You are on page 1of 12

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/227178243

Two Steps Forward, One Step Back: The Presence of Female Characters and
Gender Stereotyping in Award-Winning Picture Books Between the 1930s and
the 1960s

Article  in  Sex Roles · November 2003


DOI: 10.1023/A:1025820404277

CITATIONS READS
27 735

4 authors, including:

Roger Clark
Rhode Island College
34 PUBLICATIONS   400 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE

All content following this page was uploaded by Roger Clark on 17 January 2017.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


P1: JLS
Sex Roles [sers] pp978-sers-472553 September 12, 2003 15:9 Style file version June 3rd, 2002

Sex Roles, Vol. 49, Nos. 9/10, November 2003 (°


C 2003)

Two Steps Forward, One Step Back: The Presence


of Female Characters and Gender Stereotyping
in Award-Winning Picture Books Between
the 1930s and the 1960s

Roger Clark,1,2 Jessica Guilmain,1 Paul Khalil Saucier,1 and Jocelyn Tavarez1

Since the late 1960s, there has been steady, progressive change in the depiction of
gender in award-winning picture books for children (e.g., Clark, Almeida, Gurka, &
Middleton, 2003). Female characters in Caldecott winners and runners-up have become in-
creasingly visible and gender stereotyping has become decreasingly evident. In this article
we consider whether this steady change can be projected back into the decades before the
1960s, or whether local, temporal variation in gender norms affected less monotonic change.
We found that Caldecotts of the late 1940s and the late 1960s had fewer visible female charac-
ters than Caldecotts of the late 1930s and the late 1950s, but that characters in the 1940s and
1960s were less gender stereotyped than the characters of the 1930s and 1950s. We interpret
these findings in terms of the greater level of conflict over gender roles that existed in the
1940s and 1960s, as well as the relatively greater status enjoyed by American women in those
decades.

KEY WORDS: children’s books; visibility of female characters; gender stereotyping.

It has been just over three decades since worth of boys and girls in American society: that “boys
Weitzman, Eifler, Hokada, and Ross (1972) produced are more highly valued than girls” (p. 1125).
their classic study on gender role socialization in chil- Weitzman et al. sparked a series of follow-up
dren’s books. Weitzman et al.’s primary focus was on studies of feminist research into the gender con-
winners and runners-up of the Caldecott Medal (the tent and effects of children’s books. By 2003, no
most prestigious award for children’s picture books), fewer that 22 research teams (Ashton, 1983; Clark,
especially those selected during the 5 years from 1967 Almeida, Gurka, & Middleton, 2003; Clark, Lennon,
to 1971. Their major finding was that American pic- & Morris, 1993; Davis, 1984; Gooden & Gooden, 2001;
ture books for preschoolers depicted male and female Grauerholz & Pescosolido, 1989; Jennings, 1975;
characters in stereotyped ways and that they hardly Knopp, 1980; Koblinsky, Cruse, & Sugawar, 1978;
showed female characters at all. Weitzman et al. sug- Kolke & LaVoie, 1981; Kropp & Halverston, 1983;
gested that both the stereotyping of all characters and Lutes-Dunckley, 1978; Ochman, 1996; Peirce &
the relative invisibility of female characters taught Edwards, 1988; Peterson & Navy, 1990; Purcell &
young readers an important lesson about the relative Stewart, 1990; St. Peter, 1979; Sugino, 2000; Tepper
& Cassidy, 1999; Turner-Bowker, 1996; White, 1985;
1 Department
Williams, Vernon, Williams, & Malecha, 1987) had di-
of Sociology, Rhode Island College, Providence,
rectly followed in their footsteps in one way or an-
Rhode Island.
2 To whom correspondence should be addressed at Department of other. It has even been plausibly argued that these
Sociology, Rhode Island College, Providence, Rhode Island 02908; studies have had some effect on the kinds of books
e-mail: rclark@ric.edu. that have been chosen to be award winners (see

439 0360-0025/03/1100-0439/0 °
C 2003 Plenum Publishing Corporation
P1: JLS
Sex Roles [sers] pp978-sers-472553 September 12, 2003 15:9 Style file version June 3rd, 2002

440 Clark, Guilmain, Saucier, and Tavarez

Clark, Kulkin, & Clancy, 1999). Clark, Lennon, and past, there are some interpretations of that trend that
Morris (1993) reported that Caldecott winners and also accord with such an extrapolation. One such in-
runners-up between 1987 and 1991 made female char- terpretation, based upon the reasoning of Weitzman
acters more visible and less stereotyped than was et al. (1972), is that the portrayal of gender in chil-
the case in their 1967–1971 counterparts. Clark et al. dren’s picture books reflects other long-term socioe-
(2003) reported that winners and runners-up be- conomic and cultural trends in the relative value
tween 1997 and 2001 had even more visible and less- placed upon men and women in American society.
stereotyped female characters than the 1987–1991 One need only add the observation that women’s ac-
group did. cess to education, the polity, and the economy has gen-
The Caldecott Medal was first awarded in 1938, erally proceeded in a more-or-less monotonic fashion
and one may well wonder whether the trend of in- in the last century, then, to justify a belief that the
creasing visibility of female characters and decreas- depiction of gender in children’s picture books will
ing stereotyping in award-winning books is safely ex- also have been subject to a more-or-less monotonic
trapolated backward in time. Our goal in this study progression.
has been to examine this possibility by looking again Almost as soon as we had laid out this expec-
at award-winners and runners-up from the late 1960s tation, however, we found ourselves drawn more and
(1967–71) and at their counterparts in the late 1950s more to the adverb “more-or-less” and less and less to
(1957–61), the late 1940s (1947–51), and the late 1930s the adjective “monotonic.” Pescosolido, Grauerholz,
(1938–42).3 and Milkie (1997), after all, had found that the ap-
pearance and portrayal of Black characters in chil-
dren’s picture books varied with social and politi-
Competing Perspectives cal forces in the twentieth century. During periods
of racial conflict (notably during the 1950s and early
We came to our study with two competing sets of 1960s), Black characters virtually disappeared from
expectations. On the one hand, we thought it possible, children’s books. Could conflict over gender roles
perhaps likely, that we would find decreasing visibil- have had similar effects on the appearance of female
ity of female characters and ever more gender stereo- characters in children’s books? Could conflict over
typing as we examined older and older Caldecotts. gender roles and the relative value placed on men and
First, there were the trends of the recent decades. women in society (clearly distinguishable concepts)
Clark et al. (2003) had found, for instance, that the affect different aspects of the portrayal of gender in
percentage of Caldecotts without female characters children’s picture books? Could it be, for instance,
had decreased from 33% in the late 1960s to 25% in that conflict in society over gender roles decreases
the late 1980s to 15% in the late 1990s. Moreover, the visibility of female characters, whereas increases
using standards that we will describe in our method in the relative value of women decrease the amount of
section below, they found that whereas female and gender stereotyping? And has there been enough lo-
male characters tended to be portrayed in stereotyp- cal variation in prevailing gender norms and conflicts
ical fashion in the 1960s, they were much less stereo- by decade in the 1930s, 1940s, 1950s, and 1960s to have
typically portrayed by the late 1980s, and tended to generated significant variation from the onward and
be portrayed in reverse stereotypical fashion by the upward trend toward the depiction of gender equality
late 1990s. For example, Clark et al. (2003) found that in children’s books of the final three decades of the
late-1960s female characters were much more likely century? We thought, perhaps, that the answer to all
to be depicted as nurturant than were their male coun- of these questions might be affirmative.
terparts, whereas male characters were slightly more In particular, we wondered whether, because the
likely than female characters to be depicted as nurtu- Great Depression is associated with a degradation in
rant in the late 1990s. women’s relative status in the public sphere,4 late-
In addition to the empirical evidence from which 1930s Caldecotts might have tended to depict men
one might have extrapolated a straight-line trend be- and women in traditional roles. We also speculated
tween the late 1960s and the late 1990s back to the
4 Rowbotham (1997), for instance, observed that economic hard-
3 Because the Caldecott was first awarded in 1938, we were unable ship “reawakened antagonism to married women working”
to investigate winners from 1937 to 1941 and so chose the winners (p. 203) and led the New Deal administration to focus on men’s
from the period 1938 to 1942 instead. employment.
P1: JLS
Sex Roles [sers] pp978-sers-472553 September 12, 2003 15:9 Style file version June 3rd, 2002

Gender Stereotyping in Award-Winning Picture Books 441

about the possibility that, because the Depression ac- ally by the Caldecott Committee of the Association
tually diminished conflict over women’s roles, it may for Library Services to Children (ALSC), a division
have led to relatively high levels of female-character of the American Library Association. The 15 mem-
visibility. We wondered whether women’s greater ac- bers of this Committee each year must be members of
cess to the public sphere in the 1940s, particularly dur- the ALSC, and all will have distinguished themselves
ing World War II, might have led to a more egalitar- within this Association for their skills at evaluating
ian portrayal of gender in late-1940s children’s books. children’s books (e.g., Fader, 1999). It is therefore
But we thought it possible that the implicit conflict possible that members of the Caldecott committee
over women’s roles, especially during the late 1940s, are more responsive to social change than are other
may have led to relatively few female characters in the arbiters of popular books for children, such as the
books. Because the 1950s were a time when traditional publishers of Golden Books.
gender roles prevailed in the larger society, and when Here we briefly outline how we measured both
there was relatively little conflict over gender roles, the visibility of female characters and the degree of
we hypothesized that Caldecotts may again have de- gender stereotyping in the books we have examined.
picted men and women in conventional stereotyped Following Weitzman et al. (1972) and Clark et al.
ways and contained more female characters. Finally, (1993), we measured visibility of female characters
we wondered whether incipient Second Wave femi- in terms of four indicators: the percentage of books in
nist struggles during the 1960s might not have con- each period that have no female characters, the per-
tributed to at least marginally less stereotyping in pic- centage with a central female character, the percent-
ture books for this decade, at least in comparison with age of human single-gender illustrations that depict
earlier decades, as well as fewer female characters. girls or women, and the percentage of single-sex ani-
Consequently, we approached our data with two mal illustrations that depict females.
competing hypotheses, one of which we dub the Weitzman et al. (1972) failed to specify how they
“monotonic change” (or MC) hypothesis and the measured the degree of gender-stereotyping in the
other which we call the “local variation” (LV) hy- Caldecotts they studied. As Clark et al. (1993) did
pothesis. The MC hypothesis holds that books in later before, we relied on Davis’ refined set of variables
decades will generally have more female characters for dealing with 15 kinds of gender-related behavior
and less gender stereotyping of their characters than (Davis, 1994; see the Appendix B for variable def-
book in earlier decades. The LV hypothesis holds that initions). Given these variable traits, we posit that
books from the late 1930s and the late 1950s will have mainstream gender stereotyping entails the expecta-
more female characters but more gender stereotyping tion that female characters will be more dependent,
than books from the late 1940s and the late 1960s. cooperative, submissive, imitative, nurturant, emo-
tional, and passive and that they will be less indepen-
dent, competitive, directive, persistent, explorative,
METHOD creative, aggressive, and active than male characters.
In general, we coded whether each of four pos-
For the current project, we closely examined the sible characters in each book possessed a given trait
20 Caldecott winners and runners-up from 1938 to (e.g., dependence). We analyzed the book’s central
1942, the 28 from 1947 to 1951, the 18 from 1957 to character, the most important character of the same
1961, and the 18 from 1967 to 1971. (See Appendix sex, and the two most important characters of the
A for the books analyzed.) We reexamined the latter other sex, where supporting characters were deemed
group, the 18 from 1967 to 1971, even though it had sufficiently visible for analysis. In many books, we ex-
previously been studied by Weitzman et al. (1972) and amined fewer than four characters (sometimes as few
Clark et al. (1993), to make sure that the same readers as zero) because there simply weren’t four charac-
(the four coauthors) applied the same standards to the ters about which we were given adequate informa-
books of all four time periods. tion to do our analysis. In rare cases, such as Lawson’s
One reason for focusing on Caldecott winners is They Were Strong and Good, where there were obvi-
the unusual influence these books have had on tastes ously five equally central characters (Lawson, 1940),
for children’s literature. The Caldecott is not only we dealt with more than four characters (in this case,
the most prestigious award for preschool literature, five). If three or four of us independently decided that
but it also guarantees its winners phenomenal sales a character possessed a trait, we accorded this trait to
(Clark, 1992). The Caldecott Medal is awarded annu- the character. If three or four failed to see a trait,
P1: JLS
Sex Roles [sers] pp978-sers-472553 September 12, 2003 15:9 Style file version June 3rd, 2002

442 Clark, Guilmain, Saucier, and Tavarez

Table I. Visibility of Female Characters in Caldecott Winners and before the 1960s. It provided much more support for
Runners-Up the local variation (LV) hypothesis. Our results follow,
1930s 1940s 1950s 1960s decade by decade.
Total number 20 28 18 18
of books
Percent without 0% 22% 11% 33% The 1930s
female charactersa
Percent with 45% 14% 39% 17%
As shown in Table I, female characters of the
central female
charactersb late 1930s were more visible than those in any other
Human single-gender illustrations decade. All of the 1930s books included a female char-
Total number 335 508 163 196 acter, and 45% included a central female character.
Percent with 24% 16% 40% 10% Girls or women were shown in 24% of human single-
female charactersc
gender illustrations, and female animals were shown
Nonhuman single-sex illustrations
Total number 190 130 101 119 in 19% of the animal single-sex illustrations.
Percent femaled 19% 20% 10% 1% Weitzman et al. (1972) and Clark et al. (1993,
a Differences between 1930s and 1940s books, significant at .03
2003) have traced a pattern in which the presence
level; between 1930s and 1960s books, at .01 level; between 1940s of female characters is associated with low gender-
and 1950s books, at .11 level. No other differences statistically stereotyping in Caldecotts between the 1960s and the
significant. 1990s. But despite the high visibility of female char-
b Differences between 1930s and 1940s books, significant at .02 level;
acters in 1930s Caldecotts, we found (see Table II)
between 1930 and 1960s books, at .06 level, between 1940s and
1950s books, at .06 level; between 1950s and 1960s books, at .14
that there is a good deal of gender stereotyping in
level. No other differences close to being significant. them. Female characters were more dependent, sub-
c All differences significant at .05 level. missive, imitative, nurturant, emotional, and passively
d Differences between 1940s, 1950s, and 1960s books, significant at
active than male characters. Male characters, on the
.05 level. Differences between 1930s and 1940s books, not statis- other hand, were more independent, competitive, di-
tically significant.
rective, persistent, explorative, aggressive, and active
than female characters. Male and female characters
we decided that the trait did not exist in the charac- evinced roughly the same levels of creativity and co-
ter. If there was a tie (with two of us seeing the trait operativeness. Of the 15 traits we examined, 13 were
and two not seeing it), we assigned a missing value more likely to be found in characters of the stereo-
to the character for that trait, and the character was typically appropriate gender in the 1930s, and 2 were
deleted in analysis of that trait. Overall, the four of us about equally likely to be found in male and female
examined 133 characters and obtained an overall in- characters, which makes the 1930s the decade in which
tercoder or interrater reliability of 88%; agreements we found the greatest amount of gender stereotyping
varied between 80% (for emotionality) and 99% (for in Caldecott winners and runners-up. Although the
activeness). stereotyping is consistent with our MC hypothesis, the
We determined the statistical significance of per- degree of visibility of female characters is not.
centage differences by calculating chi-square statis- As might be expected during the tumultuous
tics. In the case of all four visibility indicators (e.g., per- 1930s, there was a considerable amount of reach-
centage of books with female characters), we looked ing back to traditional American and Western sto-
at six differences: between the 1930s and the 1940s, ries and myths in the list of 1930s Caldecotts. Every
1950s and 1960s, first; between the 1940s, 1950s, and year brought its very large (by more recent picture
1960s, next; and between the 1950s and 1960s, last. book standards) volumes, with traditional themes:
Summaries of these analyses are presented in notes Fish and Lawson’s Four and Twenty Blackbirds (Fish
at the bottom of Table I. In the case of all 15 behav- & Lawson, 1937), Fish and Lathrop’s Animals of
ioral traits, we examined differences between the per- the Bible: A Picture Book (Fish & Lathrop, 1937),
centage of male and female characters who possessed Gag’s Snow White and the Seven Dwarfs (Gag, 1938),
each trait (e.g., dependence) for each decade. Daugherty’s Andy and the Lion (Daugherty, 1938),
the D’Aulaires’ Abraham Lincoln (D’Aulaires &
RESULTS D’Aulaires, 1939), Lawson’s They Were Strong and
Good (Lawson, 1940), and the Petershams’ The
Our analysis showed almost no support for the American ABC (Petersham & Petersham, 1941). The
monotonic change (MC) hypothesis in the decades characters in these books were likely to be gender
P1: JLS
Sex Roles [sers] pp978-sers-472553 September 12, 2003 15:9 Style file version June 3rd, 2002

Gender Stereotyping in Award-Winning Picture Books 443

Table II. Percentage of Characters Exhibiting Behavioral Traits by Gender or Sex


Late-1930s Late-1940s Late-1950s Late-1960s
Male Female Male Female Male Female Male Female
Traits characters characters characters characters characters characters characters characters
Dependent 24 62 S∗ 26 54 S 11 50 S 37 29 R
Independent 90 70 S 93 90 T 100 80 S 88 83 T
Cooperative 81 85 T 83 69 R 87 92 T 79 100 S
Competitive 36 27 S 30 30 T 42 8S 18 17 T
Directive 48 27 S 36 44 R 46 27 S 59 57 T
Submissive 25 50 S 15 0R 8 27 S 0 0T
Persistent 90 50 S∗ 78 85 R 85 67 S 73 60 S
Explorative 85 46 S∗ 83 43 S∗ 36 50 R 66 33 S
Creative 58 57 T 52 33 S 64 36 S 40 80 R
Imitative 8 15 S 18 8R 0 0T 0 0T
Nurturant 43 75 S 44 75 S 33 73 S∗ 56 71 S
Aggressive 35 23 S 17 21 T 13 0S 27 14 S
Emotional 63 92 S 54 67 S 60 57 T 79 50 R
Active 100 92 S 100 85 S 100 100 T 100 100 T
Passively active 90 100 S 100 85 R 100 100 T 100 100 T
Note. ∗ indicates the difference between male and female characters is significant at the .05 level; S indicates that the difference is in
the stereotypical direction; R indicates that the difference is in the reverse stereotypical direction; T indicates that there’s essentially
no difference (less than a 5% difference) between males and females (i.e., that there is a tie).

stereotyped. Lawson’s family history, They Were visible than they were in either the late 1930s or the
Strong and Good, for instance, features three extraor- late 1950s. Fully 22% of 1940s books had no female
dinarily active men and three extraordinarily passive characters, and only 15% had a central female char-
women. (One of these women is so nearly invisible acter. Only 16% of the human single-gender illustra-
that we couldn’t code her in our analysis of gender tions on late-1940s Caldecotts were of girls or women,
stereotyping. There just wasn’t enough information and only 20% of animal single-sex illustrations were
about her.) By virtue of their length, if nothing else, of females.
however, such books could hardly have excluded fe- If the visibility of female characters were indi-
male characters altogether. There are some fascinat- rectly associated with gender stereotyping, as it is for
ing exceptions to the “traditional Western story” rule the Caldecotts from late 1960s to the late 1990s, one
among the late-1930s Caldecotts. Modern classics, would expect considerable gender stereotyping in the
which “happened” to focus on female characters, such late 1940s books, but this is not the case. In fact, con-
as McCluskey’s Make Way for Ducklings (McCluskey, sistent with the LV hypothesis, the late-1940s Calde-
1941) and Bemelman’s Madeline (Bemelman, 1939), cotts exhibited less gender stereotyping than those
were honored, and they introduced independent and of any other decade we examined. For, although fe-
nonsubmissive female characters. Similarly, interest male characters in these books remained (as they were
is shown in Native Americans (through the acknowl- in the 1930s) substantially more dependent, less ex-
edgement of books such as Holling’s, Paddle-to-the- plorative, more nurturant, and more emotional than
Sea; Holling, 1941, and Clark & Herrera’s, In My male characters, they were just as independent, com-
Mother’s House; Clark & Herrera, 1941) and Chinese petitive, and aggressive as male characters, and less
(Handforth’s, Mei Li; Handforth, 1938). Some of cooperative, more directive, less submissive, less im-
these books, such as Mei Li, featured surprisingly ac- itative, and more persistent than their male counter-
tive female characters. On the whole, though, late- parts. Are we seeing the remnants here of the egali-
1930s Caldecotts featured a good deal of gender tarian spirit that Rosie the Riveter embodied during
stereotyping. World War II?
Probably not. But we probably are seeing signs
The 1940s of ambivalence on the part of authors, publishers, and
award-givers, about gender roles. One does, of course,
As Table I indicates, we found that female char- find some very traditional, martial male characters
acters in Caldecotts of the late 1940s were more visible in Malcolmson and Burton’s Song of Robin Hood
than they were in the late-1960s Caldecotts, but less (Malcolmson & Burton, 1947) and Holbrook and
P1: JLS
Sex Roles [sers] pp978-sers-472553 September 12, 2003 15:9 Style file version June 3rd, 2002

444 Clark, Guilmain, Saucier, and Tavarez

Ward’s America’s Ethan Allen (Holbrook & Ward, character. Forty percent of the human single-gender
1949). But there is a notable turning away from adult illustrations in the late-1950s Caldecotts were of girls
human society and its troubles in these books. Read- or women. The only indicator on which we found rela-
ers were exposed to the zany, masculine world of tively low female-character visibility among the 1950s
Dr Seuss in McElligot’s Pool, If I Ran the Zoo, and Caldecotts was for animal single-sex illustrations: only
Bartholomew and the Oobleck (Seuss, 1947, 1949, 10% of these were of females.
1950). Mainly, however, children got books about Female visibility in 1950s Caldecotts was second
nature, animals, and children: 20 of the 27 award- only to its counterpart in 1930s Caldecotts, and the
winning books focused on either nature, animals, or amount of gender-stereotyping was also second only
children. Not infrequently the books were simultane- to that found in the 1930s books (see Table II). Fe-
ously about nature, animals, and children, as they are male characters were more dependent, submissive,
in McCluskey’s Blueberries for Sal (McCluskey, 1948) and nurturant than male characters, and were less in-
and Davis and Woodward’s The Wild Birthday Cake dependent, competitive, persistent, explorative, cre-
(Davis & Woodward, 1949). And children and animals ative, and aggressive. In a minor reversal of conven-
are relatively easily forgiven minor transgressions of tional stereotypes, they were slightly more likely to
gender boundaries. Thus, Sal, out picking blueberries be explorative than their male counterparts. Typical
with Mom, can be explorative and persistent, and she of the 1950s books are two in the series about Anatole
does not need to be at all nurturant, and Johnny (in the mouse, Anatole and Anatole and the Cat, by Titus
The Wild Birthday Cake) can develop a cooperative and Galdone (1956, 1957). In these books, the en-
relationship with his 75-year-old mentor, the Profes- terprising and creative Anatole makes his fortune in
sor, be submissive to him, and be nurturant (of ani- France’s cheese industry, whereas his wife, Duchette,
mals). But there are no Rosies here. In fact, if one finds keeps a fine mouse-house and minds the children, i.e.,
a working and assertive adult female, as one does in the little mice. The amount of gender stereotyping we
Brown’s Dick Whittington and His Cat (Brown, 1950), found in late-1950s books is very consistent with a
she’s likely to be an abusive servant (who is most un- view that these books reflected the “separate spheres”
appealingly directive, aggressive, and nonnurturant) ethos of the decade.
or a mouse-killing cat.
Thus, although one can argue that the late-1940s The 1960s
Caldecotts do provide support for the LV hypothesis,
we found little evidence that they might have reflected The results of our examination of the 1960s
an egalitarian Zeitgeist borne of war. On the contrary, Caldecotts are consistent with those of Weitzman
it seems more likely that these books reflected some et al.’s study, at least insofar as we also find consider-
combination of a confusion about gender norms, an able evidence of the invisibility of female characters
exhaustion with wartime preoccupations of all sorts, (see Table I). We found that fully 33% of the books
and perhaps a fear of being labeled too liberal or even had no female characters, and only 17% had a central
“communist.” (Joseph McCarthy was elected to the female character. Moreover, only 10% of all single-
Senate in 1946 and began his anticommunist hear- gender human illustrations in these books were, by
ings the next year.) There were, to be sure, some safe, our count, of girls or women, and only a miniscule
“manly” men, but there were even more nearly-as- 0.8% of single-sex animal illustrations were of fe-
safe children and animals. If there had to be an as- males. On all visibility dimensions, then, late-1960s
sertive woman, her assertiveness was made to look Caldecotts were outstanding for their capacity to keep
unappealing. female characters invisible.
On the other hand, though, we did not find as
much gender stereotyping in the 1960s Caldecotts as
The 1950s Weitzman et al. apparently did. In fact, we found these
books to be the second-least (to the 1940s books)
We found that female characters were less visi- stereotyped of the ones we examined in this study.
ble in the Caldecotts of the late 1950s than they were Female characters were more cooperative and nurtu-
in the 1930s, but that they were more visible in the rant and less persistent, explorative, and aggressive
1950s than they were in Caldecotts of the 1940s or than their male counterparts, as one would expect by
the 1960s. Only 11% of the 1950s books had no fe- stereotype, but they were also less dependent, more
male character and fully 39% had a central female creative, and less emotional. On 7 of the 15 dimen-
P1: JLS
Sex Roles [sers] pp978-sers-472553 September 12, 2003 15:9 Style file version June 3rd, 2002

Gender Stereotyping in Award-Winning Picture Books 445

sions (e.g., independence), male and female charac- invisibility of female characters and more gender-
ters had the focal trait in approximately equal mea- stereotyping farther back in time. We called this our
sure. Preston and Parker’s eponymous characters in monotonic change (MC) hypothesis. On the other
Pop Corn and Ma Goodness worked together at build- hand, we thought it possible that the depiction of
ing a house and did essentially the same kinds of work gender in such books might vary with the more gen-
(Preston & Parker, 1969). eral attitudes about gender in a particular decade.
The invisibility of female characters during the This was our local variation (LV) hypothesis. Over-
1960s does not seem to be as clearly a reflection of all, we found more support for the LV than for the
authors’ and award-givers’ avoidance of adult hu- MC hypothesis.
man society (through a focus on children and ani- The MC hypothesis was undermined, in part, by
mals) as it did in the 1940s. Late-1960s books, in fact, the complete absence of a negative correlation be-
seem to reflect the kind of social consciousness one tween the presence of female characters and gen-
might have expected from a decade of liberationist der stereotyping between the late 1930s and the late
social movements; however, they do not focus on the 1960s. Unlike what seems to occur (see, for instance,
women’s movement directly. Retellings of folktales Clark et al., 1993, 2003) in award-winners between
from “other” societies abound and constitute the basis the late 1960s and the late 1990s, when the presence
for Yashimo’s Seashore Story (Japan; Yashimo, 1967), of female characters and gender stereotyping were
Dayrell and Lent’s Why the Sun and the Moon Live in negatively correlated, we found that they were posi-
the Sky (Africa; Dayrell & Lent, 1968), Ransome and tively correlated between the late 1930s and the late
Shulevitz’s The Fool of the World and the Flying Ship 1960s. In the 1930s and 1950s, when female characters
(Russia; Ransome & Shulevitz, 1968), Sleator and were present in relatively great numbers in Calde-
Lent’s The Angry Moon (Native America; Sleator & cott winners and runners-up, gender stereotyping was
Lent, 1970), Haley’s A Story, A Story (Africa; Haley, most prevalent. In the 1940s and 1960s, when female
1970), and Yolen and Young’s The Emperor and the characters were present in relatively small numbers,
Kite (China; Yolen & Young, 1967). We also found gender stereotyping, at least as we measured it, was
among honored books Keats’ Goggles! (Keats, 1969), least obvious. The considerable presence of female
the story of two African American boys pursued by characters in 1930s and 1950s Caldecotts may have
an urban gang. None of these books, except for The had slightly different origins in the two decades. The
Emperor and the Kite, has a central female charac- presence of female characters in the books of the
ter, but none is reminiscent of the nature books of 1930s results, in part, from the fact that the Calde-
the late 1940s for their avoidance of adult human cott committee honored much longer books then than
society either. Some of the absence of female char- it has in later decades. Longer books, particularly
acters, however, seems to reflect the Caldecott com- those with multiple stories (such as books of Bible
mittee’s interest in male-centered animal stories, as stories), are simply more likely to include female
in Lionni’s Frederick (about a mouse; Lionni, 1967), characters, even central female characters, some-
Lionni’s Alexander and the Wind-up Mouse (about a where. The presence of female characters in 1950s
mouse; Lionni, 1969), Steig’s Sylvester and the Magic books, on the other hand, seems to reflect at least
Pebble (about a donkey; Steig, 1969), and Lobel’s Frog a partial focus on domestic life, the realm in which
and Toad were Friends (about a frog and a toad; Lobel, “women” were expected to be present. The Ana-
1970), many of which are Aesop-like fables designed tole books (about a mouse and his supportive wife),
to convey moral lessons to young readers. Yashimo’s Umbrella (Yashimo, 1958), Freeman’s Fly
High, Fly Low (Freeman, 1957), LaBastida and
DISCUSSION Ets’ Nine Days to Christmas (LaBastida & Ets,
1959), and Frasconi’s The House that Jack Built
We began our investigation of Caldecott award- (Frasconi, 1958), are all books that give at least di-
winners and runners-up from the late-1930s, late- vided attention to domestic matters. In any case, the
1940s, late-1950s, and late-1960s with competing authors of 1930s and 1950s award winners seem to
expectations. On the one hand, on the basis of a lit- have had no problem writing about female characters.
erature that suggests that female characters have be- The MC hypothesis was also challenged by the
come more visible and that gender-stereotyping has nonlinear pattern in gender stereotyping that oc-
become less apparent in such books since the 1960s, curred by decade between the late 1930s and the late
we thought it possible that we would find greater 1960s. The most “deviant” decade is the 1940s, when
P1: JLS
Sex Roles [sers] pp978-sers-472553 September 12, 2003 15:9 Style file version June 3rd, 2002

446 Clark, Guilmain, Saucier, and Tavarez

stereotyping reached its lowest point over the period, Another implication of our work is that the
a decade in which, given the 15 traits we examined, “trend” toward such visibility and decreased stereo-
male and female characters evinced stereotyped be- typing cannot be taken for granted and probably re-
havior on 6 traits, reversed stereotyped behavior on flects real gains in public consciousness, as well as the
6 traits, and similar behavior on 3 traits. We thought it consciousness of the award committee, about the im-
possible, as expressed in our LV hypothesis, that the portance of gender-egalitarian displays in children’s
late-1940s Caldecotts might evince less stereotyping media. Antifeminist backlash in other media (see,
than those of the late 1930s and the late 1950s, but our for instance, hooks, 1997, on film) suggests that such
reasoning behind this expectation was not borne out. a trend is reversible. Our data from the first four
Much of the nonstereotyped behavior in these books decades of Caldecott awards suggest that, under the
came from children, such as Sal in Blueberries for Sal wrong circumstances, the current 30-year trend to-
and Johnny in The Wild Birthday Cake, who were ward less stereotyped displays and more visibility of
put into settings where minor transgressions of gen- female characters in children’s award-winning picture
der boundaries are easily tolerated. We were so struck books is also potentially reversible.
by the socially-acceptable transgressions of these chil-
dren, in fact, that we encourage future researchers to APPENDIX A: BOOKS ANALYZED
do what we failed to do: analyze the differences in the
ways adults and children are stereotyped by gender Armer, L. A. (1938). The forest pool. Logmans.
in children’s books more generally. In fact, though, Artzybasheff, B. (1937). Seven Simeons: A Russian
there seems to be a turning away from the concerns tale. Viking.
of adult society in the books selected by the Caldecott Bemelmans, L. (1939). Madeline. Viking.
committee of the late 1940s, which led us to wonder Brown, M. (1949). Henry Fisherman. Scribner
whether the committee (and perhaps authors) had be- Brown, M. (1950). Dick Whittington and his cat.
come anxious in light of the political and social con- Scribner.
servatism of that particular part of the decade. Brown, M. (1947). Stone soup. Scribner
Our examination of Caldecott winners and Brown, M. W., & Weisgard, L. (1946). The little island.
runners-up from the late 1930s, 1940s, 1950s, and 1960s Doubleday.
provided support for the view that the gender rela- Clark, A. N., & Herrera, V. (1941). In my mother’s
tions depicted in award-winning picture books are house. Viking.
more likely to reflect local, temporal variations in gen- Cooney, B. (1958). Chanticleer and the fox. Crowell.
der norms than to express a long-term trend toward Daugherty, J. (1938). Andy and the lion. Viking.
increasing the increasing visibility of female charac- Davis, L. R., & Woodward, H. (1947). Roger and the
ters and decreasing gender stereotyping. One implica- fox. Doubleday.
tion of our findings is that gender stereotyping and the Davis, L. R., & Woodward, H. (1949). The wild birth-
presence of female characters in award-winning pic- day cake. Doubleday.
ture books for children may reflect different aspects of Dayrell, E., & Lent, B. (1968). Why the sun and the
gender relations in society: that they may not both, as moon live in the sky. Houghton.
Weitzman et al.’s classic study (Weitzman et al., 1972) Elkin, B., & Daugherty, J. (1956). Gillespie and the
suggested, be functions of the relative status of men guards. Viking.
and women, and of boys and girls, in society. Gen- Emberley, B., & Emberley, E. (1966). One wide river
der stereotyping may well reflect the relative status of to cross. Prentice-Hall.
women between the late 1930s and the late 1960s. The Emberley, B., & Emberley, E. (1967). Drummer Hoff.
steady decrease in the amount of gender stereotyping Prentice-Hall.
found in Caldecott winners and runners-up since the Ets, M., H., & Labastida, A. (1959). Nine days to
1960s may, then, reflect increases in women’s status Christmas. Viking.
during the period. The visibility (or invisibility) of fe- Ets, M., H. (1956). Mr. Penny’s race horse. Viking.
male characters, on the other hand, may well reflect Fish, H. D., & Lathrop, D. P. (1937). Animals of the
the degree of conflict over women’s roles in society in Bible, A picture book. Lippincott.
the decades we examined. Consequently, we specu- Fish, H. D., & Lawson, R. (1937). Four and twenty
late that the increasing visibility of female characters blackbirds. Stokes.
in books since the 1960s may reflect decreases in the Flack, M., Banum, J. H. (1946). Boats on the river.
degree of conflict over such roles since then. Viking.
P1: JLS
Sex Roles [sers] pp978-sers-472553 September 12, 2003 15:9 Style file version June 3rd, 2002

Gender Stereotyping in Award-Winning Picture Books 447

Ford, L. (1939). The ageless story. Dodd. Petersham, M., & Petersham, M. (1941). An American
Frasconi, A. (1958). The house that Jack built. ABC. MacMillan.
Harcourt. Politi, L. (1946). Pedro, the angel of Olvera street.
Freeman, D. (1957). Fly high, fly low. Viking. Scribner.
Gag, W. (1938). Snow White and the seven dwarfs. Politi, L. (1948). Juanita. Scribner.
Coward Politi, L. (1949). Song of the swallows. Scribner.
Gag, W. (1941). Nothing at all. Coward. Preston, E. M., & Parker, R. A. (1969). Pop Corn &
Geisel, T. S. (1950). If I ran the zoo. Random House. Ma Goodness. Viking.
Goudey, A., & Adams, A. (1959). Houses from the Ransome, A., & Shulevitz, U. (1968). The fool of the
sea. Scribner. world and the flying ship. Farrar.
Graham, A., & Palazzo, T. (1946). Timothy turtle. Robbins, R., & Sidjakov, N. (1960). Babouska and the
Welch. three kings. Parnassus.
Hader, B., & Hader, E. (1939). Cock-a-doodle doo. Schrieber, G. (1947). Bambino the clown. Viking.
Macmillan. Sendak, M. (1970). In the night kitchen. Harper.
Hadforth, T. (1938). Mei Li. Doubleday. Seuss, D. (1947). McElligot’s Pool. Random House.
Haley, G. E. (1970). A story, a story. Doubleday. Seuss, D. (1949). Bartholomew and the oobleck.
Holbrook, S., & Ward, L. (1949). America’s Ethan Random House.
Allen. Houghton. Sleater, W., & Lent. B. (1970). The angry moon.
Holling, H. C. (1941). Paddle-to-the-sea. Houghton. Atlantic.
Joslin, S., & Sendak, M. (1958). What do you say dear? Steig, W. (1969). Sylvester and the magic pebble.
W.R. Scott. Windmill Books.
Keats, E. J. (1969). Goggles. Macmillan. Titus, E., & Galdone, P. (1956). Anatole. McGraw-
Krauss, R., & Simont, M. (1949). The happy day. Hill.
Harper. Titus, E., & Galdone, P. (1957). Anatole and the cat.
Lawson, R. (1940). They were strong and good. McGraw-Hill.
Viking. Tresselt, A., & Weisgard, L. (1946). Rain drop splash.
Leaf, M., & Lawson, R. (1938). Wee Gillis. Viking. Lothrop.
Lionni, L. (1960). Inch by inch. Obolensky. Tresselt, A., & Duvoisin, R. (1947). White snow, bright
Lionni, L. (1967). Frederick. Pantheon. snow. Lothrop.
Lionni, L. (1969). Alexander and the wind-up mouse. Tudor, T., (1956). 1 is one. Walck.
Pantheon. Turkle, B. (1969). Thy friend, Obadiah. Viking.
Lipkind, W., & Mordvinoff, N. (1950). The two reds. Udry, J., & Simont., M. (1956). A tree is nice.
Harcourt. Harper.
Lobel, A. (1970). Frog and toad are friends. Udry, J. M., & Sendak, M. (1959). The moon jumpers.
Harper. Harper.
Malcomson, A., & Burton, V. L. (1947). Song of Robin Wheeler, O., & Torrey, M. (1946). A collection of the
Hood. Houghton. best loved hymns. Dutton.
McCloskey, R. (1941). Make way for ducklings. Wiese, K. (1948). Fish in the air. Viking.
Viking. Yashima, T. (1958). Umbrella. Viking.
McCloskey, R. (1948). Blueberries for Sal. Viking. Yashima, T. (1967). Seashore story. Viking.
McGinley, P., & Stone, H. (1948). All around the town. Yolen, J., & Young, E. (1967). The emperor and the
Lippincott. kite. World.
McGinley, P., & Stone, H. (1950). The most wonderful Zemach, H., & Zemach, M. (1969). The judge: An
doll in the world. Lippincott. untrue tale. Farrar.
Mihous, K. (1950). The egg tree. Scribner.
Ness, E. (1966). Sam, Bangs and Moonshine. Holt.
Newberry, C. T. (1938). Barkis. Harper. APPENDIX B: BEHAVIORAL DEFINITIONS
Newberry, C. T. (1940). April’s kittens. Harper. FROM DAVIS (1984)
Newberry, C. T. (1950). T-bone, the baby sitter. Harper.
Parin d’Aulaire, I., & Parin d’Aulaire, E. (1939). Abra- Dependent: seeking or relying on others for help, pro-
ham Lincoln. Doubleday. tection, or reassurance; maintaining close proxim-
Pene du Bois, W. (1956). Lion. Viking. ity to others.
P1: JLS
Sex Roles [sers] pp978-sers-472553 September 12, 2003 15:9 Style file version June 3rd, 2002

448 Clark, Guilmain, Saucier, and Tavarez

Independent: self-initiated and self-contained behav- tion to social research (2nd ed., pp. 379–385). Belmont, CA:
ior, autonomous functioning, resistance to exter- Wadsworth.
Clark, R., Kulkin, H., & Clancy, L. (1999). The liberal feminist bias
nally imposed constraints. in feminist social science research on children’s books. In B. L.
Cooperative: working together or in a joint effort to- Clark & M. R. Higgonet (Eds.), Girls, boys, books, toys: Gen-
ward a common goal, complementary division of der, children’s literature, and culture (pp. 71–82). Baltimore:
Johns Hopkins Press.
labor in a given activity. Clark, R., Lennon, R., & Morris, L. (1993). Of Caldecotts and
Competitive: striving against another in an activity or kings: Gendered images in recent American children’s books
game for a particular goal, position, reward; desire by Black and non-Black illustrators. Gender and Society, 5,
227–245.
to be first, best, winner. Daugherty, J. (1938). Andy and the lion. New York: Viking Press.
Directive: guiding, leading, impelling others toward D’Aulaire, I., & D’Aulaire, E. P. (1939). Abraham Lincoln. New
an action or goal; controlling behaviors of others. York: Doubleday.
Davis, A. (1984). Sex-differentiated behaviors in nonsexist picture
Submissive: yielding to the direction of others; defer- books. Sex Roles, 11, 1–15.
ence to wishes of others. Davis, L., & Woodward, H. (ill.). (1949). The wild birthday cake.
Persistent: maintenance of goal-directed activity de- New York: Doubleday.
Elphinstine, D., & Lent, B. (ill.). (1968). Why the sun and the moon
spite obstacles, setbacks, or adverse conditions. live in the sky. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.
Explorative: seeking knowledge or information thr- Fader, E. (1999). How you can get on the Newbery or Calde-
ough careful examination or investigation; inquis- cott Committee: An awards insider shares her secrets. Re-
trieved May, 2003, from http://slj.reviewsnews.com/index.asp?
itive and curious. layout=article&articleid=CA153022&publication=slj
Creative: producing novel idea or product; unique so- Fish, H. D., & Lathrop, D. P. (ill.). (1937). Animals of the Bible.
lution to a problem; engaging in fantasy or imagi- Philadelphia: Lippincott.
Fish, H. D., & Lawson, R. (ill.). (1937). Four and twenty blackbirds:
native play. Old nursery rhymes. Philadelphia: Lippincott.
Imitative: duplicating, mimicking, or modeling behav- Frasconi, A. (1958). The house that Jack built. New York: Harcourt,
ior (activity or verbalization) of others. Brace and World.
Freeman, D. (1957). Fly high, fly low. New York: Viking Press.
Nurturant: giving physical or emotional aid, support, Gag, W. (1938). Snow White and the seven dwarfs. New York:
or comfort to another; demostrating affection or Coward-McGunn.
compassion for another. Gooden, A., & Gooden, M. (2001). Gender representations in no-
table children’s picture books: 1995–1999. Sex Roles, 45, 89–
Aggressive: physically or emotionally hurting some- 101.
one; verbal aggression; destroying property. Grauerholz, E., & Pescosolido, B. (1989). Gender representation
Emotional: affective display of feelings; manifestation in children’s literature: 1900–1984. Gender and Society, 3, 113–
125.
of pleasure, fear, anger, sorrow, and so on via laugh- Haley, G. (1970). A story. A story. Konigstein: Atheneum.
ing, cowering, crying, frowning, violent outbursts, Handforth, T. (1938). Mei Li. New York: Doubleday.
and so on. Holbrook, S., & Ward, L. (1949). America’s Ethan Allen. Boston:
Houghton Mifflin.
Active: gross motor (large muscle) physical activity, Holling, H. C. (1941). Paddle-to-the-sea. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.
work, and play. hooks, b. (1997). bell hooks: Cultural criticism and transformation.
Passively active: fine motor (small muscle) activity; A film by Media Education Foundation, Northhampton, MA.
Jennings, S. (1975). Effects of sex typing in children’s stories on
alert, attentive, activity but with minimal or no preference recall. Child Development, 46, 220–223.
physical movements (e.g., reading, talking, think- Keats, J. (1969). Goggles! New York: MacMillan.
ing, etc.). Knopp, S. (1980). Sexism in pictures of children’s readers: East and
West Germany compared. Sex Roles, 6, 189–205.
Koblinsky, S., Cruse, D., & Sugawara, A. (1978). Sex role stereo-
types and children’s memory for story content. Child Devel-
REFERENCES opment, 49, 452–458.
Kolbe, R., & LaVoie, J. (1981). Sex-role stereotyping in preschool
children’s picture books. Social Psychology Quarterly, 44, 369–
Ashton, E. (1983). Measures of play behavior: The influence of 374.
sex-role stereotyped children’s books. Sex Roles, 9, pp. 43–47. Kropp, J., & Halverston, C. (1983). Preschool children’s preferences
Bemelman, L. (1939). Madeline. New York: Simon and Schuster. and recall for stereotyped versus nonstereotyped stories. Sex
Brown, M. (1950). Dick Whittington and his cat. New York: Roles, 9, 261–272.
Scribner. LaBastida, A., & Ets, M. H. (ill.). (1959). Nine days to Christmas
Clark, A. N., & Herrera, V. (ill.). (1941). In my mother’s house. New York: Viking Press.
New York: Simon and Schuster. Lawson, R. (1940). They were strong and good. New York: Viking
Clark, B. (1992). American children’s literature: Background Press.
and bibliography. American Studies International, 30, 4– Lionni, L. (1967). Frederick. New York: Random House.
40. Lionni, L. (1969). Alexander and the wind-up mouse. New York:
Clark, R., Almeida, M., Gurka, T., & Middleton, L. (2003). Random House.
Engendering tots with Caldecotts: An updated update. In Lobel, A. (1970). Frog and toad are friends. New York: Harper &
E. S. Adler & R. Clark (Eds.), How it’s done: An invita- Row.
P1: JLS
Sex Roles [sers] pp978-sers-472553 September 12, 2003 15:9 Style file version June 3rd, 2002

Gender Stereotyping in Award-Winning Picture Books 449

Lutes-Dunckley, C. (1978). Sex-role preferences as a function of Seuss, D. (1949). Bartholomew and the oobleck. New York: Random
sex of storyteller and story content. Journal of Psychology, House.
100, 151–158. Seuss, D. (1950). If I ran the zoo. New York: Random House.
Malcolmson, A., & Burton, V. L. (1947). Song of Robin Hood. New Sleator, W., & Lent, B. (ill.). (1970). The angry moon. Boston: Little,
York: McGraw Hill. Brown & Company.
McCluskey, R. (1941). Make way for ducklings. New York: Viking Steig, W. (1969). Sylvester and the magic pebble. New York: Simon
Press. and Schuster.
McCluskey, R. (1948). Blueberries for Sal. New York: Viking Press. St. Peter, S. (1979). Jack went up the hill . . . But where was Jill?
Ochman, J. M. (1996). The effects of nongender-role stereotyped, Psychology of Women Quarterly, 4, 256–260.
same-sex role models in storybooks on the self-esteem of Sugino, T. (2000, August). Stereotypical role models in Western and
children in grade three. Sex Roles, 35, 711–35. non-Western children’s literature. Paper presented at the Inter-
Peirce, K., & Edwards, E. (1988). Children’s construction of fantasy national Conference for Global Conversations on Language
stories: Gender Differences in conflict resolution strategies. and Literacy, Utrecht, The Netherlands.
Sex Roles, 18, 393–404. Tepper, C., & Cassidy, K. (1999). Gender differences in emotional
Pescosolido, B., Grauerholz, E., & Milkie, M. (1997). Culture and language in children’s picture books. Sex Roles, 40, 265–280.
conflict: The portrayal of Blacks in U.S. children’s picture Titus, E., & Galdone, P. (1956). Anatole. New York: McGraw Hill.
books through the mid- and late-twentieth century. American Titus, E., & Galdone, P. (1957). Anatole and the cat. New York:
Sociological Review, 62, 443–464. McGraw Hill.
Petersham, M., & Petersham, M. (1941). An American ABC. New Turner-Bowker, D. (1996). Gender stereotyped descriptors in chil-
York: MacMillan. dren’s books: Does “curious Jane” exist in the literature? Sex
Peterson, S. B., & Navy, N. A. (1990). Gender stereotypes in chil- Roles, 35, pp. 461–488.
dren’s books: Their prevalence and influence on cognitive and Weitzman, L., Eifler, D., Hokada, E., & Ross, C. (1972). Sex-role
affective development. Gender and Education, 2, 185–197. socialization in picture books for preschool children. American
Preston, E. M., & Parker, R. A. (ill.). (1969). Pop Corn and Ma Journal of Sociology, 77, 1125–1150.
Goodness. New York: Viking Press. White, H. (1985). Damsels in distress: Dependency themes in fiction
Purcell, P., & Stewart, L. (1990). Dick and Jane in 1989. Sex Roles, for adolescents. Adolescence, 21, 251–256.
22, 177–185. Williams, A., Vernon, J., Williams, M., & Malecha, K. (1987). Sex-
Ransome, A., & Shulevitz, U. (1968). The fool of the world and the role socialization in picture books: An update. Social Science
flying ship. New York: Farrar, Straus and Giroux. Quarterly, 68, 148–156.
Rowbotham, S. (1997). A century of women. The history of women Yashimo, T. (1958). Umbrella. New York: Viking Press.
in Britain and the United States in the twentieth century. Yashimo, T. (1967). Seashore story. New York: Viking Press.
New York: Penguin Books. Yolen, J., & Young, E. (ill.). (1967). The emperor and the kite.
Seuss, D. (1947). McElligot’s pool. New York: Random House. New York: World Publishing.

View publication stats

You might also like