You are on page 1of 5

Aging & Mental Health, May 2006; 10(3): 207–210

EDITORIAL

Cognitive stimulation and dementia

E. MONIZ-COOK

Department of Clinical Psychology, Post Graduate Medical Institute, Hertford Building, University of Hull, Hull, UK

The ageing society has stimulated interest in the This guideline further suggests that individual
‘use it or lose it’ approach to memory decline, where 24-hour reality orientation therapy applied by skilled
aspects of lifestyle such as mental activity (Wilson therapists is better than group classroom reality
et al., 2002) or exercise (Larson et al., 2006) are orientation (SIGN, 2006, p. 11), that is usually
thought to act as protective factors in delaying the applied by staff in care settings, but it overlooks
onset of dementia. The current drive towards early the developing evidence base for group reality
detection and diagnosis in many western health care orientation therapy or group cognitive stimulation
systems has precipitated a parallel interest in (Breuil et al., 1994; Holden & Woods, 1995;
psychological treatments for dementia, since many Spector, Davies, Woods, & Orrell, 2001; Spector
people and families often want to know what et al., 2003). Provision of individually tailored
they should do about memory difficulties, following home-based programmes involving both the person
a diagnosis of dementia. Some governments have with dementia and the family caregiver reflects the
reflected this public interest in their policies—for zeitgeist of dementia care practice (Vernooij-Dassen
example, in the UK, the National Service Framework & Moniz–Cook, 2005), but this does not have to
for Older People, recommends that ‘mental exercise’ is imply that there is no place for group intervention
offered (Department of Health [DoH], 2001, p. 98). within a given set of circumstances, for some
There is confusion about what ‘mental exercise’ people with dementia. The paper by Woods and
as a treatment for dementia might mean. Cognitive his colleagues (this issue) describes the effect of
stimulation (Breuil et al., 1994; Woods et al., this a group cognitive stimulation programme on quality
issue) or its precursor, reality orientation (Woods, of life. Given the aforementioned problems with
2001), is one type of mental ‘therapy’ for dementia terminology, definitions and practice, we will now
but other approaches such as memory training turn to the context of group cognitive stimulation by
(Zarit, S. H., Zarit, J. M., & Reever, 1982), examining its history and development as a psycho-
memory rehabilitation (Clare 1999; De Vreese, social intervention for dementia.
Neri, Fioravanti, Belloi, & Zanetti, 2001), pro-
cedural memory stimulation (Zanetti et al.,
1995), cognitive rehabilitation (Clare & Woods,
Cognitive stimulation: Connecting the past to
2001), cognitive training (Hofmann, Hock,
the present
Kuhler, & Muller-Spahn, 1996), psychosocial
rehabilitation (Bird, 2000) and memory stimulation Mental activity as a psychological ‘therapy’ for
(Grandmaison & Simard, 2003) can also be found in dementia has a long history, dating to the
the literature on interventions that target cognition development of reality orientation (RO), where
in people with dementia. These wide ranging Taulbee and Folsom (1966) noticed that it allowed
terminologies and definitions can give rise to a change from hopelessness to hopefulness for some
misunderstandings in the practice of cognition- people with dementia. It also appeared to provide
focussed treatment in dementia. This is seen for hope for care staff, with reports of improved morale
example, in a recent national guideline for the (Merchant & Saxby, 1981; Powell-Proctor & Millar,
management of dementia (Scottish Intercollegiate 1982), perhaps because it helped combat the
Guidelines network [SIGN], 2006), where cognitive hopelessness and therapeutic nihilism that was at
stimulation describing home-based individual the time associated with dementia (Holden, 1987).
cognitive training carried out with minimal training Reality orientation was a method of presenting
by families is recommended (SIGN, 2006, p. 8). information that reoriented people with dementia

Correspondence: Professor Esme Moniz-Cook, PhD, Clinical Psychology & Ageing, Post Graduate Medical Institute,
Hertford Building, University of Hull, Hull, UK. E-mail: E.D.Moniz-cook@hull.ac.uk
ISSN 1360-7863 print/ISSN 1364-6915 online ß 2006 Taylor & Francis
DOI: 10.1080/13607860600725268
208 E. Moniz-Cook

throughout the day (i.e., 24-hour RO) including ‘exercise’ runs the risk of undermining the person’s
developing a ‘prosthetic’ environment, but its more sense of competence and the finding that global
common use in research and practice remained cognitive stimulation in the form of group recrea-
within group ‘classroom’ sessions (Holden & tional activity has better outcomes for both person
Woods, 1995). By the 1980s, concerns about its and family (Farina et al., this issue), is therefore
potential to become an insensitive experience for reassuring.
people with dementia (Dietch, Hewett, & Jones, A cognition-orientated intervention that achieves
1989; Gubrium & Ksander, 1975), resulted in a fall improvements on measures of cognition may not
in its popularity, with exceptions in some countries have any practical effect on the everyday situations
such as in Italy, where group reality orientation that people with dementia and their families might
therapy (ROT) continues to be embraced face (Bird, 2000). Therefore, in addition to cogni-
(Zanetti et al., 1995; 2002). By the turn of the tion other outcomes such as emotion and behaviour
century a systematic review of classroom RO (Woods, 2001), the impact on family caregivers
(Spector et al., 2000) reported positive effects on (Richards, Duggan, Carr, Wang & Moniz-Cook,
cognition and in some cases, behaviour, in people 2003) and the relationship between the person and
with dementia. The most influential study at the their family caregiver (Charlesworth, 2001), are
time (Breuil et al., 1994) described a ‘cognitive also now seen as important outcomes for research
stimulation’ group and it also became clear that the on the cognitive management of dementia. For
content of groups were not all the same (see review example, studies of memory training have reported
by Spector, Orrell, Davies, & Woods, 2001). Many positive changes in the person’s memory perfor-
RO groups included aspects of reminiscence or other mance, but a negative effect on family caregiver
stimulating activity and there were culture-specific mood (Zarit et al., 1982), or positive outcomes for
differences in application across some countries cognition with no negative effect on caregiver mood
(Holden & Woods, 1995). What followed was the (Onder et al., 2005) or improvements in memory
development of a global Cognitive Stimulation with initial family distress, followed by positive
Therapy (CST) group programme lasting seven outcome in mood and family burden a year later
weeks (Spector et al., 2001), for people in (Moniz-Cook, Agar, Gibson, Win, & Wang, 1998).
care settings, a randomised controlled trial which The papers on cognition-orientated group interven-
demonstrated short-term positive outcomes on both tions for people with dementia in this issue cannot be
cognition and quality of life (Spector et al., 2003) criticised for ignoring real life situations for people
and persuasive arguments for the redefinition of RO and families, since they specifically measure the
as ‘cognitive stimulation’ (Woods, 2002). effects on quality of life of people with dementia
This issue contains two papers on cognition- (Woods et al., this issue), their mood, behaviour and
orientated group interventions for people their family’s burden (Farina et al., this issue).
with dementia. One study evaluates the CST However teasing out the important components of
programme (Woods et al., this issue), whilst the a group cognition-orientated programme is not easy,
other compares global cognitive stimulation training since a benefit of group work is the opportunity for
and cognitive—specific cognitive training (Farina social interaction. For people with dementia who
et al., this issue). The problem with definition and engage in cognitive management groups, social
terminology described previously has been recently interaction can act as one of the ‘active ingredients’
addressed by Clare and Woods (2004), who outline (Bird, 2000) that contribute to positive outcomes
a conceptual basis for classifying the range of on mood and quality of life. The study from Italy by
cognition-focussed interventions into three broad Farina and her colleagues is disappointing in its
but distinct categories, which they describe as failure to measure the quality of life of patients since
cognitive stimulation, cognitive training and cogni- it is not possible to use its findings to further evaluate
tive rehabilitation. Within this framework, cognitive the contribution of social interaction as a component
stimulation therapy (Woods et al., this issue) and of global cognitive stimulation therapy on positive
global cognitive stimulation training (Farina et al., quality of life outcomes (Spector et al., 2003; Woods
this issue) may be seen as ‘cognitive stimulation’ et al., this issue). The study by Woods and his
interventions, since both target global cognitive colleagues did not include a placebo social activity or
functions which are interrelated and therefore passive recreational group. Pleasurable activity
cannot be treated in isolation. In contrast, the and social interaction may have contributed to the
cognitive-specific training group, described by positive quality of life outcomes, since patients
Farina and her colleagues, is an example of cognitive reported improvements in their relationships as
training, since it involves repeated practice on well as in their memory function, energy and in
specific tasks such as analogues of activities of daily managing everyday chores, following cognitive
living (washing hands, laying tables making tea) or stimulation therapy (Woods et al., this issue).
paper exercises for enhancing attention, short–term In the Italian study, improved trends on measures
memory language, visuo-spatial abilities and cate- of behaviour for both cognitive stimulation and
gorization. This type of repeated practice or cognitive training groups were, according to the
Cognitive stimulation and dementia 209

authors, associated with non-specific psychological content is similar (see for example Zanetti et al.,
support and socialisation. An in-home programme, 2002). Traditional RO group therapy may have
where family caregivers delivered cognitive training, found new avenues for application in early
also reported positive outcomes on cognition and stage dementia, within ongoing in-home individual
interaction against a control group, but positive gains programmes provided by family caregivers, in
were also seen in a placebo condition of passive conjunction with an acetylcholinesterase inhibitor
social activity (Quayhagen, M. P., Quayhagen, M., (Onder et al., 2005). However this type of individual
Corbeil, Roth, & Rodgers, 1995; 1996). Knowing 24-hour in-home RO needs to show its efficacy on
the precise contribution of cognition and social quality of life measures for people with dementia.
interaction to the positive reported outcomes of The relative merits of 24-hour cognitive stimulation,
cognitive stimulation may well be an interesting cognitive training and cognitive rehabilitation also
pursuit for researchers (see Clare and Woods, 2004, require further investigation.
p. 387), but given that many care staff struggle to
find positive pleasurable activity for people with
dementia, CST, like its precursor ‘classroom’ RO
may well become one way of providing mental References
exercise and pleasant social activity in care home
settings. Bird, M. J. (2000). Psychosocial rehabilitation for problems
arising from cognitive deficits in dementia. In R. D. Hill,
If group reality orientation is conceptually the
L. Bäckman & A. S. Neely (Eds.), Cognitive rehabilitation in old
same as cognitive stimulation (Clare & Woods, age. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
2004), is there a place for other forms of RO, that Breuil, V., de Rotrou, J., Forette, F., Tortrat, D., Ganasia-
are not based on group activity, within the range of Ganem, A., Frambourt, A., et al. (1994). Cognitive stimulation
cognition-focussed intervention in dementia? A of patients with dementia: Preliminary results. International
recent randomised trial from Italy, used an adapted Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 9, 211–217.
Charlesworth, G. (2001). Reviewing psychosocial interventions
24-hour in-home version of RO, where families were for family carers of people with dementia. Aging & Mental
taught to apply ROT, which essentially stimulated Health, 5, 104–106.
global cognition by using graded guided practice in Clare, L. (1999). Memory rehabilitation in early dementia.
a range of cognitive tasks (Onder et al., 2005). Journal of Dementia Care, 7(6), 33–37.
Whilst this programme enhanced the effects of Clare, L., & Woods, R. T. (Eds.). (2001). Cognitive rehabilitation
in dementia. Hove, UK: Psychology Press.
donezepil on cognitive function, there were few
Clare, L., & Woods, R. T. (2004). Cognitive training and
equivalent outcomes on social functioning. In con- cognitive rehabilitation for people with early-stage Alzheimer’s
trast, home-based cognitive training for everyday disease: A review. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 14,
problems using families to carry out the programmes 385–401.
has shown promise for cognition, spousal Clare, L., Woods, R. T., Moniz-Cook, E. D., Orrell, M., &
Spector, A. (2003). Cognitive rehabilitation and cognitive
communication, and interaction outcomes (see
training interventions targeting memory functioning in early
Quayhagen et al., 1995; 1996). Again, these positive stage Alzheimer’s disease and vascular dementia (Cochrane
outcomes may be strongly influenced by social Review). Cochrane Library, Issue 4. Chichester, UK: John Wiley
engagement in meaningful activity with a caregiver & Sons Ltd.
(Bird, 2000). Department of Health. (2001). National Service Framework for
What type of mental activity is needed to Older People. London: The Stationary Office.
De Vreese, L. P., Neri, M., Fioravanti, M., Belloi, L., &
achieve the outcomes described in the studies
Zanetti, O. (2001). Memory rehabilitation in Alzheimer’s
reported in this issue, or in related systematic reviews disease: A review of progress. International Journal of Geriatric
(see Clare, Woods, Moniz-Cook, Orrell, & Psychiatry, 16, 794–809.
Spector, 2003; Spector et al., 1998)? Clearly Dietch, J. T., Hewett, L. J., & Jones, S. (1989). Adverse effects of
cognitive training in the form of mechanistic reality orientation. Journal of American Geriatric Society, 37,
974–976.
repeated paper and pencil ‘exercise’ continues
Grandmaison, E., & Simard, M. (2003). A critical review of
to lack credibility, whilst a meaningful and pleasur- memory stimulation programs in Alzheimer’s disease. Journal
able programme content remains important. of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 15, 130–144.
The framework provided by Clare and Woods Gubrium, J. F., & Ksander, M. (1975). On multiple realities and
(2004) for the range of cognition-orientated psycho- reality orientation. Gerontologist, 15, 142–145.
social interventions in dementia is a step towards Hofmann, M., Hock, C., Kuhler, A., & Muller-Spahn, F. (1996).
Interactive computer-based cognitive training in patients with
clarification of concepts, content and practice and Alzheimer’s disease. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 30,
has the potential to bridge the gap between 493–501.
research and practice. Cognitive stimulation in the Holden, U. (1987). The value of reality orientation in 1987.
twenty-first century reflects a modernisation of Clinical Rehabilitation, 1, 235–238.
reality orientation, where cognition-focussed Holden, U. P., & Woods, R. T. (1995). Positive approaches to
dementia care (3rd ed.). Edinburgh: Churchill Livingstone.
programmes are set in a social context and aim to
Larson, E. B., Wang, L., Bowen, J. D., McCormick, W. C.,
enhance cognitive and social function in people with Teri, L., Crane, P., et al. (2006). Exercise is associated with
dementia. Even in countries where the traditional reduced risks for incident dementia among persons 65 years of
name has been retained, it is fair to note that the age and older. Annals of Internal Medicine, 144, 73–81.
210 E. Moniz-Cook
Merchant, M., & Saxby, P. (1981). Reality orientation: A way for people with dementia. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 11,
forward. Nursing Times, 77, 1442–1445. 377–397.
Moniz-Cook, E. D., Agar, S., Gibson, G., Win, T., & Wang, M. Spector, A., Thorgrimsen, L., Woods, R. T., Royan, L., Davies,
(1998). A preliminary study of the effects of early intervention S., Butterworth, M., et al. (2003). Efficacy of an evidence-
with people with dementia and their families in a memory based cognitive stimulation therapy programme for people
clinic. Aging & Mental Health, 2, 199–211. with dementia: Randomised controlled trial. British Journal of
Onder, G., Zanetti, O., Giacobini, E., Frisoni, G., Bartorelli, L., Psychiatry, 183, 248–254.
Carbone, G., et al. (2005). Reality orientation therapy Taulbee, L. R., & Folsom, J. C. (1966). Reality orientation
combined with cholinesterase inhibitors in Alzheimer’s disease: for geriatric patients. Hospital and Community Psychiatry, 17,
Randomised controlled trial. British Journal of Psychiatry, 187, 133–135.
450–455. Vernooij-Dassen, M., & Moniz-Cook, E. (2005). Improving the
Powell-Proctor, L., & Miller, E. (1982). Reality orientation: quality of in home interventions in dementia care. Dementia:
A critical appraisal. British Journal of Psychiatry, 140, The International Journal of Social Research & Practice, 4,
457–463. 165–169.
Quayhagen, M. P., Quayhagen, M., Corbeil, R., Roth, P., & Wilson, R. S., Mendes de Leon, C. F., Barnes, L. L., Schneider,
Rodgers, J. A. (1995). A dydatic remediation programme for J. A., Bienias, J. L., Evans, D. A., et al. (2002). Participation
care recipients with dementia. Nursing Research, 44, 153–159. in cognitively stimulating activities and risk of incident
Quayhagen, M. P., & Quayhagen, M. (1996). Discovering life Alzheimer’s disease. The Journal of the American Medical
quality in coping with dementia. Western Journal of Nursing Association, 287, 742–748.
Research, 18, 120–135. Woods, R. T. (2001). Discovering the person with Alzheimer’s
Richards, K., Duggan, P., Carr, I., Wang, M., & disease: Cognitive, emotional and behavioural aspects. Aging &
Moniz-Cook, E. D. (2003). Early intervention for Mental Health, 5, S7–S16.
people with dementia: What are important outcomes Woods, R. T. (2002). Reality orientation: A welcome return?
for families? Aging & Mental Health, 7, 7–14. Age & Ageing, 31, 1–2, Editorial.
Scottish Intercollegiate Guidelines Network (SIGN). (2006). Zanetti, O., Frisoni, G. B., De Leo, D., Buono, M. D.,
Management of patients with dementia: A national clinical Bianchetti, A., & Trabucci, M. (1995). Reality orientation
guideline. Edinburgh: SIGN, 2006 (SIGN publication therapy in Alzheimer’s disease: Useful or not? A controlled
no. 86). study. Alzheimer’s Disease & Associated Disorders, 9, 132–138.
Spector, A., Davies, S., Woods, R. T., & Orrell, M. (2000). Zanetti, O., Oriani, M., Geroldi, C., Binetti, G., Frisoni, G.,
Reality orientation for dementia: A systematic review Giovanni, G., & De Vreese, L. P. (2002). Predictors of
of the evidence for its effectiveness. Gerontologist, 40, cognitive improvement after reality orientation in Alzheimer’s
206–212. disease. Age & Ageing, 31, 193–196.
Spector, A., Orrell, M., Davies, S., & Woods, R. T. (2001). Can Zarit, S. H., Zarit, J. M., & Reever, K. E. (1982). Memory training
reality orientation be rehabilitated? Development and piloting for severe memory loss: Effects on senile dementia patients
of an evidence-based programme of cognition-based therapies and their families. Gerontologist, 22, 373–377.

You might also like