You are on page 1of 8

Psychon Bull Rev (2014) 21:1481–1488

DOI 10.3758/s13423-014-0635-0

BRIEF REPORT

The politics of color: Preferences for Republican red versus


Democratic blue
Karen B. Schloss & Stephen E. Palmer

Published online: 15 April 2014


# Psychonomic Society, Inc. 2014

Abstract The present study reveals that Election Day differ- On election night, millions of Americans watched US maps
entially affects the color preferences of US Republicans and change color as voting results were broadcast on TV and the
Democrats. Voters’ preferences for Republican red and Dem- Internet. Little did they know that their color preferences were
ocratic blue were assessed, along with several distractor changing as well. Intense media discussion of “red states” and
colors, on and around the 2010 interim and 2012 presidential “blue states” leading up to 21st-century US elections leaves
elections. On non-Election Days, Republicans and Democrats little doubt that politically knowledgeable Americans associ-
preferred Republican red equally, and Republicans actually ate red with Republicans and blue with Democrats. Is it
preferred Democratic blue more than Democrats did. On possible that those political associations with colors would
Election Day, however, Republicans’ and Democrats’ color affect voters’ color preferences?
preferences changed to become more closely aligned with Mounting empirical evidence suggests that color prefer-
their own party’s colors. Republicans liked Republican red ences are shaped by positive/negative experiences with corre-
more than Democrats did, and no longer preferred Democratic spondingly colored objects/entities (Palmer & Schloss, 2010;
blue more than Democrats did. These results are consistent Schloss, Poggesi, & Palmer, 2011; Strauss, Schloss, &
with the hypothesis that color preferences are determined by Palmer, 2013; Taylor & Franklin, 2012). These findings are
people’s preferences for correspondingly colored objects/ consistent with the ecological valence theory’s (EVT’s) as-
entities (Palmer & Schloss in Proceedings of the National sumption that preference for a given color is determined by the
Academy of Sciences 107:8877–8882, 2010). They further combined preferences for all objects/entities associated with
suggest that color preferences are calculated at a given mo- that color (Palmer & Schloss, 2010). Supporting the EVT,
ment, depending on which color–object associations are cur- 80 % of the variance in average color preferences was ex-
rently most activated or salient. Color preferences are thus far plained by the weighted affective valence estimates (WAVEs)
more dynamic and context-dependent than has previously of 32 colors: the average positivity/negativity ratings of all
been believed. objects that people associated with the colors, weighted by
how well the colors matched the objects’ colors (Palmer &
Schloss, 2010). Further evidence suggests that object prefer-
Keywords Color preferences . Visual aesthetics . Political ences causally influence color preferences, because color
orientation . Ecological valence theory (EVT) preferences have been increased/decreased in the laboratory
by experiences with positive/negative colored objects (Strauss
et al., 2013). Furthermore, affiliation with rival universities
(UC Berkeley and Stanford) has been found to influence
preference for school colors: Students liked their own
K. B. Schloss (*)
Department of Cognitive, Linguistic, and Psychological Sciences, university’s colors more than their rivals did, to a degree that
Brown University, 190 Thayer Street, Providence, RI 02912, USA was positively correlated with self-reported school spirit
e-mail: karenschloss@gmail.com (Schloss et al., 2011).
Exactly how experiences with characteristically colored
S. E. Palmer
Department of Psychology, University of California, Berkeley, CA, objects/entities combine to produce color preferences is un-
USA known. Does the brain maintain a running average of the
1482 Psychon Bull Rev (2014) 21:1481–1488

valences (positive/negative feelings) of all previous experi-


ences with a particular color? Do some experiences with
colored objects weigh more heavily than others in the brain’s Rep-red Dem-blue
calculation of color preference? Does the impact of particular
associations matter in some contexts more than others? The
present study provides new insight into the dynamics of color
preferences by suggesting that they are determined by the
relative activation and strength of specific color–object asso-
ciations at a given moment.
Fig. 1 The 16 colors tested in this study. “Rep-red” indicates Republican
Previous findings about the color preferences of university red, and “Dem-blue” indicates Democratic blue
students (Schloss et al., 2011) led to the hypothesis that
Republicans might like red more than Democrats do, and that
parties until participants were directly asked about their polit-
Democrats might like blue more than Republicans do. Posi-
ical affiliation. It is therefore almost impossible that demand
tive affiliation with a political institution should lead to an
characteristics influenced the color preference judgments.
overall preference for colors associated with that institution, as
We tested 16 colors, including Republican red (Rep-red),
it had for universities. The present results reveal a more
Democratic blue (Dem-blue), and 14 fillers (see Fig. 1 and
interesting, dynamic pattern, with preferences for party-
Table 1). The background was white (R:255/G:255/B:255).
consistent colors appearing only on Election Day.
We based the RGB values for Rep-red and Dem-blue on the
colors used on the official Republican and Democratic
websites in 2010. We do not generally endorse specifying
Method colors using device-dependent RGB values instead of
device-independent coordinates (e.g., CIE 1931 xyY values),
Participants because this threatens the accuracy/replicability of color pro-
duction across monitors. We made an exception for this study
A total of 1,906 participants (247 in 2010; 1,659 in 2012), because forgoing rigorous calibration allowed us to test a
with a mean age of 33 years, took part in the study. Partici- broader sample of Democrats and Republicans than was
pants were classified as Republican/Democratic according to available on the Berkeley campus, especially on Election
the percentages of times they reported having previously Day. The fact that we obtained reliable group differences,
voted for Republican versus Democratic candidates. Partici- despite variations in color rendering, attests to the robustness
pants who reported equal percentages for Republican and
Democratic candidates were excluded (467/1,906 partici-
pants, 25 %), leaving a total of 1,439 participants. Participants Table 1 RGB values for the colors in this study
were tested online using the Amazon Mechanical Turk Color R G B
(mTurk) and Qualtrics.1 We did not assess color vision, be-
cause doing so online is nearly impossible. Moreover, we Republican red (Rep-red) 254 0 0
were interested in differences between the color preferences Democratic blue (Dem-blue) 0 0 254
of Democrats and Republicans, regardless of whether they Dark red (DR) 154 17 35
experienced colors with full trichromatic vision or weaker/ Pink 255 191 191
deficient color vision. All participants gave informed consent, Orange 250 126 0
and the Committee for the Protection of Human Subjects Dark yellow (DY) 255 203 57
approved the experimental protocol. Light yellow (LY) 255 255 191
Dark green (DG) 0 95 47
Light green (LG) 105 172 93
Design, displays, and procedure
Dark blue (DB) 1 40 118
Light blue 86 159 210
The experiment title on mTurk was “Color Preference Exper-
Dark purple 88 6 140
iment,” and politics was not mentioned until after participants
had rated their color preferences. No information suggested Light purple 187 125 226
that the experiment was related to Election Day or political Brown 93 47 41
Gray 128 128 128
Black 0 0 0
1
Perception and cognition experiments conducted online have produced
results comparable to the same experiments conducted in traditional Colors with abbreviated names in parentheses were also used in the
laboratory settings (Germine et al., 2012). party–color association task (see Fig. 3)
Psychon Bull Rev (2014) 21:1481–1488 1483

of the reported effects. Furthermore, people often experience influenced color preferences for Berkeley and Stanford stu-
Rep-red and Dem-blue on uncalibrated displays when view- dents, we initially concluded that those results did not gener-
ing political websites and news articles on their computer alize to political affiliations.
monitors and when watching news coverage of political Closer inspection revealed that the effects of political affil-
events on their televisions. iation on color preferences were more dynamic and context-
Participants were first shown the full 16-color array (Fig. 1, dependent than we had anticipated. On Election Day (Nov. 2),
without labels) and asked to decide which colors they liked Republicans tended to like Republican red more than Demo-
most and least, to anchor what liking “not at all” and “very crats did, and Democrats tended to like Democratic blue more
much” meant for them in the context of these colors (Palmer, than Republicans did, as initially expected. In contrast, partic-
Schloss, & Sammartino, 2013). They proceeded to rate their ipants tended to show the opposite pattern of preferences on
preferences for colors, presented individually, using a slider non-Election Days. Unfortunately, the sample included too
scale from not at all to very much. Each trial contained text at few participants for this temporal difference to reach statistical
the top of the screen, asking “How much do you like this significance (see Appendix). We therefore waited until the
color?” Below was a colored square ∼2.5 × 2.5 cm on a 1,440 2012 election to accumulate more data.
× 900-pixel resolution, 13-in. monitor. Although the size of In 2012, we repeated the same procedure as in 2010,
the colored squares varied according to each participant’s collecting data during the weeks preceding the election, on
monitor size/resolution, the sizes of color patches on monitors Election Day (Nov. 6), and over the following weeks. We only
have little impact on color preferences (Schloss, Strauss, & included participants who had unique IP addresses, to elimi-
Palmer, 2013). Below the colored square was a slider scale nate data sets from previous participants. We combined the
whose left endpoint was labeled not at all (coded as −100), 2010 and 2012 data because a four-way analysis of variance
center point was labeled neutral (coded as 0), and right end- (Year × Color × Timing × Party Affiliation) indicated that year
point was labeled very much (coded as +100). The slider did not interact with overall preference for red versus blue, and
always started at “neutral” and had to be moved before par- that none of the higher-order interactions were significant (Fs
ticipants could begin the next trial. After responding, partici- < 1.05). (See Figs. 5d–f in the Appendix for the 2012 data.)
pants clicked a “Continue” button. The colors were presented Participants generally preferred blue to red [F(1, 1438) =
in random order. 15.33, p < . 001, η2 = .01] (Figs. 2a–b), as had been found
After rating all 16 colors, participants were asked, “Con- previously in nonpolitical contexts (Eysenck, 1941; Guilford
sidering only the elections in which you voted, in what % of & Smith, 1959; McManus, Jones, & Cottrell, 1981; Palmer &
them did you vote for candidates in the following political Schloss, 2010). However, we found a significant three-way
parties: Republican ___ and Democratic___” and filled in interaction among color (red/blue), political affiliation
their estimates. Participants were classified as Republican, (Democratic/Republican), and timing (Election Day/non-
Democratic, or neither, depending on the party for which they Election Day) [F(1, 1431) = 7.07, p < .01, η2 = .005]. On
voted more frequently. Election Day, Republicans liked Rep-red more than Dem-
At the end of the experiment, participants were presented ocrats did [F(1, 93) = 7.03, p < .01, η2 = .07], but there was
with two reds (Rep-red and dark red), two blues (Dem-blue no corresponding party difference in preferences for Rep-
and dark blue), and four filler colors (light yellow, dark red on non-Election days (F < 1; Fig. 2a). The pattern of
yellow, light green, and dark green) simultaneously arranged preferences for Dem-blue was less straightforward, but still
in a vertical column. First they rated how strongly they asso- consistent with the pattern that color preferences were
ciated each color with the Democratic Party on a slider scale more party-consistent on Election Day than on other days
(scored from 0 to 10) to the right of each color. They then (Fig. 2b). On non-Election days, Republicans liked Dem-
repeated this procedure for the Republican Party. blue more than Democrats did [F(1, 1342) = 16.45, p <
.001, η2 = .01]. Possible explanations for this initially
puzzling result will be discussed below. This difference
Results and discussion diminished on Election Day (F < 1), when Democrats
tended to like blue somewhat more, and Republicans
Color preferences of Republicans and Democrats tended to like it somewhat less than on non-Election base-
line days.
The data from the 2010 US election season showed no overall Figure 2c shows (signed) changes in preference for Rep-
difference in Republicans’ and Democrats’ preferences for red and Dem-blue on Election Day relative to baseline, by
Rep-red or Dem-blue (Fs < 1) (see Figs. 5a–c in the Appendix subtracting out the corresponding baseline preferences on
for the data and numbers of participants in the groups). Be- non-Election Days. Republicans like Rep-red and dislike
cause we had expected overall group differences analogous to Dem-blue more on Election Day than on non-Election Days,
Schloss et al.’s (2011) finding that school affiliation and Democrats like Dem-blue and dislike Rep-red more on
1484 Psychon Bull Rev (2014) 21:1481–1488

A. Preference for red B. Preference for blue C. Election minus Non-Election


80 80 40

Average Preference

Average Preference
60 60

Difference Score
20
40 40
0
20 20
-20
0 0

-20 -20 -40


Rep. Dem. Rep. Dem. Rep. Dem. Rep. Dem. Rep. Dem. Rep. Dem.
N = (402) (942) (19) (76) N = (402) (942) (19) (76) Red Blue
Non-Election Election Non-Election Election Color
Timing Timing
Fig. 2 Color preferences of Republicans (gray bars) versus Democrats difference scores indicate greater preference on Election Day, and nega-
(white bars). (a) Preferences for Republican red on non-Election versus tive ones indicate lesser preference on Election Day. The numbers of
Election Days. (b) Preferences for Democratic blue on non-Election participants in different groups are displayed in parentheses, and error
versus Election Days. (c) Preferences for Republican red and Democratic bars represent standard errors of the means
blue on Election Day minus non-Election Days (baseline). Positive

Election Day than on non-Election Days. Although the only effect may also be related to asymmetries in the color prefer-
change from baseline that reached statistical significance was ences of Berkeley and Stanford students reported in Schloss
Republicans’ increase in preference for Rep-red [F(1, 419) = et al. (2011). The hypothesis that an underdog effect biases
3.95, p < .05, η2 = .009], all four difference scores were in the color preferences suggests interesting and testable predictions
predicted directions (p = .0625 by a sign test). The corre- about how personal investment or striving on behalf of a
sponding difference scores showed the same pattern in 2010 color-related socio-political institution modulates color pref-
and 2012 (Appendix), with eight out of eight differences being erences in rivalrous situations.
in the predicted directions (p < .01 by a sign test). These
results are consistent with the notion that people do not simply Explaining dynamic color preferences
judge color preference from a stored average valence for all
experiences with a particular color, but rather compute them Why are color preferences more consistent with party affiliations
on the fly, on the basis of which color associates are currently on Election Day than on non-Election Days? An EVT-based
activated. We will discuss this issue further below. explanation is that the mechanisms underlying color preference
include a dynamic activation component. The degree to which
Evidence for an “underdog effect” preference for a particular object or entity influences preference
for a specific color may depend on at least two factors at the
One perplexing aspect of the color preference data is the moment that the color preference judgment is made: (1) the
asymmetry between Republicans’ and Democrats’ preference overall activation of the object/entity in memory, and (2) the
changes for party colors on Election Day: The Republican strength and specificity of the object’s/entity’s color associations.
increase in liking red was reliably greater than the Democratic In the present study, the first factor concerns the relative
increase in liking blue (Fig. 2c). This asymmetry may be due activation of political parties, as compared with those of all
to an “underdog effect,” in which members of the party other objects/entities in memory associated with red and blue.
expected to lose the election feel stronger group affiliation To illustrate, assume that a fixed set of objects/entities are red
from pulling for their party to rally, as compared with mem- and a fixed set are blue, and that the combined preferences those
bers of the party expected to win. This hypothesis is consistent objects/entities contribute to preferences for red and blue, re-
with social threat cohesion, in which group identity spectively. If activation of a specific red entity (the Republican
strengthens when group members feel threatened (e.g., Party) and/or a blue entity (the Democratic Party) is heightened
Smeekes & Verkuyten, 2013; Turner, Hogg, Turner, & Smith, on Election Day, then those associations will have a greater
1984). In the 2012 election, from which the majority of the impact on preferences for red and/or blue than if they were less
data come, Republicans were underdogs because they were activated. Although those associations still exist on non-
predicted to lose seats in Congress and their presidential Election Days, their activation would be weaker, and other,
candidate was predicted to lose to the incumbent Democratic more salient objects would influence color preferences more
president. In contrast, Democrats were relatively confident strongly (e.g., during a berry-picking outing, preferences for
that their candidates would win and were less emotionally strawberries and blueberries would likely be more salient). This
uncertain about the outcome of the election. This underdog notion of differential activation is consistent with the hypothesis
Psychon Bull Rev (2014) 21:1481–1488 1485

that context strongly biases the associations that people have Republican Party than was dark red [F(1, 1438) = 653.63,
with colors (Elliot & Maier, 2012). Although we report no direct p < .001, η2 = .31], and Dem-blue was more strongly associ-
evidence for this activation hypothesis here, previous results ated with the Democratic Party than was dark blue [t(1438) =
have indicated that activating specific color–object associations 162.22, p < .001, η2 = .10]. However, party–color associations
biases color preferences, at least for a limited period of time were more accurate on Election Day (Fig. 4), and the ways in
(Strauss et al., 2013). Furthermore, it seems plausible that most which they were less accurate on non-Election Days can partly
American voters are more cognizant of their party affiliation on explain the color preference pattern in Fig. 2.
Election Day than on other days, except perhaps for diehard Averaged over all participants, color association ratings for
political advocates, for whom party issues are omnipresent. the correct color pairings (Republican Party with Rep-red,
The second factor concerns the strength and specificity of Democratic Party with Dem-blue) were equally strong on
party–color associations. If an object is only weakly associated Election Day and non-Election Days (Fs < 1; see Fig. 4).
with a color, then it may have less impact on preference for that Color association ratings for the incorrect pairings were lower,
color. This can be true even if an object is strongly activated. and thus more accurate, on Election Day than on non-Election
For example, the sky is strongly activated while sunbathing at Days: the Republican Party with Dem-blue, F(1, 1437) =
the beach, but it has little impact on preference for purple, 4.52, p < .05, η2 = .003, and the Democratic Party with Rep-
because purple is only weakly associated with the sky. Indeed, red, F(1, 1437) = 9.21, p < .01, η2 = .006. These results
the object–color match weighting factor in Palmer and suggest that party–color associations are indeed more specific
Schloss’s (2010) WAVE measure was designed to estimate on Election Day. People have nonnegligible baseline associa-
such differences in association strength when predicting color tions between the Republican Party and blue and between the
preferences. Concerning specificity, if an object is associated Democratic Party and red that are less evident on Election
with multiple colors, then preference for that object will impact Day, when party–color associations are particularly apparent.
the preferences for all associated colors. If Republicans asso- We will return below to why these default biases may exist.
ciate the Republican Party with both red and blue, then positive The party association data on non-Election Days revealed
affect for the Republican Party should influence preferences patterns that are consistent with two surprising aspects of the
for both red and blue, though to different degrees reflecting the preference data: (1) Republicans and Democrats liked red
relative strengths of those associations. It makes sense that equally (Fig. 2a) and (2) Republicans liked blue more than
party colors would be more ecologically evident on Election Democrats did (Fig. 2b). As compared with Democrats, Re-
Day, given the prevalence of color-coded media maps. If publicans not only had a weaker association between red and
party–color associations are stronger and/or more specific on the Republican Party [F(1, 1342) = 6.16, p < .05, η2 = .005;
Election Day than on non-Election Days, that might account Fig. 4a], but also a marginally stronger association between
for changes in color preferences. red and the Democratic Party [F(1, 1342) = 3.77, p = .052,
We investigated this possibility by analyzing participants’ η2 = .003; Fig. 4c]. The combination of associating red less
ratings of how strongly they associated each color with the with their own party and more with their rival party implies
Republican and Democratic parties. Averaged over all Repub- weaker and less specific associations with red, which may
lican and Democratic participants, Rep-red was most strongly have contributed to Republicans not liking red more than
associated with the Republican Party, and Dem-blue was most Democrats did on non-Election Days. A similar story holds
strongly associated with the Democratic Party (Fig. 3). for blue. As compared with Democrats, Republicans associ-
Rep-red was even more strongly associated with the ated blue more strongly with the Republican Party [F(1, 1342)

A. Republican Party B. Democratic Party


8 8
Average Association Rating

Average Association Rating

6 6

4 4

2 2

0 0
Rep-red DR DY LY DG LG DB Dem-blue Rep-red DR DY LY DG LG DB Dem-blue
Color Color

Fig. 3 Ratings of party–color association strengths for the Republican and Democratic parties. Error bars represent standard errors of the means. The
colors along the x-axes represent Republican red (Rep-red), dark red (DR), dark yellow (DY), light yellow (LY), dark green (DG), light green (LG), dark
blue (SB), and Democratic blue (Dem-blue)
1486 Psychon Bull Rev (2014) 21:1481–1488

Association with Red Association with Blue

Average Association Rating


Average Association Rating
10 A. Republican Party - Red 10 B. Republican Party - Blue
8 8

Republican 6 6

Party 4 4

2 2

0 0
Rep. Dem. Rep. Dem. Rep. Dem. Rep. Dem.
N = (402) (942) (19) (76) N = (402) (942) (19) (76)
Non-Election Election Non-Election Election
Timing Timing
Average Association Rating

Average Association Rating


10 C. Democratic Party - Red 10 D. Democratic Party - Blue
8 8

6 6
Democratic
Party 4 4

2 2

0 Rep. Dem. Rep. Dem.


0 Rep. Dem. Rep. Dem.
N = (402) (942) (19) (76) N = (402) (942) (19) (76)
Non-Election Election Non-Election Election
Timing Timing

Fig. 4 Party–color associations of Republicans (gray bars) versus Dem- and Republican red on non-Election versus Election Days. (d) Associa-
ocrats (white bars). (a) Associations between the Republican Party and tions between the Democratic Party and Democratic blue on non-Election
Republican red on non-Election versus Election Days. (b) Associations versus Election Days. The numbers of participants in different groups are
between the Republican Party and Democratic blue on non-Election displayed in parentheses. Error bars represent standard errors of the
versus Election Days. (c) Associations between the Democratic Party means

= 42.08, p < .001, η2 = .03; Fig. 4b] and less strongly with the to be relatively aggressive, and blue to be relatively calm and
Democratic Party [F(1, 1342) = 68.86, p < .001, η2 = .05; passive (e.g., Kaya & Epps, 2004; Palmer, Schloss, Xu, &
Fig. 4d]. These differences relative to Democrats’ associations Prado-León, 2013). People may have a default bias to associate
may have contributed to the higher Republican preference for Republicans more with blue than with red because conserva-
blue on non-Election Days. As with red, Republicans’ asso- tives are generally more threat-averse and conflict-avoidant
ciations with blue were weaker and less specific than those of than liberals (Jost & Amodio, 2012). Converging support for
Democrats. Such weaker, less specific associations may have this difference between conservatives and liberals comes from
prevented positive affect toward the Republican Party from psychological (Jost, Glaser, Kruglanski, & Sulloway, 2003),
selectively having a positive impact on preference for red and physiological (Oxley et al., 2008), neuropsychological
a negative impact on preference for blue. However, the asso- (Amodio, Jost, Master, & Yee, 2007), and neuroanatomical
ciation data do not fully explain political effects on color (Kanai, Feilden, Firth, & Rees, 2011) evidence.
preferences, because affiliates of both parties still associated Other possible influences concern cultural and historical
blue more strongly with the Democratic Party than with the factors in the political associations of colors. In many parts of
Republican Party, and red much more strongly with the Re- the world, more-liberal parties are associated with red and
publican Party than with the Democratic Party on both non- more-conservative parties with blue (Bensen, 2004; Enda,
Election and Election Days (all Fs > 15, ps < .001, η2s > .41). 2012; Farhi, 2004). Indeed, before the year 2000, US party–
color assignments varied from election to election, and the
Explaining party–color association biases on non-Election opposite of the contemporary party colors tended to be used:
Days blue for Republicans and red for Democrats. For example,
when Republican Reagan swept the 1984 presidential elec-
Finally, we return to the question of why people show tenden- tion, the election map coded Regan victories in blue and was
cies toward associating the Republican Party with blue and the referred to as a “suburban swimming pool” (Enda, 2012).
Democratic Party with red on non-Election days, despite the How might default and contemporary party–color associa-
opposite media-defined party–color assignments in the US. tions have combined to produce changes in color preferences
One possibility is the influence of default meanings of the on Election Day relative to non-Election Days? We believe that
colors (Elliot & Maier, 2012). For example, red is considered both types of associations contribute to color preferences to
Psychon Bull Rev (2014) 21:1481–1488 1487

some extent, all of the time. However, the default meanings and and e), separated by year (2010 and 2012). In 2010, members
deeply ingrained associations held on non-Election Days were of each party tended to prefer their own colors on Election
overridden on Election Day, when people carefully monitored Day and the opposite colors on non-Election Days, but too
election map colors in the media. This change was evident in few participants were sampled on Election Day for this three-
our participants’ party–color associations. As media-based ex- way interaction to reach statistical significance [F(1, 176) =
periences reinforced the new canonical color associations on 3.04, p = .08, η2 = .02].
Election Day, people’s color preferences became more aligned For 2012, we found a reliable Color × Party × Timing
with their affiliated party’s canonical colors. Perhaps in several three-way interaction [F(1, 1255) = 4.63, p < .05, η2 = .004].
years, when the notions of conservative “Red States” and On Election Day itself, Republicans liked Republican red
liberal “Blue States” becomes even more deeply embedded in more than Democrats did [F(1, 80) = 6.69, p < .05, η2 =
the US political consciousness, party-consistent color prefer- .08], but no party difference was apparent in preferences for
ences will become a more persistent norm. Republican red on non-Election Days [F(1, 1175) = 1.80, p =
.18, η2 = .002] (Fig. 5d). The pattern of preferences for
Democratic blue was also consistent with the hypothesis that
Author Note We thank Joseph Austerweil, Bill Prinzmetal, and color preferences are more party-consistent on Election Day
Melissa Ferguson for insightful discussions and Mathilde Heinemann
for help with preliminary analyses. This work was supported by than on other days (Fig. 5e). On non-Election Days, Repub-
the National Science Foundation (Grant Nos. BCS-1059088 and licans liked Democratic blue more than Democrats did [F(1,
BCS-0745820). 1175) = 16.17, p < .001, η2 = .01], and this reverse party color
difference diminished on Election Day (F < 1).
Figures 5c and f show preferences for Republican red and
Appendix: Party color preferences, separated by year Democratic blue on Election Day, after subtracting out the
respective baselines of non-Election Day preferences in 2010
Figure 5 shows Democrats’ and Republicans’ preferences for and 2012. All eight difference scores were in the predicted
Republican red (Figs. 5a and d) and Democratic blue (Figs. 5b direction (binomial sign test p < .01), with Republicans liking

A. B. C.

D. E. F.

Fig. 5 Color preferences of Republicans (gray bars) versus Democrats (baseline), from 2010. The bottom row (panels d–f) shows the corre-
(white bars). The top row shows (a) preferences for Republican red on sponding data from 2012. The large error bars in the 2010 data are largely
non-Election and Election Days, (b) preferences for Democratic blue on due to the small sample sizes. The numbers of participants in different
non-Election and Election Days, and (c) preferences for both Republican groups are displayed in parentheses, and error bars represent standard
red and Democratic blue on Election Day minus non-Election Days errors of the means
1488 Psychon Bull Rev (2014) 21:1481–1488

Republican red and disliking Democratic blue more on Elec- Jost, J. T., Glaser, J., Kruglanski, A. W., & Sulloway, F. J. (2003). Political
conservatism as motivated social cognition. Psychological Bulletin,
tion Day than on non-Election Days, and Democrats liking
129, 339–375. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.129.3.339
Democratic blue and disliking Republican red more on Elec- Kanai, R., Feilden, T., Firth, C., & Rees, G. (2011). Political orientations
tion Day than on non-Election Days in both 2010 and 2012. are correlated with brain structure in young adults. Current Biology,
However, the only change from baseline that reached statisti- 21, 677–680. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2011.03.017
Kaya, N., & Epps, H. H. (2004). Relationship between color and emotion:
cal significances was Republicans’ increase in preference for
A study of college students. College Student Journal, 38, 396–405.
Republican red in 2012 [F(1, 362) = 4.32, p < .05, η2 = .01]. McManus, I. C., Jones, A. L., & Cottrell, J. (1981). The aesthetics of
colour. Perception, 10, 651–666.
Oxley, D. R., Smith, K. B., Alford, J. R., Hibbing, M. V., Miller, J. L.,
Scalora, M., & Hibbing, J. R. (2008). Political attitudes vary with
References physiological traits. Science, 321, 1667–1670.
Palmer, S. E., & Schloss, K. B. (2010). An ecological valence theory of
human color preference. Proceedings of the National Academy of
Amodio, D. M., Jost, J. T., Master, S. L., & Yee, C. M. (2007). Sciences, 107, 8877–8882. doi:10.1073/pnas.0906172107
Neurocognitive correlates of liberalism and conservatism. Nature Palmer, S. E., Schloss, K. B., & Sammartino, J. (2013a). Visual aesthetics
Neuroscience, 10, 1246–1247. doi:10.1038/nn1979 and human preference. Annual Review of Psychology, 64, 77–107.
Bensen, C. (2004). Red state blues: Did I miss that memo? (Technical doi:10.1146/annurev-psych-120710-100504
Report). Lake Ridge, VA: POLIDATA Political Data Analysis. Palmer, S. E., Schloss, K. B., Xu, Z. X., & Prado-León, L. R. (2013b).
Retrieved from www.polidata.org/elections/red_states_blues_ Music–color associations are mediated by emotion. Proceedings of
de27a.pdf the National Academy of Sciences, 110, 8836–8841.
Elliot, A., & Maier, M. A. (2012). Color-in-context theory. Advances in Schloss, K. B., Poggesi, R. M., & Palmer, S. E. (2011). Effects of
Experimental Social Psychology, 45, 63–125. university affiliation and “school spirit” on color preferences:
Enda, J. (2012, Nov 1). When Republicans were blue and Democrats Berkeley versus Stanford. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18,
were red. Retrieved from Smithsonian.com. 498–504. doi:10.3758/s13423-011-0073-1
Eysenck, H. J. (1941). A critical and experimental study of color prefer- Schloss, K. B., Strauss, E. D., & Palmer, S. E. (2013). Object color
ence. American Journal of Psychology, 54, 385–391. preferences. Color Research and Application, 38, 393–411.
Farhi, P. (2004, Nov 2). Elephants are red, donkeys are blue; color is sweet, Smeekes, A., & Verkuyten, M. (2013). Collective self-continuity, group
so their states we hue. The Washington Post. Retrieved from www. identification and in-group defense. Journal of Experimental Social
washingtonpost.com/wp-dyn/articles/A17079-2004 Nov1.html Psychology, 49, 984–994.
Germine, L., Nakayama, K., Duchaine, B. C., Chabris, C. F., Chatterjee, Strauss, E. D., Schloss, K. B., & Palmer, S. E. (2013). Color preferences
G., & Wilmer, J. B. (2012). Is the Web as good as the lab? change after experience with liked/disliked colored objects.
Comparable performance from Web and lab in cognitive/ Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 20, 935–943. doi:10.3758/
perceptual experiments. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 19, 847– s13423-013-0423-2
857. doi:10.3758/s13423-012-0296-9 Taylor, C., & Franklin, A. (2012). The relationship between color–object
Guilford, J. P., & Smith, P. C. (1959). A system of color-preferences. associations and color preference: Further investigation of ecologi-
American Journal of Psychology, 72, 487–502. cal valence theory. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 19, 190–197.
Jost, J. T., & Amodio, D. M. (2012). Political ideology as motivated doi:10.3758/s13423-012-0222-1
social cognition: Behavioral and neuroscientific evidence. Turner, J. C., Hogg, M. A., Turner, P. J., & Smith, P. (1984). Failure and
Motivation and Emotion, 36, 55–64. doi:10.1007/s11031-011- defeat as determinants of group cohesiveness. British Journal of
9260-7 Social Psychology, 23, 97–111.

You might also like