You are on page 1of 7

Aging Clinical and Experimental Research

MINI REVIEW

Cognitive impairment: classification and open issues


Cristina Basso1, Federica Limongi1, Paola Siviero1, Giovanna Romanato1, Marianna Noale1,
Stefania Maggi1, Leontino Battistin2, and Gaetano Crepaldi1
1
CNR Aging Section, Institute of Neuroscience, Padova, 2IRCCS San Camillo, Lido di Venezia, Italy

ABSTRACT. Several clinically-defined cognitive im-


senescent forgetfulness. So dementing disorders were
pairment syndromes, with differing diagnostic criteria
considered as disease processes distinct from aging, and
and nomenclature, have been proposed to describe
the term “senility” was rejected in favor of “dementia”.
nondisabling symptomatic cognitive deficits.
Incidence and prevalence rates vary as a result of DEFINITIONS
different di- agnostic criteria and sampling Age-associated memory impairment (AAMI)
procedures across stud- ies. The incidence rates of
A number of clinical labels have been proposed to
cognitive impairment in- crease with age; but no
de- scribe subclinical cognitive deficits.
consistent data have been re- ported on the
To characterize age-related changes in memory, the
association with family history, age, sex, education,
National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH) working group
Apo E4 genotype, depression, and other traditional
defined criteria for AAMI (3): age 50 years with
risk factors for dementia. Several studies have
gradu- al onset of memory complaints substantiated by
suggested that most patients with cogni- tive psycho- logical performance tests [1.0 standard deviation
impairment clinically defined will progress to (SD) be- low the mean test value, normed on young
Alzheimer Disease (AD), but rates of conversion vary adults]. In AA- MI, memory impairment is not the
widely among studies. This review summarizes existing consequence of a specific neurological, psychiatric or
definitions and related epidemiological data. medical condition. Blackford and LaRue criticized
(Aging Clin Exp Res 2007; 19: 344-348) these criteria as being over-inclusive (4); difficult to
©
2007, Editrice Kurtis implement in epidemiologic studies (5); lacking construct
INTRODUCTION validity, in that people with higher intelligence; and
lacking predictive validity, in that subjective complaints
Cognitive impairment without dementia has common- do not predict future decline. In ad- diction, it does not
ly been considered a normal consequence of brain contemplate and does not clearly discriminate between
aging. Many attempts have been made to define the subjects who will develop dementia and those who will
clinical en- tity of cognitive decline associated with aging. remain cognitively stable.
Prichard (1), at the beginning of the 19 th century, An alternative to viewing changes associated with ag-
identified the earliest stage of dementia as impairment of ing as intrinsic to the aging process is to consider them
recent memory with in- tact remote memory. A century as harbingers of disease. Dementing diseases are more
later, Kral (2) described be- nign senescent forgetfulness common with age, and this point should be considered
(BSF), in which unimpor- tant parts of experiences of in the effort to differentiate age-associated traits that
the past are not recalled and in which the forgotten acquire disease status from those that do not. AAMI
data seem to belong to the remote past rather than the prevalence estimates have ranged from 18.5% at age
recent past. The clinical features of BSF included greater than 50 to 38.4% between ages 60 and 78 (6).
memory impairment, and particular attention was paid to
both the extent of the deficits and the patient’s Aging-associated cognitive decline (AACD)
awareness of them, but no formal criteria were proposed. In 1994, a task force of the International
BSF was characterized by an age-related decline, which
Psychogeriatric Association (IPA), in collaboration with
did not cross a disease threshold, although it was
the World Health Organisation (WHO) (7), proposed
probably a mild form of the same process giving rise to
diagnostic criteria for
malignant

Key words: Cognitive impairment, dementia.


Correspondence: Federica Limongi, CNR Aging Section, Institute of Neuroscience, Via Giustiniani 2, 35128 Padova,
Italy. E-mail: federica.limongi@in.cnr.it
Received August 10, 2006; accepted in revised form March 30, 2007.

Aging Clin Exp Res, Vol. 19, No. 5 1


C. Basso, F. Limongi, P. Siviero, et Aging Clin Exp Res 19: 344-348,
2007

AACD, in which patients score at least one SD below restric- tive definition, compared with 3% with the most
age and education-based standards (i.e., by reference to liberal
norms for elderly people) on neuropsychological tests
assessing multiple cognitive abilities (memory and
learning, attention and concentration, language and
visuospatial function- ing). The AACD criteria contain
no age restriction, al- though cognitive decline is more
prevalent in old age, and its onset may occur earlier in
life. Hanninen et al. (8) showed a prevalence of 26.6%
and Ritchie et al. (9) found a prevalence of 19.3% in
elderly subjects over 60.

Age-related cognitive decline (ARCD)


ARCD, defined by the Diagnostic and Statistical Man-
ual of mental disorders – Fourth Edition (DSM-IV) as
an objective decline in cognitive functioning consequent
up- on the aging process which is within normal limits,
given the person’s age, does not include diagnostic
criteria, and few epidemiological studies using this
definition have been conducted (10).

Mild cognitive disorder (MCD)


The International Statistical Classification of diseases
and related Health Problems, 10th Revision (ICD-10)
(6) included the definition of MCD, a diagnosis only
used when there are indications of a disease or
condition known to cause cerebral dysfunction.

Cognitive impairment, no dementia (CIND)


The category of CIND has been introduced to
classify all individuals with cognitive impairment falling
between healthy and demented states, but not meeting
criteria of dementia. It describes cognitive impairment
that may or may not progress to dementia, and
includes global cog- nitive impairment and deficits in
several cognitive do- mains. In the Canadian Study of
Health and Ageing (CSHA) (5), a nationally
representative epidemiologic study, the prevalence of
CIND was about 16%, twice that of dementia (8%). In
the Italian Longitudian Study on Ag- ing (ILSA) (11) a
prevalence rate of 10.7% was found.

Mild cognitive impairment (MCI)


The term MCI has received more attention
compared with the other terms still used. It identifies
subjects com- plaining of defective memory, is
preferably corroborated by an informant, includes
normal activities of daily living and normal general
cognitive functioning, but involves ab- normal memory
function for age and education; demen- tia is absent
(12, 13). Population-based studies estimate the
prevalence of MCI to be more than double that of de-
mentia (14) with prevalence rates ranging from 3% for age
60 years (9) to 15% for people aged 75 and older
(15). Fisk et al. (16) showed that changes in definition
changed the prevalence of MCI. The smallest preva-
lence (less than 3%) is also the result of the most
2 Aging Clin Exp Res, Vol. 19, No. 5
Aging Clin Exp Res 19: 344-348, Cognitive impairment: classification and open
2007
definition. However, these changes did not change the CIND remain sta-
proportion of subjects who progressed to dementia.
The heterogeneity of MCI required a subclassifica-
tion: amnestic MCI (a-MCI-single domain) which
focuses on the subjective memory complaint, ideally
corroborat- ed by an informant, with objective memory
impairment for age, and essentially preserved general
cognitive func- tion with largely intact functional
activities. MCI with slight impairment in multiple
domains represents a second type of MCI called md-MCI,
which involves various de- grees of impairment in
multiple cognitive domains such as language, executive
functions and visuospatial skills, with or without
memory impairment. Subjects with memory
impairment are called md-MCI+a and those without
are called md-MCI–a. The third and least common type
of MCI involves only the non-memory cognitive
domain, such as language, executive function or
visuospatial skills (non-amnestic MCI-single domain).
Other subtypes em- phasizing impairments in non-
memory domains, such as executive function and
visuospatial skills, may have a higher likelihood of
progressing to non-Alzheimer disease (AD) dementia,
such as dementia with Lewy bodies. The combination
of clinical subtypes and putative etiologies are also useful
in predicting the type of dementia which will evolve.
The clinical subtypes of MCI are defined partly by
the absence of impairment in activities of daily living
(ADL) and no more than “mild” impairment in instru-
mental activities of daily living (IADL) (17). Data shows
that IADL are lost before ADL, and there are even
hierarchi- cal patterns of functional losses within
these domains. IADL impairment may be associated
with changes in cognition and motor function, and
thus suggests that one or more items of IADL
impairment may serve as a better threshold for
cognitive decline diagnosis (17, 18).

Rate of progression to dementia


Different progression rates have been reported, de-
pending on the definition of impairment and length of
fol- low-up: 12-38% over 1 year (10), 36% over 2
years
(19) , 18% over 3 years (20) and 42% over 5 years
(21). Hogan and Ebly reported that the highest rate of
pro- gression to dementia (51% of survivors) was seen
amongst those who met the ICD-10 F06 criteria for
MCI with re- spect to subjects with specific memory
impairment, in- tellectual decline or personality
change, without func- tional impairment. The
prevalence of all types of cogni- tive impairment,
including CIND and dementias accord- ing to the
CSHA, increased with age and affected 65% of those
aged 85 and older. Subjects with CIND were about
three times more likely to be living in an institution
than those who were cognitively unimpaired. Elderly
people with CIND also show a significant degree of
functional im- pairment and need of institutional care
(21). However, a substantial number of subjects with

Aging Clin Exp Res, Vol. 19, No. 5 3


C. Basso, F. Limongi, P. Siviero, et Aging Clin Exp Res 19: 344-348,
2007

ble, or may even improve in cognitive functioning over MCI to AD (10, 27, 30) vary widely across studies
time (11-21). ranging from
A Finnish study (22) demonstrated that 22% of
subjects with CIND improved after 3 years. Another
study (23) re- ported improvement in 15%, with an
additional 29% remaining stable after 3 years, but it is
not known whether the observed improvement is stable
over time.
In a prospective population-based study, the Kung-
sholmen Project (24), 35% of individuals with CIND
pro- gressed to dementia and 11% remained stable with
an es- timated improvement of 25% between baseline
and the 3- year follow-up. Those who improved within 3
years of fol- low-up did not have a significantly higher
risk of later pro- gressing to dementia than subjects
who had never been classified as cognitively impaired
(relative risk 1.4). It was then found that the absence of
a subjective memory complaint predicts improvement
(odds ratio 5.4), and progression to dementia appeared
to be time-depen- dent, occurring within 3 years. The
relative risk of pro- gressing to dementia was lower at
the 6-year follow-up, and a similar pattern was
described by Johansson and Zarit (25), who reported
rates of progression from MCD to dementia which
were higher over 2 years than over 7 years. Moreover,
the proportions of subjects who im- proved were
similar in severity. The improvement ob- served in the
3-year follow-up is similar to the 22% ob- served by
Schonknecht et al. (26) and slightly higher than the
15% described by Daly et al. (27). In the Kung-
sholmen Project, subjects improved after 3 years, and
re- turned to a cognitively unimpaired state without a
signif- icantly higher risk of dementia with respect to
unim- paired participants.
Longitudinal population studies, using various defini-
tions of MCD adapted to epidemiological studies, have
shown a prevalence in the elderly population between
3 and 19% in adults older 65 years, with an incidence of
8- 58 per 1000 per year, and a risk of developing
demen- tia of 11-33% over 2 years (28). Population-
based stud- ies have shown that up to 44% of patients
with MCI at their first evaluation were estimated to
return to normal a year later (28, 29). Nevertheless,
data shows conflicting conclusions about why not all
individuals identified with MCI appear to progress to
AD, and it has been sug- gested that MCI may represent
early AD which would be revealed with a sufficiently
long period of follow-up (14), or that it represents a
heterogeneous group of individuals within which there
are some at increased risk of demen- tia and others
who have a more non-progressive form of cognitive
impairment which is the reason why improve- ments
are observed.
Data have shown increased rates of progression to
AD in these individuals compared with those with no
cogni- tive impairment (14, 30), which is about 8-10%
per year over 5 years (16). Rates of conversion from

346 Aging Clin Exp Res, Vol. 19, No.


Aging Clin Exp Res 19: 344-348, Cognitive impairment: classification and open
2007
about 4% to 40% per year. Estimates are difficult to
in- terpret, because the few studies have methodological
lim- itations, such as small groups of highly selected
pa- tients, lack of similar comparison groups, short
and lim- ited cognitive function evaluation, and failure
to control for demographic variables. Data show that
the relative risk of AD increases between 3 and 8
times among persons with MCI, with estimates
depending on the length of follow-up (16, 31). Only
one study is available regarding the rate of cognitive
decline in subjects with MCI and without cognitive
impairment (31). Among highly educated Catholic
clergy members with MCI, the relative risk of
developing AD is about three times high- er over an
average of almost 5 years of follow-up. A non- linear or
accelerating rate of cognitive decline among per- sons
with MCI with respect to those without cognitive im-
pairment has been assumed, although previous studies
re- ported a period of accelerated cognitive decline
both be- fore (32) and after (33) the time of clinical
diagnosis, when subjects with MCI were not separated
from those without cognitive impairment. Thus, the
non-linear de- cline prior to the onset of dementia is
presumed to be as- sociated with MCI at the transition
phase from normal- ity to dementia. Data from the
Rush Memory and Aging Project (34), a longitudinal
study on 786 community- based persons with MCI
and without cognitive impair- ment, showed that both
groups had a non-linear accel- erating rate of cognitive
decline and that MCI subjects had an additional faster
linear rate of decline.
Various longitudinal studies show that subjects who
develop dementia had cognitive decline at baseline or
during follow-up when compared with individuals
who did not become demented, and various cognitive
test scores decline more precipitously in converters
than in non-converters (35, 36, 37).
Neuropsychological evaluation can discriminate the
transient nature of the early stage of MCI, when
impairment is defined solely by the results of
neuropsychological test scores. Thus, the probability
of performing poorly on the first evalua- tion does not
exclude good performance at the next vis- it. Kryscio
et al. (38) describe several risk factors which affect
transition from a cognitively normal state to amnestic
MCI or dementia. Many potential factors for disease
progression have been identified; no single one or a
combination of factors has yet emerged as a clear
predictor. The risk of a conversion from a cognitively
normal state to MCI and risk factors, which modify the
risk of progression from MCI to dementia (prognostic
factors), are two different classes of risk factors.
Prospec- tive studies beginning at midlife can identify
true risk fac- tors for developing MCI. Depression was
associated with MCI over a 3-year follow-up in the
Mayo prospec- tive cohort (39). The relationship
between depression, cognitive decline and dementia
has not clarified (40, 41). Lesser education and
greater age are associated with

Aging Clin Exp Res, Vol. 19, No. 5


C. Basso, F. Limongi, P. Siviero, et Aging Clin Exp Res 19: 344-348,
2007

MCI (27, 42). The Cardiovascular Health Study shows


REFERENCES
an association with depression, Apo E4 genotype, cor-
1. Prichard JC. A treatise on insanity. Philadelphia: Haswell
tical atrophy, and infarcts identified by magnetic reso- Bar- rington and Hasswell, 1837.
nance imaging (42). No differences between MCI and
2. Kral VA. Senescent forgetfulness. Benign and malignant. Can
normal groups with respect to family history, age, sex, Med Assoc J 1962; 86: 257-60.
education, cognitive scores, Apo E4 genotype, depres- 3. Crook T, Bartus RT, Ferris SH, Whitehouse P, Cohen GD,
sion, medications, exposure to anesthesia, or hypop- Gershon S. Age-associated memory impairment: proposed
erfusion on single photon emission computed tomog- di- agnostic criteria and measures of clinical change: report of
raphy (SPECT) neuroimaging have been described a Na- tional Institute of Mental Health work group. Dev
Neuropsy- chol 1986; 2: 261-76.
(43). However, there are data that support the strong
4. Blackford RC, La Rue A. Criteria for diagnosing age
asso- ciation between Apo E4 genotype and the associated memory impairment. Proposed improvement
progres- sion of MCI to AD (21), as well as specific from the field. Dev Neuropsychol 1989; 5: 295-306.
memory or language test scores, which predict 5. Elby EM, Hogan DB, Parhad IM. Cognitive impairment in
cognitive decline and or progression to AD. Daly et al. non- demented elderly. Results from the Canadian Study of
(27) showed that functional impairment and Health and Ageing. Arch Neurol 1995; 52: 612-9.
difficulties with specific dai- ly functions involving 6. Feldman HH, Jacova C. Mild cognitive impairment. Am J Geri-
judgment and problem-solving discriminated subjects atr Psychiatry 2005; 13: 645-55.
who would or would not progress to AD within 3 years. 7. World Health Organisation. International Statistical Classification
of Diseases and Related Problems, 10th Revision (ICD-10),
In a Swedish community-based study (44, 45), anxiety Chapter V: Categories F00-F99: Mental, Behavioural and De-
symptoms, interpreted as part of the velopmental Disorders, Clinical Description and Diagnostic
neurodegenerative process, predicted progression to Guide- lines. Geneva: WHO, 1992.
MCI, whereas depressive symptoms were considered 8. Hanninen T, Koivisto K, Reinikainen KJ, et al. Prevalence of
to be preclinical signs of AD. Changes in cognitive per- age- ing-associated cognitive decline in an elderly population.
Age Age- ing 1996; 25: 201-5.
formance, such as episodic memory, showed a signifi-
9. Ritchie K, Artero S, Touchon J. Classification criteria for mild
cantly greater rate of decline over the follow-up period cog- nitive impairment: a population-based validation study.
among converters; executive functions were lower at Neurology 2001; 56: 37-42.
baseline in converters to AD than in stable MCI and 10. Zaudig M. A new systematic method of measurement and diag-
nor- mal subjects (46, 47). nosis of mild cognitive impairment and dementia according to
ICD-10 and DSM-II R criteria. Int Psychogeriatr 1992; 4
CONCLUSIONS (Suppl 2): 203-19.
Data show that the risk of dementia increases with 11. Di Carlo A, Baldereschi M, Amaducci L, et al. Cognitive im-
pairment without dementia in older people: prevalence
in- creasing age for all MCI definitions. The time to vascular risk factors, impact on disability. The Italian
convert to dementia is longer in younger (7.8 years) than Longitudinal Study on Aging. J Am Geriatr Soc 2000; 48: 775-
in elderly subjects (4.5-5.7 years). Sex and level of 82.
education do not appear to be associated with the risk 12. Petersen RC, Smith GE, Ivnik RJ, et al. Apolipoprotein E
of transition to de- mentia. Studies investigating the status as a predictor of the development of Alzheimer’s
disease in memory impairment individuals. JAMA 1995;
conversion from MCI to dementia should have a follow- 273: 1274-8.
up of more than 5 years, in order to detect all subjects 13. Petersen RC, Smith GE, Waring SC, Ivnik RJ, Kokmne E, Tan-
with transition (48). gelos EG. Aging, memory, and mild cognitive impairment. Int Psy-
Most subjects with MCI are aged 40 to 85 years, and chogeriatr 1997; 9 (Suppl 1): 65-9.
do not progress to dementia in the long term. The risk 14. Petersen RC, Stevens JC, Ganguli M, Tangalos EG, Cummings
of dementia in MCI subjects is closely dependent on age JL, DeKosky ST, Practice parameter: early detection of demen-
(49), and is low in subjects aged 40 to 50 and high in tia: mild cognitive impairment (an evidence based review). Report
of the Quality Standards Subcommittee of the American Acade-
those over 70. Despite what has been described in one my of Neurology. Neurology 2001; 56: 1133-42.
neu- rodegenerative study (28), MCI often represents 15. Frisoni GB, Fratiglioni L, Fastbon J, Guo Z, Viitanen M, Winblad
the pre- dementia stage of neurodegenerative disorders B. Prevalence of mild cognitive impairment in the population and
in elderly but rarely in younger subjects. physical health: data on 1,435 individuals aged 75 to 95. J
Although risk factors for AD are probably also risk Gerontol 2000; 55: M322-8.
factors for the development of MCI or progression to it, 16. Fisk JD, Merry HR, Rokwood K. Variations in case definition
af- fect prevalence but not outcomes of mild cognitive
no degree of correspondence has been found. This re- impairment. Neurology 2003; 61: 1179-84
flects the heterogeneity of MCI. Factors that predict no 17. Winblad B, Palmer K, Kivipelto M, et al. Mild cognitive
progression, as well as improvement or cognitive sta- impair- ment beyond controversies, towards a consensus: report
bility, should be identified. Recent studies demonstrate of the In- ternational Working Group on Mild Cognitive
the importance of MCI as a clinical entity which not on- Impairment. J In- tern Med 2004; 256: 240-6.
ly predicts progression to dementia, but also functional 18. Petersen RC. Mild cognitive impairment as a diagnostic entity.
J Intern Med 2004; 256: 183-94.
decline in activities that reflect autonomy and the qual-
ity of life (50). 19. Graham JE, Rockwood K, Beattie BL, et al. Prevalence and

348 Aging Clin Exp Res, Vol. 19, No.


Aging Clin Exp Res 19: 344-348, Cognitive impairment: classification and open
2007

severity of cognitive impairment with and without dementia in


an elderly population. Lancet 1997; 349: 1793-6. 36. Fabrigoule C, Rouch I, Taberly A, et al. Cognitive process in the
preclinical phase of dementia. Brain 1998; 121: 135-41.
20. Ritchie K, Ledesert BB, Touchon J. Subclinical cognitive im-
pairment: epidemiology and clinical characteristics. Compr Psy- 37. Chen P, Ratcliff G, Belle SH, Cauley JA, Dekosky ST, Ganguli
chiatry 2000; 41: 61-5. M. Cognitive tests that best discriminate between
presymptomatic AD and those who remain nondemented.
21. Hogan DB, Ebly EM. Predicting who will develop dementia in Neurology 2000; 55: 1847-53.
a cohort of Canadian seniors. Can J Neurol Sci 2000; 27: 18-
14. 38. Kryscio RJ, Schmitt FA, Salazar JC, Mendiondo MS,
Markesbery WR. Risk factors for transition from normal to
22. Helkala EL, Koivisto K, Hannine T, et al. Stability of age asso- mild cognitive im- pairment and dementia. Neurology 2006;
ciated memory impairment during a longitudinal population- 66: 828-32.
based study. J Am Geriatr Soc 1997; 45: 120-2.
39. Geda I, Knopman D, Rocca W, et al. Depression is associated
23. Daly E, Zaitchik D, Copeland M, Schmahmaa J, Aunther J, Al- with incident mild cognitive impairment (MCI): a
bert M. Predicting conversion to Alzheimer disease using stan- prospective cohort study. Int Psychogeriatr 2005; 17
darized clinical information. Arch Neurol 2000; 57: 675-80. (Suppl. 2): 170-1.
24. Palmer K, Wang HX, Bakman L, Windblad B, Fratiglioni L.
40. Ganguli M, Du Y, Dodge HH, et al. Depressive symptoms and
Differential evolution of cognitive impairment in nondemented
cognitive decline in late life. A prospective epidemiologic study.
old- er persons: results from the Kungsholmen Project. Am J
Arch Gen Psychiatry 2006; 63: 153-60.
Psy- chiatry 2002; 159: 4376-442.
41. Rapp MA, Schnaider-Beeri M, Grossman HT, et al. Increased
25. Johansson B, Zarit SH. Early cognitive markers of the
hip- pocampal plaques and tangles in patients with Alzheimer
incidence of dementia and mortality: a longitudinal study of
disease with a life time history of major depression. Arch Gen
the oldest old. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 1997; 12: 53-9.
Psychiatry 2006; 63: 161-7.
26. Schonknecht P, Pantel J, Kruse A, Schroder J. Prevalence and
42. Lopez OL, Jagust WJ, Dulberg C, et al. Risk factors for mild
natural course of aging-associated cognitive decline in a
cog- nitive impairment in the Cardiovascular Health Study
popula- tion-based sample of young-old subjects. Am J
Cogni- tion Study. Part 2. Arch Neurol 2003; 60: 1394-9.
Psychiatry 2005; 162: 2071-7.
27. Daly E, Zaitchik D, Copeland M, Schmahmann J, Gunther J, 43. Ritchie K, Arterom S, Touchon J. Classification criteria for
Al- bert M. Predicting conversion to Alzheimer disease using mild cognitive impairment: a population-based study.
stan- dardized clinical information. Arch Neurol 2000; 57: Neurology 2001; 56: 37-42.
675-80. 44. Palmer K, Berger A-K, Monastero R, et al. Neuropsychiatric
28. Ritchie K. Mild cognitive impairment. An epidemiological per- symptomatology in mild cognitive impairment: do anxiety and
spective. Dialogues Clin Neurosci 2004; 6: 401-8. de- pressive-related symptoms predict progression to
Alzheimer’s disease? Int Psychogeriatr 2005; 17 (suppl 2):
29. Ganguli M, Dodge HH, Shen C, DeKosky ST. Mild cognitive 170.
im- pairment, amnestic type: an epidemiologic study.
Neurology 2004: 63: 115-21. 45. Palmer K, Berger A-K, Monastero R, Winblad B, Backman L,
Fratiglioni L. Predictors of progression from mild cognitive im-
30. Morris JC, Storandt M, Miller P, et al. Mild cognitive pairment to Alzheimer disease. Neurology 2007; 68: 1596-
impairment represents early stage Alzheimer disease. Arch 602.
Neurol 2001; 58: 397-405.
46. Albert M, Blacker D, Moss MB, Tanzi R, McArdle JJ. Longitudinal
31. Bennet DA, Wilson RS, Schneider JA, et al. Natural history of change in cognitive performance among individuals with mild cog-
mild cognitive impairment in older persons. Neurology 2002; 59: nitive impairment. Neuropsychology 2007; 21: 158-69.
198-205.
47. Blacker D, Lee H, Muzikansky A, et al. Neuropsychological
32. Amieva H, Jacqmin-Gadda H, Orgogozo JM, et al. The 9-year measures in normal individuals that predict subsequent cognitive
cognitive decline before dementia of the Alzheimer type: a decline. Arch Neurol 2007; 64: 862-71.
prospective population based study. Brain 2005; 128: 1093-
101. 48. Visser P J, Kester A, Jolles J, Verhey F. Ten-year risk of dementia
in subjects with mild cognitive impairment. Neurology 2006;
33. Galvin JE, Powlishta KK, Wilkins K, et al. Predictors of 67: 1201-7.
preclin- ical Alzheimer disease and dementia: a
clinicopathologic study. Arch Neurol 2005; 62: 758-65. 49. Tyas SL, Salazar JC, Snowdon DA, et al. Transitions to mild
cog- nitive impairments, dementia and death: findings from
34. Petersen RC, Doody R, Kurz A, et al. Current concepts in mild the Nun Study. Am J Epidemiol 2007; 165: 1231-8.
cognitive impairment. Arch Neurol 2001, 58: 1985-92.
50. Wadley VG, Crowe M, Marsiske M, et al. Changes in everyday
35. Elias MF, Beiser A, Wolf PA, Au R, White RF, D’Agostino RB.
function in individuals with psychometrically defined mild cog-
The preclinical phase of Alzheimer Disease. A 22year
nitive impairment in the advanced cognitive training for Inde-
prospec- tive study of the Framingham cohort. Arch Neurol
pendent and Vital Elderly study. J Am Geriatr Soc 2007; 55:
2000; 57: 808-13.
1192-8.

Aging Clin Exp Res, Vol. 19, No. 5

You might also like