Clínica da Demência
Navajas, R. F. (2005). Factores de riesgo de la enfermedad [Clinical Dementia Rating Scale]. In Grupo de Estudos
de Alzheimer [Risk factors in Alzheimer disease]. Revista de de Envelhecimento Cerebral e Demências [Study Group
Neurología, 40, 613–618. on Brain Aging and Dementia] (Ed.), Escalas e testes na
Barreto, J., Leuschner, A., Santos, F., & Sobral, M. (2008). demência [Scales and tests in dementia] (pp. 17–32). Lisbon,
Escala de Depressão Geriátrica [Geriatric Depression Scale]. Portugal: GEECD.
In Grupo de Estudos de Envelhecimento Cerebral e Gauthier, S., Reisberg, B., Zaudig, M., Petersen, R. C.,
Demências [Study Group on Brain Aging and Dementia] Ritchie, K., Broich, K., et al. (2006). Mild cognitive impair-
(Ed.), Escalas e testes na demência [Scales and tests in ment. Lancet, 367(9518), 1262–1270. doi:10.1016/S0140-
dementia] (pp. 69–72). Lisbon, Portugal: GEECD. 6736(06)68542-5
Bravo, G., & Hébert, R. (1997). Age and education specific Ginó, S., Mendes, T., Ribeiro, F., Mendonça, A., Guerreiro,
reference values for the Mini-Mental and Modified Mini- M., & Garcia, C. (2008). Escala de Queixas de Memória
Mental State Examination derived from a non-demented [Memory Complaints Scale]. In Grupo de Estudos de
elderly population. International Journal of Geriatric Envelhecimento Cerebral e Demências [Study Group on
MoCA NORMATIVE STUDY 7

Brain Aging and Dementia] (Ed.), Escalas e testes na McDowell, I. (2001). Alzheimer’s disease: Insights from epidemi-
demência [Scales and tests in dementia] (pp. 117–120). ology. Aging: Milano, 13(3), 143–162.
Lisbon, Portugal: GEECD. Measso, G., Cavarzeran, F., Zappalà, G., Lebowitz, B. D.,
Guerreiro, M. (1998). Contributo da neuropsicologia para o Crook, T. H., Pirozzollo, F. J., et al. (1993). The Mini-
estudo das demências [Contribution of neuropsychology to Mental State Examination: Normative study of an Italian
the study of dementia]. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, random sample. Developmental Neuropsychology, 9(2),
University of Lisbon, Lisbon, Portugal. 77–85. doi:10.1080/87565649109540545
Han, C., Jo, S. A., Jo, I., Kim, E., Park, M. H., & Meyers, L. S., Gamst, G., & Guarino, A. J. (2006). Applied mul-
Kang, Y. (2008). An adaptation of the Korean Mini- tivariate research: Design and interpretation. Thousand Oaks,
Mental State Examination (K-MMSE) in elderly Koreans: CA: Sage Publications.
Demographic influence and population-based norms (the Moraes, C., Pinto, J. A., Lopes, M. A., Litvoc, J., & Bottino, C.
AGE study). Archives of Gerontology and Geriatrics, 47, M. (2010). Impact of sociodemographic and health variables
302–310. doi:10.1016/j.archger.2007.08.012 on Mini-Mental State Examination in a community-based
Herrera-Rivero, M., Hernández-Aguilar, M. E., Manzo, J., & sample of older people. European Archives of Psychiatry and
Aranda-Abreu, G. E. (2010). Enfermedad de Alzheimer: Clinical Neuroscience, 260(7), 535–542. doi:10.1007/s00406-
Inmunidad e diagnóstico [Alzheimer disease: Inmunidad and 010-0104-3
diagnosis]. Revista de Neurología, 51, 153–164. Morgado, J., Rocha, C. S., Maruta, C., Guerreiro, M., &
Hughes, C. P., Berg, L., Danziger, W. L., Coben, L. A., & Martin, Martins, I. P. (2009). Novos valores normativos do Mini-
R. L. (1982). A new clinical scale for the staging of dementia. Mental State Examination [New normative data for Mini-
The British Journal of Psychiatry, 140, 566–572. Mental State Examination]. Sinapse, 2(9), 10–16.
Instituto Nacional de Estatística. (2004). Projecções de popu- Morgado, J., Rocha, C. S., Maruta, C., Guerreiro, M., &
lação residente, Portugal e NUTS II: 2000–2050 [Projections Martins, I. P. (2010). Cut-off scores in MMSE: A mov-
of resident population, Portugal and NUTS II: 2000–2050]. ing target? European Journal of Neurology, 17(5), 692–695.
Lisbon, Portugal: Author. doi:10.1111/j.1468-1331.2009.02907.x
Downloaded by [Sandra Freitas] at 03:23 04 July 2011

Instituto Nacional de Estatística. (2009). Projecções de popu- Nasreddine, Z., Phillips, N. A., Bédirian, V., Charbonneau,
lação residente: 2008–2060 [Projections of resident popula- S., Whitehead, V., Collin, I., et al. (2005). The Montreal
tion: 2008–2060]. Lisbon, Portugal: Author. Cognitive Assessment, MoCA: A brief screening tool for
Instituto Nacional de Estatística. (2010). Portal de Estatísticas mild cognitive impairment. American Geriatrics Society,
Oficiais [Portuguese Official Statistics]. Retrieved June 22, 53(4), 695–699. doi:10.1111/j.1532-5415.2005.53221.x
2010, from http://www.ine.pt Nguyen, H. T., Black, S. A., Ray, L. A., Espino, D.
Ismail, Z., Rajji, T. K., & Shulman, K. I. (2009). Brief cogni- V., & Markides, K. S. (2002). Predictors of decline in
tive screening instruments: An update. International Journal MMSE scores among older Mexican Americans. Journal
of Geriatric Psychiatry, 25(2), 111–120. doi:10.1002/gps. of Gerontology, 57A(3), M181–M185. doi:10.1046/j.1532-
2306 5415.2003.51055.x
Ismail, Z., & Shulman, K. (2006). Avaliação cognitiva breve Pallant, J. (2007). SPSS survival manual: A step by step guide to
para a demência [Brief cognitive assessment for dementia]. data analysis using SPSS for Windows (3rd ed.). New York,
In H. Firmino (Ed.), Psicogeriatria [Psychogeriatrics] (pp. NY: McGraw-Hill.
513–530). Coimbra, Portugal: Psiquiatria Clínica. Petersen, R. C. (2000). Mild cognitive impairment: Transition
Jacova, C., Kertesz, A., Blair, M., Fisk, J. D., & Feldman, between aging and Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology, 15(3), 93–
H. H. (2007). Neuropsychological testing and assess- 101. Retrieved from http://www.neurology.org/
ment for dementia. Alzheimer’s Dementia, 3(4), 299–317. Petersen, R. C. (2003). Mild cognitive impairment: Aging to
doi:10.1016/j.jalz.2007.07.011 Alzheimer’s disease. New York, NY: Oxford University
Jorm, A. F., Korten, A. E., & Henderson, A. S. (1987). The Press.
prevalence of dementia: A quantitative integration of the Petersen, R. C. (2004). Mild cognitive impairment as a diag-
literature. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 76(5), 465–479. nostic entity. Journal of Internal Medicine, 256(3), 183–194.
Langa, K. M., Chernew, M. E., Kabeto, M. U., Herzog, A. R., doi:10.1111/j.1365-2796.2004.01388.x
Ofstedal, M. B., Willis, R. J., et al. (2001). National estimates Petersen, R. C. (2007). Mild cognitive impairment. Continuum:
of the quantity and cost of informal caregiving for the elderly Lifelong Learning in Neurology, 13(2), 15–38.
with dementia. Journal of General Internal Medicine, 16(11), Petersen, R. C., Stevens, J. C., Ganguli, M., Tangalos, E. G.,
770–778. doi:10.1111/j.1525-1497.2001.10123.x Cummings, J. L., & DeKosky, S. T. (2001). Practice param-
Langa, K. M., Llewellyn, D. J., Lang, I. A., Weir, D. R., Wallace, eter: Early detection of dementia: Mild cognitive impair-
R. B., Kabeto, M. U., et al. (2009). Cognitive health among ment (an evidence-based review). Neurology, 56, 1133–1142.
older adults in the United States and in England. BMC Retrieved from http://www.neurology.org/
Geriatrics, 9, 23. doi:10.1186/1471-2318-9-23 Schmand, B., Jonker, C., Hooijer, C., & Lindeboom, J. (1996).
Lieberman, D., Galinsky, D., Fried, V., Grinshpun, Y., Mytlis, Subjective memory complaints may announce dementia.
N., Tylis, R., et al. (1999). Factors affecting the results Neurology, 46(1), 121–125.
of the Clock Drawing Test in elderly patients hospital- Seshadri, S., Wolf, P. A., Beiser, A., Au, R., McNulty, K., White,
ized for physical rehabilitation. International Journal of R., et al. (1997). Lifetime risk of dementia and Alzheimer’s
Geriatric Psychiatry, 14, 325–330. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1099- disease: The impact of mortality on risk estimates in the
1166(199905)14:5<3 GPS904>3.0.CO;2-Q Framingham study. Neurology, 49(6), 1498–1504.
Lonie, J. A., Tierney, K. M., & Ebmeier, K. P. (2009). Simões, M. R., Freitas, S., Santana, I., Firmino, H., Martins,
Screening for mild cognitive impairment: A systematic C., Nasreddine, Z., et al. (2008). Montreal Cognitive
review. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 24(9), Assessment (MoCA): Versão portuguesa [Montreal
902–915. doi:10.1002/gps.2208 Cognitive Assessment (MoCA): Portuguese version].
Luck, T., Luppa, M., Briel, S., & Riedel-Heller, S. G. (2010). Coimbra, Portugal: Serviço de Avaliação Psicológica
Incidence of mild cognitive impairment: A systematic review. da Faculdade de Psicologia e de Ciências da Educação
Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders, 29(2), 164–175. da Universidade de Coimbra [Psychological Assessment
doi:10.1159/000272424 Department, Faculty of Psychology and Educational
Mathuranath, P. S., Cherian, J. P., Mathew, R., George, Sciences, University of Coimbra].
A., Alexander, A., & Sarma, S. P. (2007). Mini Mental Wimo, A., Winblad, B., Aguero-Torres, H., & von Strauss, E.
State Examination and the Addenbrooke´s Cognitive (2004). The magnitude of dementia occurrence in the world.
Examination: Effect of education and norms for a mul- Alzheimer Disease and Associated Disorders, 17(2), 63–67.
ticultural population. Neurology India, 55(2), 106–110. Winblad, B., Palmer, K., Kivipelto, M., Jelic, V., Fratiglioni,
doi:10.4103/0028-3886.32779 L., Wahlund, L. O., et al. (2004). Mild cognitive
8 FREITAS ET AL.

impairment—beyond controversies, towards a consen- World Health Organization website: http://www.who.


sus: Report of the International Working Group on Mild int/whr/2003/en/whr03_en.pdf
Cognitive Impairment. Journal of Internal Medicine, 256(3), Yesavage, J. A., Brink, T. L., Rose, T. L., Lum, O., Huang, V.,
240–246. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2796.2004.01380.x Adey, M., et al. (1983). Development and validation of a
World Health Organization. (2003). World health report geriatric depression screening scale: A preliminary report.
2003: Shaping the future. Retrieved May 2, 2008, from Journal of Psychiatric Research, 17(1), 37–49.
Downloaded by [Sandra Freitas] at 03:23 04 July 2011

You might also like