You are on page 1of 11

The Clinical Spectrum of Developmental Language Impairment in School-Aged

Children: Language, Cognitive, and Motor Findings


Richard I. Webster, Caroline Erdos, Karen Evans, Annette Majnemer, Eva Kehayia,
Elin Thordardottir, Alan Evans and Michael I. Shevell
Pediatrics 2006;118;e1541
DOI: 10.1542/peds.2005-2761

The online version of this article, along with updated information and services, is
located on the World Wide Web at:
http://pediatrics.aappublications.org/content/118/5/e1541.full.html

PEDIATRICS is the official journal of the American Academy of Pediatrics. A monthly


publication, it has been published continuously since 1948. PEDIATRICS is owned,
published, and trademarked by the American Academy of Pediatrics, 141 Northwest Point
Boulevard, Elk Grove Village, Illinois, 60007. Copyright © 2006 by the American Academy
of Pediatrics. All rights reserved. Print ISSN: 0031-4005. Online ISSN: 1098-4275.

Downloaded from pediatrics.aappublications.org by guest on May 29, 2013


ARTICLE

The Clinical Spectrum of Developmental Language


Impairment in School-Aged Children: Language,
Cognitive, and Motor Findings
Richard I. Webster, MBBS, MSc, FRACPa,b, Caroline Erdos, MScc, Karen Evans, MScc, Annette Majnemer, PhD, OTa,d,e, Eva Kehayia, PhDe,
Elin Thordardottir, PhDc, Alan Evans, PhDf, Michael I. Shevell, MD, CM, FRCPCa,d

aDepartment of Neurology/Neurosurgery, cSchool of Communications Sciences and Disorders, dDepartment of Pediatrics, eSchool of Physical and Occupational Therapy,
f
McConnell Brain Imaging Centre, McGill University, Montreal, Quebec, Canada; bDepartment of Neurology and Children’s Hospital Education Research Institute,
Children’s Hospital at Westmead, Westmead, New South Wales, Australia

The authors have indicated they have no financial relationships relevant to this article to disclose.

ABSTRACT
OBJECTIVE. Our goal was to evaluate detailed school-age language, nonverbal cogni-
tive, and motor development in children with developmental language impair-
www.pediatrics.org/cgi/doi/10.1542/
ment compared with age-matched controls. peds.2005-2761
METHODS. Children with developmental language impairment or normal language doi:10.1542/peds.2005-2761
development (controls) aged 7 to 13 years were recruited. Children underwent Key Words
cognitive development, language
language assessment (Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals-4, Peabody disorders, developmental delay, motor
Picture Vocabulary-3, Goldman-Fristoe Test of Articulation-2), nonverbal cogni- development, language development
tive assessment (Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-IV), and motor assess- Abbreviations
ment (Movement Assessment Battery for Children). Exclusion criteria were non- SLI—specific language impairment
DLI— developmental language
verbal IQ below the 5th percentile or an acquired language, hearing, autistic impairment
spectrum, or neurologic disorder. CELF-4 —Clinical Evaluation of Language
Fundamentals-4
RESULTS. Eleven children with developmental language impairment (7:4 boys/girls; PPVT-3—Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test,
3rd Edition
mean age: 10.1 ⫾ 0.8 years) and 12 controls (5:7 boys/girls; mean age: 9.5 ⫾ 1.8 GFTA-2—Goldman-Fristoe Test of
years) were recruited. Children with developmental language impairment showed Articulation-2
WISC-IV—Wechsler Intelligence Scale for
lower mean scores on language (Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals- Children, 4th Edition
4 — developmental language impairment: 79.7 ⫾ 16.5; controls: 109.2 ⫾ 9.6; PRI—Perceptual Reasoning Index
Goldman-Fristoe Test of Articulation-2— developmental language impairment: M-ABC—Movement Assessment Battery
for Children
94.1 ⫾ 10.6; controls: 104.0 ⫾ 2.8; Peabody Picture Vocabulary-3— developmen-
Accepted for publication May 30, 2006
tal language impairment: 90.5 ⫾ 13.8; controls: 100.1 ⫾ 11.6), cognitive (Wech- Address correspondence to Michael I. Shevell,
sler Intelligence Scale for Children-IV— developmental language impairment: 99.5 MD, CM, FRCPC, Montreal Children’s Hospital,
⫾ 15.5; controls: 113.5 ⫾ 11.9), and motor measures (Movement Assessment 2300 Tupper St, Room A-514, Montreal,
Quebec, Canada H3H 1P3. E-mail: michael.
Battery for Children percentile— developmental language impairment: 12.7 ⫾ shevell@muhc.mcgill.ca
16.7; controls: 66.1 ⫾ 30.6) and greater discrepancies between cognitive and PEDIATRICS (ISSN Numbers: Print, 0031-4005;
Online, 1098-4275). Copyright © 2006 by the
language scores (Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-IV/Clinical Evaluation of American Academy of Pediatrics
Language Fundamentals-4 — developmental language impairment: 17.8 ⫾ 17.8;

PEDIATRICS Volume 118, Number 5, November 2006 e1541


Downloaded from pediatrics.aappublications.org by guest on May 29, 2013
controls: 1.2 ⫾ 12.7). Motor impairment was more com- der.5 Consistent with this, in studies that have recruited
mon in children with developmental language impair- children with greater degrees of language impairment, a
ment (70%) than controls (8%). higher incidence of motor disorders has been reported.12
However, it is still unclear whether motor impairment in
CONCLUSIONS. Developmental language impairment is char- children with DLI is the result of a more global develop-
acterized by a broad spectrum of developmental impair- mental impairment or reflects a biological function that
ments. Children identified on the basis of language im- has greater effects on language and motor function than
pairment show significant motor comorbidity. Motor nonverbal cognition.
assessment should form part of the evaluation and fol- Most previous studies that have evaluated motor per-
low-up of children with developmental language im- formance in DLI have used measures of isolated motor
pairment. functions (eg, rate of tapping or peg-moving10,13,14) or
have used combined measures of neurologic soft signs
that are not standardized and do not necessarily predict

D EVELOPMENTAL DISORDERS LEADING to language im-


pairment are probably the most common form of
childhood developmental disability. The prevalence of
a child’s functional motor ability.15,16 Four studies have
used standardized measures of motor function to evalu-
ate children with DLI.4,6,11,12 In one of these studies a
language delay in preschool-aged children has been es- proportion of children would not meet criteria for DLI,12
timated to be 7.6%.1 In kindergarten-aged children, and the other studies focused on the motor findings of
7.4% were found to meet criteria for specific language children with DLI but provided limited data on the chil-
impairment (SLI).2,3 The term SLI has been used to iden- dren’s language and cognitive skills.4,6,11 To our knowl-
tify children with language impairment in the context of edge, no study has evaluated the developmental profiles
normal nonverbal cognitive function; however, evi- of children with DLI across language, nonverbal cogni-
dence is increasing that SLI is associated with a range of tive, and motor domains.
impairments in other developmental domains.4–6 Thus, With this study we aimed to further the understand-
in this article, we use the term developmental language ing of DLI by evaluating the developmental profiles (ie,
impairment (DLI) to describe children who would oth- phenotypes) of children with DLI across language, mo-
erwise meet criteria for SLI. Children with an acquired tor, and nonverbal cognitive domains using standardized
language disorder or language impairment secondary to instruments with relevance for clinicians.
an autistic spectrum disorder, a hearing disorder, or a
known neurologic disorder are usually not considered to
have DLI. METHODS
Previous studies that examined the clinical pheno- Population
types of children with DLI have identified a range of Children between the ages of 7 and 13 years who were
impairments in domains other than language. There is considered to have DLI and controls were prospectively
an increased incidence of attention-deficit disorders recruited for this study. Children were recruited through
among children with DLI7 and an increased incidence of the neurology and developmental clinics of Montreal
language impairment among children with attention- Children’s Hospital, private speech/language patholo-
deficit disorders.5 Children with DLI are frequently gists, and from a class for children with language disor-
found to have impairments in socialization skills.8 More- ders. For the DLI group, clinicians were asked to refer
over, despite the requirement for normal nonverbal cog- children if they had an impairment in language but were
nitive function, nonverbal cognitive impairments have considered to have otherwise-normal nonverbal cogni-
been reported at school-age follow-up in children with a tive development. For the control group, clinicians were
diagnosis of DLI made at preschool age.9 Despite the asked to refer children with headache disorders (ie, mi-
range of impairments seen in children with DLI, it is graines, considered unlikely to have a structural basis)
unclear whether these deficits are secondary to the ef- who were considered to have normal development in all
fects over time of the underlying communication disor- developmental domains. Children with headache disor-
der or whether they are a separate but intrinsic part of ders were selected as the comparison group because
the underlying disorder that leads to language impair- these children are regularly assessed through the hospi-
ment. tal’s neurology clinics. These children had already un-
There is increasing evidence that motor impairment is dergone assessment by a neurologist and, thus, were
a common comorbidity in children with DLI.4,6,10,11 Mo- unlikely to have unrecognized developmental problems.
tor impairment is less likely to be secondary to a com- Given that the normative data for the language and
munication disorder than the impairments in other de- cognitive measures were available for English-speaking
velopmental domains. Among children with DLI, motor children, only children with English as their dominant
impairment has been found to correlate most strongly language were recruited.
with the observed severity of the child’s language disor- Children were excluded from this study if they were

e1542 WEBSTER et al
Downloaded from pediatrics.aappublications.org by guest on May 29, 2013
known to have an underlying neurologic or autistic Goldman-Fristoe Test of Articulation-2 (GFTA-2).19 This
spectrum disorder per Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of test is a systematic means of assessing each child’s spon-
Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition, Revised criteria (ie, per- taneous articulation of the consonant sounds of standard
vasive developmental disorder or pervasive develop- American English in single words. The GFTA-2 has ex-
mental disorder-not otherwise specified), a hearing im- cellent test-retest, interrater, and internal reliability and
pairment, or on subsequent testing were found to have is intended for use in children ⬎2 years of age.19
a nonverbal IQ below the 5th percentile. Approval was The children’s nonverbal cognition was assessed by
obtained from the hospital’s institutional review board using the block-design, matrix-reasoning, and picture-
before commencing the study. Written informed con- concepts subtests of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for
sent from the children’s parents and the child’s assent Children, 4th Edition (WISC-IV).20 These 3 subtests al-
were necessary preconditions for participation in this low the calculation of a child’s Perceptual Reasoning
study. Index (PRI), which is a measure of nonverbal and fluid
reasoning (the ability to deduce the relationship be-
Clinical Assessment and Measures Used tween stimuli and to draw conclusions from this infor-
Before assessment, the parents of all children were in- mation). It shows excellent reliability and convergent
terviewed by telephone by a research assistant to con- validity with the previously validated 3rd edition of the
firm that they met broad eligibility criteria. All children WISC.20
underwent screening audiometry with a portable audi- Children’s motor function was measure by using the
ometer. A threshold of 20 dB (at least in 1 ear) at all Movement Assessment Battery for Children (M-ABC).21
frequencies tested was considered to represent adequate The M-ABC is a commonly used instrument for the
hearing for conversational speech. A neurologic exami- identification of motor impairment in children and pro-
nation was performed by a pediatric neurologist to iden- vides measures of movement competence and manual
tify signs suggestive of possible underlying structural dexterity as well as ball skills and static and dynamic
neurologic disease. Parents were asked to complete a balance. The M-ABC represents a minor revision of a
questionnaire providing details about the child’s medical previously validated test of motor impairment (Test of
and developmental history as well as the Pragmatics Motor Impairment [Henderson Revision])22; this test had
Profile from the Clinical Evaluation of Language Funda- a minimum test-retest reliability for any item of 0.75 and
mentals-4 (CELF-4).17 All speech/language, psychologi- a minimum interrater reliability of 0.70. A recent vali-
cal, and occupational therapy assessments were per- dation study of the M-ABC in Chinese preschool-aged
formed by appropriately qualified therapists who were children reported a mean intraclass correlation coeffi-
blinded to the children’s group assignment (suspected cient of 0.96 across items and a test-retest reliability of
DLI or control) and to clinical information. 0.77.23 The M-ABC has demonstrated validity in identi-
Language was assessed by using the CELF-4 and the fying motor impairments in at-risk24 populations and has
Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test, 3rd Edition (PPVT-3).18 previously been reported to be a useful instrument for
The CELF-4 is an instrument that was designed to iden- detecting motor difficulties in children with SLI.4
tify language disorders or delays in children ⬎5 years of Normative values were taken from the data set pro-
age. The CELF-4 shows excellent split-half and interrater vided by the tests’ publishers. The CELF-4, WISC-IV, and
reliability, and detailed evidence supporting its validity is the GFTA-2 allow the calculation of standard scores
presented in its published manual.17 The PPVT-3 is a (mean: 100; SD: 15). The M-ABC generates an age-
well-established measure of receptive vocabulary for standardized impairment score from which a child’s per-
children and adults aged ⬎21⁄2 years. It shows excellent centile can be calculated.
internal consistency and test-retest reliability, and valid-
ity is supported by strong correlations with other mea- Statistical Analysis
sures of language.18 The core subtests and the Pragmatics Statistical analysis was performed by using SPSS 11.5
Profile of the CELF-4 were administered. The core software.25 Descriptive statistics were used to describe
subtests of the CELF-4 involve 2 tests of receptive lan- the population characteristics and the range of scores on
guage (concepts and following directions, word classes the measures used. The distributions of scores are shown
[receptive, for children ⬎8 years old]) and 3 tests of graphically by using box plots (see Figs 1– 4). Compari-
expressive language (word structure [children ⬍8 years sons between children with DLI and controls were per-
old], word classes [children ⬎8 years old], formulated formed by using t tests for normally distributed data (age
sentences and recalling sentences). A parent-completed at assessment, CELF-4 language scores, GFTA-2 scores,
questionnaire, the Pragmatics Profile, was used to iden- WISC-IV PRI scores, nonverbal cognitive discrepancy)
tify children who had evidence of pragmatic impairment and the Mann-Whitney tests (M-ABC impairment
(ie, the rules that determine how language is used in scores) for data that were not normally distributed.
different social contexts and environments). Speech was Given the difficulties inherent in comparing percentile
assessed by using the “sounds-in-words” section of the ranks, statistical tests on the M-ABC were performed by

PEDIATRICS Volume 118, Number 5, November 2006 e1543


Downloaded from pediatrics.aappublications.org by guest on May 29, 2013
using the total impairment score. To compare the distri- control children (9.5 ⫾ 1.8 years) was more widely
bution of scores with expected normative values, the distributed than that of the children with DLI (10.1 ⫾
sign test was used because of the relatively low sample 0.8 years), although the mean values did not differ sig-
size. The ␹2 statistic (or Fisher’s exact test) was used to nificantly. Children with DLI were significantly more
compare parental income brackets, the reported fre- likely that control children to have a family history of
quency of a family history of language disorders, and the language disorders (DLI: 6 of 11; controls: 0 of 10; P ⫽
frequency of motor impairments. .006), which is consistent with previous studies. All chil-
dren passed screening audiometry. No child had clinical
RESULTS findings suggestive of a focal neurologic disorder (other
Thirty children (16 DLI, 14 controls) were originally than language impairment).
recruited to participate in this study. Five children re- All parents of children referred with a diagnosis of DLI
ferred with a diagnosis of DLI were excluded from the considered their children to have problems with speech
study: 2 children did not undergo complete clinical test- and language. The mother of 1 child in the control group
ing, 1 child was found to have a PRI below the 5th reported that her child had difficulties with articulating
percentile, 1 child had an acquired language disorder, some words. The majority of children (10 of 12) in the
and 1 child was considered to have normal language control group had a clinical diagnosis of migraine. Three
after assessment. Two other children (1 with DLI, 1 children with DLI (3 of 11) had infrequent headaches;
control) who met criteria for inclusion in the study but none were considered to have migraine. Data on the
were the older siblings of children assessed were ex- combined parental income were available for 9 children
cluded to avoid biasing the data as a result of familial with DLI and 10 controls. The families of controls tended
factors. Thus, 11 children with DLI (7 boys) and 12 to have higher incomes than that of the families of
controls (5 boys) met eligibility criteria for this study. children with DLI, although this did not reach statistical
The demographic details and parental assessment of significance (P ⫽ .11).
the children’s attention, social, and reading skills for The distributions of language (CELF-4), vocabulary
each child are presented in Table 1. A comparison of (PPVT-3), and articulation (GFTA-2) scores for the DLI
controls and children with DLI at the time of their clin- and control groups are shown in Figs 1–3. The test and
ical assessment is shown in Table 2. The age range of subtest scores for the CELF-4 and the PPVT-3 for indi-

TABLE 1 Demographic Characteristics and Parental Assessment of Children Studied


ID Gender Age, y Referring Family Attentionb Social Reading Problemsd Parental Incomee
Diagnosis Historya Problemsc
1 F 7.7 Control ND ND ND ND ND
2 M 11.0 Control ND ND ND ND ND
3 F 10.5 Control ⫺ Normal None Mild 20 000–39 000
4 M 8.2 Control ⫺ Normal None Normal 40 000–59 000
5 F 8.1 Control ⫺ Normal Minor ND 60 000–79 000
6 M 10.9 Control ⫺ Normal None Normal 60 000–79 000
7 F 8.6 Control ⫺ Normal None Normal 60 000–79 000
8 F 7.0 Control ⫺ Normal None Normal 60 000–79 000
9 M 9.2 Control ⫺ Normal None Normal 60 000–79 000
10 M 10.7 Control ⫺ Normal Minor Normal 60 000–79 000
11 F 9.1 Control ⫺ Normal None Normal ⬎80 000
12 F 13.2 Control ⫺ Normal None Normal ⬎80 000
13 M 10.6 DLI ⫹ Normal Minor Mild ND
14 M 8.8 DLI ⫹ Normal None Moderate/Severe ND
15 F 8.8 DLI ⫹ Poor Minor Moderate/Severe ⬍20 000
16 M 9.1 DLI ⫺ Poor None Moderate/Severe 20 000–39 000
17 M 10.8 DLI ⫺ Normal None Normal 40 000–59 000
18 F 10.7 DLI ⫺ Normal None Moderate/Severe 40 000–59 000
19 M 10.0 DLI ⫹ Normal Minor Moderate/Severe 40 000–59 000
20 M 10.8 DLI ⫺ Poor Minor Mild 60 000–79 000
21 M 9.8 DLI ⫹ Poor Major Moderate/Severe 60 000–79 000
22 F 10.1 DLI ⫹ Normal Minor Mild ⬎80 000
23 F 11.1 DLI ⫺ Normal Minor Mild ⬎80 000
Shown is a summary of the demographic characteristics of the children included in this sample. Included are data from a questionnaire given to the children’s parents. ND indicates data not supplied.
a Family history: parents were asked if there were other family members who had problems with speech and language.

b Attention: parents were asked if their child’s general ability to pay attention was normal/good or poor for their age.

c Social problems: parents were asked if their child had difficulties making friends with children of the same age.

d Reading problems: parents were asked to rate their child’s reading skills, allowing for their age.

e Parental income: parents were asked in which income bracket their total family income was.

e1544 WEBSTER et al
Downloaded from pediatrics.aappublications.org by guest on May 29, 2013
TABLE 2 Comparison of Children With DLI and Controls
DLI (n ⫽ 11) Controls (n ⫽ 12) P
Age, mean ⫾ SD, y 10.1 ⫾ 0.8 9.5 ⫾ 1.8 .36
Boys, n/N (%) 7/11 (64) 5/12 (42) .29
Family income more than $60 000, n/N (%) 4/9 (44)a 8/10 (80)a .11
Family history of language disorders, n/N (%) 5/11 (55) 0/10 (0)a .006
a Not all parents responded to these items on the questionnaires.

FIGURE 1
Scores on language measures. A, Core language score, CELF-4; B, articulation, Goldman-Fristoe Test of Articulation-2; C, receptive vocabulary, PPVT-3. Shown are box plots of scores on
language and articulation measures. The box shows the 25th to 75th percentile ranges, the solid line within the box is the median score, and the range is illustrated by the horizontal
lines at the ends of the range bars.

vidual children are shown in Table 3. Children with DLI 109.2 ⫾ 9.6; P ⬍ .001). They also had significantly lower
showed significantly lower total language scores on the scores on tests of articulation (GFTA-2—DLI: 94.1 ⫾
CELF-4 than did controls (DLI: 79.7 ⫾ 16.5; controls: 10.6; controls: 104.0 ⫾ 2.8; P ⫽ .003). Children with DLI

PEDIATRICS Volume 118, Number 5, November 2006 e1545


Downloaded from pediatrics.aappublications.org by guest on May 29, 2013
(PPVT-3) was lower than expected on the basis of nor-
mative values, the distribution of scores did not differ
significantly from normative data (P ⫽ .11). By contrast,
the language scores of controls were significantly higher
than would be expected on the basis of normative data
(P ⫽ .04); however, scores on the PPVT-3 did not differ
significantly from normative data.
The distributions of nonverbal cognitive testing scores
and the results for individual children are shown in Fig
2 and Table 3, respectively. Children with DLI had sig-
nificantly lower WISC-IV PRI scores than did controls
(DLI: 99.5 ⫾ 15.5; controls: 113.5 ⫾ 11.9; P ⫽ .009).
When compared with controls, children with DLI
showed significantly poorer performance on the matrix-
reasoning (DLI: 9.1 ⫾ 3.2; controls: 12.7 ⫾ 1.9; P ⫽ .002)
and block-design (DLI: 9.3 ⫾ 3.5; controls: 11.9 ⫾ 2.6; P
FIGURE 2 ⫽ .05) subtests of the WISC-IV. However, the total PRI
Nonverbal cognition: PRI (WISC-IV). Shown are box plots of scores on the WISC-IV PRI. The
box shows the 25th to 75th percentile ranges, the solid line within the box is the median scores and subtest scores of children with DLI did not
score, and the range is illustrated by the horizontal lines at the ends of the range bars. differ significantly from published normative values. By
contrast, control children had significantly higher total
PRI scores (P ⫽ .01) and higher scores on the picture-
concepts subtests than would be expected on the basis of
normative data. Children with DLI showed significantly
greater discrepancies between nonverbal cognitive and
core language scores than controls (WISC-IV [PRI]/
CELF-4 [core language score]—DLI: 17.8 ⫾ 17.8; con-
trols: 1.2 ⫾ 12.7; P ⫽ .02) (see Fig 3).
The distribution of children’s scores on the M-ABC is
shown in Fig 4, and results for individual children are
shown in Table 3. One child in the DLI group did not
complete assessment with the M-ABC. The mean im-
pairment score for children with DLI was 14.1 ⫾ 5.3 and
for controls was 3.9 ⫾ 5.2 (P ⫽ .001). When percentile
ranks were considered, the mean M-ABC percentile for
children with DLI was 12.7 ⫾ 16.7, whereas for controls
the mean was 66.1 ⫾ 30.6. Using an M-ABC score below
FIGURE 3 the 15th percentile to identify impairment, 70% of chil-
Discrepancy between language and nonverbal cognitive scores (WISC-IV PRI/CELF-4 dren with DLI met criteria for motor impairment,
core language score). Shown are box plots of children’s nonverbal cognitive/language
discrepancy (WISC-IV PRI/CELF-4 total score). The box shows the 25th to 75th percentile whereas 8% of controls met this criteria (P ⫽ .003).
ranges, the solid line within the box is the median score, and the range is illustrated by the More than half of the children (6 of 10) with DLI had
horizontal lines at the ends of the range bars. M-ABC scores that fell below the 5th percentile.

DISCUSSION
tended to have poorer receptive vocabulary scores, al- Despite a history of delayed language development and
though this did not reach statistical significance (PPVT- ongoing language difficulties in all children with DLI,
3—DLI: 90.5 ⫾ 13.8; controls: 100.1 ⫾ 11.6; P ⫽ .06). not all children had abnormal results on the core lan-
Children with DLI had significantly poorer scores on all guage tests of the CELF-4. The mean language score fell
the administered subtests of the CELF-4 other than word 1.25 SDs below the expected population mean on the
classes (P ⫽ .10). The subtest that caused the greatest basis of normative data. This corresponds to the 10th
difficulty was recalling sentences (DLI: 5.2 ⫾ 3.2; con- percentile cutoff that has been suggested to correlate
trols: 10.9 ⫾ 2.8; P ⫽ .001). Two children, both with a well with the clinical identification of meaningful lan-
diagnosis of DLI, met the criterion for pragmatic impair- guage impairment.26 However, several children had core
ment on the CELF-4 Pragmatics Profile. language scores that fell within the reference range, just
When compared with expected normative values, short of the population mean. Consistent with the clin-
children with DLI had significantly poorer core language ical diagnosis of DLI, children with DLI showed much
scores (P ⫽ .001). Although the mean vocabulary score greater degrees of discrepancy between language and

e1546 WEBSTER et al
Downloaded from pediatrics.aappublications.org by guest on May 29, 2013
TABLE 3 Test and Subtest Scores of the Participants: CELF-4, WISC-IV, and M-ABC
ID Group Age, y CELF-4 PPVT-3 WISC-IV M-ABC
CFD WS/WC RS FS Total BD PC MR PRI Percentile IS
1 Control 7.7 10 12 11 13 109 85 11 10 11 104 65 3
2 Control 11.0 10 10 10 12 102 114 9 7 9 90 36 6
3 Control 10.5 9 7 7 10 90 87 14 13 12 119 79 2
4 Control 8.2 13 13 12 12 115 112 9 13 14 112 93 0.5
5 Control 8.1 12 11 10 13 109 98 15 11 8 108 29 7
6 Control 10.9 12 9 5 13 98 83 11 12 12 110 89 1
7 Control 8.6 14 13 15 13 123 93 15 11 18 129 84 1.5
8 Control 7.0 11 11 13 10 108 103 12 11 16 119 96 0
9 Control 9.2 10 9 14 15 112 115 14 12 13 119 49 4.5
10 Control 10.7 11 11 13 13 112 107 14 16 15 131 79 2
11 Control 9.1 11 10 10 10 108 102 7 13 10 100 1 19
12 Control 13.2 12 14 11 14 124 111 12 13 15 121 93 0.5
13 DLI 10.6 8 10 8 12 97 89 12 15 8 110 1 23
14 DLI 8.8 7 2 1 5 62 100 8 13 11 104 1 19
15 DLI 8.8 3 1 1 1 44 65 6 6 6 75 1 18
16 DLI 9.1 4 8 5 9 79 94 7 9 9 90 4 14.5
17 DLI 10.8 6 10 9 9 91 91 13 10 9 104 29 7
18 DLI 10.7 12 7 3 6 82 79 13 15 11 119 13 10.5
19 DLI 10.0 11 8 9 11 99 102 10 12 10 104 2 16.5
20 DLI 10.8 9 10 7 9 93 98 7 5 10 84 49 4.5
21 DLI 9.8 6 9 5 3 75 87 14 10 12 112 26 7.5
22 DLI 10.1 4 8 8 10 85 115 3 9 7 77 ND ND
23 DLI 11.1 11 5 1 3 70 76 9 11 7 94 1 20.5
Presented are the scores of the individual participants on the tests and subtests of the CELF-4, WISC-IV, and M-ABC. CFD indicates concepts and following directions; WS/WC, word structure (⬍9
years)/word classes (⬎9 years); RS, recalling sentences; FS, formulated sentences; BD, block design; PC, picture concepts; MR, matrix reasoning; IS, impairment score (higher scores are associated
with greater degrees of motor impairment); ND, no data.

ceptive vocabulary are less sensitive in identifying chil-


dren with language impairment than more broadly
based tests of language.9 Although the PPVT-3 is a rela-
tively easy instrument to administer, it is likely to miss
problems with syntax and word structure that are com-
monly seen in children with DLI.
The range of language findings in this group of chil-
dren illustrates the heterogeneity of language disorders
in school-aged children. The profile of the language abil-
ities of children with DLI has been shown to evolve.27 As
children age, earlier problems in areas such as phonol-
ogy and morphosyntax may improve; however, more
detailed testing may show problems with higher-level
language (eg, understanding of humor and idioms28).
The core language subtests of the CELF-4 (which test
receptive and expressive morphosyntax, verbal memory,
FIGURE 4 and verbal working memory and the relationship be-
Distribution of children’s motor scores (M-ABC percentiles). Shown are box plots of per-
centiles on the M-ABC. The box shows the 25th to 75th percentile ranges, the solid line
tween words) are insufficient to identify problems with
within the box is the median score, and the range is illustrated by the horizontal lines at these higher-level language functions. Tests of reading
the ends of the range bars. potentially would have an increased sensitivity for de-
tecting language disorders; however, given considerable
variability in classroom exposure to reading between
nonverbal cognitive scores than control children. For a families and school environments, we decided not to
number of children, this discrepancy was the major include these tests.
marker of language impairment. In this study, articulation was assessed by using the
The PPVT-3 (a test of receptive vocabulary) proved to GFTA-2.19 Most of the control children were performing
be a less sensitive instrument for the identification of at a ceiling level on this test. Although children with DLI
language impairment than the CELF-4. This is consistent did more poorly than controls, most did not have major
with previous studies that have shown that tests of re- problems with their production of individual speech

PEDIATRICS Volume 118, Number 5, November 2006 e1547


Downloaded from pediatrics.aappublications.org by guest on May 29, 2013
sounds. The sounds-in-words section of the GFTA-2 child’s ability to participate in sporting and playground
only examines a child’s ability to produce individual activities. Children with language disorders are fre-
speech sounds. A more demanding test of verbal se- quently relatively socially isolated. Motor competence is
quencing may be required to demonstrate oromotor lan- important for the participation in normal playground
guage impairments in these children. activities (eg, ball games, running, and climbing). Chil-
For children with DLI, the mean PRI (99.5) was on dren with motor impairments have been found to have
the 50th percentile on the basis of normative data. The lower self-worth and higher levels of anxiety than con-
PRI scores of children with DLI had an almost normal trols.30 As such, it is possible that comorbid motor im-
distribution and ranged from the 5th percentile to the pairments exacerbate the social isolation commonly seen
90th percentile. This finding is not surprising and is in children with DLI. Although the effectiveness of ther-
consistent with our recruitment criteria (language im- apy for motor impairment in DLI is uncertain, there is
pairment in the context of normal nonverbal intelli- some evidence that treatment can lead to improvements
gence). Although the scores of children with DLI were in a child’s motor skills.11 Moreover, knowledge of po-
significantly poorer than those of the control population, tential motor impairments in children with DLI may lead
there was evidence to suggest that the control children to targeted surveillance of areas that may cause potential
were “supernormal.” difficulties (ie, pen grip) and guidance regarding physical
Motor impairment proved to be a common comor- activities that present fewer motor challenges.
bidity in this group of children. The identification of a
group of children on the basis of language impairment CONCLUSIONS
also identified a group of children with considerable This study illustrates the considerable clinical heteroge-
motor impairment. When the mean percentiles were neity seen in school-aged children with DLI. Although
plotted for the domains tested (Fig 5), levels of motor motor impairment proved to be a common and signifi-
performance were commensurate with language perfor- cant comorbidity, it was by no means universal. The
mance in children with DLI; however, these were unre- range of developmental impairments seen in this group
lated to nonverbal cognitive function. This lends further of children suggests that the care of children with DLI
support to the hypothesis that common biological factors would be enhanced by multidisciplinary developmental
critical to language and motor function (but of relatively surveillance (eg, speech/language, educational psychol-
less importance for nonverbal cognition) may be the ogy, and occupational therapy). However, given the rel-
etiologic factors in DLI.14 The number of children en- atively low number of children with DLI assessed, there
rolled in this study was relatively small; thus, further is need for additional studies with larger sample sizes to
exploration of the correlation between language, non- determine if these findings are generally applicable to
verbal cognitive, and motor variables was not possible. children with DLI.
The relatively high frequency of motor impairments
in this cohort is consistent with previous research in this ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
area.4,6,11,12,29 Motor impairment has important conse- Dr Webster was a research fellow supported by the Mon-
quences for a child’s elementary school academic per- treal Children’s Hospital Research Institute and the John
formance (writing, drawing, and coloring) and for a Yu Scholarship (Children’s Hospital at Westmead, Syd-
ney, Australia). Dr Shevell is a chercheur boursier cli-
nicien (clinical research scholar) of the Fonds de Recher-
che en Santé du Québec. Dr Shevell is also grateful for
the support of the Montreal Children’s Hospital Foun-
dation. This project was supported by the Réseau Pro-
vincial de Recherche en Adaptation-Réadaptation.
We acknowledge the contributions of Nancy Marget,
Cynthia Pearlman, Rina Birnbaum, Nicholas Hall, and
Lisa Steinbach to this study.

REFERENCES
1. Silva PA. A study of the prevalence stability and significance of
developmental language delays in preschool children. Dev Med
Child Neurol. 1980;22:768 –777
FIGURE 5
2. Tomblin JB, Records NL, Zhang X. A system for the diagnosis of
Comparison of mean percentiles. The mean language (CELF-4 core language score),
specific language impairment in kindergarten children. J Speech
nonverbal cognitive (WISC-IV PRI), and motor (M-ABC) scores of children with DLI and
controls are plotted as percentiles. It can be seen that although both groups have relative Lang Hear Res. 1996;39:1284 –1294
strengths in their nonverbal cognitive skills, there is almost a 40-percentile difference 3. Tomblin JB, Records NL, Buckwater P, Smith E, O’Brien M.
between language and cognitive scores and cognitive and motor scores in the group of Prevalence of specific language impairment in kindergarten
children with DLI. children. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 1997;40:1245–1260

e1548 WEBSTER et al
Downloaded from pediatrics.aappublications.org by guest on May 29, 2013
4. Hill EL. A dyspraxic defect in specific language impairment and 16. Johnston RB, Stark RE, Mellits ED, Tallal P. Neurological status
developmental coordination disorder? Evidence from hand of language-impaired and normal children. Ann Neurol. 1981;
and arm movements. Dev Med Child Neurol. 1998;40:388 –395 10:159 –163
5. Tannock R, Brown TE. Attention-deficit disorders with learn- 17. Semel E, Wiig E, Secord WA. Clinical Evaluation of Language
ing disorders in children and adolescents. In: Brown TE, ed. Fundamentals 4 (CELF-4). San Antonio, TX: The Psychological
Attention Deficit Disorders and Cognitive Co-morbidities in Children, Corporation; 2003
Adolescents and Adults. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric 18. Dunn LM, Dunn LM. Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test. 3rd ed.
Press; 2000 Circle Pines, MN: AGS Publishing; 1997
6. Webster RI, Majnemer A, Platt RW, Shevell MI. Motor func- 19. Goldman R, Fristoe M. Goldman-Fristoe Test of Articulation. 2nd
tion in school age children with a preschool diagnosis of spe- ed. Circle Pines, MN: AGS Publishing; 2000
cific language impairment (SLI). J Pediatr. 2005;146:80 – 85 20. Wechler D. Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-IV. Toronto,
7. Manor O, Shalev RS, Joseph A, Gross-Tsur V. Arithmetic skills Ontario, Canada: The Psychological Corporation; 2003
in kindergarten children with developmental language disor- 21. Henderson SE, Sugden DA. Movement Assessment Battery for
ders. Eur J Paediatr Neurol. 2001;5:71–77 Children Checklist. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological
8. Shevell MI, Majnemer A, Webster RI, Platt RW, Birnbaum R. Corporation; 1992
Outcomes at school-age of preschool children with develop- 22. Stott DH, Moyes FA, Henderson SE. The Test of Motor Impair-
mental language impairment. Pediatr Neurol. 2005;32:264 –269
ment. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation; 1984
9. Webster RI, Majnemer A, Platt RW, Shevell MI. The predictive
23. Chow SMK, Henderson SE. Interrater and test-retest reliability
value of a preschool diagnosis of developmental language im-
of the Movement Assessment Battery for Chinese preschool
pairment. Neurology. 2004;63:2327–2331
children. Am J Occup Ther. 2003;57:574 –577
10. Bishop DVM, Edmundson A. Specific language impairment as
24. Foulder-Hughes L, Cooke A. Do mainstream schoolchildren
a maturational lag: evidence from longitudinal data on lan-
who were born preterm have motor problems? Br J Occup Ther.
guage and motor development. Dev Med Child Neurol. 1987;29:
2003;66:9 –16
442– 459
11. Rintala P, Pienimäki K, Ahonen T, Cantell M, Kooistra L. The 25. Statistical Package for Social Sciences [computer program]. Version
effects of a psychomotor training programme on motor skill 11.5. Chicago, IL: SPSS Inc; 2002
development in children with developmental language disor- 26. Records NL, Tomblin JB. Clinical decision making: describing
ders. Hum Mov Sci. 1998;17:721–737 the decision rules of practicing speech-language pathologists.
12. Robinson RJ. Causes and associations of severe and persistent J Speech Hear Res. 1994;37:144 –156
specific speech and language disorders in children. Dev Med 27. Leonard LB. Children With Specific Language Impairment. Cam-
Child Neurol. 1991;33:943–962 bridge, MA: MIT Press; 1998
13. Preis S, Schittler P, Lenard HG. Motor performance and hand- 28. Norbury CF. Factors supporting idiom comprehension in chil-
edness in children with developmental language disorder. Neu- dren with communication disorders. J Speech Lang Hear Res.
ropediatrics. 1997;28:324 –327 2004;47:1179 –1193
14. Bishop DVM. Motor immaturity and specific language 29. Powell RP, Bishop DV. Clumsiness and perceptual problems in
impairment: evidence for a common genetic basis. Am J Med children with specific language impairment. Dev Med Child
Genet. 2001;114:56 – 63 Neurol. 1992;34:755–765
15. Trauner D, Wulfeck B, Tallal P, Hesselink J. Neurological and 30. Skinner RA, Piek JP. Psychosocial implications of poor motor
MRI profiles of children with developmental language impair- coordination in children and adolescents. Hum Mov Sci. 2001;
ment. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2000;42:470 – 475 20:73–94

PEDIATRICS Volume 118, Number 5, November 2006 e1549


Downloaded from pediatrics.aappublications.org by guest on May 29, 2013
The Clinical Spectrum of Developmental Language Impairment in School-Aged
Children: Language, Cognitive, and Motor Findings
Richard I. Webster, Caroline Erdos, Karen Evans, Annette Majnemer, Eva Kehayia,
Elin Thordardottir, Alan Evans and Michael I. Shevell
Pediatrics 2006;118;e1541
DOI: 10.1542/peds.2005-2761
Updated Information & including high resolution figures, can be found at:
Services http://pediatrics.aappublications.org/content/118/5/e1541.full.
html
References This article cites 21 articles, 5 of which can be accessed free
at:
http://pediatrics.aappublications.org/content/118/5/e1541.full.
html#ref-list-1
Citations This article has been cited by 4 HighWire-hosted articles:
http://pediatrics.aappublications.org/content/118/5/e1541.full.
html#related-urls
Subspecialty Collections This article, along with others on similar topics, appears in
the following collection(s):
Developmental/Behavior
http://pediatrics.aappublications.org/cgi/collection/developme
ntal:behavior
Permissions & Licensing Information about reproducing this article in parts (figures,
tables) or in its entirety can be found online at:
http://pediatrics.aappublications.org/site/misc/Permissions.xht
ml
Reprints Information about ordering reprints can be found online:
http://pediatrics.aappublications.org/site/misc/reprints.xhtml

PEDIATRICS is the official journal of the American Academy of Pediatrics. A monthly


publication, it has been published continuously since 1948. PEDIATRICS is owned, published,
and trademarked by the American Academy of Pediatrics, 141 Northwest Point Boulevard, Elk
Grove Village, Illinois, 60007. Copyright © 2006 by the American Academy of Pediatrics. All
rights reserved. Print ISSN: 0031-4005. Online ISSN: 1098-4275.

Downloaded from pediatrics.aappublications.org by guest on May 29, 2013

You might also like