You are on page 1of 14

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/292989338

Experienced and Novice Officers' Generalized Communication Suspicion and


Veracity Judgments

Article  in  Law and Human Behavior · February 2016


DOI: 10.1037/lhb0000169

CITATIONS READS
12 193

4 authors, including:

Jaume Masip Eugenio Garrido


Universidad de Salamanca Universidad de Salamanca
80 PUBLICATIONS   935 CITATIONS    69 PUBLICATIONS   842 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Psicología jurídica View project

Autoeficcacia View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Jaume Masip on 26 March 2016.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Masip, J., Alonso, H., Herrero, C., & Garrido, E. (2016). Experienced and novice officers' generalized communication suspicion and
veracity judgments. Law and Human Behavior, 40(2), 169-181.
Law and Human Behavior © 2016 American Psychological Association
2016, Vol. 40, No. 2, 169 –181 0147-7307/16/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/lhb0000169

Experienced and Novice Officers’ Generalized Communication Suspicion


and Veracity Judgments
Jaume Masip, Hernán Alonso, Carmen Herrero, and Eugenio Garrido
University of Salamanca, Spain

Deception detection research has shown that police officers are less truth-biased and make their veracity
judgments with greater confidence than do nonofficers. Here we examined nonofficers, novice officers,
and experienced officers’ response bias, confidence, and generalized communicative suspicion. In
Experiment 1, novice officers aligned with nonofficers in terms of both generalized communicative
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

suspicion scores and confidence, with both these groups scoring lower than experienced officers.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Generalized communicative suspicion scores and veracity judgments were not significantly related for
either sample. However, novice officers aligned with experienced officers in terms of judgments: both
police groups were lie-biased, whereas nonofficers were truth-biased. These findings suggest that unlike
experienced officers, who have embraced the police culture to a greater degree, novice officers are not
dispositionally suspicious (generalized communicative suspicion); however, they are able to mirror the
prototypical police behavior (deception judgments) in police-related contexts. Experiment 2 supported
these notions.

Keywords: lie bias, deception, GCS, police, police culture

“We don’t interrogate innocent people.” This was the reply judgments than truth judgments (see reviews by Alonso, Masip, &
given by Joseph Buckley, president of John E. Reid and Associ- Garrido, 2009; Garrido & Masip, 1999; Meissner & Kassin, 2002).
ates, a U.S. police training firm, when someone asked him whether In his major exploration of the deception detection literature, Vrij
his interrogation methods could cause innocent suspects to confess (2008) concluded that
(Kassin & Gudjonsson, 2004, p. 36). Buckley said this because in
order for a suspect to be questioned with Reid’s confrontational [c]ompared to laypersons, professionals seem to be somewhat better
in detecting lies and somewhat worse in detecting truths. This is, at
interrogation techniques, the suspect must first have been judged
least in part, because professional lie catchers do not appear to show
guilty on the grounds of either his or her reactions to a preinter- a truth-bias. In fact, many studies have demonstrated that police
rogation interview (called Behavior Analysis Interview; see Inbau, officers show a lie-bias, judging the majority of the fragments they
Reid, Buckley, & Jayne, 2013), or some kind of evidence. How- were exposed to as being deceptive. Moreover, in every study where
ever, Buckley’s statement also reflects the view of many seasoned both professional lie catchers and laypersons have participated, the
law enforcement officers—the view that suspects are typically professional lie catchers were more inclined to judge a fragment as
guilty and lie during police questioning. In this study, we exam- deceptive than laypersons. (p. 164)
ined the sources of this bias.
C. F. Bond and DePaulo’s (2006) meta-analysis did not find a
lie bias among “experts” (i.e., people whose occupations expose
Investigator Bias Effect them to lies, such as police officers, judges, auditors, job inter-
Unlike the general public, which displays a truth bias when viewers or psychiatrists); however, between-study comparisons
judging someone else’s veracity (e.g., C. F. Bond & DePaulo, revealed that such experts were less biased toward truthfulness
2006; Levine, Park, & McCornack, 1999), police officers have judgments than nonexperts.
often been found to show a lie bias—that is, they make more lie Not only are officers less inclined to make truth judgments than
are nonofficers; they also see themselves as skilled lie- and truth-
detectors and make their veracity judgments with great confidence
(e.g., DePaulo & Pfeiffer, 1986; Garrido & Masip, 1999; Garrido,
This article was published Online First February 4, 2016. Masip, & Herrero, 2004; Meissner & Kassin, 2002). Meissner and
Jaume Masip, Hernán Alonso, Carmen Herrero, and Eugenio Garrido, Kassin (2002; see also Masip, 2014b) coined the term investigator
Department of Social Psychology and Anthropology, University of Sala- bias effect to refer to the officers’ tendency to make lie judgments
manca. (often with high confidence).
Hernán Alonso is now at the Pedagogical Medical Institute San Juan de
Dios, Valladolid, Spain; Eugenio Garrido is now retired. Behavior, Personality, and Situations
The authors are grateful to Professor Sigi Sporer for his help with the
signal detection theory formulas. There are some crucial notions in this paper that should not be
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Jaume confused. These notions are lie bias, generalized communicative
Masip, Faculty of Psychology, University of Salamanca, Avenida de la suspicion, and police-related situations. Lie bias refers to behavior,
Merced, 109-131, 37005 Salamanca, Spain. E-mail: jmasip@usal.es generalized communicative suspicion is a personality variable, and

169
170 MASIP, ALONSO, HERRERO, AND GARRIDO

police-related situations are a type of situation. We think it is findings. These findings suggest confirmation bias was in opera-
important to clarify these notions. tion: officers appeared to enter the veracity assessment situation
First, calling someone a liar (or, for that matter, a truth teller) is with the belief the sender was going to lie, and they appeared to
a behavior. Repeatedly calling other people liars is performing the search for supportive evidence. But why would officers assume the
same behavior repeatedly. Thus, the lie bias pertains to the behav- sender was going to lie?
ioral domain. Masip et al. (2005) reasoned that the police are often involved
Behavior may be caused by personality, by situations, or by in competitive interactions (Robinson, 1996). In cooperative inter-
their interaction (e.g., Bandura, 1986). Concerning personality, actions all participants collaborate to attain a common goal. Con-
some people may be more distrustful and suspicious than others, versely, in competitive interactions different participants have
which may lead them more often to judge others’ statements as different goals and everyone is pursuing his or her own benefit at
deceptive. Levine and McCornack (1991) posited that individuals the expense of the others. The nature of policing, as well as the
systematically differ in terms of their generalized communicative police goals of maintaining order and enforcing the law lead the
suspicion (GCS) —that is, their “predisposition towards believing police to be frequently involved in competitive interactions. Over
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

that the messages produced by others are deceptive” (p. 328). the years, police involvement in situations like these may generate
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Levine and McCornack conceptualized this construct as a rela- chronic distrust and suspiciousness.
tively enduring personality characteristic and created a scale to More generally, distrust or suspiciousness may also reflect po-
measure it. They expected that generalized communicative suspi- lice culture. Pioneering work by Skolnick (1966), Westley (1970),
cion scores (personality) would predict deception judgments (be- and Rubinstein (1973) depicted the police as an isolated, cohesive,
havior). distrustful, secretive, and authoritarian professional group exposed
Also, some situations or contexts may be more likely to prompt to multiple dangers in their interactions with citizens. More re-
deception judgments than others. Imagine that on Friday morning, cently, some researchers have argued that police culture may be
a police officer is interviewing a suspect at the police station. The more diverse than what early researchers thought, and that the
next day, the same officer, who is off duty, is visiting a distant central characteristics of the culture may have changed somewhat
town with his or her family and asks a pedestrian for directions. It (see, e.g., O’Neill, Marks, & Singh, 2007; Paoline, 2003, 2014;
is reasonable to assume that the first situation is more likely to Scaramella et al., 2010; Yüksel & Tosun, 2015). However, Wad-
prompt deception judgments from the officer than the second dington (1999) maintains that the core elements of the police
situation. Interestingly, situations may affect not only behaviors culture remain essentially the same across time and nations.
but also personality (e.g., Bandura, 1986). For instance, over the Among these elements are (a) position of authority; (b) glorifica-
long run, repeated exposure to situations arousing suspicion may tion of violence, action and excitement (celebrating the “real”
turn the individual into a suspicious person. police job); (c) crime-fighting image; (d) social isolation; (e)
The main point of Experiment 1 was to examine (a) whether protective solidarity with fellow officers; and (f) “dirty work” and
officers (particularly the most experienced ones) displayed a stron- knowledge of the miseries of the “underworld” (see Waddington,
ger lie bias (behavior) than non-officers; (b) whether experienced 1999, for the rationale behind these and other characteristics of the
officers, in part because of their exposure to suspicion-arousing police culture, as well as their psychological benefits for officers).
situations, were more dispositionally (personality) suspicious (in It is important to note that distrust, cynicism, and suspicion have
terms of their generalized communicative suspicion scores) than also been associated with police culture:
novice officers and nonofficers; and (c) whether officers’ gener-
alized communicative suspicion (personality) could explain their An integral part of the police personality is cynicism: the notion that
all people are motivated by evil and selfishness. Police cynicism
tendency to make lie judgments (behavior). In Experiment 2, we
develops among many officers through the nature of police work. . . . By
examined the influence of the situation on novice officers’ decep-
constantly dealing with crime and the most unsavory aspects of social
tion judgments. In the following sections, we describe the back- life . . . [police officers’] faith in humanity seems to diminish.
ground of this study and make specific predictions. (McNamara, 1999, pp. 8 –9)

According to Caplan (2003), cynicism is an inevitable conse-


Officers’ Suspicion
quence of police work, and “a cynic expects nothing but the worst
Masip, Alonso, Garrido, and Antón (2005) suggested that the in human behavior” (p. 304). Skolnick (1994) argued that “the
officers’ lie bias could be a result of greater communicative element of danger seems to make the policeman especially atten-
suspicion among officers than among lay people. In a prior study, tive to signs indicating a potential for violence and lawbreaking.
Garrido et al. (2004) had shown video clips with an honest or As a result, the policeman is generally a ‘suspicious’ person” (p.
deceptive sender (i.e., a person giving a statement) to both police 43). Johnson, Todd, and Subramanian (2005) argued that danger-
officers and students. They found that officers made more decep- ous and unpredictable persons cannot be identified in advance;
tion judgments than students. Furthermore, relative to students, because of this, officers “treat each individual with suspicion and
officers mentioned more cues associated with deception judg- caution” (p. 104). Paoline (2014) described the police “as a so-
ments, fewer cues associated with truthfulness judgments, and cially isolated group that are distrustful and suspicious of their
interpreted ambiguous behaviors as deception indicators rather primary clientele, as they continually attempt to maintain the upper
than as truth indicators (see Masip, Garrido, Herrero, Antón, & hand in utilizing their coercive authority” (p. 3579).
Alonso, 2006). Subsequently, Nahari (2012), who also examined In short, frequent exposure to situations involving crime, fre-
law enforcement officers’ and students’ credibility judgments and quent competitive interactions with suspected offenders, the dan-
the reasons they provided for their judgments, reported similar gers of police work, the authority of the police, and the resulting
OFFICERS’ GENERALIZED COMMUNICATIVE SUSPICION AND VERACITY JUDGMENTS 171

social isolation of officers engender suspicion and distrust directed mental overconfidence would be expected to be hallmarks of a
toward the general public. An essential component of suspicion is veteran officer’s personality.
the belief that other people have something to hide and will not be
fully honest, which might generate a lie bias.
Rationale for the Present Study
It is important to note here that these effects are more likely to
be apparent among experienced officers than among police re- One weakness in Masip et al.’s (2005) study is that even though
cruits. The cumulative effect of frequent involvement in compet- they measured participants’ generalized communicative suspicion,
itive interactions happens in the long run. Also, it is reasonable to they did not examine the relation between generalized communi-
assume that senior officers embrace the police culture—which cative suspicion scores and veracity judgments. Masip et al. as-
involves suspicion—to a greater degree than police recruits. In- sumed that higher scores lead to more lie judgments and fewer
deed, the police culture is transmitted from one generation to the truth judgments, but is this assumption correct? If it is, would the
next through socialization within the police force, first via formal more seasoned officers (who scored higher on the scale) thus make
training at the academy, and later on by more experienced peers at more lie judgments than their less experienced peers? The current
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

the workplace (McNamara, 1999; Paoline, 2003; Sato, 2003; study addressed these issues.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Scaramella et al., 2010; Van Maanen, 1975). Scholars such as


Paoline (2003, 2014) and Waddington (1999) argue that the main Generalized Communicative Suspicion and
function of police culture is to allow officers to cope with the
Deception Judgments
hazards of police work and social pressures. Therefore, novice
officers will progressively embrace the police culture as they leave The notion that generalized communicative suspicion scores
the police academy and get into real policing. Also, increased predict deception judgments is not undisputed. Levine and Mc-
informal interactions with senior officers contribute to the novices’ Cornack (1991) found that individuals scoring moderate and high
socialization (e.g., Paoline, 2003). Police socialization models on the scale were more likely to make deception judgments than
suggest that skepticism, distrust and disenchantment appear some- individuals scoring low. However, McCornack and Levine (1990)
what late in a police career (e.g., Barker, 1999; Van Maanen, found no effect of generalized communicative suspicion scores on
1975). It can thus be said that officers’ immersion into police the number of lie judgments, and two studies conducted in prisons
culture increases gradually over their police career. Therefore, revealed no significant relation between inmates’ scores on the
there is no reason to expect much distrust among novice officers. scale and their deception judgments (G. D. Bond, Malloy, Arias,
These are personality changes that happen over time through a Nunn, & Thomson, 2005; G. D. Bond & Lee, 2005). In another
lengthy, gradual process. study, G. D. Bond, Thompson, and Malloy (2005) showed that
Indeed, Masip et al. (2005) asked undergraduates, novice offi- prisoners had higher generalized communicative suspicion scores
cers, and veteran officers to complete Levine and McCornack’s and made more deception judgments than nonprisoners; however,
(1991) Generalized Communicative Suspicion Scale (see left col- a regression analysis across groups revealed that generalized com-
umn in Appendix). Because of experienced officers’ greater ex- municative suspicion did not predict deception judgments. Hurst
posure to competitive interactions and police culture, Masip et al. and Oswald (2012) found that students and police recruits were
(2005) predicted that experienced officers would score signifi- truth-biased, whereas police officers were neither truth- nor lie-
cantly higher than would novice officers and students on the scale. biased. Correlations between generalized communicative suspi-
The data supported this prediction. Further, the generalized com- cion and preliminary (not final) deception judgments were signif-
municative suspicion scores of novice officers and students did not icant only for the students, but not for police officers or recruits.
differ significantly. Hurst and Oswald (2012) subsequently repli- Hurst and Oswald ran regression analyses entering concern about
cated this pattern of results. These findings supported the notion misjudging truths or lies, age, and generalized communicative
that seasoned officers are more distrustful than their less experi- suspicion scores as predictors of final deception judgments. The
enced peers and the general public, and suggested that the officers’ effect of generalized communicative suspicion scores was not
lie bias may be caused by generalized suspicion. significant for either sample. In short, if there is any link between
Officers’ judgmental confidence may also increase over time. generalized communicative suspicion scores and deception judg-
Paoline (2003) contended that officers believe they can minimize ments, this link seems to be weak (see Masip, 2014a), and other
the dangers to which they are exposed by “reading people and variables of a different kind may also influence judgments. In this
situations” (p. 202). This belief may contribute to increasing senior study, besides examining the influence of generalized communi-
officers’ misplaced confidence in “reading people” to detect de- cative suspicion (personality) on the kind of judgment, we also
ception. Also, a number of factors may gradually increase officers’ examined the influence of police experience, and the situation.
reliance on inaccurate beliefs about deceptive behavior over their
career—factors such as the absence of disconfirmatory feedback
Novice Versus Experienced Officers’ Lie Bias
when officers’ veracity judgments are inaccurate paired with con-
firmatory feedback when judgments are accurate (see DePaulo & Whereas much research has been conducted comparing the
Pfeiffer, 1986), schema-consistent memory, confirmation bias, and veracity judgments of police officers versus nonofficers, to our
belief perseverance (see Garrido et al., 2004, pp. 267–268, for a knowledge little research exists comparing novice versus experi-
discussion). Overreliance on inaccurate beliefs may increase offi- enced officers’ judgments (for an exception, see Hurst and Os-
cers’ perceived ability to detect truths and lies, as well as the wald’s [2012] Study 1). In the current study, nonofficers, novice
confidence they place in their veracity judgments (while not in- officers, and veteran officers completed the Generalized Commu-
creasing accuracy). Thus, both communication suspicion and judg- nicative Suspicion Scale and judged the veracity of a number of
172 MASIP, ALONSO, HERRERO, AND GARRIDO

videotaped statements. In terms of communicative suspicion, we Method


expected to replicate Masip et al.’s (2005) findings: Novice offi-
cers were expected to have scores that did not differ from those of
Participants
nonofficers, with both these groups scoring significantly lower
than the experienced officers. This would reflect the influence of Seventy-two nonofficers (Mage ⫽ 22 years, SD ⫽ 1.61, SEM ⫽
policing situations and police culture on personality. However, in 0.19), 72 novice officers (Mage ⫽ 28 years, SD ⫽ 1.79, SEM ⫽
terms of deception judgments, it was unclear whether novice 0.21; 64 with less than one year of experience and none with more
officers would align with nonofficers or with their veteran peers. than five years of experience), and 72 experienced officers
If novice officers make as few deception judgments as nonof- (Mage ⫽ 44 years, SD ⫽ 3.96, SEM ⫽ 0.47; Mexperience ⫽ 22 years,
ficers, this would show that novice officers are like nonofficers in SD ⫽ 3.93, SEM ⫽ 0.46, experience range: 16 to 32 years)
terms of both (a) generalized, slow changes in personality over participated in the study. Nonofficers were undergraduate students
time (generalized communicative suspicion), and (b) context- whereas police officers were studying at the national police acad-
dependent, immediate changes in behavior (deception judgments). emy of Spain to become or to be promoted to police inspectors. In
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

On the other hand, if novice officers make as many deception the Spanish National Police Force, inspectors are of a higher rank
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

judgments as their more seasoned colleagues, then this would than plain officers. Experienced plain officers can apply to be
reveal that (a) despite novice officers being like nonofficers in promoted to inspectors after a number of years of service. Lay
terms of generalized communicative suspicion (because they have people can enter the police after passing a competitive examination
not yet undergone any lengthy socialization process within the to become either plain officers or inspectors. The requirements to
police force), they (b) try to act (behavior) as a prototypical “real” become an inspector are much more stringent than those to become
officer, that is, showing harshness, distrust, suspicion, and there- a plain officer. Some officers with not enough experience to be
fore making many deception judgments. promoted to inspector may also take this competitive examination
to have access to an inspector position (for more detail, see Masip
et al., 2005, Note 3 on p. 1052).
Experiment 1
We asked nonofficers, novice officers, and experienced officers Materials
to complete the Generalized Communicative Suspicion Scale. We used Masip et al.’s (2005) Spanish adaptation of Levine and
Later on, and with no explicit connection to the prior task, all McCornack’s (1991) Generalized Communicative Suspicion
participants judged the veracity of a number of videotaped truthful Scale. The scale contains 11 items, and Cronbach’s alpha for the
and deceptive statements. We predicted the following: Spanish adaptation is .82 (Masip et al., 2005).
In the present study, we used four videotapes previously used by
Hypothesis 1: Experienced officers would score significantly Masip and his colleagues (Masip, Garrido, & Herrero, 2006, 2009,
higher than both novice officers and nonofficers on the Gen- 2010; Street & Masip, 2015). The videotapes contain the state-
eralized Communicative Suspicion Scale. No difference was ments of 24 individuals who lied and told the truth (in counter-
expected between novice officers and nonofficers. balanced order) answering three questions about either an at-
tempted or successful theft they had seen on videotape. The 48
Hypothesis 2: Despite the mixed evidence concerning the resulting statements were arranged in four different videotapes,
relationship between generalized communicative suspicion each containing six liars and six truth tellers. All relevant vari-
scores and veracity judgments, we tentatively predicted that ables, such as senders’ gender, question order, and so forth were
higher scores would be significantly related to a greater ten- counter balanced, and the same person never appeared in the same
dency to make deception judgments. tape lying and telling the truth. Specifically, the videos were labeled A1,
A2, B1, and B2. Videos A1 and B1 contained the statements of the
Hypothesis 3: Police officers would make more deception same 12 senders, but those who lied in A1 told the truth in B1, and
judgments (and fewer truth judgments) than nonofficers. It those who told the truth in A1 lied in B1. Videos A2 and B2
remained an open question whether novice officers would contained the statements of the other 12 senders and, again, those
differ or not from experienced officers in terms of truth/ who lied in one of the videos told the truth in the other video (see
deception judgments. Masip et al., 2006).

Hypothesis 4: Relatedly, because of the judgmental tendencies


predicted in Hypothesis 3, we expected police officers to be
Procedure
more accurate than nonofficers in judging lies and less accu- All participants first completed Levine and McCornack’s (1991)
rate than nonofficers in judging truths. It remained an open Generalized Communicative Suspicion Scale. Then, at a different
question whether novice and experienced officers would differ time in the same academic year, they judged the veracity of 12
in this regard. videotaped statements. At the time of data collection, the partici-
pants were unaware of the connection between the two tasks,
Hypothesis 5: Experienced officers would show more confi- which were administered by different researchers at different
dence in their veracity judgments than would both novice times. Within each sample, 18 participants watched each of the
officers and nonofficers. No difference was expected between four 12-statement videos. Both students in the same year at the
the latter two groups. university and police students in the same year at the police
OFFICERS’ GENERALIZED COMMUNICATIVE SUSPICION AND VERACITY JUDGMENTS 173

academy are normally divided into smaller groups on the basis of ANOVAs on the percentage of judgments of deceptiveness and on
the initial letter of their family name. A different video was shown B” (to test Hypothesis 3), on the percentage of correct judgments
to each group. Groups did not differ in terms of gender or age for (accuracy; to test Hypothesis 4), and on confidence (to test Hy-
either sample. pothesis 5). Using ANOVAs is in keeping with the analytical
The videos were shown in a classroom at the School of Psy- approach normally used in deception research.
chology (nonofficers) or at the police academy (officers). Empty
seats were left between one participant and the next, and the Results
researcher asked participants to keep silent and do the task indi-
vidually. The researcher handed out an answer booklet to partici- Generalized Communicative Suspicion
pants. The instructions, which very closely resembled those used
by Masip et al. (2006), were printed on the front page and the Cronbach’s alpha of the Generalized Communicative Suspicion
researcher read them aloud. The participants learned that they were Scale for the current study samples was .84 for nonofficers and .85
about to watch 12 people (senders), each making a statement about for both novice officers and experienced officers. The ANOVA
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

a videotape that they (senders) had seen, and that the statement of revealed that the three samples differed significantly in their
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

each particular sender could be either truthful or deceptive. The scores, F(2, 213) ⫽ 4.69, p ⫽ .010, ␩2 ⫽ .042, 90% CI [.006, .089]
participants’ task was to determine whether each sender was lying (see Table 1). Post hoc Sheffé tests revealed that nonofficers (M ⫽
or telling the truth. The experimenter told the participants that after 34.94, SD ⫽ 9.68, SEM ⫽ 1.14) and novice officers (M ⫽ 33.90,
each statement he would pause the tape so they could provide their SD ⫽ 9.70, SEM ⫽ 1.14) did not differ significantly, p ⫽ .827;
answers by ticking the “Lying” or “Telling the truth” box for the however, experienced officers’ scores (M ⫽ 38.81, SD ⫽ 10.95,
particular sender in the response booklet and by indicating their SEM ⫽ 1.29) were significantly higher than were those of novice
confidence in each judgment on a 1 (not at all confident) to 7 officers (p ⫽ .016) and marginally higher than those of nonofficers
(absolutely confident) scale. After both tasks had been completed, (p ⫽ .075). Overall, the pattern of results supported Hypothesis 1.
the participants were debriefed. Remarkably, the descriptive statistics are strikingly similar to those
reported by Masip et al. (2005; for nonofficers, M ⫽ 35.57, SD ⫽
Overview of Analyses 9.36; for novice officers, M ⫽ 33.13, SD ⫽ 10.11; for experienced
officers, M ⫽ 39.42, SD ⫽ 11.09).
We conducted a one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) to In line with Hypothesis 2, we expected generalized communi-
examine whether nonofficers, novice officers, and experienced cative suspicion to explain the percentage of judgments of decep-
officers differed in their generalized communicative suspicion tiveness, false alarms, and B”. The results of the linear regression
scores (Hypothesis 1). To test Hypothesis 2, we calculated linear analyses are displayed in Table 2. It is apparent that the predictive
regression analyses to see how well generalized communicative value of the generalized communicative suspicion scores was
suspicion scores could explain (a) the raw percentage of judgments essentially null. Hypothesis 2 was not supported by the data.
of deceptiveness made by participants, (b) B”, and (c) false alarms.
B” is a signal detection theory (SDT) measure of bias. According
Judgments
to SDT (e.g., Green & Swets, 1966; Swets, Dawes, & Monahan,
2000), a detection task depends on two distinct processes, namely, The Sample ⫻ Veracity ANOVA run on the percentage of
discrimination (accuracy) and decision criterion (response bias). judgments of deceptiveness showed that deceptive statements were
Unlike raw percentages, SDT makes it possible to evaluate each of judged as deceptive (M ⫽ 57.64, SD ⫽ 18.31, SEM ⫽ 1.25) more
these processes independently. A measure of response bias that is often than truthful statements (M ⫽ 46.91, SD ⫽ 21.16, SEM ⫽
independent of accuracy is B”. We calculated B” with Donaldson’s 1.44), F(1, 213) ⫽ 38.88, p ⬍ .001, ␩p2 ⫽ .154, 90% CI [.086,
(1992) formula. An unbiased response would yield B” ⫽ 0; neg- .227]. What is more interesting is that the sample main effect was
ative values would reflect a lie bias and positive values a truth bias. also significant, F(2, 213) ⫽ 8.98, p ⬍ .001, ␩p2 ⫽ .078, 90% CI
False alarms were the percentage of truths misjudged as lies. [.026, .135]. Post hoc Sheffé tests revealed that both experienced
For the other hypotheses, because the independent variables officers (M ⫽ 56.60, SD ⫽ 14.19, SEM ⫽ 1.67) and novice
were categorical and the dependent measures were quantitative, we officers (M ⫽ 53.70, SD ⫽ 13.55, SEM ⫽ 1.60) made more
ran 3 (sample: nonofficers vs. novice officers vs. experienced deception judgments than did students (M ⫽ 46.53, SD ⫽ 16.18,
officers) ⫻ 2 (veracity: truthful vs. deceptive statement) mixed SEM ⫽ 1.91); p ⬍ .001 and p ⫽ .015, respectively; however, the

Table 1
Means (Standard Deviations) for the Dependent Variables in Study 1

Measure Nonofficers Novice officers Experienced officers

Generalized Communication Suspicion scores 34.94ab (9.68) 33.90a (9.70) 38.81b (10.95)
Percentage of judgments of deceptiveness 46.53a (16.18) 53.70b (13.55) 56.60b (14.19)
B” .14a (.56) ⫺.14b (.50) ⫺.22b (.48)
Accuracy 54.86a (12.80) 57.18a (12.22) 54.05a (12.89)
Confidence 4.17a (0.83) 4.51a (0.75) 4.99b (0.86)
Note. Within each row, scores with different subscripts differ at the p ⬍ .05 level.
174 MASIP, ALONSO, HERRERO, AND GARRIDO

Table 2 F[1, 123] ⫽ 4.58, p ⫽ .033, ␩p2 ⫽ .021, 90% CI [.001, .063])
Outcomes of the Linear Regression Analyses to Explain the displayed greater accuracy in judging lies than truths, nonofficers
Percentages of Judgments of Deceptiveness, B”, and False were somewhat more accurate in judging truths (M ⫽ 58.33, SD ⫽
Alarms Based on Generalized Communication Suspicion Scores 22.38, SEM ⫽ 2.64) than lies (M ⫽ 51.39, SD ⫽ 18.71, SEM ⫽
2.21), F(1, 123) ⫽ 4.03, p ⫽ .046, ␩p2 ⫽ .019, 90% CI [.0002,
Measure F df p R R2 B .0586]. Additional analyses revealed that experienced officers’
Percentage of judgments of accuracy was significantly above chance in judging lies, t(71) ⫽
deceptiveness 5.15, p ⬍ .001, d ⫽ 0.61, 95% CI [0.25, 0.96], but not in judging
Nonofficers 0.59 1, 70 .447 .09 .01 0.00 truths, t(71) ⫽ ⫺1.05, p ⫽ .300, d ⫽ ⫺0.12, 95% CI [⫺0.48,
Novice officers 0.26 1, 70 .613 .06 .00 0.00 0.23]. The same pattern emerged among novice officers, t(71) ⫽
Experienced officers 0.00 1, 70 .969 .01 .00 0.00
Across all samples 0.48 1, 214 .490 .05 .00 0.00 5.34, p ⬍ .001, d ⫽ 0.63, 95% CI [0.28, 0.98], and t(71) ⫽ 1.55,
B” p ⫽ .129, d ⫽ 0.18, 95% CI [⫺0.17, 0.53], respectively. However,
Nonofficers 0.67 1, 70 .416 .10 .01 ⫺0.01 nonofficers’ accuracy was significantly above chance in judging
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Novice officers 0.24 1, 70 .626 .06 .00 0.00 truths, t(71) ⫽ 3.16, p ⫽ .002, d ⫽ 0.37, 95% CI [0.02, 0.73], not
Experienced officers 0.01 1, 70 .941 .01 .00 0.00
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

lies, t(71) ⫽ 0.63, p ⫽ .531, d ⫽ 0.07, 95% CI [⫺0.28, 0.43].


Across all samples 0.38 1, 214 .539 .04 .00 0.00
False alarms These findings support Hypothesis 4 in that both police groups
Nonofficers 1.08 1, 70 .303 .12 .02 0.00 were more accurate in judging lies than truths, whereas nonofficers
Novice officers 0.43 1, 70 .517 .08 .01 0.00 were more accurate in judging truths than lies.
Experienced officers 0.00 1, 70 .951 .01 .00 0.00
Across all samples 2.11 1, 214 .147 .10 .01 0.00
Confidence
Hypothesis 5 was tested with a 3 (Sample) ⫻ 2 (Veracity) mixed
difference between experienced and novice officers was not sig- ANOVA on judgmental confidence. The sample main effect was
nificant, p ⫽ .498 (see Table 1). significant, F(2, 213) ⫽ 18.18, p ⬍ .001, ␩p2 ⫽ .146, 90% CI [.076,
An additional Sample ⫻ Veracity ANOVA revealed that sam- .214]. Post hoc Scheffé tests revealed that experienced officers’
ples also differed in their B” scores, F(2, 213) ⫽ 9.56, p ⬍ .001, confidence (M ⫽ 4.99, SD ⫽ 0.86, SEM ⫽ 0.10) was significantly
␩p2 ⫽ .082, 90% CI [.029, .141], with both experienced greater than both nonofficers’ (M ⫽ 4.17, SD ⫽ 0.83, SEM ⫽
(B” ⫽ ⫺.22, SD ⫽ .48, SEM ⫽ .06) and novice officers 0.10), p ⬍ .001, and novice officers’ confidence (M ⫽ 4.51, SD ⫽
(B” ⫽ ⫺.14, SD ⫽ .50, SEM ⫽ .06) displaying a lie bias, whereas 0.75, SEM ⫽ 0.09), p ⫽ .002. Novice officers’ confidence was just
nonofficers displayed a truth bias (B” ⫽ .14, SD ⫽ .56, SEM ⫽ slightly (p ⫽ .053) greater than the confidence of nonofficers. A
.07). All three B” figures were significantly different from 0 (no significant veracity main effect revealed that participants were
bias), for experienced officers: t(71) ⫽ ⫺3.82, p ⬍ .001, more confident in judging lies (M ⫽ 4.62, SD ⫽ 0.93, SEM ⫽
d ⫽ ⫺0.45, 95% CI [⫺0.80, ⫺0.10]; for novice officers: 0.06) than truths (M ⫽ 4.50, SD ⫽ 0.96, SEM ⫽ 0.07), F(1,
t(71) ⫽ ⫺2.28, p ⫽ .025, d ⫽ ⫺0.27, 95% CI [⫺0.62, 0.08]; for 213) ⫽ 6.55, p ⫽ .011, ␩2 ⫽ .030, 90% CI [.004, .076]. In line
nonofficers: t(71) ⫽ 2.14, p ⫽ .036, d ⫽ 0.25, 95% CI [⫺0.10, with previous research (Aamodt & Custer, 2006; DePaulo, Char-
0.61]. Scheffé tests revealed no significant difference between lton, Cooper, Lindsay, & Muhlenbruck, 1997), the confidence-
experienced and novice officers (p ⫽ .630), but nonofficers dif- accuracy correlation was not significant (among nonofficers,
fered significantly from both the experienced (p ⬍ .001) and the r(N ⫽ 72) ⫽ .11, p ⫽ .343; among novice officers, r(N ⫽
novel officers (p ⬍ .001). 72) ⫽ ⫺.21, p ⫽ .082; among experience officers, r(N ⫽ 72) ⫽
In short, whereas nonofficers displayed a significant truth bias, .04, p ⫽ .712; across all samples, r(N ⫽ 216) ⫽ ⫺.02, p ⫽ .770).
both police groups were significantly biased toward making de- Hypothesis 5 was supported in that confidence was greater among
ception judgments. These findings supported Hypothesis 3. experienced officers than among novice officers and nonofficers.

Accuracy Discussion
To test Hypothesis 4, we ran a Sample ⫻ Veracity mixed All our predictions except Hypothesis 2 were supported. In
ANOVA on percentage correct judgments (accuracy). The sample terms of generalized communicative suspicion scores, experienced
main effect was not significant, F(2, 213) ⫽ 1.19, p ⫽ .308, ␩p2 ⫽ officers were the most suspicious of all participants. The fact that
.011, 90% CI [.000, .039]—accuracy did not differ significantly novice officers scored lower than experienced officers on the
across samples (see Table 1). A significant main effect of veracity Generalized Communicative Suspicion Scale suggests that the
revealed that lies (M ⫽ 57.64, SD ⫽ 18.32, SEM ⫽ 1.25) were increased suspicion and distrust of the police is not a result of
better detected than truths (M ⫽ 53.09, SD ⫽ 21.16, SEM ⫽ 1.44), police selection practices. Instead, what appears to increase suspi-
F(1, 213) ⫽ 5.19, p ⫽ .024, ␩p2 ⫽ .024, 90% CI [.002, .067]. cion is lengthy police experience and socialization. Over the years,
However, this was moderated by sample, F(2, 213) ⫽ 8.98, p ⬍ officers experience many competitive interactions that might in-
.001, ␩p2 ⫽ .078, 90% CI [.026, .135]. Whereas experienced crease their skepticism concerning other people’s truthfulness.
officers (Mlies ⫽ 60.65, SD ⫽ 17.54, SEM ⫽ 2.07; Mtruths ⫽ 47.45, Similarly, progressive socialization into the police culture may
SD ⫽ 20.68, SEM ⫽ 2.44; F[1, 123] ⫽ 14.54, p ⬍ .001, ␩p2 ⫽ .064, lead police officers to become more distrustful.
90% CI [.021, .123]) and novice officers (Mlies ⫽ 60.88, SD ⫽ However, this increased suspicion did not explain the police’s
17.28, SEM ⫽ 2.04; Mtruths ⫽ 53.47, SD ⫽ 19.16, SEM ⫽ 2.26; lie bias. Even though it may seem reasonable to posit that police
OFFICERS’ GENERALIZED COMMUNICATIVE SUSPICION AND VERACITY JUDGMENTS 175

culture and its influence on personality have an impact on behav- 203). Pressure toward uniformity is great, and new recruits
ior, causing a lie bias among experienced officers, the present “actively seek to assimilate themselves into the organization at
findings do not support this notion. First of all, linear regression every available opportunity” (Sato, 2003, p. 291). Also, the
analyses revealed that generalized communicative suspicion could novice officers get in touch and interact with senior officers in
not explain lie judgments (measured either as raw percentages of this police-only environment. All of these circumstances help to
deception judgments or with more sophisticated SDT measures) explain why the self-identity of new recruits is already that of
for either group. Thus, the notion that officers’ lie bias is caused by a police officer. Novice officers perceive themselves as officers
their increased communicative suspicion is unsupported (see and seek to learn to act as officers.
also Hurst & Oswald, 2012). Second, experienced officers (who But the situation matters. In police-related situations, novice
had higher generalized communicative suspicion scores than officers may feel they are performing a professional task (e.g.,
novice officers) did not display a stronger lie bias than did running an interrogation or assessing veracity) and will play
novice officers. In fact, novice officers made as many deception their roles, acting the way they think others expect them to act
judgments as the experienced officers. Consequently, both po- (e.g., being inquisitive and questioning the truthfulness of the
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

lice groups were more accurate in judging lies than truths. In suspects’ answers). In most respects, their behavior is very
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

contrast, nonofficers displayed a truth bias and were better at much like that of their experienced peers. However, differences
identifying truths than lies. may emerge between novice and experienced officers in more
It is thus apparent that the lie bias is not caused by a police ordinary, nonpolice-related situations. There, police recruits
personality but by other factors. The nature of the task (deception may feel no need to display prototypical officers’ behaviors.
detection) is relevant for police work. Situational demands placed In the context of the current experiment, judging the veracity of
upon the police’s behavior could be a good candidate to explain speakers describing a theft is related to police work. That is why
the current pattern of results. novice officers made as many deception judgments as experienced
Novice officers were aligned with experienced officers in terms officers. However, the Generalized Communicative Suspicion
of deception judgments (percentage of judgments of deceptiveness Scale is not a scale to measure communicative suspicion in police-
and B”) and with nonofficers in terms of generalized communica- related situations. Instead, it measures personality (see Levine &
tive suspicion scores and confidence (see Table 1). This shows that McCornack, 1991). It measures generalized communicative sus-
even though the novice officers had not yet experienced the slow picion, which is dispositional, not situational. This is one possible
changes that happen over a police career (suspicion and confi- explanation why experienced officers scored significantly higher
dence), in this police-related situation they were nevertheless ca- on this scale than did novice officers; the latter did not feel the
pable of behaving like “real officers,” displaying skeptical and need to play the role of an officer when filling in a scale about
distrustful behaviors prototypically attributed to the police. In general—rather than situation-specific and police-related— com-
other words, novice officers are not (personality) “real officers” municative suspicion.
yet, but they may act (behavior) as such in a police-related situa- This line of reasoning suggests that if we had used a scale
tion (see Goffman, 1959). designed specifically to tap suspicion in police-related situations
The reason for this may be that novice officers feel they are only instead of using the Generalized Communicative Suspicion
officers. To enter the police force, applicants have to spend a Scale, then novice officers would have scored as high as experi-
substantial amount of time (ranging from months to years) prepar- enced officers. We decided to conduct a follow up study to test this
ing to pass a harsh competitive examination that many fail. Passing suggestion.
the exam may create a sense of pride and achievement and may
also establish a psychological separation in the minds of those who
Experiment 2
pass between themselves, who are no longer ordinary citizens but
carefully selected “officers,” and those who failed. Once at the We created a new scale we named Interrogation Suspicion Scale
academy, formal socialization begins. The police academy is a (see Appendix). We built the scale by rewriting each item in
total institution, that is, “a place of residence and work where a Levine and McCornack’s (1991) Generalized Communicative Sus-
large number of like-situated individuals, cut off from the wider picion Scale so that the item specifically referred to a police
society for an appreciable period of time, together lead an en- interrogation situation (see Appendix). Therefore, whereas the
closed, formally administered round of life” (Goffman, 1961, p. Generalized Communicative Suspicion Scale items do not refer to
xiii). In a total institution, all aspects of life are tightly scheduled, any specific situation, the Interrogation Suspicion Scale specifi-
happen in the same place, under the same authority, and in the cally focused on a police-related situation.
company of other similar individuals, and all the enforced activi-
ties are designed to fulfill the aims of the institution (Goffman,
Participants and Procedure
1961; see also Davies, 1989). As in other total institutions, in the
police academy first “the attitudes, worldviews, and behavior We collected new data from 74 novice officers (Mage ⫽ 27
patterns of the individual are stripped away” (McNamara, 1999, years, SD ⫽ 1.97, SEM ⫽ 0.23; 56 with less than one year of
p. 3) to later provide the novice officer with “a new set of experience and none with more than three years of experience),
attitudes, values, and beliefs” (McNamara, 1999, p. 4). Police and 107 experienced officers (Mage ⫽ 45 years, SD ⫽ 3.89,
recruits are forced to adopt “uniformity in appearance, attitude, SEM ⫽ 0.38; Mexperience ⫽ 23 years, SD ⫽ 3.95 years, SEM ⫽
and behavior, as well as strict adherence to rules and proce- 0.38; experience range: 14 to 31 years). They all were at the
dures” (Paoline, 2003, p. 203) and “group cohesion and loyalty national police academy of Spain studying to become or to be
are stressed in a paramilitary environment” (Paoline, 2003, p. promoted to police inspectors. They completed the Interroga-
176 MASIP, ALONSO, HERRERO, AND GARRIDO

tion Suspicion Scale (13 items) in class and were debriefed experienced and novice officers completing the Interrogation Sus-
afterward. picion Scale (which measures suspicion in police-related situa-
tions only) would not differ and would all have high scores. To test
Results for these predictions, we ran an ANOVA to compare the interro-
gation suspicion scores of the current experienced and novice
Internal Consistency of the Interrogation officers with the generalized communicative suspicion scores of
experienced and novice officers of Experiment 1. More specifi-
Suspicion Scale
cally, we conducted a 2 (Scale) ⫻ 2 (Officers’ Experience: novice
We ran principal component factor analyses setting the program vs. experienced officers) independent groups ANOVA on gener-
to obtain a single factor solution. In shaping or adapting the alized communicative suspicion scores and interrogation suspicion
Generalized Communicative Suspicion Scale, both Levine and scores. Because the total number of items was the same (11) both
McCornack (1991) and Masip et al. (2005) adopted the criterion of in Masip et al.’s (2005) adaptation of the Generalized Communi-
retaining only those items with factor loadings of .40 or greater. To cative Suspicion Scale and in the current Interrogation Suspicion
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

remain in line with previous research, and also because of statis- Scale, the sum scores could be compared directly—that is, there
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

tical considerations, we adopted the same criterion: The greater the was no need to average across items.
loading, the more the variable reflects the factor. According to
Tabachnick and Fidell (2013), only variables with loadings of .32 Findings
and above should be interpreted. We also calculated Cronbach’s
alphas and removed those items that limited the scale reliability. The main effect of scale was significant, F(1, 321) ⫽ 276.94,
All these items happened to have factor loadings smaller than .40; p ⬍ .001, ␩p2 ⫽ .463, 90% CI [.400, .517], with interrogation
thus, our two item-retention criteria converged. The process of suspicion scores (M ⫽ 53.37, SD ⫽ 8.01, SEM ⫽ 0.59) being
item deletion is detailed in Table 3 where, for each variation of the higher than generalized communicative suspicion scores (M ⫽
scale, the factor loading of each item is displayed along with the 36.35, SD ⫽ 10.60, SEM ⫽ 0.88). This reveals that officers’
scale reliability (alpha) if each item were removed. We can see that distrust is higher in situations specifically related to their profes-
an initial factor analysis revealed that Items 9 and 10 had factor sional role, such as police interrogations. A small but significant
loadings lower than required and scale reliability would increase if main effect for experience revealed that novice officers (M ⫽
they were removed (see Table 3). After removing these two items, 43.99, SD ⫽ 13.36, SEM ⫽ 1.11) scored lower than experienced
we obtained a unidimensional scale with 11 items and Cronbach’s officers (M ⫽ 47.32, SD ⫽ 11.62, SEM ⫽ 0.87), F(1, 321) ⫽ 4.08,
alpha ⫽ .73 (see Table 3). Alpha was remarkably similar to the p ⫽ .044, ␩p2 ⫽ .013, 90% CI [.0002, .040]. However, as expected,
figures reported by Levine and McCornack (1991) in validating this effect was qualified by a significant Scale ⫻ Experience
the original Generalized Communicative Suspicion Scale (.71 in interaction, F(1, 321) ⫽ 7.60, p ⫽ .006, ␩p2 ⫽ .023, 90% CI [.004,
Study 1 and .75 in Study 3). .057] (see Figure 1). Scheffé tests showed that although experi-
enced officers (M ⫽ 38.81, SD ⫽ 10.95, SEM ⫽ 1.29) scored
higher than did novice officers (M ⫽ 33.90, SD ⫽ 9.07, SEM ⫽
Data Analysis
1.14) on the Generalized Communicative Suspicion Scale (p ⫽
We expected that, unlike experienced and novice officers com- .001), scores of experienced (M ⫽ 53.06, SD ⫽ 8.00, SEM ⫽ 0.77)
pleting the Generalized Communicative Suspicion Scale (which and novice officers (M ⫽ 53.81, SD ⫽ 8.04, SEM ⫽ 0.93) did not
measures generalized suspicion), who differed in their scores, differ on the Interrogation Suspicion Scale (p ⫽ .585).

Table 3
Factor and Scale Reliability Analyses and Item Deletion Process for the Interrogation Suspicion Scale

All items Item 10 deleted Items 10 and 9 deleted


Alpha if item Alpha if item Alpha if item
Item M SD Factor loadings deleted Factor loadings deleted Factor loadings deleted

1 3.86 1.54 .505 .6863 .500 .7057 .513 .7098


2 4.03 1.43 .522 .6815 .519 .7009 .509 .7103
3 5.44 1.49 .507 .6906 .509 .7054 .511 .7121
4 5.02 1.40 .525 .6867 .525 .7026 .535 .7075
5 5.02 1.55 .457 .6956 .460 .7086 .451 .7187
6 4.36 1.47 .557 .6765 .551 .6972 .556 .7024
7 5.39 1.44 .452 .6901 .449 .7078 .440 .7172
8 4.92 1.38 .528 .6868 .530 .7008 .523 .7090
9 4.95 1.72 .298 .7126 .298 .7293 — —
10 2.12 1.18 .050 .7234 — — — —
11 4.95 1.24 .547 .6866 .549 .7017 .547 .7089
12 5.08 1.16 .595 .6857 .598 .6992 .610 .7031
13 5.30 1.27 .521 .6949 .526 .7075 .541 .7116
Cronbach’s alpha .7093 .7234 .7293
Note. The numbers in italics indicate that the corresponding items did not meet the retention criteria and needed to be removed.
OFFICERS’ GENERALIZED COMMUNICATIVE SUSPICION AND VERACITY JUDGMENTS 177

Prior research has found that police officers place more confi-
dence in their veracity judgments than do nonofficers (e.g., De-
Paulo & Pfeiffer, 1986; Garrido et al., 2004; Meissner & Kassin,
2002). To our knowledge, the question of whether experienced and
novice officers’ confidence scores differ has remained untapped.
We tackled this issue here and found that experienced officers are
more confident in their veracity judgments than are both novice
officers and nonofficers.
This pattern of results provides interesting insights into novice
and experienced officers’ behaviors and personalities. Generalized
communicative suspicion and confidence reflect the influence of
police work and police socialization on the officers’ personality.
However, deception judgments are context-dependent behaviors.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

We argued that in indisputably police-related situations (e.g., in


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

assessing the veracity of a testimony about a theft), novice officers


may embrace the stereotypical role of a “real” officer (e.g., by
showing distrust and skepticism); however, in more unspecific or
Figure 1. Novice and experienced officers’ generalized communicative ambiguous contexts (e.g., when filling in a scale about general
suspicion and interrogation suspicion scores. Error bars attached to each communicative suspicion), novice officers do not see the need to
column denote standard errors of the means. play that role. However, experienced officers behave in a distrust-
ful and skeptical way in both kinds of situations because this
behavior is an expression of their personalities.
General Discussion
Experiment 2 supported these notions. We built the Interroga-
Prior research has shown that the truth bias typically found in tion Suspicion Scale, which was parallel to the Generalized Com-
deception detection research vanishes or even reverses when de- municative Suspicion Scale except that it focused on a police-
ception raters are police officers rather than lay people (Alonso et relevant situation (i.e., interrogations). As predicted, unlike
al., 2009; C. F. Bond & DePaulo, 2006; Garrido & Masip, 1999; generalized communicative suspicion scores, which were lower for
Meissner & Kassin, 2002; Vrij, 2008). Nonetheless, the question novice than for experienced officers, interrogation suspicion scores
of whether novice and experienced officers differ in terms of did not differ between the two police groups, and were high for
veracity judgments has been largely unexplored. Here we found both groups. This effect was not due to a ceiling effect for the
that in assessing veracity, novice officers are as biased toward interrogation suspicion scores (i.e., of all participants giving ex-
deception as experienced officers. Conversely, nonofficers are tremely high scores on all Interrogation Suspicion Scale items), as
biased toward making truth judgments. average interrogation suspicion scores (just above 53) were still far
Also, prior research demonstrated that experienced officers dis- below the highest possible score (77) (see also the mean score for
play greater generalized communicative suspicion than both nov- each item, on a 1-to-7 scale, in Table 3). The Interrogation Sus-
ice officers and nonofficers (Hurst & Oswald, 2012; Masip et al., picion Scale was thus a sensitive instrument.
2005). Here we found support for this difference. Police officers’
daily involvement in competitive interactions and the progressive
Limitations
endorsement of police culture through the officers’ careers may
explain why experienced officers scored higher on the Generalized The current study is a laboratory study with no serious conse-
Communicative Suspicion Scale. Generalized communicative sus- quences for being caught lying (i.e., no threats of punishment were
picion was originally expected and found to predict lie judgments used to motivate senders). It has been argued that in real life, indi-
(Levine & McCornack, 1991); however, research with both lay viduals questioned by the police face much more serious conse-
people (McCornack & Levine, 1990) and prisoners (G. D. Bond & quences than in typical laboratory studies. This increased motivation
Lee, 2005; G. D. Bond, Malloy, et al., 2005; G. D. Bond, Thomp- to be believed would produce more deception cues in liars, which in
son, et al., 2005) raised questions about this relationship. Here we turn would increase observers’ accuracy (e.g., Carlucci, Compo, &
examined the relationship between generalized communicative Zimmerman, 2013; Wright Whelan, Wagstaff, & Wheatcroft, 2015).
suspicion and kind of judgment with lay people, novice officers However, researchers have also argued that in high stake situations,
and experienced officers. All effects were null. Hurst and Oswald even truth tellers might be afraid of being disbelieved and may thus
(2012) also found no significant impact of officers’ generalized come to show the stereotypical behavior of a liar (C. F. Bond &
communicative suspicion scores on judgments. Together, these DePaulo, 2006). Even though this might be particularly so among
results cast doubt on the usefulness of Levine and McCornack’s individuals pertaining to certain minority groups that may feel appre-
(1991) scale to predict lie judgments either among officers or hension of being perceived in terms of a negative stereotype (Naj-
among nonofficers (see Masip, 2014a). However, both the current dowski, 2011; see Fenn, Blandón-Gitlin, Pezdek, & Yoo, 2015, for
study and previous research (G. D. Bond, Thompson, et al., 2005; empirical support), truth tellers’ increased apprehension or fear might
Hurst & Oswald, 2012; Masip et al., 2005) show that generalized be a general phenomenon when the stakes or motivation to be be-
communicative suspicion scores appear to differentiate between lieved are high (see Hartwig, 2014). This phenomenon would make
presumably suspicious individuals (e.g., prisoners or experienced truth tellers look like liars, which would in turn increase the lie bias
officers) and ordinary individuals. (rather than increasing accuracy), particularly in judging truths. Does
178 MASIP, ALONSO, HERRERO, AND GARRIDO

the available empirical evidence support these contentions? In their This has a number of practical implications. No matter whether a
meta-analysis, C. F. Bond and DePaulo (2006) found that in the suspect is interviewed by an experienced or a novice officer, the
relatively few studies where motivation had been manipulated likelihood of this suspect’s statement being disbelieved is high re-
(within-study comparisons), stronger motivation to be believed led to gardless of actual veracity. This may set into motion a process that
higher accuracy rates. However, when comparing across the many may produce false confessions and eventually wrongful convictions.
studies with different motivation levels (between-study comparisons), According to Leo and Drizin (2010), the lie bias—which may lead
C. F. Bond and DePaulo found that higher motivation did not increase officers to erroneously decide that an innocent person is guilty—may
accuracy; rather, it reduced truth judgments, particularly when judges lead the police in some countries to use an accusatorial interrogation
had access to visible cues. This evidence suggests that the lie bias to obtain a confession. Unlike information-gathering interview ap-
would be stronger in real life (high stakes) compared with the present proaches, accusatorial approaches increase the risk of false confes-
experiment (low stakes). It is uncertain whether the increased stakes sions (Meissner et al., 2014). Therefore, the innocent suspect may end
and sender’s motivation would affect the judgments of officers and up confessing. Police detectives can also influence the post admission
nonofficers differently. narrative (Leo & Drizin, 2010). Finally, confessions have a huge
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

In a series of studies with real-life, high stake truths and lies told influence over conviction rates (Kassin & Neumann, 1997). Thus, the
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

by offenders, Mann and Vrij (e.g., Vrij, Mann, Robbins, & Rob- initial misclassification of an innocent suspect caused by police offi-
inson, 2006; see Alonso et al., 2009, for a comprehensive review) cers’ lie bias may have negative far-reaching consequences.
did not find any lie bias among officers (no nonofficers partici- To solve this problem, the police should use systematic lie-
pated in these studies). More recently, both Carlucci et al. (2013) detection tools that work better than their intuitions and gut feelings.
and Wright Whelan et al. (2015) found a stronger lie bias among If the officers’ veracity judgments were the result of using structured,
officers than among nonofficers judging high stakes, real-life, systematic, and valid procedures, then they would be less biased.
crime-related truths and lies. Therefore, we do not think the Recently, psychologists started to develop and test a number of
officers’ lie bias we found is limited to low stake, laboratory interviewing approaches to detect deception by creating or increasing
situations. We do agree, however, that more ecologically valid observable differences between truth tellers and liars (see Vrij &
studies are necessary in the deception detection domain and ap- Granhag, 2012, for a review). For example, because often lying is
plaud Carlucci et al.’s (2013) and Wright Whelan et al.’s (2015)
cognitively more demanding than truth telling (Gombos, 2006;
efforts in this direction.
Hauch, Blandón-Gitlin, Masip, & Sporer, 2015; Walczyk, Igou,
Another limitation of this study is that the Interrogation Suspicion
Dixon, & Tcholakian, 2013), further increasing the interviewees’
Scale (Experiment 2) was administered at a later time than the
cognitive demand will be more detrimental to liars than truth tellers,
Generalized Communicative Suspicion Scale. This time delay could
such that behavioral signs of cognitive load will be displayed by liars
presumably have influenced the results such that the lack of a signif-
(Vrij et al., 2008). One way of increasing cognitive load during the
icant difference between novice and experienced officers in Experi-
interview is asking interviewees to describe the target event in the
ment 2 could be a result of the data collection timing rather than a
reverse order rather than in the normal chronological order (Vrij et al.,
result of using the Interrogation Suspicion Scale instead of the Gen-
2008). Walczyk et al. (2005, 2012) developed a promising interview
eralized Communicative Suspicion Scale. We believe this explanation
procedure to detect deception based on a cognitive model of decep-
is extremely unlikely to account for our results. First, there is no
theoretical reason why a few months’ time should obliterate the tion. Also, depleting the cognitive resources of suspects before the
difference between the scores of novice and experienced officers. interview negatively affects lying behavior and improves lie detection
Second, the generalized communicative suspicion difference between (Blandon-Gitlin, Echon, & Pineda, 2013). Withholding the incrimi-
groups made sense theoretically and replicated prior findings by Hurst nating information that the interviewer possesses until the end of the
and Oswald (2012) and Masip et al. (2005); this shows that the effect interview may result in liars providing statements that contradict the
does not depend on the moment of data collection. Third, the pattern available evidence (Hartwig, Granhag, & Luke, 2014). Finally, asking
of results found in Experiment 2 was in line with our hypothesis. unexpected questions to pairs of suspects when interviewing them
separately results in liars giving different answers to the same ques-
tions; conversely, truth tellers, who simply describe the real event
Conclusions and Practical Implications honestly, provide the same answers (Vrij et al., 2009).
The current findings suggest that, over time, experienced officers If experienced officers were given specific guidelines based on
become more dispositionally suspicious and confident, presumably these evidence-based lie-detection approaches and could be con-
because of their exposure to competitive interactions, their socializa- vinced to closely follow them, and if novice officers internalized the
tion and experience within the police force, and their embracement of notion that “the right thing to do” when questioning someone’s
police culture. Thus, socialization and exposure to certain situations credibility in police-related situations is to use systematic scientific
may cause changes in the personality of officers. However, in this approaches (rather than being skeptical or distrustful), then the police
study generalized communicative suspicion (personality) was unre- lie bias would presumably decrease and detection accuracy would
lated with lie bias (behavior). Instead, situational factors appeared to increase. The influence of veteran officers’ personality on their be-
have a greater impact. Indeed, novice officers, who were not dispo- havior would be circumvented because veteran officers judging ve-
sitionally suspicious, made as many deception judgments as experi- racity could follow a set of available systematic guidelines instead of
enced officers. In police-related situations, novice officers were as having to resort to their overall impressions or gut feelings. Concern-
suspicious as experienced officers. These findings suggest that novice ing novice officers, their tendency to act “as an officer” in police-
officers have not yet acquired the inner characteristics of their expe- related situations would be beneficial if this implied using systematic
rienced peers but are able to behave like them. procedures rather than behaving in a distrustful way.
OFFICERS’ GENERALIZED COMMUNICATIVE SUSPICION AND VERACITY JUDGMENTS 179

Unfortunately, the limits of these new interviewing approaches Fenn, E., Blandón-Gitlin, I., Pezdek, K., & Yoo, A. (2015, March). “I’m
to detect deception and their vulnerability to countermeasures are innocent, but look guilty”: Ethnicity matters during a cognitive load
still not well known. Therefore, at present, their widespread use approach interview. Paper presented at the meeting of the American
may be premature (see Blandón-Gitlin, Fenn, Masip, & Yoo, Psychology-Law Society, San Diego, CA.
2014). However, they are promising tools for the future. It is our Garrido, E., & Masip, J. (1999). How good are police officers at spotting
hope that they will contribute to eradicate the police’s lie bias and lies? Forensic Update, 58, 14 –21.
will help improve the justice system. Garrido, E., Masip, J., & Herrero, C. (2004). Police officers’ credibility
judgments: Accuracy and estimated ability. International Journal of Psy-
chology, 39, 254 –275. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00207590344000411
References Goffman, E. (1959). The presentation of self in everyday life. New York,
Aamodt, M., & Custer, H. (2006). Who can best catch a liar? A meta- NY: Anchor Books.
analysis of individual differences in detecting deception. Forensic Ex- Goffman, E. (1961). Asylums: Essays on the social situation of mental
aminer, 16, 6 –11. patients and other inmates. New York, NY: Anchor Books.
Alonso, H., Masip, J., & Garrido, E. (2009). La capacidad de los policías Gombos, V. A. (2006). The cognition of deception: The role of executive pro-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

para detectar mentiras [Police officers’ ability to detect lies]. Revista de cesses in producing lies. Genetic, Social, and General Psychology Monographs,
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Derecho Penal y Criminología, 2, 159 –196. 132, 197–214. http://dx.doi.org/10.3200/MONO.132.3.197-214


Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action. Englewood Green, D. M., & Swets, J. A. (1966). Signal detection theory and psycho-
Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. physics. New York, NY: Wiley.
Barker, J. C. (1999). Danger, duty, and disillusion. The worldview of Los Hartwig, M. (2014, November 26). Telling lies: Fact, fiction, and non-
Angeles police officers. Prospect Heights, IL: Waveland Press. sense. Retrieved from https://www.psychologytoday.com/blog/living-
Blandón-Gitlin, I., Echon, R., & Pineda, C. (2013, June). Detecting decep- single/201411/telling-lies-fact-fiction-and-nonsense-maria-hartwig
tion: The benefit of depleting executive control in liars. Paper presented Hartwig, M., Granhag, P. A., & Luke, T. (2014). Strategic use of evidence
at the biannual meeting of the Society of Applied Research in Memory during investigative interviews: The state of the science. In D. C. Raskin,
& Cognition (SARMAC), Rotterdam, the Netherlands. C. R. Honts, & J. C. Kircher (Eds.), Credibility assessment: Scientific
Blandón-Gitlin, I., Fenn, E., Masip, J., & Yoo, A. H. (2014). Cognitive- research and applications (pp. 1–36). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
load approaches to detect deception: Searching for cognitive mecha- http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-394433-7.00001-4
nisms. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18, 441– 444. http://dx.doi.org/10 Hauch, V., Blandón-Gitlin, I., Masip, J., & Sporer, S. L. (2015). Are computers
.1016/j.tics.2014.05.004
effective lie detectors? A meta-analysis of linguistic cues to deception.
Bond, C. F., Jr., & DePaulo, B. M. (2006). Accuracy of deception judg-
Personality and Social Psychology Review, 19, 307–342. http://dx.doi.org/
ments. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 10, 214 –234. http://
10.1177/1088868314556539
dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15327957pspr1003_2
Hurst, M., & Oswald, M. (2012). Mechanisms underlying response bias in
Bond, G. D., & Lee, A. Y. (2005). The darkest side of trust: Validating the
generalized communication suspicion scale with prison inmates. Per- deception detection. Psychology, Crime & Law, 18, 759 –778. http://dx
sonality and Individual Differences, 38, 1429 –1438. http://dx.doi.org/ .doi.org/10.1080/1068316X.2010.550615
10.1016/j.paid.2004.09.008 Inbau, F. E., Reid, J. E., Buckley, J. P., & Jayne, B. C. (2013). Criminal
Bond, G. D., Malloy, D. M., Arias, E. A., Nunn, S. N., & Thompson, L. A. interrogation and confessions (5th ed.). Sudbury, MA: Jones and Bartlett.
(2005). Lie-biased decision making in prison. Communication Reports, Johnson, L. B., Todd, M., & Subramanian, G. (2005). Violence in police
18, 9 –19. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/08934210500084180 families: Work-family spillover. Journal of Family Violence, 20, 3–12.
Bond, G. D., Thompson, L. A., & Malloy, D. M. (2005). Vulnerability of http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10896-005-1504-4
older adults to deception in prison and nonprison contexts. Psychology Kassin, S. M., & Gudjonsson, G. H. (2004). The psychology of confessions. A
and Aging, 20, 60 –70. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.20.1.60 review of the literature and issues. Psychological Science in the Public
Bull, R., & Soukara, S. (2010). Four studies of what really happens in Interest, 5, 33– 67. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1529-1006.2004.00016.x
police interviews. In G. D. Lassiter & C. A. Meissner (Eds.), Police Kassin, S. M., & Neumann, K. (1997). On the power of confession
interrogations and false confessions: Current research, practice, and evidence: An experimental test of the fundamental difference hypothe-
policy recommendations (pp. 81–95). Washington, DC: American Psy- sis. Law and Human Behavior, 21, 469 – 484. http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/
chological Association. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/12085-005 A:1024871622490
Caplan, J. (2003). Police cynicism: Police survival tool? The Police Jour- Leo, R. A. (1996). Inside the interrogation room. The Journal of Criminal
nal, 76, 304 –313. http://dx.doi.org/10.1350/pojo.76.4.304.25821 Law & Criminology, 86, 266 –303. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/1144028
Carlucci, M. E., Compo, N. S., & Zimmerman, L. (2013). Lie detection Leo, R. A., & Drizin, S. A. (2010). The three errors: Pathways to false confession
during high-stakes truths and lies. Legal and Criminological Psychol- and wrongful conviction. In G. D. Lassiter & C. A. Meissner (Eds.), Police
ogy, 18, 314 –323. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8333.2012.02064.x
interrogations and false confessions. Current research, practice, and
Davies, C. (1989). Goffman’s concept of the total institution: Criticisms
policy recommendations (pp. 9 –30). Washington, DC: American Psy-
and revisions. Human Studies, 12, 77–95. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/
chological Association. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/12085-001
BF00142840
Levine, T. R., & McCornack, S. A. (1991). The dark side of trust:
DePaulo, B. M., Charlton, K., Cooper, H., Lindsay, J. J., & Muhlenbruck,
L. (1997). The accuracy-confidence correlation in the detection of de- Conceptualizing and measuring types of communicative suspicion.
ception. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 1, 346 –357. http:// Communication Quarterly, 39, 325–339. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/
dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15327957pspr0104_5 01463379109369809
DePaulo, B. M., & Pfeiffer, R. L. (1986). On-the-job experience and skill Levine, T. R., Park, H. S., & McCornack, S. A. (1999). Accuracy in detecting
at detecting deception. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 16, 249 – truths and lies: Documenting the “veracity effect.” Communication Mono-
267. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1559-1816.1986.tb01138.x graphs, 66, 125–144. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/03637759909376468
Donaldson, W. (1992). Measuring recognition memory. Journal of Exper- Masip, J. (2014a). Generalized communicative suspicion. In T. Levine
imental Psychology: General, 121, 275–277. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/ (Ed.), Encyclopedia of deception (Vol. 1, pp. 404 – 406). Thousand
0096-3445.121.3.275 Oaks, CA: Sage. http://dx.doi.org/10.4135/9781483306902.n156
180 MASIP, ALONSO, HERRERO, AND GARRIDO

Masip, J. (2014b). Investigator bias. In T. Levine (Ed.), Encyclopedia of Scaramella, G. L., Cox, S. M., & McCamey, W. P. (2010). Introduction to
deception (Vol. 2, pp. 538 –540). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. http://dx policing. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
.doi.org/10.4135/9781483306902.n203 Skolnick, J. H. (1966). Justice without trial: Law enforcement in demo-
Masip, J., Alonso, H., Garrido, E., & Antón, C. (2005). Generalized cratic society. New York, NY: Wiley.
communicative suspicion (GCS) among police officers: Accounting for Skolnick, J. H. (1994). Justice without trial: Law enforcement in demo-
the investigator bias effect. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 35, cratic society (3rd ed.). New York, NY: Wiley.
1046 –1066. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1559-1816.2005.tb02159.x Street, C. N. H., & Masip, J. (2015). The source of the truth bias: Heuristic
Masip, J., Garrido, E., & Herrero, C. (2006). Observers’ decision moment processing? Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 56, 254 –263. http://
in deception detection experiments: Its impact on judgment, accuracy, dx.doi.org/10.1111/sjop.12204
and confidence. International Journal of Psychology, 41, 304 –319. Swets, J. A., Dawes, R. M., & Monahan, J. (2000). Psychological science
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00207590500343612 can improve diagnostic decisions. Psychological Science in the Public
Masip, J., Garrido, E., & Herrero, C. (2009). Heuristic versus systematic Interest, 1, 1–26. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1529-1006.001
processing of information in detecting deception: Questioning the truth
Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2013). Using multivariate statistics (6th
bias. Psychological Reports, 105, 11–36. http://dx.doi.org/10.2466/PR0
ed.). Boston, MA: Pearson.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

.105.1.11-36
Van Maanen, J. E. (1975). Breaking in: A consideration of organizational
Masip, J., Garrido, E., & Herrero, C. (2010). Regression toward the mean
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

socialization. In R. Dubin (Ed.), Handbook of work, organization, and


or heuristic processing in detecting deception? Reply to Elaad (2010).
society (pp. 407– 418). Chicago, IL: Rand-McNally.
Psychological Reports, 107, 587–592. http://dx.doi.org/10.2466/03.07
Vrij, A. (2008). Detecting lies and deceit. Pitfalls and opportunities.
.PR0.107.5.587-592
Masip, J., Garrido, E., Herrero, C., Antón, C., & Alonso, H. (2006). Chichester, UK: Wiley.
Officers as lie detectors. Guilty before charged. In D. Chadee & J. Vrij, A., & Granhag, P. A. (2012). Eliciting cues to deception and truth: What
Young (Eds.), Current themes in social psychology (pp. 187–205). matters are the question asked. Journal of Applied Research in Memory &
Mona, Jamaica: The University of the West Indies Press. Cognition, 1, 110 –117. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jarmac.2012.02.004
McCornack, S. A., & Levine, T. R. (1990). When lovers become leery: The Vrij, A., Leal, S., Granhag, P. A., Mann, S., Fisher, R. P., Hillman, J., &
relationship between suspicion and accuracy in detecting deception. Sperry, K. (2009). Outsmarting the liars: The benefit of asking unantic-
Communication Monographs, 57, 219 –230. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/ ipated questions. Law and Human Behavior, 33, 159 –166. http://dx.doi
03637759009376197 .org/10.1007/s10979-008-9143-y
McNamara, R. P. (1999). The socialization of the police. In D. J. Kenney Vrij, A., Mann, S. A., Fisher, R. P., Leal, S., Milne, R., & Bull, R. (2008).
& R. P. McNamara (Eds.), Police and policing. Contemporary issues Increasing cognitive load to facilitate lie detection: The benefit of
(pp. 1–12). Westport, CT: Greenwood. recalling an event in reverse order. Law and Human Behavior, 32,
Meissner, C. A., & Kassin, S. M. (2002). “He’s guilty!” Investigator bias 253–265. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10979-007-9103-y
in judgments of truth and deception. Law and Human Behavior, 26, Vrij, A., Mann, S., Robbins, E., & Robinson, M. (2006). Police officers
469 – 480. http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/A:1020278620751 ability to detect deception in high stakes situations and in repeated lie
Meissner, C. A., Redlich, A. D., Michael, S. W., Evans, J., Camilletti, detection test. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 20, 741–755. http://dx.doi
C. R., Bhatt, S., & Brandon, S. (2014). Accusatorial and information- .org/10.1002/acp.1200
gathering interrogation methods and their effects on true and false Waddington, P. A. J. (1999). Police (canteen) sub-culture. An appreciation.
confessions: A meta-analytic review. Journal of Experimental Criminol- The British Journal of Criminology, 39, 287–309. http://dx.doi.org/10
ogy, 10, 459 – 486. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11292-014-9207-6 .1093/bjc/39.2.287
Nahari, G. (2012). Elaborations on credibility judgments by professional Walczyk, J. J., Griffith, D. A., Yates, R., Visconte, S. R., Simoneaux, B.,
lie detectors and laypersons: Strategies of judgment and justification. & Harris, L. L. (2012). Lie detection by inducing cognitive load. Eye
Psychology, Crime & Law, 18, 567–577. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/ movements and other cues to the false answers of “witnesses” to crimes.
1068316X.2010.511222
Criminal Justice and Behavior, 39, 887–909. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/
Najdowski, C. J. (2011). Stereotype threat in criminal interrogations: Why inno-
0093854812437014
cent Black suspects are at risk for confessing falsely. Psychology, Public Policy,
Walczyk, J. J., Igou, F. P., Dixon, A. P., & Tcholakian, T. (2013).
and Law, 17, 562–591. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0023741
Advancing lie detection by inducing cognitive load on liars: A review of
O’Neill, M., Marks, M., & Singh, A. (Eds.). (2007). Police occupational
relevant theories and techniques guided by lessons from polygraph-
culture: New debates and directions. New York, NY: Emerald.
based approaches. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 14.
Paoline, E. A., III. (2003). Taking stock: Toward a richer understanding of
police culture. Journal of Criminal Justice, 31, 199 –214. http://dx.doi Walczyk, J. J., Schwartz, J. P., Clifton, R., Adams, B., Wei, M., & Zha, P.
.org/10.1016/S0047-2352(03)00002-3 (2005). Lying person to person about life events: A cognitive framework
Paoline, E. A., III. (2014). Police culture. In G. Bruinsma & D. Weisburd for lie detection. Personnel Psychology, 58, 141–170. http://dx.doi.org/
(Eds.), Encyclopedia of criminology and criminal justice (pp. 3577– 10.1111/j.1744-6570.2005.00484.x
3586). New York, NY: Springer. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4614- Westley, W. A. (1970). Violence and the police: A sociological study of
5690-2_394 law, custom, and morality. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Robinson, W. P. (1996). Deceit, delusion, and detection. Thousand Oaks, Wright Whelan, C., Wagstaff, G., & Wheatcroft, J. M. (2015). High stakes lies:
CA: Sage. Police and non-police accuracy in detecting deception. Psychology, Crime &
Rubinstein, J. (1973). City police. New York, NY: Ballantine Books. Law, 21, 127–138. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/1068316X.2014.935777
Sato, M. (2003). Police recruits’ training and the socialisation process: Yüksel, Y., & Tosun, H. (2015). Understanding organizational socializa-
From the network perspective. The Police Journal, 76, 289 –303. http:// tion process of police officers and job satisfaction. Electronic Journal of
dx.doi.org/10.1350/pojo.76.4.289.25826 Social Sciences, 14, 170 –182.
OFFICERS’ GENERALIZED COMMUNICATIVE SUSPICION AND VERACITY JUDGMENTS 181

Appendix

Generalized Communication Suspicion Scale and Interrogation Suspicion Scale

Generalized Communication Suspicion Scale Interrogation Suspicion Scale

1. Everyone lies, the person who says that they don’t is the biggest 1. Every suspect lies during an interrogation, the suspect who says
liar of all that they don’t is the biggest liar of allⴱ
2. I often feel as if people aren’t being completely truthful with meⴱ 2. When in my daily work I request information that may somehow
be related to a crime I feel that that people aren’t being completely
truthful with meⴱ
3. Most people only tell you what they think you want to hearⴱ —
4. When I am in a conversation with someone, I frequently wonder 3. When I am interrogating someone, I frequently wonder whether
whether they are really telling me the truthⴱ they are really telling me the truthⴱ
5. People rarely tell you what they’re really thinkingⴱ 4. People rarely tell you what they’re really thinking when they have
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

to answer a police officers’ questions about their potential


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

involvement in specific criminal activitiesⴱ


6. The best policy is to trust people until proven wrong 5. The best policy as a police officer is to trust the people one is
interrogating until proven wrongⴱ
7. Dishonesty is a part of human natureⴱ 6. Dishonesty is a part of human nature when people are submitted to
a police interrogationⴱ
8. When I first meet someone, I assume that they are probably lying 7. When I know someone from prior interrogations, I assume that
to me about some thingsⴱ they are probably lying to me about some thingsⴱ
9. Most people are basically honestⴱ 8. Most people are basically honest when an officer interrogates them
about certain actions they are suspected of having committedⴱ
10. Anyone who completely trusts someone else is asking for troubleⴱ 9. Any police officer who completely trusts a suspect’s statement is
asking for trouble
11. When I ask a stranger for directions, I frequently wonder whether 10. When an officer is on duty and asks a stranger for directions, the
they are being truthful officer frequently should wonder whether the stranger is being
truthful
12. When I am talking to others, I tend to believe what they sayⴱ 11. When I am interrogating a suspect, I tend to believe what they sayⴱ
13. People seldom lie to meⴱ 12. People whom I have to interrogate seldom lie to meⴱ
14. Most people follow the saying “honesty is the best policy”ⴱ 13. Most suspects who know they are going to be interrogated follow
the saying “honesty is the best policy”ⴱ
Note. Asterisks denote items retained in Masip et al.’s (2005) Generalized Communication Suspicion Scale or in the current Interrogation Suspicion Scale.
For both scales, agreement with each item is measured on a 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree) scale. Overall score is obtained by adding up the
ratings across all items after reversing ratings for items 6, 9, 12, 13, and 14 (Generalized Communication Suspicion Scale) or 5, 8, 11, 12, and 13
(Interrogation Suspicion Scale).
In creating the Interrogation Suspicion Scale, we could not adapt Item 3 of the Generalized Communication Suspicion Scale: “Most people only tell you
what they think you want to hear.” The higher the scores on this item, the more the respondent believes others lie. The version of this item for the
Interrogation Suspicion Scale would read, “During an interrogation, most suspects only tell you what they think you want to hear.” However, it may seem
far too obvious to most potential respondents that during a police interview most suspects do not tell the interviewer what he or she wants to hear (i.e.,
that they are guilty, which the officer thinks is the truth). Instead, suspects tend deny the police accusations and are reluctant to confess. Therefore, in the
Interrogation Suspicion Scale, the higher the scores on this item, the more the respondent believes others (here, suspects) tell the truth. So, the two versions
of the same item would measure opposite things in each scale. This posed a serious problem.
Some may argue that confession research shows that suspects readily tell the police what they want to hear (i.e., that suspects readily confess no matter
whether they are guilty or innocent). However, legal safeguards in Spain prevent the police from using confrontational interviewing approaches, which are
the ones that might increase the false confessions rate (Meissner et al., 2014). Also, Leo’s (1996) field research on police interrogations revealed that 36%
of suspects made no incriminating statement at all, and that only 24% made a full confession. More recently, Bull and Soukara (2010) reported real suspects’
confession rates of 39% (Study 2) and 38% (Study 3). Notwithstanding the fact that false confessions are a legitimate serious concern, the data suggest that
few officers would endorse the notion that suspects readily tell the interviewer what the interviewer wants to hear (i.e., a full confession). Even if a suspect
eventually confesses, she or he often first denies his/her involvement in the crime (Bull & Soukara, 2010).

Received April 2, 2015


Revision received October 9, 2015
Accepted October 29, 2015 䡲

View publication stats

You might also like