You are on page 1of 9

JOURNAL OF CLINICAL AND EXPERIMENTAL NEUROPSYCHOLOGY

2010, 32 (4), 350–357

Attention and working memory deficits in mild


NCEN

cognitive impairment

Nichole L. J. Saunders1 and Mathew J. Summers1,2


Attention and Working Memory in MCI

1
University of Tasmania, Launceston, Tasmania, Australia
2
Wicking Dementia Research and Education Centre, Menzies Research Institute, Hobart,
Tasmania, Australia

Mild cognitive impairment (MCI) has emerged as a classification for a prodromal phase of cognitive decline
preceding the emergence of Alzheimer’s disease (AD). We examined neuropsychological functioning in a sample
of 60 adults with amnestic-MCI (a-MCI), 32 with subjective complaints of memory impairment (subjective-MCI,
s-MCI), 14 with mild AD, and 25 age-matched controls. Both the a-MCI and s-MCI groups displayed impaired
attentional processing, working memory capacity, and semantic language, with a-MCI displaying additional
impairments to verbal and/or visual memory. These results indicate that further research is needed to examine
cognitive decline in nonamnestic variants of MCI.

Keywords: Mild cognitive impairment; Memory; Executive function; Attention; Working memory.

INTRODUCTION demographically matched control group on a verbal


memory test); preserved general cognitive functioning;
The notion of a preclinical stage of Alzheimer’s disease intact activities of daily living (ADL); and no dementia.
(AD) has been variously described, with the more recent A potential problem with the definition of MCI is the
term, mild cognitive impairment (MCI; Flicker, Ferris, & dependence on subjective reports of memory deficits. Stud-
Reisberg, 1991), appearing to be favored amongst ies indicate that 25–34.3% (Jonker, Launer, Hooijer, &
researchers in the field. MCI is an evolving construct that Lindeboom, 1996; Tobiansky, Blizard, Livingston, &
refers to a transitional cognitive state between normal Mann, 1995) of community samples aged over 65 years
aging and the early stages of dementia (Petersen, 2004), report memory complaints. Whether these subjective mem-
for which different criteria have been proposed. Research ory complaints are predictive of objective cognitive deficits
into MCI is complicated by disagreements about the crite- remains contentious (Farias, Mungas, & Jagust, 2005).
ria utilized, the classification process employed, the rela- However, a variety of other cognitive problems experi-
tive instability of the MCI classification over time, and a enced by individuals may mistakenly be attributed to “poor
lack of agreement regarding the neuropsychological memory” such as an underlying impairment to attention,
instruments and cutoff scores that yield the most sensitive executive control, or information processing (Howieson &
and reliable assessment (Petersen, 2004; Rabin et al., Lezak, 2002). Consequently, the reliability and validity of
2006). Among the various criteria for MCI, those initially subjective reports of memory impairment are uncertain.
proposed by Petersen and colleagues (Petersen et al., 2001; When Petersen and colleagues’ original criteria
Petersen et al., 1999) have been widely adopted, requiring (Petersen et al., 2001; Petersen et al., 1999) are applied,
evidence of: informant-corroborated complaints of MCI is considered to have a very high rate of
memory loss; objective evidence of memory impairment progression to clinical AD (Alladi, Arnold, Mitchell,
(typically defined as a performance 1.5 SDs below a Nestor, & Hodges, 2006). However, research indicates

There was no financial support received for this study. The authors wish to thank the Alzheimer’s Association of Tasmania as well as
various General Practice Surgeries in Northern Tasmania for their assistance in recruitment of participants in this study. We also wish
to thank the participants and their families for their enthusiasm in taking part in this study. The authors report no conflicts of interest.
This manuscript has not been published elsewhere, nor has it been submitted simultaneously for publication elsewhere.
Address correspondence to Mathew J. Summers, School of Psychology, University of Tasmania, Locked Bag 1342, Launceston,
Tasmania 7250, Australia (E-mail: Mathew.Summers@utas.edu.au).

© 2009 Psychology Press, an imprint of the Taylor & Francis Group, an Informa business
http://www.psypress.com/jcen DOI: 10.1080/13803390903042379
ATTENTION AND WORKING MEMORY IN MCI 351

that single-domain amnestic-MCI (a-MCI) is a rare and AD (n = 14); and healthy controls (n = 25)—took part in
unstable condition, with longitudinal clinic-based studies this study. Participants provided informed consent prior
indicating that while 10–15% of a-MCI patients progress to to the commencement of the study, in accordance with the
AD annually (Roach, 2005), some progress to other requirements of the Human Research Ethics Committee
dementia types, remain stable, or even recover (Gauthier & (Tasmania) Network and National Health and Medical
Touchon, 2005). It has been suggested that the variability Research Council (NHMRC) Human Research guide-
and lack of specificity of the MCI criterion in accurately lines. The healthy control group consisted of adults with
predicting the risk for developing AD may reflect the heter- no cognitive complaints or medical history of significance
ogeneity of the clinical and etiological presentation of MCI and matched the mean age and level of education of the
(Arai, 2005). Recently, attempts have been made to address MCI group. A total of 14 adults with probable AD
these issues by reconceptualizing MCI as consisting of two (National Institute of Neurological and Communicative
major subtypes: amnestic-MCI and nonamnestic-MCI. Disorders and Stroke–Alzheimer’s Disease and Related
These subtypes can be further subclassified into single- or Disorders Association, NINCDS-ARDRA, criteria as
multiple-domain categories (Petersen & Morris, 2005; independently assessed by a psychogeriatrician and/or
Winblad et al., 2004). However, neuropsychological tests neuropsychologist with access to each participant’s full
assessing cognitive deficits in nonmemory domains have not medical and psychiatric history) were also recruited.
been specified, and cutoff scores have yet to be determined. MCI participants were recruited via local medical
Recent research suggests that the earliest cognitive practices and advertising in print and radio media seek-
deficits in the preclinical stage of AD may not be ing adults over 60 years of age with memory problems.
detectable using tests of verbal memory (Backman, This recruitment campaign resulted in 275 respondents,
Jones, Berger, Laukka, & Small, 2004; Bondi et al., 2008; who underwent a two-stage screening process to identify
Loewenstein, Acevedo, Agron, Martinez, & Duara, those with probable MCI. Stage 1 involved a structured
2007), with deficits to visual memory (Alladi et al., 2006; telephone interview assessing evidence of subjective and
de Jager, Hogervorst, Combrinck, & Budge, 2003), informant-corroborated reports of: (a) memory prob-
attention (Belleville, Chertkow, & Gauthier, 2007; Rapp lems, with a history of decline from a former level; (b)
& Reischies, 2005), working memory and executive preserved cognitive functioning; (c) intact activities of
functions (Grober et al., 2008; Rapp & Reischies, 2005), daily living; (d) no history of significant medical, neuro-
and semantic knowledge/language (Cuetos, Arango- logical, or psychiatric condition; (e) no history of major
Lasprilla, Uribe, Valencia, & Lopera, 2007) co-occurring risk factors for vascular disease; and (f) no history of
with or preceding verbal memory impairments. A review alcohol abuse, sensory impairment, or impairment of
of 73 neuropsychological studies investigating preclinical hand mobility. Of the 275 respondents, 135 successfully
AD supports these findings, indicating that this preclinical completed Stage 1 of screening and underwent Stage 2
stage is characterized by subtle deficits in attention, learn- assessment of psychological and neuropsychological
ing and memory, executive functioning, processing speed, functioning. Stage 2 assessment consisted of the
and semantic knowledge/language (Twamley, Ropacki, & Wechsler Test of Adult Reading (WTAR; Psychological
Bondi, 2006). Other studies indicate that deficits to cogni- Corporation, 2001) to estimate premorbid Wechsler
tive functions other than memory (Storandt, Grant, Adult Intelligence Scale–Third Edition full-scale intelli-
Miller, & Morris, 2006), such as attention and executive gence quotient (WAIS–III FSIQ); the Geriatric Depres-
functions (Rapp & Reischies, 2005; Storandt, 2008), are sion Scale (GDS; Yesavage, 1983) to measure level of
affected years before a clinical diagnosis of AD. depression; Mattis Dementia Rating Scale −2 (DRS−2;
The aim of the present study is to assess working Mattis, 2001) to assess global cognitive functioning and
memory and attentional processing capacities in dementia severity; the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning
participants with subjective-MCI (s-MCI, with no Test (RAVLT; Lezak, Howieson, & Loring, 2004;
objective memory impairment) and amnestic-MCI. It is Strauss, Sherman, & Spreen, 2006) as a measure of
hypothesized that participants with s-MCI will have episodic verbal memory; the Paired Associates Learning
significantly greater objective deficits in one or more of test (PAL; Cambridge Cognition Ltd, 2004) as a meas-
the cognitive domains implicated in the preclinical stages ure of episodic visual memory; and the Boston Naming
of AD: attention (selective, sustained, and/or divided) or Test (BNT; Kaplan, Goodglass, & Weintraub, 1983) as a
working memory (central executive functioning). measure of language retrieval from semantic memory.
Further, it is hypothesized that participants with a-MCI DRS−2 scores were converted to age- and education-
will display objective impairments to memory and not to corrected Mayo Older American Normative Studies
attention and working memory functioning. (MOANS) scaled scores (AEMSS).
Stage 2 test results were examined by an experienced
clinical neuropsychologist (M.S.). A total of 4 partici-
METHOD pants were referred for further medical assessment and
were independently diagnosed by a psychogeriatrician
Participants with mild probable AD, and 4 were excluded due to
other factors (illicit drug use, depression). A total of 92
A total of 131 participants aged between 60 and 90 years (63 MCI participants were screened into the study and
male and 68 female), comprising four subgroups—amnestic- were subdivided into two groups on the basis of the fol-
MCI (a-MCI; n = 60); subjective-MCI (s-MCI; n = 32); mild lowing criteria:
352 SAUNDERS AND SUMMERS

Amnestic-MCI (a-MCI) frontal (but not temporal) lobe damage (Robbins &
Sahakian, 1994).
1. Informant-corroborated subjective complaint of
declining memory functioning.
Procedure
2. Objective memory impairment (defined as a test
score on the RAVLT and/or PAL <10th percentile
After completion of the clinical and neuropsychological
below age norms).
tests, the CANTAB tests were administered in the
3. Normal general cognitive functioning (DRS−2
following order: RTI, PAL, MTS, SSP, RVP, SWM,
AEMSS score ≥9).
IED. The test battery took approximately 90 min to
4. No evidence of dementia (DRS−2 AEMSS score
administer. Halfway through the testing procedure all
≥9).
participants were given a 10-min break to minimize the
5. Intact ADL.
possibility of negative practice effects (i.e., fatigue). The
CANTAB subtests were administered on a laptop com-
Subjective-MCI (s-MCI)
puter attached to a separate 17-inch touch-sensitive
screen and a response pad. Participants were seated
1. Informant-corroborated subjective complaint of
approximately 50 cm from the touch-screen, and the
declining memory functioning.
response pad was positioned 15 cm from the bottom
2. No objective memory impairment.
right (or left for left-handed participants) corner of the
3. Normal general cognitive functioning (DRS−2
touch screen. A standardized script was used to adminis-
AEMSS score ≥9).
ter the CANTAB tests.
4. No evidence of dementia (DRS−2 AEMSS score
≥9).
5. Intact ADL. Statistical analyses

Statistical computations were performed using SPSS for


Executive, attention, and working memory
Windows (Version 16.0).
assessment
Analyses of the hypotheses were conducted by
multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA)/multivari-
Materials
ate analysis of covariance (MANCOVA), followed by
Measures of executive functioning, attention, and one-way analyses of variance (ANOVAs) to test for
working memory capacity were selected from the specific group differences and post hoc pairwise
Cambridge Automated Neuropsychological Assessment comparisons.
Battery (CANTAB), a valid and reliable measure of neu-
ropsychological functions developed specifically for use
with aged populations and populations suffering from RESULTS
dementia and related disorders (Fray, Robbins, &
Sahakian, 1996). The CANTAB has subtests found to be The neuropsychological tests used in the present study
sensitive to the progressive decline of cognitive functions are susceptible to variations between groups due to
early in the course of AD (Fray et al., 1996; Sahakian & discrepancies in age, gender, premorbid IQ, and demen-
Owen, 1992). tia severity (Strauss et al., 2006). One-way ANOVA
Four CANTAB subtests were used to assess identified significant group differences in age (Table 1),
attentional functions in the present study (Reaction with the AD group being significantly older than the
Time, RTI; Match to Sample Visual Search, MTS; other groups. Correlational analyses indicated that signi-
Rapid Visual Processing, RVP; Intra–Extra Dimen- ficant relationships exist between age and WTAR (r =
sional Set Shifting, IED). The RTI test consists of two .195), BNT (r = −.388), RAVLT Trial 5 (r = −.387),
subcomponents: simple RTI (SRTI) assessing simple RAVLT delayed recall (r = −.411), PAL 6 shapes
sustained attention, and 5-choice RTI (CRTI) assessing adjusted (r = .319), and PAL 8 shapes adjusted (r = .453).
divided attention. MTS tests the participant’s ability to Analysis of group differences across these variables was
match visual samples. The RVP task is a test of sustained conducted by analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) with age
attention, with a small working memory component, as the covariate (Table 1). Correlational analyses revealed
and is sensitive to dysfunction in the parietal and frontal no significant relationships between age and GDS, educa-
lobe areas of the brain (Sahakian & Coull, 1993). IED tion level, or DRS−2, with ANOVA indicating no signific-
assesses rule acquisition, flexibility of thinking, and ant group differences in GDS score or level of education
attentional set shifting and is a computerized equivalent (Table 1). A 2 × 4 chi-square analysis identified significant
of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test. Two CANTAB sub- differences in gender ratio between the four groups, c2(3)
tests were used to assess working memory functions in = 11.362, p = .010, reflecting a higher proportion of males
the present study (Spatial Span, SSP; Spatial Working in the mild AD group.
Memory, SWM). SSP is a test of visuospatial span and is Significant group differences were detected for age
a computerized equivalent of the Corsi Blocks task. The and BNT, DRS−2, RAVLT, and PAL scores (Table 1).
SWM test assesses retention and manipulation of spatial Post hoc analyses indicated that group differences on
information in working memory and is sensitive to these tests were in expected directions.
ATTENTION AND WORKING MEMORY IN MCI 353

TABLE 1
Group differences in age, education, estimated premorbid FSIQ, and GDS, DRS−2, BNT, RAVLT, and PAL scores

Control s-MCI a-MCI AD Post hoc Effect size


(n = 25) (n = 32) (n = 60) (n = 14) (at p < .05) (hp2)

Measure M SD M SD M SD M SD p

Age 69.32 5.83 71.16 7.08 71.18 7.02 76.29 7.04 .026 CONT < AD 0.070
Education 13.56 3.14 13.00 3.52 13.13 3.35 12.00 2.72 .561 0.016
WTAR (est. FSIQ) 113.32 6.67 110.91 6.78 108.48 8.75 107.71 7.73 .009 CONT, s-MCI > 0.088
a-MCI, AD
GDS 1.60 1.80 1.56 1.46 1.50 1.58 2.29 1.33 .409 0.022
DRS–2 (AEMSS) 11.80 1.71 12.09 1.77 10.82 1.87 5.43 1.83 <.001 AD < a-MCI < 0.536
s-MCI, CONT
BNT 57.48 1.73 55.78 2.88 54.40 4.24 44.21 8.69 <.001 AD < a-MCI < 0.382
s-MCI, CONT
RAVLT Trial 5 12.29 1.90 11.66 1.91 8.97 1.87 6.07 1.94 <.001 AD < a-MCI < 0.505
recall s-MCI, CONT
RAVLT delayed 10.36 2.50 9.50 2.77 6.57 2.35 2.64 2.41 <.001 AD < a-MCI < 0.454
recall s-MCI, CONT
PAL 6 shapesa 4.40 3.48 5.63 4.15 11.53 9.85 30.50 20.13 <.001 AD > a-MCI > 0.346
s-MCI, CONT
PAL 8 shapesa 14.84 8.32 15.78 9.24 31.63 16.04 52.71 22.21 <.001 AD > a-MCI > 0.391
s-MCI, CONT

Note. WTAR = Wechsler Test of Adult Reading; est. FSIQ = estimated full-scale intelligence quotient; GDS = Geriatric Depression
Scale; DRS−2 = Dementia Rating Scale−2; AEMSS = age- and education-corrected MOANS (Mayo Older American Normative
Studies) scaled scores; BNT = Boston Naming Test; RAVLT = Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test; PAL = Paired Associates
Learning. MCI = mild cognitive impairment. s-MCI = subjective-MCI. a-MCI = amnestic-MCI. AD = Alzheimer’s disease. CONT =
controls.
aTotal errors adjusted.

Correlational analyses revealed no significant mean latency the a-MCI, s-MCI, and AD groups were
relationships between age and simple RTI, IED total significantly slower than the control group (p < .05);
errors, or RVP A′. Analysis of group differences by with no other significant group differences being
MANOVA across these three variables indicated a signi- detected. On IED total errors (adjusted) the AD group
ficant group difference: Pillai’s trace = .474, F(9, 381) = made significantly more errors than the control, s-MCI,
7.937, p < .001, power = 1.00, hp2 = 0.158. Subsequent and a-MCI groups (p < .05); with no other group differ-
analyses of the group differences within each of these ences reaching significance. On RVP A′ the s-MCI,
dependent variables were examined by ANOVA (Table 2). a-MCI, and AD groups displayed significantly lower
Correlational analyses identified significant relationships detection thresholds than the control group; the AD
between age and MTS correct latency (r = .267); RVP group displayed a significantly lower detection threshold
latency (r = .191); and 5-choice RTI (r = 240). Analysis than the a-MCI group (all p < .05); with no difference
of group differences across these three variables by between the a-MCI and s-MCI groups being detected.
MANCOVA with age entered as the covariate, identified Correlational analyses indicated that significant rela-
a significant group effect: Pillai’s trace = .289, F(9, 378) tionships existed between age and both measures of
= 4.486, p < .001, power = 1.00, hp2 = 0.096. Subsequent working memory: SWM total errors and SSP. Group
analyses of the group differences within each of the depend- differences on the two measures of working memory
ent variables was conducted by ANCOVA (Table 2). (CANTAB) were examined by MANCOVA with age
Post hoc analyses were conducted to examine group entered as the covariate. A significant group effect was
differences on MTS correct latency, 5-choice RTI, detected across the two dependent variables of working
simple RTI, RVP latency, IED total errors (adjusted), memory: Pillai’s trace = .339, F(6, 252) = 8.580, p < .001,
and RVP A′. For MTS mean correct latency the a-MCI power = 1.00, hp2 = 0.170. Subsequent analyses of the
and AD groups were significantly slower than the con- group differences within each of the dependent variables
trol and s-MCI groups, with the AD group being signifi- were conducted by ANCOVA (Table 3).
cantly slower than the a-MCI group (p < .05). On the Post hoc analyses were conducted to examine the
CRTI task the AD group was significantly slower than group differences identified for SWM total errors and
the s-MCI, a-MCI, and control groups (p < .05), with no SSP span length. On SWM total errors, significant
other group differences detected. On SRTI the a-MCI differences were detected between each of the groups,
and AD groups were significantly slower than the con- with the AD group making more errors than the
trol and s-MCI groups; with the AD group being signifi- a-MCI group, the a-MCI group making more errors
cantly slower than the a-MCI group (p < .05). On RVP than the s-MCI group, and the s-MCI group making
354 SAUNDERS AND SUMMERS

TABLE 2
CANTAB attention test performances in control, s-MCI, a-MCI, and AD groups

Control s-MCI a-MCI AD Effect


(n = 25) (n = 32) (n = 60) (n = 14) size
Attention (hp2)
test M SD M SD M SD M SD p Power

Selective MTS correct 2,836.07 750.43 3,524.89 1,368.69 3,768.82 1,554.22 5,780.33 2,970.71 <.001 0.161 .990
latency (ms)
Divided 5-choice RTI 338.33 42.53 379.17 53.37 374.85 69.60 475.28 152.78 <.001 0.161 .990
(ms)
Sustained Simple RTI 300.96 40.96 328.72 60.68 336.51 74.51 406.26 105.58 <.001 0.139 .975
(ms)
RVP latency 447.50 77.51 530.70 123.75 519.68 123.57 574.25 172.20 .034 0.066 .692
(ms)
Attention IED total 14.84 13.18 16.44 28.24 18.64 12.20 37.71 14.58 .001 0.120 .946
shifting errors
(adjusted)
Target RVP A′ 0.93 0.04 0.86 0.05 0.87 0.05 0.83 0.05 <.001 0.299 1.00
detection

Note. CANTAB = Cambridge Automated Neuropsychological Assessment Battery. MTS = Match to Sample Visual Search. RTI =
Reaction Time. RVP = Rapid Visual Processing. IED = Intra–Extra Dimensional Set Shifting. MCI = mild cognitive impairment.
s-MCI = subjective-MCI. a-MCI = amnestic-MCI. AD = Alzheimer’s disease.

TABLE 3
CANTAB working memory performances in control, s-MCI, a-MCI, and AD groups

Control s-MCI a-MCI AD Effect


(n = 25) (n = 32) (n = 60) (n = 14) size (hp2)

Working memory test M SD M SD M SD M SD p Power

Spatial working SWM total 33.68 13.08 42.06 15.05 51.82 15.15 66.21 19.29 <.001 0.248 1.00
memory errors
Visuospatial span SSP 5.80 0.76 5.20 0.69 4.96 0.64 4.50 0.85 <.001 0.197 .998

Note. CANTAB = Cambridge Automated Neuropsychological Assessment Battery. SWM = Spatial Working Memory. SSP = Spatial
Span. MCI = mild cognitive impairment. s-MCI = subjective-MCI. a-MCI = amnestic-MCI. AD = Alzheimer’s disease.

more errors than the control group (p < .05). On SSP against age-based normative data) to verbal and/or vis-
span length, the a-MCI and AD groups displayed a sig- ual memory or other cognitive domains are displayed in
nificantly shorter span than the s-MCI and control Table 4.
groups (p < .05), with the s-MCI displaying a signifi- Only 10 participants in the a-MCI group displayed
cantly shorter span than the control group. an isolated impairment to episodic memory function-
It was predicted that a large proportion of the 32 s- ing (verbal and/or visual). The remaining 50 partici-
MCI participants, who present with subjective experi- pants exhibited deficits in one or more additional
ences of memory problems without objective evidence of cognitive domains (Table 4) and could be reclassified
memory impairment, would perform below the 10th per- as multiple-domain a-MCI. These results highlight the
centile for their age on one or more attention and/or need for the use of sensitive and specific neuropsycho-
working memory tests. It was found that all 32 s-MCI logical tests of other cognitive functions in the classifi-
participants performed below the 10th percentile on one cation of single-domain a-MCI. These findings also
or more of these measures. That is, these participants suggest that previous research into a-MCI may have
displayed a level of impairment (<10th percentile) on inadvertently examined a group with heterogeneous
specific tests of nonmemory functions equivalent in cognitive impairment.
severity to the level of amnestic impairment used to
classify a-MCI. Consequently, these participants could
be considered to be displaying a nonamnestic form of DISCUSSION
MCI (na-MCI). The number of participants in the con-
trol, s-MCI, a-MCI, and AD groups with impairments The present study objectively measured memory
(defined as performances below the 10th percentile performance in individuals with informant-corroborated
ATTENTION AND WORKING MEMORY IN MCI 355

TABLE 4
Proportion of s-MCI, a-MCI, and AD participants performing at <10th percentile on memory and
nonamnestic measures

Impairmenta Control s-MCI a-MCI AD

n % n % n % n %

Verbal memory 38 63 14 100


Visual memory 35 58 10 71
Semantic memory/language 1 3 5 8 7 50
Attention/executive function 2 8 31 97 44 73 13 93
Working memory 2 6 22 37 9 64

Memory (visual or verbal) only 10 17


(single-domain)
Attention/executive only 29 91
(single-domain)
Working memory only (single-domain) 1 3
Multiple-domain amnestic 50 83 14 100
(memory + other)
Multiple-domain nonamnestic 2 6
(2 or more other cognitive)

Note. MCI = mild cognitive impairment. s-MCI = subjective-MCI. a-MCI = amnestic-MCI. AD = Alzheimer’s
disease.
aImpairment defined as performance <10th percentile compared to age-based normative data.

subjective complaints of declining memory functioning. a-MCI group displayed an equivalent level of impair-
Of the 92 participants, 32 were found to have no objec- ment on at least one other cognitive domain. These par-
tive impairment to verbal or visual episodic memory and ticipants meet the notional criteria for multiple-domain
were classified as s-MCI. The remaining 60 participants a-MCI; suggesting that the original criteria for a-MCI
displayed an objective impairment to verbal and/or (Petersen et al., 2001) are insufficient, as the criteria for
visual episodic memory with otherwise intact general impairment to other cognitive domains are discrepant
cognitive functioning (DRS−2) and were classified as a- from the criteria for detecting memory impairment.
MCI. Relative to the control group, both the s-MCI and Finally, the apparent low rate of single-domain a-MCI
a-MCI groups displayed impairments to language in the present study is consistent with previous findings
retrieval from semantic memory, complex sustained that “pure” or single-domain a-MCI is rare (Alladi et al.,
attention, target detection, spatial working memory, and 2006). Such findings raise concerns regarding previous
visuospatial span, with the a-MCI group displaying research employing Petersen’s original criteria for a-MCI,
greater deficits to selective attention, simple sustained in that the participants recruited may predominantly
attention, and language retrieval. The s-MCI and a-MCI represent multiple-domain a-MCI and not single-
groups were indistinguishable on measures of complex domain a-MCI.
sustained attention, target detection, or divided atten- Current classification criteria for MCI consistently
tion. Further, the a-MCI group did not differ from the refer to the presence of memory impairment as the core
AD group on a measure of visuospatial span, and the s- defining characteristic feature of MCI (Petersen et al.,
MCI group did not differ from the AD group on a meas- 2001; Petersen et al., 1999). As our research and other
ure of complex sustained attention. When the s-MCI and recent studies indicate (Alladi et al., 2006; Belleville et
a-MCI participants’ performance on the battery of al., 2007; Herukka et al., 2007), MCI classification crite-
attention and working memory tests was examined, it ria based exclusively on the presence of a memory
was found that all (100%) of the s-MCI group and 50 impairment alone exclude a significant number of aging
(83%) of the a-MCI group also displayed an impaired adults with significant decline in other cognitive
performance on at least one measure of attention or domains. We found that 35% of our sample with inform-
working memory function, despite no evidence of ant-corroborated memory decline did not display an
impairment to these cognitive domains being detected on objectively measured impairment to memory but did dis-
the DRS−2. play impaired attention or working memory functioning.
Our sample of 60 a-MCI participants met Petersen’s That such a sizable proportion of our MCI sample did
(Petersen et al., 2001; Petersen et al., 1999) original crite- not display objective memory impairment raises
ria for a-MCI: an objective memory impairment in the concerns regarding previous studies utilizing exclusively
absence of other cognitive impairments (as measured using amnestic variants of MCI to examine the trajectory of
the DRS−2 as a global measure of cognitive functioning). MCI. As this nonamnestic subgroup of adults have
However, when sensitive and specific neuropsychologi- generally been excluded from previous studies of MCI,
cal tests of other cognitive functions are used, 83% of the the risk for sufferers of na-MCI in developing a
356 SAUNDERS AND SUMMERS

neurodegenerative disorder such as Alzheimer’s Gauthier, S., & Touchon, J. (2005). Mild cognitive impairment
dementia has yet to be established. Recent propositions is not a clinical entity and should not be treated. Archives of
Neurology, 62(7), 1164–1166.
by several authors have led to a reconceptualization and Grober, E., Hall, C. B., Lipton, R. B., Zonderman, A. B.,
broadening of MCI classification criteria (Petersen, Resnick, S. M., & Kawas, C. (2008). Memory impairment,
2004; Winblad et al., 2004). The recognition of various executive dysfunction, and intellectual decline in preclinical
subtypes of MCI other than amnestic-MCI is a Alzheimer’s disease. Journal of the International Neuropsy-
progressive step forward; however, to date few studies chological Society, 14(2), 266–278.
Herukka, S.-K., Helisalmi, S., Hallikainen, M., Tervo, S.,
exist examining these MCI subtypes in relation to Soininen, H., & Pirttila, T. (2007). CSF Aβ42, tau and phos-
progression to dementia. Further, the relationship phorylated tau, APOEε4 allele and MCI type in progressive
between the various subtypes of MCI has not been MCI. Neurobiology of Aging, 28(4), 507–514.
examined: It is possible that na-MCI represents an Howieson, D. B., & Lezak, M. D. (2002). Separating memory
from other cognitive problems. In A. Baddeley (Ed.), Hand-
alternate early phase of MCI equivalent to single- book of memory disorders (2nd ed.). Oxford, UK: John
domain a-MCI, but that both progress independently to Wiley & Sons.
multiple-domain a-MCI and then to AD, or alterna- Jonker, C., Launer, L. J., Hooijer, C., & Lindeboom, J. (1996).
tively that na-MCI represents a prodromal phase for Memory complaints and memory impairment in older
neurodegenerative disorders other than AD. Further individuals. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society,
44(1), 44–49.
research is required to explore MCI in all its variations Kaplan, E., Goodglass, H., & Weintraub, S. (1983). Boston
and to move beyond a narrow definition based upon an Naming Test. Philadelphia: Lea & Febiger.
amnestic syndrome. Lezak, M. D., Howieson, D. B., & Loring, D. W. (2004).
Neuropsychological assessment (2nd ed.). Oxford, UK:
Oxford University Press.
Original manuscript received 24 February 2009
Loewenstein, D. A., Acevedo, A., Agron, J., Martinez, G., &
Revised manuscript accepted 11 May 2009 Duara, R. (2007). The use of amnestic and nonamnestic
First published online 25 September 2009 composite measures at different thresholds in the neuropsy-
chological diagnosis of MCI. Journal of Clinical and
Experimental Neuropsychology, 29(3), 300–307.
Mattis, S. (2001). Dementia Rating Scale-2 (DRS-2). Odessa,
REFERENCES FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.
Petersen, R. C. (2004). Mild cognitive impairment as a diagnos-
Alladi, S., Arnold, R., Mitchell, J., Nestor, P. J., & Hodges, J. R. tic entity. Journal of Internal Medicine, 256(3), 183–194.
(2006). Mild cognitive impairment: Applicability of research Petersen, R. C., Doody, R., Kurz, A., Mohs, R., Morris, J. C.,
criteria in a memory clinic and characterisation of cognitive Rabins, P. V., et al. (2001). Current concepts in mild cogni-
profile. Psychological Medicine, 36, 507–515. tive impairment. Archives of Neurology, 58(12), 1985–1992.
Arai, H. (2005). Current concept and future research directions Petersen, R. C., & Morris, J. C. (2005). Mild cognitive impair-
of mild cognitive impairment. Psychogeriatrics, 5(3), 83–88. ment as a clinical entity and treatment target. Archives of
Backman, L., Jones, S., Berger, A. K., Laukka, E. J., & Small, Neurology, 62(7), 1160–1163.
B. J. (2004). Multiple cognitive deficits during the transition Petersen, R. C., Smith, G. E., Waring, S. C., Ivnik, R. J.,
to Alzheimer’s disease. Journal of Internal Medicine, 256(3), Tangalos, E. G., & Kokmen, E. (1999). Mild cognitive
195–204. impairment: Clinical characterization and outcome.
Belleville, S., Chertkow, H., & Gauthier, S. (2007). Working Archives of Neurology, 56(3), 303–308.
memory and control of attention in persons with Alzheimer’s Psychological Corporation. (2001). Wechsler Test of Adult
disease and mild cognitive impairment. Neuropsychology, Reading. San Antonio, TX: Harcourt Assessment.
21(4), 458–469. Rabin, L. A., Roth, R. M., Isquith, P. K., Wishart, H. A.,
Bondi, M. W., Jak, A. J., Delano-Wood, L., Jacobson, M. W., Nutter-Upham, K. E., Pare, N., et al. (2006). Self- and
Delis, D. C., & Salmon, D. P. (2008). Neuropsychological informant reports of executive function on the BRIEF-A in
contributions to the early detection of Alzheimer’s disease. MCI and older adults with cognitive complaints. Archives of
Neuropsychology Review, 18(1), 73–90. Clinical Neuropsychology, 21, 721–732.
Cambridge Cognition Ltd. (2004). CANTABeclipse. Rapp, M. A., & Reischies, F. M. (2005). Attention and execut-
Cambridge, UK: Author. ive control predict Alzheimer disease in late life: Results
Cuetos, F., Arango-Lasprilla, J. C., Uribe, C., Valencia, C., from the Berlin Aging Study (BASE). American Journal of
& Lopera, F. (2007). Linguistic changes in verbal expres- Geriatric Psychiatry, 13(2), 134–141.
sion: A preclinical marker of Alzheimer’s disease. Journal Roach, E. S. (2005). Treating mild cognitive impairment: The
of the International Neuropsychological Society, 13(3), absence of evidence. Archives of Neurology, 62, 1167.
433–439. Robbins, T. W., & Sahakian, B. J. (1994). Computer methods
de Jager, C. A., Hogervorst, E., Combrinck, M., & Budge, M. of assessment of cognitive function. In J. R. M. Copeland,
M. (2003). Sensitivity and specificity of neuropsychological M. T. Abou-Saleh, & D. G. Blazer (Eds.), Principles and
tests for mild cognitive impairment, vascular cognitive practice of geriatric psychiatry (pp. 205–209). Chichester,
impairment and Alzheimer’s disease. Psychological Medi- UK: John Wiley & Sons.
cine, 33(6), 1039–1050. Sahakian, B. J., & Coull, J. T. (1993). Tetrahydroaminoacridine
Farias, S. T., Mungas, D., & Jagust, W. (2005). Degree of (THA) in Alzheimer’s disease: An assessment of attentional
discrepancy between self and other-reported everyday func- and mnemonic function using CANTAB. Acta Neurologica
tioning by cognitive status: Dementia, mild cognitive Scandinavica, 88, 29–35.
impairment, and healthy elders. International Journal of Sahakian, B. J., & Owen, A. M. (1992). Computerized assess-
Geriatric Psychiatry, 20(9), 827–834. ment in neuropsychiatry using CANTAB: Discussion paper.
Flicker, C., Ferris, S. H., & Reisberg, B. (1991). Mild cognitive Journal of the Royal Society of Medicine, 85(7), 399–402.
impairment in the elderly: Predictors of dementia. Neurology, Storandt, M. (2008). Cognitive deficits in the early stages of
41(7), 1006–1009. Alzheimer’s disease. Current Directions in Psychological Sci-
Fray, P. J., Robbins, T. W., & Sahakian, B. J. (1996). ence, 17(3), 198–202.
Neuropsychiatric applications of CANTAB. International Storandt, M., Grant, E. A., Miller, J. P., & Morris, J. C. (2006).
Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 11(4), 329–336. Longitudinal course and neuropathologic outcomes in
ATTENTION AND WORKING MEMORY IN MCI 357

original vs. revised MCI and in pre-MCI. Neurology, 67(3), Alzheimer’s disease. Journal of the International Neuropsy-
467–473. chological Society, 12(5), 707–735.
Strauss, E., Sherman, E. M. S., & Spreen, O. (2006). A compen- Winblad, B., Palmer, K., Kivipelto, M., Jelic, V., Fratiglioni, L.,
dium of neuropsychological tests: Administrations, norms, and Wahlund, L. O., et al. (2004). Mild cognitive impair-
commentary (3rd ed.). New York: Oxford University Press. ment: Beyond controversies, towards a consensus:
Tobiansky, R., Blizard, R., Livingston, G., & Mann, A. (1995). Report of the International Working Group on Mild
The Gospel Oak Study stage IV: The clinical relevance of Cognitive Impairment. Journal of Internal Medicine,
subjective memory impairment in older people. Psychologi- 256(3), 240–246.
cal Medicine, 25(4), 779–786. Yesavage, J. (1983). Development and validation of a geriatric
Twamley, E. W., Ropacki, S. A. L., & Bondi, M. A. (2006). depression screening scale: A preliminary report. Journal of
Neuropsychological and neuroimaging changes in preclinical Psychiatric Research, 17, 37–49.
Copyright of Journal of Clinical & Experimental Neuropsychology is the property of Psychology Press (UK)
and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright
holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use.

You might also like