You are on page 1of 7

Article

Body Position Influences


Cardiovascular Disgust Reactivity
A Tilt Table Experiment
Anne Schienle,1 Sonja Übel,1 Andreas Rössler,2 Andreas Schwerdtfeger,3
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

and Helmut Karl Lackner2


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

1
Department of Clinical Psychology, University of Graz, Austria, 2Institute of Physiology,
Medical University Graz, Austria, 3Department of Health Psychology, University Graz, Austria

Abstract. It has been suggested that elevated trait disgust constitutes a vulnerability factor for fainting episodes. We tested the hypothesis that
disgust-prone individuals are susceptible to vasovagal syncope by means of a tilt table experiment, during which 30 women were presented with
disgusting pictures in a supine and a 70 upright position. The results showed that relative to disgust elicitation in the supine position, tilting
reduced diastolic blood pressure during disgust elicitation, which could indicate increased risk for presyncope. Moreover, self-reported disgust
proneness was positively correlated with heart rate during disgust induction in the tilted position. This association may point to a compensatory
mechanism that aims at stabilizing mean arterial pressure. Disgust-prone individuals possibly utilized this mechanism more extensively to
prevent fainting. Future investigations with a longer duration should follow up on this finding and compare the onset of presyncope between
high and low disgust-prone individuals.

Keywords: disgust, disgust proneness, tilt table, cardiovascular, pictures

In his initial formulation of the disgust theory of fainting, The discrepant findings may be related to different
Page (1994) proposed that disgust-prone individuals show methodological factors. First, a sufficient exposure time to
pronounced parasympathetic activation when confronted disgust elicitors is necessary in order to be able to observe
with repulsive stimuli. He argued that the associated a prolonged heart rate deceleration and to be able to com-
marked heart rate deceleration and blood pressure reduction pute additional cardiac indices like heart rate variability.
may even lead to fainting. Consequently, the personality However, in early investigations the chosen indicators were
trait disgust proneness (the tendency of a person to experi- limited to heart rate (e.g., Levenson, Ekman, & Friesen,
ence disgust across different situations) was conceptualized 1990). This approach does not provide a detailed picture
as a vulnerability factor for fainting episodes (Page, 2003; of the cardiovascular processes associated with disgust.
Page & Tan, 2009). More recent studies that included a variety of cardiac
There is however mixed empirical evidence that the parameters described a more complex picture. de Jong
experience of disgust is indeed associated with parasympa- et al. (2011) observed a disgust-related increase in heart rate
thetic activation. The most commonly applied experimental together with an increase in high frequency band power
design for studying the psychophysiology of disgust has of heart rate variability during the presentation of repulsive
been passive picture viewing. The participants are presented video clips (de Jong et al., 2011). Thus, there were simulta-
with aversive scenes or video clips while their cardiovascu- neous sympathetic and parasympathetic influences.
lar responses (e.g., heart rate) are recorded. In line with Vossbeck-Elsebusch, Steinigweg, et al. (2012) recorded
Page’s hypothesis many studies observed heart rate reduc- several measures of (para)sympathetic activation (e.g., heart
tion during visual disgust elicitation (e.g., Rohrmann & rate, skin conductance, blood pressure, respiratory sinus
Hopp, 2008; Schienle, Stark, & Vaitl, 2001; Stark, Walter, arrhythmia [RSA]) while they confronted their participants
Schienle, & Vaitl, 2005). However, some authors reported with disgusting pictures. The pattern of results did not indi-
no heart rate changes (e.g., Sarlo, Buodo, Munafò, cate an increase of parasympathetic activation, but an
Stegagno, & Palomba, 2008; van Overveld, de Jong, & increase in sympathetic tone (increase in heart rate and skin
Peters, 2009; Vossbeck-Elsebusch, Steinigweg, Vögele, & conductance, minor RSA changes relative to the baseline).
Gerlach, 2012) or even heart rate acceleration (e.g., de Another reason for inconsistent findings refers to the fact
Jong, van Overveld, & Peters, 2011). that the disgust sensitivity of individuals as a moderating

 2015 Hogrefe Publishing Journal of Psychophysiology 2015; Vol. 29(2):73–79


DOI: 10.1027/0269-8803/a000136
74 A. Schienle et al.: Disgust and Body Position

personality factor of their disgust reactivity has widely been 22.9 kg/m2 (SD = 4.0; range: 17.9–32). The participants
neglected with only a few exceptions (e.g., de Jong et al., had been asked for the presence of somatic and/or mental
2011; Gerlach et al., 2006; Rohrmann & Hopp, 2008). disorders and the intake of medication. Positive answers
It can be assumed that only individuals with elevated disgust led to exclusion from the study.
proneness show substantial physiological changes during We only included participants with at least moderate
disgust exposure (Schienle, Walter, & Vaitl, 2002). This disgust proneness in our sample in order to assure sufficient
hypothesis is in line with the typical finding for unselected psychophysiological disgust responses to the pictures.
samples that alterations in (para)sympathetic activation dur- The selection was based on participants’ scores on the
ing visual disgust induction are rather small. For example, Questionnaire for the Assessment of Disgust Proneness
the disgust-related heart rate reduction was not even two (QADP; Schienle, Walter, et al., 2002). In the construction
beats per minute in the study by Stark et al. (2005). sample, women had obtained a mean QADP score of
Another moderating factor that is directly related to dis- M = 2.28 (SD = 0.52). The participants of the present study
gust proneness is female gender. Women consistently report were required to have the same or higher scores. The sam-
higher disgust proneness compared to men (e.g., Schienle, ple had been restricted to females because there are consid-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Walter, et al., 2002). Several psychophysiological disgust erable gender differences in disgust proneness. Females
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

studies were conducted with gender-mixed groups, describe themselves as more disgust-sensitive than males
sometimes with unequal proportions of female and male (Schienle, Walter, et al., 2002).
participants (e.g., de Jong et al., 2011; Rohrmann & Hopp,
2008). This fact may (at least partially) explain why cardio-
vascular studies examining disgust have been inconclusive. Material and Design
Based on these observations, we only tested women with
an above average disgust proneness in the present investiga- The participants viewed 40 disgusting and 20 neutral
tion. They were exposed to a set of validated disgust pictures pictures. The disgust scenes showed repulsive animals
that are able to specifically elicit this emotion with sufficient (e.g., maggots, snails), poor hygiene (e.g., dirty toilet,
intensity (Schienle, Stark, et al., 2002). By means of a tilt garbage), and unusual/spoiled food (e.g., a man eats a
table the participants were brought from a supine to an grasshopper). The neutral pictures depicted geometric
upright position. This leads to a rapid translocation of blood figures, nature scenes, and household items. All pictures
to the lower body. As a response individuals typically acti- were taken from a validated picture set (Schienle, Stark,
vate their sympathetic nervous system. Tachycardia et al., 2002). Previous investigations were able to demon-
(increase in heart rate of 10–30 beats per minute) and vaso- strate that these images are able to induce the target
constriction occur as part of the compensatory response to emotion disgust to a significantly higher degree than any
orthostasis (Lackner, Goswami, Papousek, et al., 2010). other basic emotion, especially fear (e.g., Schienle, Übel,
Avery different response pattern is displayed by individu- Schöngassner, Ille, & Scharmüller, in press), and therefore
als who suffer from orthostatic intolerance. Their reaction is can be considered specific disgust elicitors.
characterized by a decrease in blood pressure and heart rate The design consisted of two disgust-picture blocks and
during the tilt, which can even result in fainting. It has been one neutral-picture block. Each block comprised 20 pic-
shown that patients suffering from blood phobia have a ten- tures of the same type, which were shown in random order.
dency to not only faint in disorder-relevant situations, but also Each image was presented for a period varying between
during orthostatic stress. Accurso et al. (2001) reported that 6 and 12 s (M = 9 s) to prevent effects of paced stimuli
during a 70 head-up tilt, 82% of the examined blood-phobic (Lackner, Goswami, Hinghofer-Szalkay, et al., 2010).
subjects experienced presyncope or syncope, in contrast to The picture-block duration was 3 min.
only 9% of the control subjects. Moreover, this patient group The participants viewed the disgust picture blocks in
typically reports elevated disgust proneness, which points to a two body positions; in a lying position (Supine_Disgust),
possible association between disgust and faint proneness (for a and in a 70 upright position (Tilted_Disgust). They were
summary see Schienle & Leutgeb, 2012). randomly assigned to one of two sequence groups (tilting
To the best of our knowledge, it has not been studied first or tilting second). Both groups started the experiment
whether disgust-prone individuals (not afflicted with blood with the neutral condition in a lying position (Supine_
phobia) suffer from orthostatic intolerance. Therefore, we Neutral) followed by the disgust conditions.
tested the hypothesis that individuals with elevated disgust
proneness are susceptible to vasovagal syncope during head-
up tilt, especially when disgust-relevant pictures are shown.
Self-Report Measures

Methods (a) The Questionnaire for the Assessment of Disgust


Proneness (QADP; Schienle, Walter, et al., 2002)
Participants describes 37 situations (e.g., ‘‘You touch the toilet seat
with part of your body in a public restroom’’; ‘‘You
We studied 30 healthy female students with a mean age smell vomit’’), that have to be rated on 5-point
of M = 23.0 years (SD = 2.4) and an average BMI of scales (0 = not disgusting; 4 = very disgusting).

Journal of Psychophysiology 2015; Vol. 29(2):73–79  2015 Hogrefe Publishing


A. Schienle et al.: Disgust and Body Position 75

The Cronbach’s a for the total scale is .90.


(b) The German version of the Multidimensional Blood/
Injury Phobia Inventory (MBI; Gebhardt et al.,
2010; short form) consists of 20 items and 4 subscales
(injections, blood, fainting, hospital). The rater indi-
cates whether a statement applies to herself/himself
(0 = not at all; 3 = very often). The Cronbach’s a
for the total scale is .96. This questionnaire was used
to exclude participants with blood-phobic symptoms.
(c) All participants rated how often they had experienced
a (pre)syncope when standing upright for a longer per-
iod of time. This was done on a 5-point Likert scale
(1 = never; 5 = often).
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

(d) Picture ratings: Subsequent to the viewing of a picture


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

block, the participants verbally indicated to the exper-


imenter to what degree the images elicited disgust,
nausea, dizziness, and sweating. This was done on
9-point scales (1 = low intensity; 9 = high intensity). Figure 1. Study design.

Procedure vascular unloading technique and corrected to absolute val-


ues with oscillometric BP measurement on the contralateral
Both sequence groups started the experiment with a 16-min upper arm. The method is based on concentrically inter-
resting period (without picture presentation) in supine posi- locking control loops for correct long-term tracing of finger
tion on the tilt table. Then they were presented with the BP and delivers, in contrast to intermittent set point
neutral pictures (3 min), which were projected to the wall re-adjustments of the conventional vascular unloading tech-
by means of a beamer. The projected picture size was nique, BP without interruptions (Fortin, Marte, et al., 2006).
1.3 m (width) and 80 cm (height); the distance from the tilt For thoracic impedance measurement, three CNSystems
table (head of the participant) to the wall was 2 m. Subse- short band electrodes (two electrode bands set at a
quent to the viewing of the picture block, the participants predefined distance onto a common adhesive medium;
rated the pictures (1-min rating period). sampling rate = 500 Hz, Ieff < 400 lA, f = 45 kHz,
Subsequently, the table was brought into a 70 upright Z0,range = 10–75 X, dZ/dt = ±10 X/s) were placed onto
position for one group (n = 15), whereas the other group the participant: One placed at the nape of the neck close
(n = 15) remained in the lying position. After 6 min, the to the glottis, two others placed on the thorax close to the
participants were presented with the disgust-picture block xiphoid (see Fortin, Habenbacher, et al., 2006). The respira-
followed by the rating. tory signal was derived from the raw data of thoracic
Finally, the body position was changed again (the lying impedance (i.e., Ernst, Litvack, Lozano, Cacioppo, &
group was tilted, and the tilted group was brought into a Berntson, 1999).
supine position). After 6 min, the women again viewed For heart rate and blood pressure beat-to-beat values
20 disgusting pictures followed by the rating. The design were used. Artifact handling of beat-to-beat values
of the study is displayed in Figure 1. was done semi-automatically with a signal analyzer
The study had been approved by the ethics committee of developed in MATLAB (MATLAB, MathWorks Natick,
the University of Graz. All participants gave their written MA) which identifies artifacts by the physiological limits
informed consent after the study had been explained to and the maximal percentage of change in relationship to
them in detail. standard deviation of the signal, using the time series with
equidistant time steps after resampling beat-to-beat values
with 4 Hz. Single artifacts were replaced by linear interpo-
Physiological Recordings and Analysis lation and their appearance was recorded.
The time domain variable of heart rate variability was
Continuous hemodynamic monitoring of heart rate (HR), computed as the standard deviation (SDNN, standard devi-
blood pressure (BP), and thoracic impedance was car- ation of normal-to-normal beat), the frequency domain vari-
ried out with the Task Force Monitor (TFM; CNSystems, ables as the power in the high/ low frequency band of heart
Graz, Austria). HR was recorded by 3-lead electro- rate variability. In more detail, the autoregressive Burg
cardiography (ECG; sampling rate = 1 kHz, fcut-off = algorithm (model order = 24) after resampling with 4 Hz
0.08–150 Hz) using CNSystems ECG-electrodes placed using piecewise cubic spline interpolation after above-
at the thoracic region. Continuous BP (sampling mentioned artifact correction and removing the trend
rate = 100 Hz, BPrange = 50–250 mmHg, ±5 mmHg) was of 2nd order for frequency domain variables of heart
derived from the finger using a refined version of the rate variability was used. Low frequency was defined as

 2015 Hogrefe Publishing Journal of Psychophysiology 2015; Vol. 29(2):73–79


76 A. Schienle et al.: Disgust and Body Position

0.04–0.15 Hz (LFrr), high frequency was defined as 0.15– p < .001, gp2 = .859), nausea, F(2, 58) = 47.0, p < .001,
0.40 Hz (HFrr), according to published recommendations gp2 = .619, dizziness, F(2, 58) = 11.7, p < .001, gp2 = .286,
(Task Force, 1996). Because of skewed distributions of and sweating, F(2, 58) = 15.3, p < .001, gp2 = .345.
the frequency domain variables, a natural logarithmic trans- The conducted post hoc t-tests indicated that experi-
formation (ln) was applied. enced disgust did not differ between the two Disgust condi-
All variables were computed across the 3-min epochs of tions, t(29) = 0.67, p = .51, which were both rated as more
picture presentation and for 3-min epochs preceding each of repulsive than the Neutral condition (both p’s < .001).
the three picture conditions (baselines), in order to control Similarly, experienced nausea, sweating, and dizziness only
for effects of initial transient responses due to the ortho- differed between the two Disgust and the Neutral conditions
static challenge (Goswami et al., 2010; Lackner, Goswami, (all p’s < .001), whereas the two Disgust conditions were
Papousek, et al., 2010). comparable (all p’s > .02). The ratings are displayed in
Table 1.

Statistical Analysis
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Physiological Responses
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

We conducted analyses of variance for repeated measures


with the within-subjects factor picture-condition We first analyzed the baseline data and observed significant
(Supine_Neutral, Supine_Disgust, Tilted_Disgust) for the main effects for all recorded variables: heart rate,
subjective ratings as well as for the physiological data. F(2, 58) = 207.4, p < .001, gp2 = .877, heart rate variability,
Moreover, we computed an ANOVA for the physiological F(2, 58) = 13.4, p < .001, gp2 = .317, the high frequency
data of the three baseline conditions (without picture pre- component of heart rate variability, F(2, 58) = 67.5,
sentation) in order to determine the effects of tilting. p < .001, gp2 = .700, the low frequency component of heart
Observed significant main effects were followed up by rate variability, F(2, 58) = 3.5, p = .038, gp2 = .107, systolic
paired t-tests with Bonferroni correction for the three con- blood pressure, F(2, 58) = 16.7, p < .001, gp2 = .374, and
ditions (significance cut-off: .05/3 = .016). diastolic blood pressure, F(2, 58) = 50.5, p < .001,
The main effects for the between-subject factor gp2 = .643, as well as breathing frequency, F(2, 58) = 3.9,
sequence as well as the interactions Sequence · Picture- p = .02, gp2 = .121.
condition were always nonsignificant (and are therefore The conducted post hoc t-tests indicated that the tilting
not reported). condition was associated with increased heart rate, systolic/
diastolic blood pressure, and lowered heart rate variability
as well as lowered power in the high frequency band of
heart rate variability compared to both lying conditions
Results (all p’s < .001). The post hoc t-tests for the low frequency
component and for breathing frequency were nonsignificant
Self-Report Measures (all p’s > .03).
In order to identify the additional effects of disgust elic-
Questionnaires itation, we conducted analyses of variance for repeated
measures for baseline-corrected values for each of the three
In the present investigation, the average score on the conditions (e.g., Tilted_Disgust minus Tilted_Baseline).
Questionnaire for the Assessment of Disgust Proneness The main effect for condition was statistically significant
(QADP; Schienle, Walter, et al., 2002) was M = 2.71 for diastolic blood pressure, F(2, 58) = 5.34, p = .008,
(SD = 0.34), which was significantly higher than the score gp2 = .160, and for breathing frequency, F(2, 58) = 18.3,
of the construction sample (p < .001). p < .001, gp2 = 387. All other main effects were nonsignif-
The scores (M, SD) on the subscales of the MBPI were icant (all p’s > .08).
as follows: injections, 5.3 (5.1); blood, 5.5 (4.9); fainting, The post hoc t-tests indicated that the Tilted_Disgust
1.8 (2.5); hospital, 5.2 (4.3). These values did not differ condition was associated with a lower diastolic blood pres-
from the mean scores of the non-anxious participants sure compared to Supine_Disgust, t(29) = 3.9, p = .001.
(n = 536) tested for the construction of the questionnaire The breathing frequency differed between Neutral and
(all p’s > .38). Supine_Disgust as well as between Neutral and Tilted_
The mean rating for experienced faintness symptoms Disgust (both p’s < .001). The difference between the two
during orthostatic load was M = 1.63 (SD = 0.99; range: disgust conditions was statistically nonsignificant (p = .04).
1–4). 57% of the participants indicated that they had never As an exploratory approach, we correlated self-reported
experienced a (pre)syncope after standing upright for a disgust proneness (QADP scores) with the six baseline-
longer period of time. corrected cardiovascular variables in the three conditions
(Bonferroni cutoff: .05/18 = .0027). We observed a signif-
icant correlation between disgust proneness and heart rate
Affective Ratings in the Tilted_Disgust condition (r = .55, p = .002). Further,
QADP scores were positively associated with experienced
The main effect for picture-condition was statistically disgust in the Supine_Disgust (r = .41, p = .02) and in
significant for experienced disgust, F(2, 58) = 177.1, the Tilted_Disgust condition (r = .44, p = .01).

Journal of Psychophysiology 2015; Vol. 29(2):73–79  2015 Hogrefe Publishing


A. Schienle et al.: Disgust and Body Position 77

Table 1. Self-report and physiological data for the experimental conditions


Supine_Neutral Supine_Disgust Tilted_Disgust
M (SD) M (SD) M (SD)
Self-Report (ratings for picture blocks)
Experienced intensity of
Disgust 1.2 (0.6) 6.7 (2.1) 6.9 (1.9)
Dizziness 1.0 (0.2) 2.0 (1.1) 2.7 (2.4)
Sweating 1.1 (0.3) 2.0 (1.2) 2.8 (2.3)
Nausea 1.0 (0.2) 3.9 (2.2) 4.6 (2.5)
Physiological variables
Baseline (before picture viewing)
Heart rate [bpm] 68.9 (7.7) 66.5 (8.1) 91.1 (11.9)
Heart rate variability [SDNN: ms] 58.7 (29.0) 60.7 (29.7) 38.4 (15.2)
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

High frequency power [HFrr: ms2] 6.6 (1.1) 6.8 (1.2) 4.5 (1.2)
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Low frequency power [LFrr: ms2] 6.6 (0.9) 6.6 (1.1) 6.2 (0.9)
Systolic blood pressure [mmHg] 122.4 (10.6) 123.1 (11.6) 127.4 (11.5)
Diastolic blood pressure [mmHg] 79.9 (9.0) 79.0 (9.9) 87.4 (9.6)
Breathing frequency [1/min] 17.5 (3.7) 17.6 (3.3) 16.5 (3.6)
Effects of picture viewing (minus baseline)
Heart rate [bpm] 0.58 (2.9) 1.07 (2.1) 0.35 (3.8)
Heart rate variability [SDNN: ms] 0.95 (8.8) 0.70 (14.4) 1.25 (7.0)
High frequency power [HFrr: ms2] 0.11 (0.4) 0.08 (0.6) 0.07 (0.5)
Low frequency power [LFrr: ms2] 0.12 (0.6) 0.21 (0.6) 0.09 (0.5)
Systolic blood pressure [mmHg] 0.36 (3.5) 0.81 (2.9) 0.16 (3.5)
Diastolic blood pressure [mmHg] 0.01 (3.1) 1.39 (2.0) 1.20 (4.1)
Breathing frequency [1/min] 0.98 (1.6) 0.28 (1.5) 1.02 (1.8)
Notes. SDNN = standard deviation for normal to normal beat; HFrr = power in the high frequency band of heart rate variability
[0.15–0.40 Hz] component; LFrr = power in the low frequency band of heart rate variability component [0.04–0.15 Hz].

Finally, we compared those participants who had in the Supine_Disgust condition did not differ from the
reported no faintness symptoms during long standing Tilted_Disgust condition. This also applied to perceived
(n = 17) or at least some (n = 13) with regard to their somatic symptoms such as dizziness, nausea, and sweating.
QADP scores, disgust ratings in the Supine_Disgust and In contrast, the diastolic blood pressure, an indicator of
in the Tilted-Disgust condition. The results of the three con- peripheral resistance, differed between the two disgust con-
ducted t-tests were all nonsignificant (all p’s > .09). ditions. This parameter was smaller in the tilted position
reflecting reduced sympathetic efferent activity. Obviously,
disgust induction was associated with less effective vaso-
constriction in the upright relative to the supine condition.
Discussion From a physiological standpoint, a typical syncope devel-
ops because of vasodilatation and venous blood pooling
The aim of the present study was to investigate whether in the lower body. This ultimately reduces brain perfusion
body position influences cardiovascular disgust reactivity below a certain level, which in turn triggers fainting.
and whether disgust-proneness is associated with the sus- The additional reduction of diastolic blood pressure due
ceptibility for vasovagal syncope during head-up tilt. to disgust elicitation in the upright position may therefore
The tilting procedure (without picture presentation) constitute a risk factor for presyncope.
resulted in pronounced cardiovascular effects. The change Moreover, disgust-proneness was positively correlated
from a supine to an upright position led to a mean heart rate with heart rate, but only during tilting. This association
increase of almost 25 beats per minute. Also, blood pres- might point to a compensatory mechanism which aims at
sure increased, and heart rate variability as well as its high stabilizing mean arterial pressure, the primary regulated
frequency power decreased. These changes are indicators of variable during stress induction (Julius, 1988; Lackner,
sympathetic activation and parasympathetic deactivation Goswami, Papousek, et al., 2010). It is a typical observation
(Goswami et al., 2009; Lackner, Goswami, Papousek, that participants of tilt-table experiments display continuous
et al., 2010). heart rate increases during the first phase of orthostatic load
The main purpose of the present study was to analyze (Goswami et al., 2009; Hinghofer-Szalkay et al., 2011;
the effects of disgust elicitation in combination with tilting. Lackner, Goswami, Papousek, et al., 2010). This response
The disgust induction was successful according to the self- can be understood as a compensatory mechanism. Due to
report of the participants. The women perceived the aver- the postural change, the blood tends to pool in the legs,
sive pictures as very repulsive. However, the disgust ratings potentially reducing the amount of blood available to return

 2015 Hogrefe Publishing Journal of Psychophysiology 2015; Vol. 29(2):73–79


78 A. Schienle et al.: Disgust and Body Position

to the heart and the brain. Increases in heart rate help to Ethics and Disclosure Statements
redistribute blood volume to the upper part of the body.
However, this negative feedback loop only works up to a All participants of the study provided written informed con-
certain point. When the individual compensatory threshold sent and the study was approved by the Ethics Committee
is reached, and the postural orthostatic hypotension of the University of Graz. All authors disclose no actual
becomes too pronounced, then the syncope occurs. Depend- or potential conflicts of interest including any financial,
ing on the disgust proneness of individuals this mechanism personal, or other relationships with other people or organi-
might be used differentially. It can be assumed that espe- zations that could inappropriately influence (bias) their
cially women with very high trait disgust counter-regulated work.
more extensively to prevent fainting.
Although body position and trait disgust were associated
with cardiovascular disgust reactivity, we were not able to References
observe a correlation between faint and disgust proneness
on the self-report level. Participants with and without expe- Accurso, V., Winnicki, M., Shamsuzzaman, A. S. M., Wenzel, A.,
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

rience of faintness symptoms during long standing in every- Johnson, A. K., & Somers, V. K. (2001). Predisposition to
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

day life (as rated with the Likert scale) did neither differ in vasovagal syncope in subjects with blood/injury phobia.
their questionnaire scores nor affective ratings. The latter Circulation, 104, 903–907.
findings are in line with reports by several authors (e.g., de Jong, P. J., van Overveld, M., & Peters, M. L. (2011).
Sympathetic and parasympathetic responses to a core disgust
Olatunji et al., 2006; Vossbeck-Elsebusch & Gerlach, video clip as a function of disgust propensity and disgust
2012), who were not able to identify a disgust-faint relation- sensitivity. Biological Psychology, 88, 174–179. doi:
ship. For example, Olatunji et al. (2006) computed a struc- 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2011.07.009
tural equation model that revealed that the relation between Ernst, J. M., Litvack, D. A., Lozano, D. L., Cacioppo, J. T., &
disgust proneness and fainting symptoms was fully medi- Berntson, G. G. (1999). Impedance pneumography: Noise as
ated by blood-related fears. However, the mentioned studies signal in impedance cardiography. Psychophysiology, 36,
333–338. doi: 10.1017/S0048577299981003
were only based on self-report and it cannot be excluded Fortin, J., Habenbacher, W., Heller, A., Hacker, A., Grüllenberger, R.,
that the participants’ interoceptive capabilities were not suf- Innerhofer, J., & Wach, P. (2006). Non-invasive beat-to-beat
ficient to detect symptoms of presyncope reliably. The asset cardiac output monitoring by an improved method of transtho-
of the present investigation is that the susceptibility for racic bioimpedance measurement. Computers in Biology
vasovagal syncope during orthostatic load was directly and Medicine, 36, 1185–1203. doi: 10.1016/j.compbiomed.
tested for the first time in disgust-prone individuals. 2005.06.001
We have to mention several limitations of our study. Fortin, J., Marte, W., Grüllenberger, R., Hacker, A., Habenbacher, W.,
Heller, A., . . . , Skrabal, F. (2006). Continuous non-invasive
We had selected disgust-prone individuals for our experi- blood pressure monitoring using concentrically interlocking
ment in order to secure a sufficient reactivity to the visual control loops. Computers in Biology and Medicine, 36, 941–957.
stimuli. However, it cannot be excluded that these individ- doi: 10.1016/j.compbiomed.2005.04.003
uals exhibited special physiological patterns. Therefore, Gebhardt, C., Kämpfe-Hargrave, N., & Mitte, K. (2010).
future studies should compare two extreme groups with German version of the Multidimensional Blood/lnjury
high and low disgust proneness in their onset of presyncope Phobia lnventory. Zeitschrift für Klinische Psychologie und
Psychotherapie, 39, 97–106.
during orthostatic load. For this, the duration of the exper- Gerlach, A. L., Nat, R., Spellmeyer, G., Vögele, C., Huster, R.,
iment should be extended. Another option is the investiga- Stevens, S., & Deckert, J. (2006). Blood-injury phobia with
tion of a group with high trait disgust variance. We have to and without a history of fainting: Disgust sensitivity does not
admit that the observed correlations between the measured explain the fainting response. Psychosomatic Medicine, 68,
cardiovascular variables and disgust proneness were only 331–339. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000203284.53066.4b
moderate, which might be due to the range restriction, Goswami, N., Lackner, H. K., Papousek, I., Jezova, D.,
Hinghofer-Szalkay, H., & Montani, J. P. (2010). Rate of
because only women with elevated QADP scores were cardiovascular recovery to combined or separate orthostatic
included. and mental challenges. International Journal of Psycho-
Further, we did not control respiration, although this physiology, 75, 54–62. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2009.11.005
parameter influences measures of cardiac vagal tone (Ritz Goswami, N., Roessler, A., Lackner, H. K., Schneditz, D.,
& Dahme, 2006). We had refrained from using a respiratory Grasser, E., & Hinghofer-Szalkay, H. G. (2009). Heart rate
control method since this might have interfered with and stroke volume response patterns to augmented ortho-
emotion elicitation. By demonstrating that the baseline- static stress. Clinical Autonomic Research, 19, 157–165.
doi: 10.1007/s10286-009-0001-x
corrected breathing frequency did not differ between the Hinghofer-Szalkay, H., Lackner, H. K., Rössler, A., Narath, B.,
two disgust conditions, we believe that our approach can Jantscher, A., & Goswami, N. (2011). Hormonal and plasma
be considered acceptable. volume changes after presyncope. European Journal of
In summary, we were able to demonstrate that body Clinical Investigation, 41, 1180–1185. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-
position influenced cardiovascular disgust reactivity, which 2362.2011.02523.x
was additionally associated with participants’disgust prone- Julius, S. (1988). The blood-pressure seeking properties of the
central nervous-system. Journal of Hypertension, 6,
ness. Our data point to a possible disgust-faint relationship 177–185.
that is only present in individuals with a particular predispo- Lackner, H. K., Goswami, N., Hinghofer-Szalkay, H., Papousek, I.,
sition (elevated disgust sensitivity), who experience disgust Scharfetter, H., Furlan, R., & Schwaberger, G. (2010). Effects
during somatic (orthostatic) stress. of stimuli on cardiovascular reactivity occurring at regular

Journal of Psychophysiology 2015; Vol. 29(2):73–79  2015 Hogrefe Publishing


A. Schienle et al.: Disgust and Body Position 79

intervals during mental stress. Journal of Psychophysiology, 24, Schienle, A., & Leutgeb, V. (2012). Blut-Spritzen-Verletzungs-
48–60. doi: 10.1027/0269-8803/a000006 phobie (Fortschritte der Psychotherapie, Vol. 50). Göttingen,
Lackner, H. K., Goswami, N., Papousek, I., Roessler, A., Germany: Hogrefe.
Grasser, E. K., Montani, J. P., Jezova, D., & Hinghofer- Schienle, A., Übel, S., Schöngassner, F., Ille, R., & Scharmüller,
Szalkay, H. (2010). Time course of cardiovascular responses W. (2014). Disgust regulation via placebo: An fMRI study.
induced by mental and orthostatic challenges. International Social, Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 9, 985–990.
Journal of Psychophysiology, 75, 48–53. doi: 10.1016/ doi: 10.1093/scan/nst072
j.ijpsycho.2009.11.003 Stark, R., Walter, B., Schienle, A., & Vaitl, D. (2005).
Levenson, R. W., Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1990). Voluntary Psychophysiological correlates of disgust and disgust
facial action generates emotion-specific autonomic nervous sensitivity. Journal of Psychophysiology, 19, 50–60.
system activity. Psychophysiology, 27, 363–384. doi: 10.1027/0269-8803.19.1.50
Olatunji, B. O., Williams, N. L., Sawchuk, C. N., & Lohr, J. M. Task Force of the European Society of Cardiology and the North
(2006). Disgust, anxiety and fainting symptoms associated American Society of Pacing and Electrophysiology. (1996).
with blood-injection-injury fears: A structural model. Heart rate variability: Standards of measurement, physio-
Journal of Anxi Disorders, 20, 23–41. logical interpretation, and clinical use. Circulation, 93,
Page, A. C. (1994). Blood-injury phobia. Clinical Psychology 1043–1065.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Review, 14, 443–461. doi: 10.1016/0272-7358(94)90036-1 van Overveld, W. J., de Jong, P. J., & Peters, M. L. (2009).
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Page, A. C. (2003). The role of disgust in faintness elicited by Digestive and cardiovascular responses to core and animal-
blood and injection stimuli. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, reminder disgust. Biological Psychology, 80, 149–157.
17, 45–58. doi: 10.1016/S0887-6185(02)00169-X doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2008.08.002
Page, A. C., & Tan, B. J. (2009). Disgust and blood-injury- Vossbeck-Elsebusch, A. N., & Gerlach, A. L. (2012). The
injection phobia. In B. O. Olatunji & D. McKay (Eds.), relation between disgust sensitivity, blood-injection-injury
Disgust and its disorders: Theory, assessment, and treatment fears and vasovagal symptoms in blood donors: Disgust
(pp. 191–210). Washington, DC: American Psychological sensitivity cannot explain fainting or blood donation-related
Association. symptoms. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental
Ritz, T., & Dahme, B. (2006). Implementation and interpretation Psychiatry, 43, 607–613. doi: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2011.08.005
of respiratory sinus arrhythmia measures in psychosomatic Vossbeck-Elsebusch, A. N., Steinigweg, K., Vögele, C., &
medicine: Practice against better evidence? Psychosomatic Gerlach, A. L. (2012). Does disgust increase parasympa-
Medicine, 68, 617–627. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000228010. thetic activation in individuals with a history of fainting? A
96408.ed psychophysiological analysis of disgust stimuli with and
Rohrmann, S., & Hopp, H. (2008). Cardiovascular indicators of without blood-injection-injury association. Journal of
disgust. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 68, Anxiety Disorders, 26, 849–858. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.
201–208. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2008.01.011 2012.07.003
Sarlo, M., Buodo, G., Munafò, M., Stegagno, L., & Palomba, D.
(2008). Cardiovascular dynamics in blood phobia: Evidence
for a key role in sympathetic activity in vulnerability to
syncope. Psychophysiology, 45, 1038–1045. doi: 10.1111/ Accepted for publication: July 10, 2014
j.1469-8986.2008.00713.x Published online: March 25, 2015
Schienle, A., Stark, R., & Vaitl, D. (2001). Evaluative condi-
tioning: A possible explanation for the acquisition of disgust
responses? Learning and Motivation, 32, 65–83. doi:
10.1006/lmot.2000.1067 Anne Schienle
Schienle, A., Stark, R., Walter, B., Blecker, C., Ott, U., Sammer,
G., & Vaitl, D. (2002). The insula is not specifically involved University of Graz
in disgust processing: An fMRI study. NeuroReport, 13, Department of Psychology
2023–2026. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200211150-00006 Universitätsplatz 2/III
Schienle, A., Walter, B., & Vaitl, D. (2002). Ein Fragebogen zur 8010 Graz
Erfassung der Ekelempfindlichkeit (FEE) [Questionnaire for Austria
the assessment of disgust sensitivity]. Zeitschrift für Tel. +43 316 380-5086
Klinische Psychologie und Psychotherapie, 31, 110–120. Fax +43 316 380-9808
doi: 10.1026/1616-3443.31.2.110 E-mail anne.schienle@uni-graz.at

 2015 Hogrefe Publishing Journal of Psychophysiology 2015; Vol. 29(2):73–79

You might also like