You are on page 1of 12

REVIEW ARTICLE

Working memory in early Alzheimers disease:


a neuropsychological review
J. D. Huntley and R. J. Howard
Section of Old Age Psychiatry, Institute of Psychiatry, London, UK
Correspondence to: Dr J. D. Huntley, E-mail: jonathan.huntley@iop.kcl.ac.uk

Background: Reports of the extent of working memory (WM) impairment in early Alzheimers disease
(AD) have been inconsistent. Using the model of WM proposed by Baddeley, neuropsychological
evidence for the impairment of WM in early AD is evaluated.
Method: Literature searches were performed using Medline, PsycINFO and Embase databases. Individual
papers were then examined for additional references not revealed by computerised searches.
Results: Phonological loop function is intact at the preclinical and early stages of AD, becoming more
impaired as the disease progresses. In mild AD, there is impairment on tasks assessing visuospatial
sketchpad (VSS) function; however, these tasks also require executive processing by the central executive
system (CES). There is evidence that the CES is impaired in mild AD and may be affected in the earlier
preclinical stage of the disease. Episodic buffer function may be impaired but further research is required.
Conclusions: Future research into central executive functioning at the earliest stages of the disease,
combined with further longitudinal studies, needs to be carried out. Tasks to assess the proposed
functions of the episodic buffer and specific tests of the VSS suitable for AD subjects need to be developed
and validated. Learning more about these processes and how they are affected in AD is important in
understanding and managing the cognitive deficits seen in the early stages of AD. Copyright # 2009 John
Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
Key words: working memory; Alzheimers Disease; central executive
History: Received 20 October 2008; Accepted 8 April 2009; Published online 11 August 2009 in Wiley InterScience
(www.interscience.wiley.com).
DOI: 10.1002/gps.2314

Introduction impairments of everyday tasks, such as keeping track


of conversations (Alberoni et al., 1992), walking whilst
The predominant cognitive deficit in Alzheimers talking (Camicioli et al., 1997) or packing a bag
disease (AD) is episodic memory impairment. This (Ramsden et al., 2008). Clarifying how WM is affected
inability to acquire, encode and retrieve memories is in early AD is therefore important in understanding
present at the earliest stages of the disease (Aggarwal the cognitive and functional difficulties faced by
et al., 2005) and progresses with disease severity. individuals with the disease and may aid early
There has been growing interest in how other diagnosis (Belleville et al., 2007).
cognitive processes, such as working memory (WM) Different theoretical models of WM have been
are affected in the early stages of the disease. WM proposed including the hypothesis that active repres-
encompasses aspects of attentional and executive entations of long term memory constitute WM
control and is involved in many cognitive processes. (Cowan, 1995; Ruchkin et al., 2003).
Deficits of WM in AD are therefore thought to However, in this review we use the influential model
contribute to a range of significant problems. For of WM proposed by Baddeley, which provides a useful
example, difficulties in dividing attention and manip- framework to evaluate neuropsychological evidence for
ulating remembered information are reflected in the impairment of WM in AD.

Copyright # 2009 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 2010; 25: 121132.
122 J. D. Huntley and R. J. Howard

Method As the length of the span sequence increases, the ability


to correctly recall the sequences decreases, with the
Literature searches were performed using Medline, length of the longest list a subject can correctly recall
PsycINFO and Embase databases combining search referred to as their memory span. These tasks have
terms Alzheimer disease, mild cognitive impairment or demonstrated that WM is limited in capacity with a
MCI, with WM, short term memory (STM), phono- span length of approximately 7 digits (Miller, 1956;
logical loop, phonological store, visuospatial sketchpad Baddeley, 2000). Subjects can be asked to recall the
(VSS), central executive, episodic buffer, digit span, dual span sequence in reverse, referred to as the backward
task, primary memory and short term forgetting. span. This is thought to require increased processing and
Individual papers were then examined for relevance therefore to involve the CES as well as the subsidiary
and for additional references not revealed by compu- system (Carlesimo et al., 1994; Cherry et al., 1996).
terised searches.

Models of working memory Impairment of memory span tasks in AD

The concept of STM was based on the model of Several studies using digit and word spans have found
Atkinson and Shiffrin (1968). STM was conceptualised them reduced in AD subjects compared to elderly
as a temporary store, through which information passed controls (Miller, 1973; Morris, 1984; Kopelman, 1985;
prior to being encoded in long term memory or retrieved Morris, 1987a; Becker, 1988; Spinnler et al., 1988;
from long-term memory stores (Atkinson and Shiffrin, Hulme et al., 1993; Cherry et al., 1996), with no
1968). Neuropsychological data cast doubts as to the role significant difference between elderly and young
of STM as a gatekeeper controlling input and output control groups (Belleville et al., 1996).
from long term memory through a simple serial process. However, as can be seen from Table 1, some studies
Patients were described with specific STM deficits but have found normal digit and word spans in individuals
who appeared to have preserved long term memory at the early or mild stages of AD (Martin et al., 1985;
(Shallice and Warrington, 1970) and vice versa Carlesimo et al., 1994; Perry et al., 2000). It appears
(Baddeley and Warrington, 1970). that rather than reflecting heterogeneity of WM deficits
These findings led Baddeley and Hitch to suggest a in AD, some of these conflicting results may be
new model of STM, consisting of subcomponents, explained by differences in disease severity amongst
which they termed the working memory model subjects. Some investigators have examined subjects at
(Baddeley and Hitch, 1974). According to this model, the very early stages of AD, referred to as minimal AD
information is held in subsidiary slave systems, the and at the preclinical or mild cognitive impairment
phonological loop, which holds verbal information (MCI) stage. There is evidence that digit spans are
and the visuospatial sketchpad (VSS), which holds preserved in MCI and minimal AD but become signi-
visual images. A third component of the model, the ficantly impaired across mild and moderate groups
central executive system (CES), acts as an attentional (Corkin, 1982; Orsini et al., 1988; Lines et al., 1991;
controller, allowing manipulation and executive Greene et al., 1995; Hodges and Patterson, 1995;
control of information within WM (Baddeley, 1996). Traykov et al., 2007). This may reflect initially normal
The functions of the central executive overlap conside- functioning within the phonological loop and CES
rably with other models of attention, including Norman which becomes impaired as the disease progresses.
and Shallices (1980) model of a supervisory attentional Longitudinal studies have found that digit span
system (SAS) (Norman and Shallice, 1980) and involve performance is normal at the preclinical stage of the
executive processes, such as the ability to shift and divide disease (Backman et al., 2001; Twamley et al., 2006)
attention, inhibit irrelevant information and manipu- and does not predict advancement to AD (Bondi et al.,
late information within verbal and visual stores. In 2000, 1994).
the model was extended by Baddeley to include an The Corsi block tapping test is used as a measure of
episodic buffer (Baddeley, 2000), placing WM at the spatial memory span. This involves encoding of visual
interface of episodic memory and executive function. stimuli, short term storage of spatial location and
sequence order and maintenance of information over
Investigating working memory time (Fischer, 2001). As reviewed in Table 2, impair-
ment on this task has been found in both mild and
Prior to the WM model, the function of STM was moderate AD groups compared to both young and
investigated using memory span tasks (Miller, 1956). elderly controls but not in MCI (Corkin, 1982; Orsini

Copyright # 2009 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 2010; 25: 121132.
Working memory in early Alzheimers disease 123

Table 1 Studies of digit and word span in AD

Study Subjects Severity of AD (scale used if not MMSE) Results AD (vs. EC)

(Miller, 1973) 15 AD Presenile dementia Impaired


15 EC No comment on severity
(Corkin, 1982) 18 AD MILD (n 1) Intact
15 EC MOD (n 8) Impaired
5 Global amnesia SEV (n 9) Impaired
(WAIS and clinical/functional evidence)
(Morris, 1984) 17 AD MOD Impaired
17 EC (Moderate severe anterograde amnesia
and moderately disorientated)
(Martin et al., 1985) 14 AD MILD Intact
11 EC (WAIS, WMS)
(Kopelman, 1985) 16 AD MILDMOD Impaired
16 Korsakoff (NART, IQ, Wechsler logical memory)
16 EC
(Morris, 1987a) 21 AD MILD Impaired
21 EC (WAIS and clinical evidence)
(Becker, 1988) 71 AD MILD (MMSE 22.5 (5.2)) Impaired
89 EC
(Spinnler et al., 1988) 29 AD MILDMOD Impaired
42 YC (Psychometric assessment)
58 EC
(Orsini et al., 1988) 51 AD MILD (n 24) Intact
30 EC MOD (n 27) Impaired
(History, orientation, visual retention test,
clinical judgement)
(Lines et al., 1991) 16 AD MILD (n 8, MMSE 22 (2026)) Intact
8 EC MOD (n 8, MMSE 1119) Impaired
(Hulme et al., 1993) 14 AD MILD Impaired
14 EC (relatively early stage of dementia,
Clifton assessment procedure)
(Carlesimo et al., 1994) 18 AD MILD Intact
18 MID (Global performance index)
26 EC
(Hodges and Patterson, 1995) 52 AD MIN (n 17, MMSE 25.6 (1.8)) Intact
24 EC MILD (n 17, MMSE 20.9 (1.7)) Impaired
MOD (n 18, MMSE 10 (4.6)) Impaired
(Greene et al., 1995) 33 AD MIN (n 17, MMSE 26.2 (2.2) Intact
30 EC MILD (n 16, MMSE 20.3 (3.3) Impaired
(Belleville et al., 1996) 20 AD MILD (n 9), Impaired
12 EC MOD (n 8),
12 YC MODSEV: (n 3)
(Reisberg global deterioration scale)
(Cherry et al., 1996) 49 AD MILD (n 26), Impaired
49 EC MOD (n 20),
SEV (n 3)
(Clinical dementia rating scale)
(Perry et al., 2000) 27 AD MIN (n 13 MMSE 26.08 (1.6)) Intact
77 EC MILD (n 14 MMSE 20.4 (2)) Intact
(Traykov et al., 2007) 20 MCI MMSE 28.95 (1.1) Intact
20 EC

AD: Alzheimers disease; EC: elderly controls; YC: young controls; MMSE: mini mental state examination (score (SD)); MIN: minimal; MOD: moderate;
WAIS: Wechsler adult intelligence scale; WMS: Wechsler memory scale; MCI: mild cognitive impairment; MID: multi infarct dementia.

Copyright # 2009 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 2010; 25: 121132.
124 J. D. Huntley and R. J. Howard

Table 2 Studies of spatial span in AD

Study Subjects Severity of AD (scale used if not MMSE) Results AD (vs. EC)

(Corkin, 1982) 18 AD MILD (n 1) Impaired


15 EC MOD (n 8) Impaired
5 Global amnesia SEV (n 9) Impaired
(WAIS and clinical/functional evidence)
(Spinnler et al., 1988) 29 AD MILDMOD Impaired
42 YC (Psychometric assessment)
58 EC
(Orsini et al., 1988) 51 AD MILD (n 24) Impaired
30 EC MOD (n 27)
(History, orientation, visual retention test,
clinical judgement)
(Lines et al., 1991) 16 AD MILD (n 8, MMSE 22 (2026)) Intact
8 EC MOD (n 8, MMSE 1119) Impaired
(Sahgal et al., 1992) 15 AD MILD (n 8, MMSE 2024) Impaired
8 EC MOD (n 7, MMSE 1519) Impaired
(Grossi et al., 1993) 39 AD MILD (MMSE21.4 (4.9)) Impaired
62 EC
(Carlesimo et al., 1994) 18 AD MILD Impaired
18 MID (Global performance index)
26 EC
(Trojano et al., 1994) 30 EC MILD (MMSE 22.6) Impaired
38 AD
(Cherry et al., 1996) 49 AD MILD (n 26) Impaired
MOD (n 20)
49 EC SEV (n 3)
(Clinical dementia rating scale)
(Traykov et al., 2007) 20 MCI MMSE 28.95 (1.1) Intact
20 EC

AD: Alzheimers disease; EC: elderly controls; YC: young controls; MMSE: mini mental state examination (score (SD)); MIN: minimal; MOD: moderate;
SEV: severe; WAIS: Wechsler adult intelligence scale; MID: multi infarct dementia; MCI: mild cognitive impairment.

et al., 1988; Spinnler et al., 1988; Sahgal et al., 1992; dissimilar items in STM (Conrad and Hull, 1964;
Grossi et al., 1993; Carlesimo et al., 1994; Trojano et al., Morris, 1994). Memory span capacity is therefore
1994; Cherry et al., 1996). As the Corsi block tapping greater when tested with sequences of phonologically
task requires both visuospatial storage and executive dissimilar items, than when tested with phonologically
processing (temporal sequencing), the impairment similar items, referred to as the phonological similarity
seen in early AD, on both forward and backward spatial effect (PSE).
spans, has been interpreted as reflecting deficits in both The second component is the articulatory rehearsal
the VSS and central executive (Carlesimo et al., 1994). mechanism (ARM). The function of the ARM has been
investigated using the word length effect (WLE)
(Baddeley et al., 1975). This refers to the observation
Specific subcomponents of WM that normal subjects are more able to recall lists of
shorter, compared to longer words, which require
Phonological loop. It is thought that two functional more time to be rehearsed and can therefore be
components make up the phonological loop. The first refreshed less frequently than short words. Another
is a phonological short term store (PSS); a temporary method for investigating ARM function is to measure
storage system, from which verbal information traces the degree to which memory span decreases when the
spontaneously fade after a few seconds. Evidence for ARM is suppressed by concurrent articulation (e.g.
this component comes from observations that similar mouthing an irrelevant word), which interferes with
sounding items are more difficult to remember than subvocal articulation by the ARM (Morris, 1994).

Copyright # 2009 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 2010; 25: 121132.
Working memory in early Alzheimers disease 125

Phonological loop function in AD (alphabet span and dual task paradigms). However,
when case severity was taken into account, as measured
The PSE, WLE and suppression of the ARM by by span performance, only those subjects with a low
concurrent articulation, have been used to investigate span had phonological loop deficits, whilst subjects
subcomponents and functioning of the phonological with high span generally had an intact phonological
loop in individuals with AD with inconsistent results, loop but showed impairment on CES tasks. The
reviewed in Table 3. authors therefore concluded that the earliest deficits in
Morris (1984) has attributed Millers (1972) findings WM may be due to impairments of the central
of a reduced PSE to a floor effect in individuals with executive, with phonological loop deficits occurring
severe AD and concluded that the phonological loop only in more severely demented patients. There was
was intact in AD, with observed deficits in memory some heterogeneity between AD subjects, however,
span tasks attributed to impairments in the central with three subjects showing deficits of the phonological
executive (Morris, 1994). loop but no difficulties on the tasks assessing the CES.
By contrast, other experimenters have found evidence (Collette et al., 1999b).
of phonological loop dysfunction in AD. Belleville et al. In a recent study, Peters et al. (2007) assessed
(1996) found evidence of a decreased PSE in AD but a subjects with mild AD and compared them to elderly
normal WLE. However, individual case analysis of AD and young healthy controls. AD subjects demonstrated
subjects revealed that, of those classed as having mild a similar PSE and WLE to those of control groups,
AD, all had normal WLE and 5 out of 6 of them had suggesting that the phonological loop is preserved in
normal PSE. All subjects with phonological loop the early stages of AD. Central executive function,
deficits also showed accompanying CES dysfunction assessed using a dual task paradigm, was significantly
(Belleville et al., 1996). impaired in AD subjects, again suggesting that in the
Collette et al. (1999b) examined subjects with mild early stages of AD the phonological loop is intact,
AD and normal elderly controls and found that AD however the CES is impaired, which may be
subjects had a decreased PSE and WLE and were also responsible for deficits seen on some verbal WM
impaired on tests of central executive function tasks. (Peters et al., 2007).

Table 3 Studies of phonological loop in AD

Study Subjects Severity of AD (scale used if not MMSE) Tasks Results AD (vs. EC)

(Miller, 1972) 14 AD Presenile dementia PSE Impaired


14 EC No comment on severity
(Morris, 1984) 17 AD MOD PSE Intact
17 EC (Moderately severe anterograde WLE Intact
amnesia and moderately disorientated)
(Morris, 1987b) 14 AD MILD CAE Intact
14 EC (WAIS, clinical evidence)
(Morris, 1987a) 21 AD MILD Articulation rate Intact
21 EC (WAIS, clinical evidence)
(Hulme et al., 1993) 14 AD MILD Speech rate Impaired
14 EC (relatively early stage of dementia,
Clifton assessment procedure)
(Belleville et al., 1996) 20 AD MILD (n 9), MOD (n 8) WLE Impaired
12 EC MODSEV (n 3)
12 YC (Reisberg global deterioration scale) PSE Intact
(Collette et al., 1999b) 20 AD MILD (MMSE 21.80 (4.75)) WLE Impaired
20 EC PSE Impaired
(Peters et al., 2007) 20 AD MILD (MMSE < 24) PSE Intact
20 EC WLE Intact
20 YC

AD: Alzheimers disease; EC: elderly controls; YC: young controls; MMSE: mini mental state examination (score (SD)); MIN: minimal; MOD: moderate;
SEV: severe; PSE: phonological similarity effect; WLE: word length effect; CAE: concurrent articulation effect.

Copyright # 2009 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 2010; 25: 121132.
126 J. D. Huntley and R. J. Howard

In summary, the available evidence suggests that in VSS functioning in AD


the early stages of AD the phonological loop is intact
and becomes impaired when the disease has progressed As reviewed above, and in Table 2, visuospatial memory
to the mild to moderate stage. Longitudinal studies span tasks, such as the Corsi block tapping task, have
specifically looking at the function of the phonological been used to show impairment in visuospatial WM in
loop and its subcomponents would be helpful in AD. Such impairment, however, is thought to reflect
clarifying the effects of disease progression. Although potential deficits in the functioning of both the VSS
the tasks described above seek to selectively test and central executive.
subcomponents of the phonological loop, it is Macpherson et al. (2007) have recently found that
impossible to entirely remove the contribution of AD subjects are impaired on the visual patterns task
attentional and executive processes. Therefore any suggesting VSS impairment in AD. Function within the
impairment of central executive function may still VSS has also been assessed using the immediate recall
cause deficits in those tasks used to predominantly stage of a delayed matching to sample task in AD
investigate subsidiary system function. patients. It is not clear, however, whether deficits on
this task seen in subjects with AD are due to specific
Visuospatial sketchpad. Logie (1995) proposed that VSS impairment. Money et al. (1992) reported a deficit
the VSS is comprised of distinct visual and spatial in an AD group irrespective of whether the stimuli
components, a visual temporary store where visual were shown simultaneously (therefore only requiring
information is represented and a spatial temporary matching) or sequentially (requiring use of memory
subsystem which rehearses the contents of the visual processes). It is therefore possible that the impairment
store and can be used to plan movement (Logie, 1995). was due to a perceptual deficit rather than an impaired
In order to investigate visual WM, a visual patterns VSS (Money et al., 1992).
task has been devised (Della Sala et al., 1999) which Due largely to the difficulties inherent in designing
involves exposing subjects to a pattern for a few tasks suitable for AD patients that satisfactorily separate
seconds before removing it and asking subjects to a passive visuospatial subsidiary storage system from
reproduce it. Della Sala (1999) reported two subjects more active executive processes, it has not been clearly
who were able to perform this task normally, but were established whether the VSS itself is specifically
impaired on the Corsi blocks task, and a subject who impaired in AD.
had the reverse impairment pattern (Della Sala et al.,
1999). Episodic buffer. Baddeley (2000) extended the WM
Further evidence for the distinction of visual and model to include a further subcomponent, the episodic
spatial components in the VSS comes from selective buffer. This was in response to various experimental
interference (Hecker and Mapperson, 1997). Spatial findings, for example, that grouping or chunking of
WM is impaired by a concurrent movement discrimi- information, which requires integration of informa-
nation task but not by a colour discrimination task, tion from LTM, improves recall and that articulatory
whereas visual object memory shows the opposite suppression has only limited effects on tasks involving
pattern of interference (Tresch et al., 1993). This visually presented information (Baddeley, 2000).
double dissociation provides evidence for the existence Given the complexity of the cognitive processes
of separate visual and spatial stores and also for ascribed to the episodic buffer, it is not surprising that
separate rehearsal mechanisms for visual and spatial it is difficult to identify clear ways of examining its
information, both independent of the central executive function. The processes involved in the grouping or
(Klauer and Zhao, 2004). chunking of information have been explored (Bor
In terms of visual WM, there is some evidence that et al., 2003; Bor et al., 2004). Attempts to separate the
basic features (e.g. colour, shape, orientation) are automatic binding that occurs in normal perception
independently stored in a set of parallel feature specific from the more active and demanding integrative
stores (Wheeler and Treisman, 2002). It is assumed processes that are assumed to play such an important
that these features can be bound together by a role in the episodic buffer, would be informative
mechanism that integrates various components of an (Baddeley, 2000). Tasks have been designed that assess
object for storage in WM, using attentional resources. a subjects ability to maintain verbal, object and spatial
This concept overlaps with the proposed functioning information in WM, either separately or combined, by
of a further component of the WM modelthe presenting objects in various locations on a grid and
episodic buffer (Baddeley, 2000; Repovs and Baddeley, assessing the ability to integrate and recall both object
2006). and spatial features. This approach has been used to

Copyright # 2009 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 2010; 25: 121132.
Working memory in early Alzheimers disease 127

investigate normal individuals (Prabhakaran et al., the CES in Baddeleys WM model has been its lack of
2000), the effects of age (Mitchell et al., 2000), schizo- specificity. A wide range of different tasks have been
phrenia (Burglen et al., 2004) and transient global used to assess attention and executive function and
amnesia (Quinette et al., 2006) on the integrative evidence for general attentional and executive deficits
abilities of the episodic buffer. in AD has been well reviewed elsewhere (Perry and
Hodges, 1999; Baddeley et al., 2001; Amieva et al.,
Episodic buffer function in AD 2004). Executive tasks often assess multiple cognitive
processes, some of which are incidental to their stated
There is some evidence that individuals with AD are purpose (Burgess, 1997) and are therefore difficult to
impaired in the integration and organisation of interpret. Factor analysis techniques have been used to
relevant information. Peters et al. (2007) reported identify key subprocesses underlying performance on a
that AD subjects were impaired on a task requiring range of executive tasks and have concluded that
integration of auditory and visual information (Peters executive functions are marked by both unity and
et al., 2007). Grober et al. (1985) investigated the ability diversity of function (Miyake et al., 2000) and therefore
to rank information by concept in subjects with AD no underlying factor can be considered completely
and found that they had difficulty organising material independent (Collette and Van der Linden, 2002).
(Grober et al., 1985). The ability of AD subjects to It has been repeatedly asserted, however, that a
group or chunk information on a word list learning major role of the CES is co-ordination of the subsidary
task has also been examined. AD subjects were systems and therefore testing dual task performance
impaired in their ability to use inherent associations provides a specific marker of CES function within WM
between words, which resulted in a lack of chunking (Baddeley et al., 1997). We have therefore concentrated
behaviour (Carlesimo et al., 1998). In a recent study, on dual task paradigms that assess divided attention
mild AD (mean MMSE 20.3) and very mild AD (mean and tasks that assess manipulation of information in
MMSE 26.39) subjects were examined in their ability to WM (alphabet span) as markers of CES function in
use organisational skills to cluster semantically similar AD.
words together and improve recall. This ability to
strategically organise information was interpreted as a CES function in AD
marker of episodic buffer function. Both elderly
controls and very mild AD subjects were able to use Using alphabet span tasks, patients with mild AD have
inherent organisational structures in the word list task been found to have impaired central executive function
to improve performance, however mild AD subjects (Collette et al., 1999b; Belleville et al., 2003). Belleville
were not. This was interpreted as reflecting relatively et al. (2007) studied mild AD patients, MCI subjects
preserved episodic buffer function in very mild AD, and elderly controls and found that AD patients, but
which becomes impaired as the disease progresses to not MCI subjects, were impaired on an alphabet span
the mild stage (Germano et al., 2008). The strategic task, suggesting that there is an impairment in the
organisation of information and use of relationships manipulation of information in WM in mild AD but
and associations between stimuli to support learning not in MCI (Belleville et al., 2007).
and recall in subjects with AD, involves many cognitive Dual task performance has been consistently
steps and goes beyond WM research. However, it has reported as impaired in AD (Baddeley et al., 1986;
been suggested that the inability to strategically Baddeley et al., 1991; Collette et al., 1999a; Baddeley
integrate information into a coherent, meaningful et al., 2001; Logie et al., 2004; Sebastian et al., 2006;
episodic representation may reflect impairment of the MacPherson et al., 2007; Peters et al., 2007). Although
episodic buffer in AD, which in turn may be involved it was an initial finding that performance on Brown-
in the episodic memory deficits seen in AD (Germano Peterson paradigms was sensitive to the degree of
and Kinsella, 2005). processing demands required (Morris, 1986; Morris
and Kopelman, 1986), there is evidence that impaired
Central executive. The main functions of the CES have dual task performance in AD reflects a specific deficit
been proposed to be co-ordination of the subsidiary in dividing attention in AD, rather than the result of a
systems, focusing, switching and dividing attention, more general processing speed deficit (Baddeley et al.,
inhibition and updating and manipulation of infor- 2001) or effect of task difficulty (Baddeley et al., 1991;
mation (Baddeley, 1996; Collette and Van der Linden, Logie et al., 2004).
2002). The CES therefore provides many attentional The studies reviewed in Table 4 suggest that there is
and executive functions within WM and a criticism of a deficit in dividing attention as measured using

Copyright # 2009 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 2010; 25: 121132.
128 J. D. Huntley and R. J. Howard

Table 4 Studies of CES in AD (divided attention and alphabet span tasks)

Study Subjects Severity of AD (scale used if not MMSE) Tasks Results AD


(vs. EC)

(Corkin, 1982) 18 AD MILD (n 1) BrownPeterson task Impaired


15 EC MOD (n 8)
5 Global amnesia SEV (n 9)
(WAIS and clinical/functional evidence)
(Kopelman, 1985) 16 AD MILDMOD BrownPeterson task Impaired
16 EC (NART, IQ, Wechsler logical memory)
16 Korsakoff
(Baddeley et al., 1986) 28 AD MILDMOD Dual task Impaired
28 EC (Ravens matrices, token test, orientation
20 YC task, everyday coping ability)
(Dannenbaum et al., 1988) 15 AD MILDMOD BrownPeterson task Impaired
15 EC (Mattis dementia rating scale)
15 depressed
(Kopelman, 1991) 6 AD MILDMOD BrownPeterson task Impaired
6 Korsakoff (Ravens matrices, logical memory, (using Corsi blocks)
6 EC object learning, IQ)
(Baddeley et al., 1991) 15 AD MILDMOD Dual task Impaired
18 EC (Ravens matrices, token test, orientation
task, everyday coping ability)
(Greene et al., 1995) 33 AD MIN (n 17, MMSE 26.2 (2.2)) Dual task Intact
30 EC MILD (n 16, MMSE 20.3 (3.3)) Impaired
(Collette et al., 1999b) 20 AD MILD (MMSE 21.80 (4.75)) Alphabet span Impaired
20 EC Dual task Impaired
(Perry and Hodges, 2000) 12 AD MIN (MMSE 26.2 (1.6) at baseline) Dual task Intact at
20 EC MILD (MMSE 23.3 (2.4) at year 1) baseline
Impaired
at year 1
(Perry et al., 2000) 31 AD MIN (n 17 MMSE 26.08 (1.6)) Dual task Intact
77 EC MILD (n 14 MMSE 20.4 (2)) Impaired
(Baddeley et al., 2001) 36 AD MILD (MMSE 19.94 (1.78)) Dual task Impaired
36 EC
36 YC
(Belleville et al., 2003) 23 AD MILD (MMSE 22.57 (1829)) Alphabet span Impaired
23 EC
15 YC
(Logie et al., 2004) 8 AD MILD (MMSE 21.9 (2.3)) Dual task Impaired
8 EC
8 YC
(Sebastian et al., 2006) 27 AD MILD (MMSE 20.37 (2.20)) Dual task Impaired
27 EC
30 YC
(Peters et al., 2007) 20 AD MILD (MMSE < 24) Dual task Impaired
20 EC
20 YC
(MacPherson et al., 2007) 15 AD MILD (MMSE 22.1 (1.8)) Dual task Impaired
20 EC
20 YC
(Belleville et al., 2007) 28 MCI MCI (MMSE 28.36 (1.98)) Alphabet span AD impaired
19 AD Adapted MCI intact
29 EC MILD (MMSE 24.65 (3.6)) BrownPeterson AD/MCI
task impaired

AD: Alzheimers disease; EC: elderly controls; YC: young controls; MMSE: mini mental state examination (score (SD)); MIN: minimal; MOD: moderate;
SEV: severe; WAIS: Wechsler adult intelligence scale; MID: multi infarct dementia. NART: national adult reading test, MCI: mild cognitive impairment.

Copyright # 2009 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 2010; 25: 121132.
Working memory in early Alzheimers disease 129

BrownPeterson and dual task paradigms in mild and Brocas area and premotor cortex, both in normal
moderate AD and that this worsens over the course of subjects (Paulesu et al., 1993; Salmon et al., 1996) and
the disease. However, there are conflicting reports of in AD (Collette et al., 1997).
the presence of impairments at the minimal AD/MCI Functional imaging studies have implicated several
stage. Greene et al. (1995) tested groups of minimal AD locations involved in the operation of the VSS, inclu-
subjects and mild AD subjects on a divided attention ding areas in frontal, posterior parietal and occipital
task and found that, although the minimal AD group cortex (Jonides et al., 1993; Smith and Jonides, 1998).
did show some impairment, only mild AD patients There is also evidence that premotor and right superior
were significantly impaired on divided attention tasks parietal cortex may mediate spatial storage and
(Greene et al., 1995). Perry et al. (2000) investigated rehearsal, while inferior parietal areas mediate object
subjects with minimal AD, mild AD and elderly storage (Smith and Jonides, 1998; Wager and Smith,
controls using forward and backward digit spans and a 2003). Although a wide range of brain areas are
dual task paradigm. There was no significant difference activated in WM tasks, the localisation of areas
in dual task performance between the minimal AD and underlying basic verbal and visuospatial WM is likely
control groups, however the mild AD subjects were to contribute to variability in performance on verbal
significantly impaired (Perry et al., 2000). and nonverbal WM tasks in AD. There is some
Perry and Hodges (2000), in a longitudinal study of evidence of asymmetry of regional hypometabolism, as
minimal AD patients, examined dual task performance demonstrated by PET, at mild-moderate stages of the
at baseline (mean MMSE 26.2) and at 1 year (mean disease, with left hypometabolism associated with
MMSE 23.3) compared with controls and found no impairment on verbal compared to visuospatial tasks
significant difference at baseline but a significant in AD (Haxby et al., 1990; Zahn et al., 2004). A further
difference after 1 year (Perry and Hodges, 2000). Peters observation is that individuals with early onset AD
et al. (2007) investigated a group of mild AD subjects have more pronounced left sided hypometabolism of
and young and elderly controls on a dual task frontal and parietal areas, compared to late onset AD
paradigm and found that AD subjects were signifi- patients, and this has been associated with impaired
cantly impaired compared to elderly controls (Peters verbal WM performance (Kalpouzos et al., 2005).
et al., 2007). Belleville et al. (2007) used alphabet span Therefore, although general patterns of verbal and
and dual task paradigms on early AD patients, MCI visuospatial WM impairment can be seen in early AD,
subjects and elderly controls. Both the AD and MCI factors such as age of onset and asymmetry of cortical
groups were significantly impaired compared to the pathology are likely to contribute to the heterogeneity
controls on the dual task and, whilst MCI subjects were of WM deficits reported at the early stage of the disease.
not significantly impaired on the alphabet span task, a Although the CES is a theoretical concept, several
third did show some impairment, with disease severity studies have attempted to identify the neurological
positively correlated with deficits on both tasks basis of CES function. Using alphabet span tasks,
(Belleville et al., 2007). several groups have found activation in dorsolateral
These results strongly suggest that early AD subjects prefrontal cortex (PFC) and left parietal cortex during
show impairment in the ability to perform two tasks the manipulation of information in WM (Collette
simultaneously and to manipulate information main- et al., 1999a; DEsposito et al., 1999; Postle et al., 1999).
tained in WM. Both of these are considered to be Dual task paradigms have also been examined. An
functions of the central executive, independent of early fMRI study identified activation in dorsolateral
storage deficits or processing speed (Peters et al., 2007). PFC and anterior cingulate gyrus when tasks were
It is clear these deficits are present at an early stage of performed together but not when performed separ-
the disease and it has been suggested that dual task ately, interpreted as reflecting CES functioning
impairment may be a useful diagnostic marker for early (DEsposito et al., 1995). However, subsequent studies
AD (Belleville et al., 2007) although there are conflic- have reported no additional activations during dual
ting reports of the presence of impairment at an earlier task co-ordination, with dual task performance
minimal AD or MCI stage. associated with activation in a network of frontal
and parietal areas already activated by single tasks
Neurological correlates of working memory (Klingberg, 1998; Adcock et al., 2000; Bunge et al.,
2000). This suggests that the dual task co-ordination
A number of studies of verbal WM using PET have role of the CES relies on interactions between cerebral
localised the PSS to the left supramarginal gyrus and areas already activated in single tasks rather than on the
the ARM to left hemisphere speech areas, such as recruitment of additional cortical areas.

Copyright # 2009 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 2010; 25: 121132.
130 J. D. Huntley and R. J. Howard

Key Points Declaration of Interest


 Phonological loop function is intact at the None.
preclinical and early stages of AD.
 Central executive function is impaired in mild AD
and may be affected during the preclinical stage.
 Further research into central executive and Acknowledgements
episodic buffer function at the earliest stages of
AD is required. We thank Richard Brown and two anonymous
reviewers for helpful comments on an earlier draft.

The N back task has also been used in fMRI studies


as a test of WM, and activations have been found in a References
range of areas including dorsolateral PFC, inferior
frontal cortex, anterior cingulate and posterior parietal Adcock RA, Constable RT, Gore JC, Goldman-Rakic PS. 2000. Functional
neuroanatomy of executive processes involved in dual-task performance.
cortex (Cohen et al., 1997; Owen et al., 2005). Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 97: 35673572.
Therefore it seems likely that the function of the CES Aggarwal NT, Wilson RS, Beck TL, Bienias JL, Bennett DA. 2005. Mild
as measured by alphabet span, dual task and N back cognitive impairment in different functional domains and incident
Alzheimers disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 76: 14791484.
tasks involves a network of prefrontal, frontal and Alberoni M, Baddeley A, Della Sala S, Logie R, Spinnler H. 1992. Keeping
parietal areas. track of a conversation: Impairments in Alzheimers Disease. Int J Geriatr
In AD, there is evidence of regional frontal lobe Psychiatry 7: 639646.
Amieva H, Phillips LH, Della Sala S, Henry JD. 2004. Inhibitory functioning
degeneration and hypoperfusion (Waldemar et al., in Alzheimers disease. Brain 127: 949964.
1994), however, there is increasing evidence that AD Atkinson R, Shiffrin R. 1968. Human memory: a proposed system and its
pathology is associated with breakdown of effective control processes. In The Psychology of learning and motivation: advances
in research and theory, Spence JT, Spence KW (eds). Academic Press:
connectivity between neuronal systems (Delbeuck London. 89195.
et al., 2003). Neuroimaging connectivity studies also Backman L, Small BJ, Fratiglioni L. 2001. Stability of the preclinical
provide evidence for disconnection in AD (Grady et al., episodic memory deficit in Alzheimers disease. Brain 124: 96102.
Baddeley A. 1996. The fractionation of working memory. Proc Natl Acad
2001) and this may play a key role in the deficits Sci U S A 93: 1346813472.
observed in the CES (Collette et al., 1999c). Baddeley A. 2000. The episodic buffer: a new component of working
memory? Trends Cogn Sci 4: 417423.
Baddeley AD, Baddeley HA, Bucks RS, Wilcock GK. 2001. Attentional
control in Alzheimers disease. Brain 124: 14921508.
SUMMARY Baddeley AD, Bressi S, Della Sala S, Logie R, Spinnler H. 1991. The decline
of working memory in Alzheimers disease. A longitudinal study. Brain
114(Pt 6): 25212542.
There is considerable evidence that the central Baddeley A, Della Sala S, Gray C, Papagno C, Spinnler H. 1997. Testing
executive is affected at an early stage of AD, with central executive functioning with a pencil-and-paper test. In Method-
subsidiary systems affected later, as the disease ology of Frontal and Executive Function, Rabbitt P (ed.). Psychology Press
Ltd.: Hove.
becomes moderately severe. The evidence that central Baddeley AD, Hitch GJ. 1974. Working memory. In The Psychology of
executive function shows some impairment at the very Learning and Motivation: Advances in Research and Theory. Bower GH
early stages of the disease makes investigation of this (ed.). Academic Press: New York.
Baddeley A, Logie R, Bressi S, Della Sala S, Spinnler H. 1986. Dementia
component an important part of the assessment of and working memory. Q J Exp Psychol A 38: 603618.
subjects at early stages of AD. Future research into the Baddeley AD, Thomson N, Buchanan M. 1975. Word length and the
extent of central executive impairment at the earliest structure of short-term memory. Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal
Behavior 14: 575589.
stages of the disease, combined with further longi- Baddeley AD, Warrington EK. 1970. Amnesia and the distinction between
tudinal studies, needs to be carried out. Tasks to assess long- and short-term memory. Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal
the proposed functions of the episodic buffer and Behavior 9: 176189.
Becker JT. 1988. Working memory and secondary memory deficits in
specific tests of the VSS suitable for AD subjects need to Alzheimers disease. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 10: 739753.
be developed and validated. Belleville S, Chertkow H, Gauthier S. 2007. Working memory and control
Learning more about these processes and how they of attention in persons with Alzheimers disease and mild cognitive
impairment. Neuropsychology 21: 458469.
are affected in AD may enable the development of Belleville S, Peretz I, Malenfant D. 1996. Examination of the working
management strategies, both pharmacological and memory components in normal aging and in dementia of the Alzheimer
non-pharmacological, to overcome some of the effects type. Neuropsychologia 34: 195207.
Belleville S, Rouleau N, Van der Linden M, Collette F. 2003. Effect of
of the cognitive impairments faced by individuals manipulation and irrelevant noise on working memory capacity of
suffering from AD. patients with Alzheimers dementia. Neuropsychology 17: 6981.

Copyright # 2009 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 2010; 25: 121132.
Working memory in early Alzheimers disease 131

Bondi MW, Monsch AU, Galasko D, et al. 1994. Preclinical cognitive Germano C, Kinsella G, Storey E, Ong B, Ames D. 2008. The episodic buffer
markers of dementia of the Alzheimer type. Neuropsychology 8: 374384. and learning in early Alzheimers disease. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 30:
Bor D, Cumming N, Scott CE, Owen AM. 2004. Prefrontal cortical 627638.
involvement in verbal encoding strategies. Eur J Neurosci 19: 33653370. Grady CL, Furey ML, Pietrini P, Horwitz B, Rapoport SI. 2001. Altered
Bor D, Duncan J, Wiseman RJ, Owen AM. 2003. Encoding strategies brain functional connectivity and impaired short-term memory in
dissociate prefrontal activity from working memory demand. Neuron 37: Alzheimers disease. Brain 124: 739756.
361367. Greene JD, Hodges JR, Baddeley AD. 1995. Autobiographical memory and
Bunge SA, Klingberg T, Jacobsen RB, Gabrieli JD. 2000. A resource model executive function in early dementia of Alzheimer type. Neuropsychologia
of the neural basis of executive working memory. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 33: 16471670.
97: 35733578. Grober E, Buschke H, Kawas C, Fuld P. 1985. Impaired ranking of
Burgess P. 1997. Theory and methodology in executive function research. semantic attributes in dementia. Brain Lang 26: 276286.
In Methodology of Frontal and Executive Function, Rabbitt P (ed.). Grossi D, Becker JT, Smith C, Trojano L. 1993. Memory for visuospatial
Psychology Press Ltd.: Hove. patterns in Alzheimers disease. Psychol Med 23: 6570.
Burglen F, Marczewski P, Mitchell KJ, et al. 2004. Impaired performance in Haxby JV, Grady CL, Koss E, et al. 1990. Longitudinal study of cerebral
a working memory binding task in patients with schizophrenia. Psy- metabolic asymmetries and associated neuropsychological patterns in
chiatry Res 125: 247255. early dementia of the Alzheimer type. Arch Neurol 47: 753760.
Camicioli R, Howieson D, Lehman S, Kaye J. 1997. Talking while walking: Hecker R, Mapperson B. 1997. Dissociation of visual and spatial processing
the effect of a dual task in aging and Alzheimers disease. Neurology 48: in working memory. Neuropsychologia 35: 599603.
955958. Hodges JR, Patterson K. 1995. Is semantic memory consistently impaired
Carlesimo GA, Fadda L, Lorusso S, Caltagirone C. 1994. Verbal and spatial early in the course of Alzheimers disease? Neuroanatomical and diag-
memory spans in Alzheimers and multi-infarct dementia. Acta Neurol nostic implications. Neuropsychologia 33: 441459.
Scand 89: 132138. Hulme C, Lee G, Brown GD. 1993. Short-term memory impairments in
Carlesimo GA, Mauri M, Graceffa AM, et al. 1998. Memory performances Alzheimer-type dementia: evidence for separable impairments of
in young, elderly, and very old healthy individuals versus patients with articulatory rehearsal and long-term memory. Neuropsychologia 31:
Alzheimers disease: evidence for discontinuity between normal and 161172.
pathological aging. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 20: 1429. Jonides J, Smith EE, Koeppe RA, et al. 1993. Spatial working memory in
Cherry BJ, Buckwalter JG, Henderson VW. 1996. Memory span pro- humans as revealed by PET. Nature 363: 623625.
cedures in Alzheimers disease. Neuropsychology 10: 286293. Kalpouzos G, Eustache F, De La Sayette V, et al. 2005. Working memory
Cohen JD, Perlstein WM, Braver TS, et al. 1997. Temporal dynamics of and FDG-PET dissociate early and late onset Alzheimer disease patients.
brain activation during a working memory task. Nature 386: 604608. J Neurol 252: 548558.
Collette F, Van der Linden M. 2002. Brain Imaging of the central executive Klauer KC, Zhao Z. 2004. Double dissociations in visual and spatial short-
component of working memory. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 26: 105125. term memory. J Exp Psychol Gen 133: 355381.
Collette F, Salmon E, Van der Linden M, Degueldre C, Franck G. 1997. Klingberg T. 1998. Concurrent performance of two working memory tasks:
Functional anatomy of verbal and visuospatial span tasks in Alzheimers potential mechanisms of interference. Cereb Cortex 8: 593601.
disease. Hum Brain Mapp 5: 110118. Kopelman MD. 1985. Rates of forgetting in Alzheimer-type dementia and
Collette F, Salmon E, Van der Linden M, et al. 1999a. Regional brain Korsakoffs syndrome. Neuropsychologia 23: 623638.
activity during tasks devoted to the central executive of working memory. Kopelman MD. 1991. Non-verbal, short-term forgetting in the alcoholic
Brain Res Cogn Brain Res 7: 411417. Korsakoff syndrome and Alzheimer-type dementia. Neuropsychologia 29:
Collette F, Van der Linden M. 2002. Brain imaging of the central executive 737747.
component of working memory. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 26: 105125. Lines CR, Dawson C, Preston GC, et al. 1991. Memory and attention in
Collette F, Van der Linden M, Bechet S, Salmon E. 1999b. Phonological patients with senile dementia of the Alzheimer type and in normal elderly
loop and central executive functioning in Alzheimers. disease. Neurop- subjects. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 13: 691702.
sychologia 37: 905918. Logie R. 1995. Visuo-spatial Working Memory. Erlbaum: Hove, UK.
Collette F, Van der Linden M, Salmon E. 1999c. Executive dysfunction in Logie RH, Cocchini G, Delia Sala S, Baddeley AD. 2004. Is there a specific
Alzheimers disease. Cortex 35: 5772. executive capacity for dual task coordination? Evidence from Alzheimers
Conrad R, Hull AJ. 1964. Information, acoustic confusion and memory disease. Neuropsychology 18: 504513.
span. Br J Psychol 55: 429432. Macpherson SE, Della Sala S, Logie RH, Wilcock GK. 2007. Specific AD
Corkin S. 1982. Some relationships between global amnesias and the impairment in concurrent performance of two memory tasks. Cortex 43:
memory impairments in Alzheimers disease. In Alzheimers Disease: A 858865.
Report of Progress in Research, Vol. 19, Corkin S Davis KL Growdon JH Martin A, Brouwers P, Cox C, Fedio P. 1985. On the nature of the verbal
Usdin E Wurtman RJ (eds). Raven Press: New York. memory deficit in Alzheimers disease. Brain Lang 25: 323341.
Cowan N. 1995. Attention and Memory: An Integrated Framework. Oxford Miller GA. 1956. The magical number seven, plus or minus two: some
University Press: New York. limits on our capacity for processing information. Psychol Rev 63:
DEsposito M, Detre JA, Alsop DC, et al. 1995. The neural basis of the 8197.
central executive system of working memory. Nature 378: 279281. Miller E. 1972. Efficiency of coding and the short-term memory deficit in
DEsposito M, Postle BR, Ballard D, Lease J. 1999. Maintenance versus presenile dementia. Neuropsychologia 10: 221224.
manipulation of information held in working memory: an event-related Miller E. 1973. Short and long-term memory in patients with presenile
fMRI study. Brain Cogn 41: 6686. dementia (Alzheimers disease). Psychol Med 3: 221224.
Dannenbaum S, Parkinson S, Inman V. 1988. Short-term forgetting: Mitchell KJ, Johnson MK, Raye CL, DEsposito M. 2000. fMRI evidence of
comparisons between patients with dementia of the Alzheimer type, age-related hippocampal dysfunction in feature binding in working
depressed, and normal elderly. Cogn Neuropsychol 5: 213233. memory. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res 10: 197206.
Delbeuck X, Van der Linden M, Collette F. 2003. Alzheimers disease as a Miyake A, Friedman NP, Emerson MJ, et al. 2000. The unity and diversity
disconnection syndrome? Neuropsychol Rev 13: 7992. of executive functions and their contributions to complex Frontal Lobe
Della Sala S, Gray C, Baddeley A, Allamano N, Wilson L. 1999. Pattern tasks: a latent variable analysis. Cognit Psychol 41: 49100.
span: a tool for unwelding visuo-spatial memory. Neuropsychologia 37: Money EA, Kirk RC, Mcnaughton N. 1992. Alzheimers dementia
11891199. produces a loss of discrimination but no increase in rate of
Fischer MH. 2001. Probing spatial working memory with the Corsi Blocks memory decay in delayed matching to sample. Neuropsychologia 30:
task. Brain Cogn 45: 143154. 133143.
Germano C, Kinsella GJ. 2005. Working memory and learning in early Morris RG. 1984. Dementia and the functioning of the articulatory loop
Alzheimers disease. Neuropsychol Rev 15: 110. system. Cogn Neuropsychol 1: 143157.

Copyright # 2009 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 2010; 25: 121132.
132 J. D. Huntley and R. J. Howard

Morris RG. 1986. Short-Term Forgetting in Senile Dementia of the Repovs G, Baddeley A. 2006. The multi-component model of working
Alzheimers Type. Cogn Neuropsychol 3: 7797. memory: explorations in experimental cognitive psychology. Neuro-
Morris RG. 1987a. Articulatory rehearsal in Alzheimer type dementia. Brain science 139: 521.
Lang 30: 351362. Ruchkin DS, Grafman J, Cameron K, Berndt RS. 2003. Working memory
Morris RG. 1987b. The effect of concurrent articulation on memory span in retention systems: a state of activated long-term memory. Behav Brain Sci
Alzheimer-type dementia. Br J Clin Psychol 26: 233234. 26: 709728 (Discussion 728777).
Morris RG. 1994. Working memory in Alzheimer-type dementia. Neu- Sahgal A, Lloyd S, Wray CJ, et al. 1992. Does visuospatial memory in senile
ropsychology 8: 544554. dementia of the Alzheimer type depend on the severity of the disorder?
Morris RG, Kopelman MD. 1986. The memory deficits in Alzheimer-type Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 7: 427436.
dementia: a review. Q J Exp Psychol A 38: 575602. Salmon E, Van der Linden M, Collette F, et al. 1996. Regional brain activity
Norman DA, Shallice T. 1980. Attention to action: willed and automatic during working memory tasks. Brain 119(Pt 5): 16171625.
control of behaviour. University of California, San Diego. CHIP Report 99. Sebastian MV, Menor J, Elosua MR. 2006. Attentional dysfunction of the
Orsini A, Trojano L, Chiacchio L, Grossi D. 1988. Immediate memory central executive in AD: evidence from dual task and perseveration
spans in dementia. Percept Mot Skills 67: 267272. errors. Cortex 42: 10151020.
Owen AM, McMillan KM, Laird AR, Bullmore E. 2005. N-back working Shallice T, Warrington EK. 1970. Independent functioning of verbal
memory paradigm: a meta-analysis of normative functional neuroima- memory stores: a neuropsychological study. Q J Exp Psychol 22: 261273.
ging studies. Hum Brain Mapp 25: 4659. Smith EE, Jonides J. 1998. Neuroimaging analyses of human working
Paulesu E, Frith CD, Frackowiak RS. 1993. The neural correlates of the memory. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 95: 1206112068.
verbal component of working memory. Nature 362: 342345. Spinnler H, Della Sala S, Bandera R, Baddeley A. 1988. Dementia, ageing,
Perry RJ, Hodges JR. 1999. Attention and executive deficits in Alzheimers and the structure of human memory. Cogn Neuropsychol 5: 193
disease: a critical review. Brain 122(Pt 3): 383404. 211.
Perry RJ, Hodges JR. 2000. Fate of patients with questionable (very mild) Traykov L, Raoux N, Latour F, et al. 2007. Executive functions deficit in
Alzheimers disease: longitudinal profiles of individual subjects decline. mild cognitive impairment. Cogn Behav Neurol 20: 219224.
Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 11: 342349. Tresch MC, Sinnamon HM, Seamon JG. 1993. Double dissociation of
Perry RJ, Watson P, Hodges JR. 2000. The nature and staging of attention spatial and object visual memory: evidence from selective interference in
dysfunction in early (minimal and mild) Alzheimers disease: relationship intact human subjects. Neuropsychologia 31: 211219.
to episodic and semantic memory impairment. Neuropsychologia 38: Trojano L, Chiacchio L, De Luca G, Fragassi NA, Grossi D. 1994. Effect of
252271. testing procedure on Corsis block-tapping task in normal subjects and
Peters F, Majerus S, Olivier L, et al. 2007. A multicomponent exploration Alzheimer-type dementia. Percept Mot Skills 78: 859863.
of verbal short-term storage deficits in normal aging and Alzheimers Twamley EW, Ropacki SA, Bondi MW. 2006. Neuropsychological and
disease. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 29: 405417. neuroimaging changes in preclinical Alzheimers disease. J Int Neurop-
Postle BR, Berger JS, DEsposito M. 1999. Functional neuroanatomical sychol Soc 12: 707735.
double dissociation of mnemonic and executive control processes con- Wager TD, Smith EE. 2003. Neuroimaging studies of working memory: a
tributing to working memory performance. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 96: meta-analysis. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci 3: 255274.
1295912964. Waldemar G, Bruhn P, Kristensen M, et al. 1994. Heterogeneity of
Prabhakaran V, Narayanan K, Zhao Z, Gabrieli JD. 2000. Integration of neocortical cerebral blood flow deficits in dementia of the Alzheimer
diverse information in working memory within the frontal lobe. Nat type: a [99mTc]-d,l-HMPAO SPECT study. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychia-
Neurosci 3: 8590. try 57: 285295.
Quinette P, Guillery-Girard B, Noel A, et al. 2006. The relationship Wheeler ME, Treisman AM. 2002. Binding in short-term visual memory.
between working memory and episodic memory disorders in transient J Exp Psychol Gen 131: 4864.
global amnesia. Neuropsychologia 44: 25082519. Zahn R, Juengling F, Bubrowski P, et al. 2004. Hemispheric asymmetries of
Ramsden CM, Kinsella GJ, Ong B, Storey E. 2008. Performance hypometabolism associated with semantic memory impairment in
of everyday actions in mild Alzheimers disease. Neuropsychology 22: Alzheimers disease: a study using positron emission tomography with
1726. fluorodeoxyglucose-F18. Psychiatry Res 132: 159172.

Copyright # 2009 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 2010; 25: 121132.

You might also like