You are on page 1of 10

Modelling in Science Lessons: Are There Better Ways

to Learn With Models?

Allan G. Harrison David F. Treagust


Central Queensland University Curtin University of Technology

Modelling is the essence of scientific thinking, andmodels are both the methods and products of science.
However, secondary students usually view science models as toys or miniatures of real-life objects, and
few students actually understand why scientists use multiple models to explain concepts. A conceptual
typology of models is presented and explained to help teachers select models appropriate to the cognitive
ability of their students. An example explains how the systematic presentation of analogical models
enhanced an llth-grade chemistry student’s understanding of atoms and molecules. The article
recommends that teachers encourage their students to use and explore multiple models in science lessons
at all levels.

The ways in which students use models to leam the word model, they must ask the questions, "Is it
science and mathematics have interested teachers and concrete or abstract?" "Is it a concept or a behavior?"
researchers for over 30 years (Black, 1962, Hesse, If teachers and researchers have to stop and ask which
1963). Two recent papers in this journal (Hodgson, way the term model is being used, imagine how con-
1995; Hodgson & Harpster, 1997) address the question fused teenage students must be! Teachers know what
ofwhat modelling is. As Hodgson and Harpster (1997) they mean when they talk about models, but research
explain, classroom modelling can be either a multistep shows that students do not (e.g., Gilbert & Boulter,
problem solving process or it can be a specific model, 1998; Harrison & Treagust, 1996).
like a graph or an equation. However, many more Therefore, this paper explores the ways model and
models than these are used in science and mathematics, modelling are used in science lessons and the ways in
and the school modelling spectrum includes both im- which secondary students understand the models fea-
plicit and explicit models. The implicit iconic symbols tured in textbooks and their teacher’s explanations. In
used each day in mathematics and science (e.g., y = x2 trying to make sense of models and modelling, the
NaCI) are models, because they represent functions, paper has two interests: It proposes that modelling is a
variables, particles, and processes. Indeed, some math- sophisticated thinking process that should be an ex-
ematical process symbols and chemical formulae (e.g., plicit feature of the science curriculum, and it argues
H^O) have been used so frequently for so long that they that teachers should be sensitive to the similarities and
have become part of the language of mathematics and differences between the models they use in their peda-
science. At the explicit level, science often uses con- gogical content explanations.
cept-building analogical models like scale models,
pedagogical analogical models, maps and diagrams, Models Representing Reality
mathematical and theoretical models, and simulations
to represent objects, ideas, and processes. There are good reasons to believe that many sci-
In education, the terms model and modelling can be ence students view models as reality and that student
quite ambiguous: a model may represent a concrete modelling often is more algorithmic than relational. It
object or a process (e.g., a model heart or a chemical is likely that this view also applies to mathematical
bond), an algorithm (e.g., computer programming syn- problem solving models. However, research conducted
tax), a problem solving process (e.g., factoring a qua- by Finster (1991) and Perry (1970) showed that stu-
dratic equation) or a teaching-learning process (like the dents can leam to think critically and creatively. Simi-
teaching-with-analogies model, Glynn, 1991). When larly, empirical studies in secondary science classes
the terms model and modelling are used in unqualified have shown that students can leam to think in sophis-
ways in teaching and research contexts, semantic and ticated ways at an earlier age than was previously
real confusion can result. When teachers read or hear thought possible (Harrison & Treagust, in press). In

School Science and Mathematics


Modelling in Science Lessons

this study 1 Ith-grade chemistry students who became desired concept learning almost always lies in the
creative multiple modellers realized that no model is systematic process similarities, and students usually
wholly right and appreciated that science is more about need guidance in mapping these relationships (Gentner,
process thinking than object description. 1983; Zook, 1991). This helps explain Glynn’s (1991)
claim that analogies are "two-edged swords," because
Modelling in Science some students map the surface analogy instead of the
Various studies show that school students and systematic process analogy, or the invalid rather than
some teachers think about scientific models in me- the valid attributes.
chanical terms and believe that models are true pictures
of nonobservable phenomena and ideas. (Abell & Modelling in School Science Lessons
Roth, 1995; Gilbert, 1991). But models are not "right How, then, can teachers describe or explain atoms,
answers." They are scientists’ and teachers’ attempts genes, chemical reactions, electricity, weather pat-
to represent difficult and abstract phenomena in every- terns, or continental drift without using one or more
day terms for the benefit of their students. John Gilbert models? Teachers consistently use models to explain
(1993) well stated the case by saying that models are immaterial processes, like equilibrium (e.g., a bal-
simultaneously "one of the main products of science," anced seesaw), and nonobservable entities, like elec-
important "elements] in scientific methodology," and trons flowing in a wire (e.g., a water circuit). Is it
"major learning [and teaching] tools in science educa- possible to explain the flow of energy and matter
tion" (pp. 9-10). Even the renowned physicist, Richard through an ecosystem without using a food web, a food
Feynman (1994) found it quite impossible to explain pyramid, or a carbon cycle? Can students understand
concepts in his physics lectures without constructing the circulation of blood, the solar system, or chemical
and using models. Similarly, many famous scientists families without using diagrammatic models? Teach-
have written popular books about their scientific expe- ers consistently use ecological, anatomical, and astro-
riences and discoveries, and each of these stories used nomical diagrams and the periodic table to teach these
models in exactly the ways proposed by Gilbert (1993). concepts. Indeed, what do teachers do when they see
Probably the best example is Watson’s (1968) The the worried looks on their students’ faces in the middle
Double Helix, wherein he attributed Crick’s and his ofexplanating an abstract concept? They reach for an
success to model building and model-based thinking. analogy or a model.
But modelling was not their original idea: The tradition Curiously, many teachers are wary ofverbal analo-
rests with great model builders like Maxwell and gies (Glynn, 1991) and do not use them often (Treagust,
Pauling. Duit, Joslin, & Lindauer, 1992), yet they use physical
models, diagrams, and iconic symbols on a daily basis.
Analogical Modelling Perhaps teachers are conscious of the unreliable way
Of the models used to represent science concepts, students interpret spoken analogies because of their
analogical models are frequently used to model macro- immaterial form. However, the common occurrence of
scopic, microscopic, and symbolic entities. Analogical models in textbooks, in classroom displays, and as
models can be concrete (e.g., atoms represented as lesson "motivators" attests to teachers’ and curriculum
balls, Keenan, Kleinfelter & Wood, 1980), abstract (a writers’ willingness to use models. Maybe the concrete
simple tube for an earthworm’s gut, Ogbom, Kress, form of many models desensitizes teachers and writers
Martins, & McGillicuddy, 1996), or mixed (a ball-and- to the insecurity felt by students when faced with many
stick molecular model, Keenan et al., 1980). Analogi- different models (Bent, 1984; Carr, 1984). To help
cal models are always simplified and enhanced in some teachers understand model differences, a typology of
way to emphasize the attributes shared between the the concept-building analogical models used in sci-
analog model and the target concept. Despite careful ence lessons was developed and is described under the
planning to reduce the unshared attributes, analogical next heading. The typology aims to do three things: (a)
models always "break down" somewhere, because it describes the similarities and differences inherent in the
there are always some analog attributes that do not models that teachers use in their lessons; (b) it attempts to
apply to the target. Two types of analogy operate alertteachers and writers to the variety ofmodels that may
between the analog model and the target concept: confuse some of their audience; and (c), it generally
surface similarities that quickly attract students to the orders the model types in increasing conceptual diffi-
intended analogy, and deep systematic process simi- culty. Recommendations for enhancing the teaching of
larities that develop conceptual understanding. The these models accompany most of the explanations.

Volume 98(8), December 1998


Modelling in Science Lessons

A Typology of Concept-building Analogical gas volume, temperature, and pressure, belong to this
Models category. Also, simplifying kinetic theory particles as
spheres qualifies them as pedagogical analogical mod-
Concrete and Concrete/Abstract Models Designed to els. Some phenomena may belong to, or contain, both
Represent Reality theoretical and mathematical models. Whenever pos-
Scale models. Scale models of animals, plants, sible, then, students and teachers should negotiate
cars, and boats are often used to depict colors, external qualitative explanations of theoretical models.
shape, and structure. Such models carefully reflect
external proportions but rarely show internal structure, Models Depicting Multiple Concepts and/or Processes
functions, and use, nor are they made of the same Maps, diagrams, and tables. These models repre-
materials as the target. Also, size-for-size, a scale sent patterns, pathways, and relationships easily visu-
model bridge is stronger than the actual bridge (Hewitt, alized by students. Examples are the periodic table,
1987, pp. 259-263)! Teachers need to highlight this phylogenetic trees, weather maps, circuit diagrams,
difference and the unshared attributes of scale models, metabolic pathways, blood circulation, nervous sys-
because scale models look so realistic. tems, pedigrees, food chains, webs, and pyramids.
Pedagogical analogical models. These are the Note that the simplifiedand enhanced nature of parts of
concrete models that teachers often use to depict ab- these diagrams make them two-dimensional models,
stract or nonobserveable entities like atoms and mol- and individual students interpret diagram items and
ecules. One or more target attributes dominate the colors in different ways.
analog’s concrete structure; e.g., ball-and-stick and Concept-process model. Most science concepts
space-filling molecular models, or a simple tube repre- are processes rather than objects. Teachers explain
senting an earthworm’s gut (Ogbom et al., 1996). immaterial processes to students (most of whom think
Because these analogical models reflect point-by- in concrete terms) using concept-process models like
point correspondences between the analog and the the multiple models of acid and bases and oxidation-
target for a limited set of attributes, they can be grossly reduction. Further, the only explanation available for
oversimplified to highlight conceptual attributes. Such the refraction of light uses concept-process models
oversimplifications should be carefully discussed with like a pair of wheels crossing a hard-soft interface
the students. (Hewitt, 1987), marching soldiers, and rolling balls.
The analogical, concrete, and dynamic nature of these
Abstract Models Designed to Communicate Theory analogies means that they integrate multiple peda-
Iconic and symbolic models. Chemical formulae gogical analogical, symbolic, theoretical, and math-
and chemical equations are symbolic models of com- ematical models.
pound composition and chemical reactions, respec- Simulations. A unique category of multiple dy-
tively. Formulae and equations are so embedded in namic models is simulation. Simulations model
chemistry’s language that school students and nonspe- highly sophisticated processes, like aircraft flight,
cialist teachers mistake them for reality when they are, global warming, nuclear reactions and accidents,
in fact, explanatory and communicative models. and population fluctuations. Simulations let nov-
Mathematicalmodels. Physicalproperties, changes, ices and researchers develop and hone skills without
and processes (e.g., k = PV, F = ma), can be represented risking life and property and increasingly include
as mathematical equations and graphs that elegantly "virtual reality" experiences; for instance, com-
depict conceptual relationships (e.g., Boyle’s Law, puter games and computer-based interactive multi-
exponential decays, etc.). However, F = ma only func- media employing stylized and real-life situations.
tions in frictionless situations, which never exist in As with scale models and pedagogical analogical
classrooms; therefore, the ideal nature of these models models, the analogical nature and unshared attributes
should always be discussed with students. It is also of simulations are easily missed.
important that students construct, for themselves, quali-
tative explanations for these mathematical models. Learning with Models
Theoretical models. Analogical representations of Models can only act as aids to memory, explana-
electromagnetic lines of force and photons are theo- tory tools, and learning devices if they are easily
retical, because the models are human constructions understood and remembered by students. Analogical
describing well-grounded theoretical entities. Theo- models need to be familiar, logical, and useful to the
retical explanations, like the kinetic theory model of students. Fruitful application seems to be strongest

School Science and Mathematics


Modelling in Science Lessons

when students generate their own analogies; however, modellers because these students believed there is a
reports of student-generated analogies are rare, and one-to-one correspondence between models and real-
only Cosgrove (1995) reports success at this level. ity (models are toys or small incomplete copies of
Students more easily map self-generated analogies actual objects), models should be "right," and items are
than teacher-supplied analogies, because their per- missing because the modeller wanted the model that
sonal analogies are more familiar and easier to under- way. Students also did not look for ideas or purposes in
stand (Zook, 1991). However, students find it hard to the model’s form. Some secondary students achieved
generate or select appropriate analogies for a given Level 2, in which models fundamentally remain real-
problem and are more likely to apply an analogy or world objects or events rather than representations of
model to a problem when the teacher supplies the ideas, models are incomplete or different depending on
analog, even though they find mapping it difficult. This the context, and the model’s main purpose is commu-
highlights the need for teachers to systematically plan nication rather than the exploration of ideas. Experts
model and analogy use in their lessons and recom- alone satisfied Level 3 criteria, believing that models
mends the use of an approach involving the focus, should be multiple, models are thinking tools, and models
action, and reflection (FAR) aspects of expert teaching can be purposefully manipulated by the modeller to suit
(Treagust, Harrison & Venville, 1998). Focus involves epistemological needs. Some students fell into mixed
prelesson planning, in which the teacher focuses on the Level 1/2 and 2/3 classifications. Because the levels are
concept’s difficulty, the students’ prior knowledge and derived from the ways students described, explained, and
ability, and the analog model’s familiarity. Action used models, the levels also provide useful information
deals with the in-lesson presentation of the familiar about the status of students’ conceptual development.
analogy or model and stresses the need to coopera-
tively map the shared and unshared attributes. Reflec- Concept-building Analogical Models
tion is the postlesson evaluation of the analogy’s or All the models described in the typology are con-
model’s effectiveness and identifies modifications cept-building analogical models, because they represent
necessary for subsequent lessons or next time the aspects ofactual science objects and processes. Analogi-
analogy or model is used. The FAR guide for system- cal models range from "concrete" scale models (like
atically presenting analogies and models is summa- model cars and boats) to highly "abstract" theoretical
rized in Appendix A. models (like magnetic fields and the kinetic theory). As
It also is important to recognize that effective observed earlier, inexperienced modellers who can un-
analogical learning requires more than systematic pre- derstand pedagogical analogical models like a model
sentations by the teacher. Studies claim that conceptual heart or eye should not be expected to understand mag-
understanding is maximized when relevant analogical netic field models without much more experience and
models are socially discussed and negotiated (Treagust, help. Yet even elementary and middle school science
Harrison, Venville, & Dagher, 1993). Searching for in- textbooks introduce and use the magnetic field meta-
class consensus is scientific in the sense that it models phor and rarely explain its origin or meaning. Students
what communities of scientists do: They argue and should not be expected to understand theoretical mod-
negotiate meaning. However, classroom negotiation els simply because curriculum materials and teachers
will not construct scientists’ knowledge per se, be- use them in descriptions and explanations!
cause there are vast differences between the prior A concrete-concrete/abstract-abstract continuum
knowledge and experiences of scientists and students. for classifying the cognitive demands of models is
Still, negotiation does help students construct the sci- useful only if it encourages teachers and writers to
ence understanding expected by the school and their think about the modelling experience and expertise of
teacher. their audience. Grosslight et al. (1991) found that most
students up to and including 10th grade are Level 1 or
Student Modelling Abilities Level 1/2 modellers; that is, they are concrete or
Students are poorer modellers than teachers ex- occasionally concrete/abstract modellers. These stu-
pect, and younger secondary students usually do not dents believe that a one-to-one correspondence exists
look further than a model’s surface similarities. between the model and reality. While these students
Grosslight, Unger, Jay, and Smith (1991) studied stu- see differences between each model and reality, they
dent-expert modelling abilities in terms of students’ cannot give reasons for their ideas, nor do they search
beliefs about the structure and purpose of models. They for reasons to explain the obvious differences between
classified many lower secondary students as Level 1 the analog and its target.

Volume 98(8), December 1998


Modelling in Science Lessons

Concept-process Modelling molecules, forces and nerve circuits), because each


The most abstract models are concept-process model elaborates but a fraction of the target’s at-
models. These are process thinking models for under- tributes. In many cases, the sum of the models is less
standing and applying important concepts, like physi- than the whole phenomenon for two reasons: (a) the
cal and chemical equilibrium, biological classifica- concept itself is not fully understood, and (b) the
tion, and current flow in network circuits. Carr (1984) models tend to overlap. There are sound reasons why
pointed out that concept-process models, like the three no single model can fully illustrate an object or process.
models of acids they are sour and react with metals If it did, it would be an example not a model (Bent,
to produce hydrogen, Arrhenius acids produce H+ ions, 1984). Expert teachers mostly use models to stress and
and Bronsted-Lowrey acids are proton donors con- explore important and difficult aspects of a concept,
fuse many chemistry students. Some of the models and this is best achieved by oversimplifying the model
used in different parts of the science syllabus are even to emphasize key ideas (e.g., the simple tube for an
contradictory; for example, the use of conventional earthworm’s gut). A series of simplified models can be
current (a flow of positive charge) in physics clashes used to explain, one at a time, the key ideas. Multiple
with the flow of negative electrons used in electro- simplified models also signal to students that no indi-
chemistry. And then there is the conflict between the vidual model is "right."
four models of oxidation-reduction. Which is oxida- Nearly every textbook we have examined, how-
tion: gain of oxygen, loss of hydrogen, increase in ever, failed to warn its readers that models are human
oxidation number, or loss of electrons? Each model inventions that break down at some point. Teachers
describes oxidation, but often students cannot under- may assume that their students understand the limits of
stand why the teacher has introduced another model with models; but Grosslight et al. (1991) showed that this
an opposite action (loss instead of gain) for the same beliefis too ambitious. This raises a major thinking and
process. Maybe we should be more surprised when learning problem for students. Students need time and
students are not confused by this model swapping! help in coming to realize that models are contrived and
limited representations of reality. According to
Summary Grosslight et al., the legitimacy of multiple scientific
The preceding evidence suggests that teachers models is a function of epistemological expertise;
may enhance their students’ learning by using the however, middle school students are usually Level 1
model typology to assess the conceptual demands of modellers who believe that one model is right. It is not
the analogical models they plan to use in their lessons. surprising, then, that students are perplexed when
From an Ausubelian perspective, model-based learn- teachers and textbooks at this level move from one
ing should be most effective when learning builds on model to another without explanation. Inexperienced
what the student already knows. If this be the case, students believe that the teacher knows the right model,
introducing complex maps and diagrams, simulations, and the trick for them is to discover which model is
and concept-process models containing multiple simple right (Perry, 1970). Yet, modelling that is multiple,
models before the students have mastered the analogi- flexible, purposeful, and relational is the essence of
cal nature of the simpler models will be detrimental. scientific thought (e.g., Gilbert, 1993), although the
Research supports teaching students simple model ability to model in these ways is rarely found in schools
forms before advancing to the more difficult and ab- students. The pressing question for school science
stract models. Learning to model also should be overtly education is "How can students with naive and realist
social and involve discussion and negotiation of mean- world views be encouraged to progressively adopt
ing, because this provides the best opportunity for each expert modelling skills?"
student to construct the desired knowledge. Such an This is why the typology of school science models
approach provides formative feedback to students, while is useful. The typology outlines the level of conceptual
helping teachers monitor their students’ learning. difficulty inherent in each model type, and the model
types are generally ordered in terms of increasing
Multiple Explanatory Models conceptual demand. Awareness of these demands
should encourage teachers to match the model types
Many science concepts depend on multiple models they choose to use in their lessons to their students’
for their description and explanation. The more ab- cognitive ability. As an aid to teachers, the FAR guide
stract and nonobservable a phenomenon, the more is a systematic framework within which teachers can
likely it will require multiple models (e.g., atoms and structure their students’ model-based learning. Another

School Science and Mathematics


Modelling in Science Lessons

issue of importance is whether teachers should teach multimedia simulations have the potential to make
with models situated at the students’ intellectual level topics like equilibrium more understandable at the
or higher than the students’ intellectual level. Finster particle level. Modem chemistry simply cannot be
(1991) claims that intellectual progress is maximized taught without models, and the ubiquitous presence of
when teaching is situated just ahead of the students’ atomic and molecular models in chemistry lessons is
current cognitive ability. evidence of their necessity. Diagrams like those in Figure
In psychological terms, this means challenging 1 feature in many chemistry textbooks and illustrate the
students to think within their "zone ofproximal devel- "taken-for-granted" role of models in chemistry.
opment" (vanderVeer&Valsiner, 1991,pp. 336-340).
Vygotsky described this zone as the intellectual range Observations from Sth-llth-grade
bounded at the lower level by what students can do on
their own and at the upper level by what they can To determine how 8th- throughlOth-grade chem-
achieve with teacher cues or peer help. This is why istry students reacted to scientific models of atoms and
socially negotiating the meaning of difficult concept molecules, we surveyed 48 Australian science students
and abstract models is so important. Vygotsky argued attending three different schools (a prestigious girls
that students’ intellectual growth is optimized when college, a large city high school and a rural high school)
they are challenged to do, with help, what they cannot and found many common model-based alternative
do on their own. Perry’s (1970) model of intellectual conceptions (Harrison & Treagust, 1996). Language
and ethical development made similar claims, and common to both biology and chemistry (e.g., nucleus
Grosslight et al.’s (1991) modelling levels suggested and shells) is a major source of confusion for some
that modelling is an intellectual skill that develops with students. Several students concluded that atoms can
help and experience. reproduce and grow and that atomic nuclei divide-
Electron shells were visualized as shells that enclosed
Models and Modelling in Learning Chemistry and protected atoms, while electron clouds were struc-
tures in which electrons were embedded. These syn-
Apart from its macroscopic properties, chemistry thetic models are likely generated during discussion as
relies on models to describe and explain all its chemical a result of semantic differences between teachers’ and
and physical changes. Symbolic models chemical students’ understanding of concept-metaphors. Stu-
formulae and equations supply chemistry’s special dents also expressed a strong preference for space-
language, and mathematical, theoretical, and concept- filling molecular models, and only two students held a
process models explain fundamental concepts like satisfactory model of the spaciousness of atoms.
atomic theory and reaction mechanics. How well could The alternative conceptions seemed to be related to
chemistry be taught without the periodic table model of the students’ believing that there is a one-to-one cor-
element properties? At yet another level, interactive respondence between the models used and reality.

Figure 1. Five different analogical models used to represent molecules of ammonia.

^
NH,

6
H : N : H

Volume 98(8), December 1998


Modelling in Science Lessons

Fifty-eight percent of the students were found to be The conceptual development and modelling abil-
Level 1 modellers. ity of every student was monitored for 36 weeks, and
This raised the question as to whether teaching case studies were written for 7 students ranging from
chemistry using systematically presented models could the highest to the lowest achiever. Three of the 11
improve a class of eleven 1 Ith-grade students’ under- students became competent Level 2/3 modellers by the
standing of model structure and purpose. A decision end of 1 Ith-grade. One student, called "Alex," became
was made to present chemistry’ s commonly used analo- so^dept at using multiple models that by the course’s
gies and models of atoms and molecules using the end, he appropriately used six different analogical
systematic FAR teaching framework and to socially models (see Figure 3) in an essay and an independent
negotiate the shared and unshared attributes of each interview to describe covalently bonded organic mol-
analogy and significant model. Whenever molecular ecules. Evidence of changes in the way Alex inter-
models were used in class, especially in the organic preted and used models emerged around week 24. An
chemistry unit, Allyn and Bacon modelling sets were interesting characteristic of the three Level 2/3 modellers
available on each student’s bench, and the students was the ease with which they identified and talked
were encouraged to make every molecule discussed. about the limitations of the models they were using.
The students were keen to build these molecules and Based on the first author’s anecdotal experience of
spent most of their spare time playing with the model more than 10 years’ experience teaching senior chem-
sets. Special pedagogical analogical models like the istry, these enhanced modelling skills were likely a
balloons model of the tetrahedral shape of sp3 mol- result of the systematic and negotiated use of analo-
ecules (e.g., methane. Figure 2) were demonstrated and gies. The literature lacks similar accounts, and
discussed in class. Cosgrove’s (1995) study is the only other reported
Furthermore, the teacher made a conscious effort instance of a lengthy intervention aimed at enhancing
to discuss the various attributes of and reasons for students’ analogical reasoning.
using the different models found in chemistry. Discus- Alex’s essay filled five pages. In a separate interview
sions covered atomic and molecular analogical models about atoms, molecules, and chemical bonds, he fluently
(see Figures 1 and 2), and special care was taken when and confidently talked about atomic structure and the
using concept-process models of acid/base and redox bonding found in a substituted alkane, a trans-aSkene, and
chemistry. Several lengthy, philosophical in-class ar- an alkyne. In the interview lasting 20 minutes, Alex did
guments probed the limits of common models like the most of the talking, and he employed each of the models
solar system atom. Some of the higher achieving stu- shown in Figure 3. Some of the models used by Alex in
dents were curious and argumentative and seemed as the interview were explained by him in detail and the
though they had ceased looking for definitive or correct following excerpt demonstrates his understanding:
models. The curiosity of these students catalyzed the These.. .are all models of molecules. Theball-and-
discussions and likely raised questions and ideas that stick method is too rigid and doesn’t show that the
benefited the less outgoing students. atom is mobile. The balloon method is too out of

Figure 2. The balloons model for tetrahedral, planar, and linear molecular shapes.

School Science and Mathematics


Modelling in Science Lessons

Figure 3. Eight models used by Alex late in the llth-grade chemistry course.

’. H ^-:- H

(a) electron cloud model of H2(b) electron-dot model of H2

\AdA\o^y
-p^
(c) ball-and-stick molecular model (d) balloons model of methane

(e) ball-and-spring model for ethane(f) electron-dot model of ethene

H \\ \\
1 /
H- c cn H
"h
H
C -
L-’
1
H
c
VH
-H

(g) line-bond model of ethyne(h) Alexis ^simplest’ model of propane

proportion. The hydrogens are huge compared two electrons being shared. These are both
to the carbon and the bonds. Some ways the good methods of representing the bonding go-
atoms can be represented on paper are electron ing on, because they show you where the bonds
dots [Figure 3(f)], and this is a good representa- are and give you clues why....! think the most
tion of where the electrons are bonding to give appropriate of these is [Figure 3 (h)]...because it
a better idea of what is going on and the bonds is one of the simplest ways of drawing the
[are drawn] as (<." This shows the types of molecules, and it also shows the position and
bonds between the atomseach line represents nature of the bonds involved...

Volume 98(8), December 1998


Modelling in Science Lessons

Another significant feature of Alex’s knowledge models break down somewhere and that no model is
was its relational nature and the way he qualified the right. Alex suggests that llth-grade students can
applicability of each model. He was comfortable with become competent multiple modellers and that they
each model5 s form, did not treat any model as right, and can realize that knowledge is relative and contextually
used the shared features of each model to explain but a bound. Whenever the social learning environment is
part of his conception. Based on Perry’s (1970) model supportive and multiple models are used, students will
of intellectual and ethical development, Alex was likely rise to the challenge and surprise their teachers.
likely a relativist, because he understood that each
model was contextually bound; that is, each model was
legitimized by the ideas it contained and the part it References
played in framing his overall conception of atoms,
molecules, and chemical bonds. Abell, S. K., & Roth, M. (1995). Reflections on a
Each of the seven case studies averaged almost 20 fifth-grade life science lesson: Making sense ofchildren’s
pages; for this reason, no case study can be presented understanding of scientific models. International
in full in this paper, but several detailed cases (includ- Journal of Science Education. 17(1), 59-74.
ing Alex’s) are presented in Harrison and Treagust (in Bent, H. (1984). Uses (and abuses) of models in
press). Our tentative claim is that the systematic pre- teaching chemistry. Journal of Chemical Education,
sentation of analogies and models and the social nego- 61, 774-777.
tiation of model meaning in a constructivist setting did Black, M. (1962). Models and metaphors. Ithaca,
enhance these students’ understanding of and ability to NY: Comell University Press.
manipulate scientific models. These claims, however, Carr, M. (1984). Model confusion in chemistry.
are limited to this study, and we go no further than to Research in Science Education, 14, 97-103.
suggest that similar strategies may produce similar Cosgrove, M. (1995). A case study of science-in-
results in other chemistry classes. The detailed study of the-making as students generate an analogy for
8th- through llth-grade students’ modelling experi- electricity. International Journal of Science Education,
ences was instrumental in helping us synthesize the 17, 295-310.
typology of concept-building analogical models re- Feynman, R. P. (1994). Six easy pieces. Reading,
ported in this paper. MA: Helix Books.
Finster, D. C. (1991). Developmental instruction.
Conclusion Part 2. Application of the Perry model to general
chemistry. Journal of Chemical Education, 70,752-756.
This article claims that many quite different Gentner, D. (1983). Structure mapping: A
analogical models are used to teach secondary science theoretical framework for analogy. Cognitive Science,
concepts. These models can range from concrete scale 7, 155-170.
models depicting no more than superficial features to Gilbert, J. K. (Ed.) (1993). Models and modelling
abstract concept-process models using multiple models in science education. Hatfield, Herts: Association for
to represent scientific processes. The discussion focused Science Education.
on two main themes: First, the concept-building Gilbert, J. K. & Boulter, C. J. (1998). Learning
analogical models used in science can be arranged in a science through models and modelling. In B. J. Eraser
typology that helps teachers understand the conceptual & K. G. Tobin (Eds.), International handbook of
demands placed on students by different model types. science education (pp. 53-66). Dordrecht, Netherlands:
This finding highlights the needforteachers to gradually Kluwer Academic Publishers.
challenge students to use more abstract and difficult Gilbert, S. W. (1991). Model building and a
models to develop student modelling skills. Second, definition of science. Journal of Research in Science
the article claims that no single model can adequately Teaching, 28, 73-79.
model a science concept; therefore, students should be Glynn, S. M. (1991). Explaining science concepts:
encouraged to use multiple explanatory models A teaching-with-analogies model. In S. Glynn, R.
whenever possible. In its simplest form, this requires Yeany, & B. Britton (Eds.), The psychology of learning
teachers to avoid early closure in discussions by asking science (pp. 219-240). Hillsdale, NJ, Eribaum.
the students for "another model please." It also asks Grosslight, L., Unger, C., Jay, E., & Smith, C.
teachers to socially negotiate model meanings with (1991). Understanding models and their use in science:
their students and regularly remind students that all Conceptions of middle and high school students and

School Science and Mathematics


Modelling in Science Lessons

experts. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 25, Watson, J. D. (1968). The double helix. London:
799-822. Penguin.
Harrison, A. G., & Treagust, D. F. (1996). Zook, K. B. (1991). Effect of analogical processes
Secondary students mental models of atoms and on learning and misrepresentation. Educational
molecules: Implications for teaching science. Science Psychology Review, 3(1) 41-72.
Education, 50, 509-534.
Harrison, A. G., & Treagust, D. F. (in press).
Learning about atoms, molecules and chemical bonds:
A case study of multiple model use in Grade-11
chemistry. Science Education.
Hesse, M. B. (1963). Models and analogies in Appendix A
science. London: Seed and Ward. The Three Aspects of the FAR Guide for Teaching and
Hewitt, P. G. (1987). Conceptual physics. Menio Learning With Analogies and Models.
Park, CA: Addison-Wesley.
Hodgson, T. (1995). Secondary mathematics FOCUS
modeling: Issues and challenges. School Science and Concept Is it difficult, unfamiliar, or abstract?
Mathematics, 95(7) 351-358. Students What ideas do the students already know
Hodgson, T., & Harpster, D. (1997). Looking back about the concept?
in mathematical modelling: Classroom observations Analog Is it something your students are familiar
and instructional strategies.. School Science and with?
Mathematics, 97(5) 260-267.
ACTION
Keenan, C. W., Kleinfelter, D. C., & Wood, J. H. Likes Discuss the features of the analog/model
(1980). General college chemistry (6th ed.). San and the science concept.
Francisco, CA: Harper and Row, Publishers. Draw similarities between them.
Ogbom, J., Kress, G., Martins, I., & McGillicuddy, Unlikes Discuss where the analog/model is un-
K. (1996). Explaining science in the classroom. like the science concept.
Buckingham: Open University Press.
Perry, W. G. (1970). Forms of intellectual and REFLECTION
ethical development in the college years. New York: Conclusions Was the analogy/model clear and useful
Holt, Rinehart and Winston. or confusing?
Treagust, D., Duit, R., Joslin, P., & Lindauer, I. Improvements Refocus as above in light of outcomes.
(1992). Science teachers’ use of analogies: Observations
from classroom practice. International Journal of
Science Education, 14( 4) 413-422.
Treagust, D. F., Harrison, A. G., & Venville, G.
(1998). Teaching science effectively with analogies:
An approach for pre-service and in-service teacher
education. Journal of Science Teacher Education, 9,
85-101.
Treagust, D. F., Harrison, A. G., Venville, G. J., &
Dagher, Z. (1996). Using an analogical teaching Author Note: Correspondence concerning this
approach to engender conceptual change. International article should be addressed to Allan G. Harrison,
Journal of Science Education, 18, 213-229. Faculty of Education and Creative Arts, Central
van der Veer, R., & Valsiner, J. (1991). Queensland University, North Rockhampton,
Understanding Vygotsky: A questfor synthesis. Oxford: Queensland, Western Australia 4702. Electronic mail
Blackwell. may be sent via Internet to a-harrison@cqu.edu.au

Volume 98(8), December 1998

You might also like