You are on page 1of 13

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/230067128

A Review of the Literature: Fraction Instruction for Struggling Learners in


Mathematics

Article  in  Learning Disabilities Research and Practice · May 2011


DOI: 10.1111/j.1540-5826.2011.00330.x

CITATIONS READS
49 13,811

1 author:

Radhika Misquitta
The Gateway School of Mumbai
6 PUBLICATIONS   83 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

FABLe (Fluency Assessment and Benchmarking for Literacy in Education) View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Radhika Misquitta on 15 October 2017.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, 26(2), 109–119

C 2011 The Division for Learning Disabilities of the Council for Exceptional Children

A Review of the Literature: Fraction Instruction for Struggling Learners in


Mathematics
Radhika Misquitta
University of Texas at Austin

Fractions are an essential foundational skill for future mathematics success (NMAP, 2008).
The purpose of this article was to review current instructional practices for teaching fractions
to struggling learners and to examine the quality and effectiveness of contemporary research
with a view to indicating directions for future research. A comprehensive search of literature
published between 1990 and 2008 resulted in the identification of 10 empirical studies that
targeted fraction skills for struggling learners. Results indicated that three interventions, found
to be effective for improving outcomes in mathematics for struggling learners, were also
effective for teaching fractions: graduated sequence, strategy instruction, and direct instruction.
In addition, explicit instruction was identified as necessary for improving students’ performance
in fractions. Overall, this review highlighted the paucity of research in this critical mathematical
content area.

Historically, fractions have been one of the most difficult dren struggle with fractions. Hiebert (1985) attributes the
mathematical skills to master, for children with and with- difficulty with fractions to a failure to connect form and
out difficulties (Behr, Wachsmuth, Post, & Lesh, 1984; understanding. Children have an intuitive or informal knowl-
Hiebert, 1985; McLeod & Armstrong, 1982; Ni, 2001). edge of fractions (Mack, 1990, 1995) but may not necessarily
Struggling learners in mathematics (students with learning associate their intuitive understanding of fractions with the
disabilities [LD], mathematics learning disability (MLD), formal representation of fractions (NMAP, 2008).
low-achievement in mathematics, and at-risk for failure in Prior mathematical experiences, especially with whole
mathematics) are at an even greater disadvantage, as their per- numbers, have been noted to hamper students’ understand-
formance in mathematics has traditionally lagged at least two ing of fractions (Ni & Zhou, 2005; NMAP, 2008; Pitkethly
grade levels below their peers (Wagner & Blackorby, 1996). & Hunting, 1996). Gallistel and Gelman (1992) reasoned
The National Mathematics Advisory Panel (NMAP, 2008) that because of students’ previous experiences with count-
was established to identify areas of concern in mathematical ing in discrete wholes, students fail to see how fractions fit
education in the United States and to provide directions to on the number line, and instead misread fractional quanti-
ensure that graduates attain international standards in math- ties as two wholes, for example, reading 14 simply as "one
ematics. The panel’s recent report indicated that at least and four." Furthermore, computing with fractions, unlike
40 percent of middle school students experienced difficulty counting with whole numbers is not intuitive (Pitkethly
with fractions, and nearly 50 percent of middle and high & Hunting, 1996), and children are at a loss when they
school students struggled with elementary level fraction con- can no longer use their fingers to compute (Wu, 2008).
tent. This finding poses a problem as fractions are considered Other familiar activities, such as halving values, a frac-
an essential foundational skill for “successful participation tion activity in the early grades (Pothier & Sawada, 1983),
in the contemporary American workforce” (NMAP, 2008, can function as a barrier when children have to create
p. 3–1) for completing day-to-day activities (e.g., modifying fractions with odd denominators (Pitkethly & Hunting,
recipes, ordering supplies) and for making important deci- 1996).
sions such as assessing risks associated with medical treat- Another obstacle to mastery is the representation of frac-
ments (Reyna & Brainerd, 2008; Subramaniam & Verma, tions as part–whole relationships (NMAP, 2008). Pictorial
2009). representations of fractions in early grades usually depict
fractions as being parts of a whole, such as slices of a pie,
making it difficult for children to conceptualize improper
Difficulties with Fractions fractions such as 98 . This part–whole model may negatively
impact students’ understanding of fractions as continuous
Several researchers (e.g., Hiebert, 1985; Mack, 1995; Mix, and infinitely divisible (Behr, Lesh, Post, & Silver, 1983;
Levine, & Huttenlocher, 1999) have investigated why chil- Hiebert & Tonnessen, 1978). The part–whole model also
inhibits other interpretations of fractions, such as fractions
Requests for reprints should be sent to Radhika Misquitta, CUZ-INNS, as fair shares (Pitkethly & Hunting, 1996). For example,
Bandra, Mumbai 400050, India. Electronic inquiries should be sent to rad- the fraction two-thirds is understood only as two parts of
hikamisquitta@gmail.com. a pie divided into thirds, and not as each person’s share
110 RADHIKA MISQUITTA: FRACTION INSTRUCTION FOR STRUGGLING LEARNERS

when two wholes are shared among three (Charalambous & formation (Miller & Hudson, 2007). Conceptual knowledge
Pitta-Pantazi, 2007). of fractions includes comparing and judging the magnitudes
of fractions, understanding fraction representation, and de-
termining fraction equivalence (NMAP, 2008). Procedural
Struggling Learners knowledge refers to the “ability to follow a set of sequential
steps to solve a mathematical task” (Miller & Hudson, 2007,
In addition to difficulties all students face with fractions, cer- p. 50). Procedural knowledge with fractions involves adding,
tain characteristics of students with LD as well as of strug- subtracting, multiplying, and dividing fractional quantities
gling learners further contribute to their difficulties working and algorithms to solve computation and word problems.
with fractions. Students with LD have limited working mem- The development of conceptual and procedural knowledge is
ory capacity (Geary, Hoard, Byrd-Craven, Nugent, & Num- an iterative process and gains in one type of knowledge lead
tee, 2007; NMAP, 2008) that impacts their ability to compute to an improvement in the other (Rittle-Johnson, Siegler, &
with fraction problems as well as to solve word problems Alibali, 2001).
(Hecht, Close, & Santisi, 2003). Grobecker (1999) noted that Specifically for struggling learners, while no review to
when compared to their peers without disabilities, students date has examined effective instructional practices in frac-
with LD tended to use inferior strategies for solving prob- tions for this category of students, syntheses of effective
lems involving fractions. Woodward, Baxter, & Robinson instructional practices in mathematics have incorporated in-
(1999), in their analyses of students’ problem-solving pro- terventions targeting fractions (e.g., Baker, Gersten, & Lee,
cesses, noted that children with LD tended to overgeneralize 2002; Gersten et al., 2009; Kroesbergen & Van Luit, 2003;
whole number strategies to fractions, simply adding denom- Maccini, Mulcahy, & Wilson, 2007; Miller, Butler & Lee,
inators and numerators as separate wholes. 1998; NMAP, 2008; Swanson & Hoskyn, 1998). The NMAP
While low-achievers and students at-risk for failure may (2008), in its meta-analysis of effective instructional prac-
perform relatively better in mathematics when compared with tices, recommended the following to enhance mathematics
students with LD (Fletcher, Lyon, Fuchs, & Barnes, 2007; instruction for struggling learners: using explicit and sys-
NMAP, 2008), fluency in processing numerical information tematic instruction including step-by-step explanations by
for low-achievers and students at-risk is still less than that teachers, providing explicit modeling and demonstration of
of their average-achieving peers. Low-achievers experience strategies, controlling task difficulty, providing specific feed-
difficulties due to inappropriate and inadequate prior expe- back, and multiple opportunities for guided and independent
riences, cognitive processes such as attention, and poor mo- practice; using concrete and visual representations; imple-
tivation (NMAP, 2008), characteristics that impact perfor- menting collaborative learning practices; using think alouds;
mance in fractions. Given the challenges students with LD setting high but reasonable expectations; and providing fre-
and other struggling learners face with fractions, it is essen- quent practice computing basic facts. Recommendations by
tial to identify interventions that target performance in this the Panel (NMAP, 2008) are consistent with the What Works
critical foundational skill. Clearinghouse (WWC) Response to Intervention (RtI) guide
to effective mathematics instruction for struggling learners
(Gersten et al., 2009). In particular, the guide strongly recom-
Fraction Instruction mends the use of explicit teaching practices including mod-
eling, guided practices, independent practice, feedback, and
The National Council of Teachers of Mathematics Stan- cumulative reviews of previous mathematics skills taught.
dards (NCTM, 1989, 2000, 2006), a guiding force in math-
ematics curriculum development (Blank and Dalkilic, 1992;
Woodward & Montague, 2002) recommends that fractional Statement of Purpose
content incorporates understanding of fractions as part of the
number line, understanding of the relationship of fractions The purpose of this review is to examine current practices
to whole numbers, fraction equivalence, and proficiency and for teaching fractions to struggling learners. Given the im-
fluency with addition, subtraction, multiplication, and divi- portance and need for fractions in our lives, it would be ben-
sion of proper, improper, and mixed fractions. According eficial to examine whether effective practices for teaching
to the benchmarks outlined by the NMAP (2008), students mathematics in general are effective for teaching fractions.
should be fluent in identifying and representing fractions by A secondary and related purpose is to investigate the quality
the end of grade 4, comparing magnitudes of fractions and and effectiveness of the existing research in order to pro-
adding and subtracting of fractions by the end of grade 5, vide suggestions for future directions in research on fraction
multiplication and division of fractions by the end of grade 6, instruction.
and all operations with positive and negative fractions by the
end of grade 7.
The NMAP (2008) recommended that development of Operational Definitions
conceptual and procedural knowledge was essential in mas-
tery of fractions. Conceptual knowledge refers to creating Fraction Instruction
links between discrete pieces of knowledge, linking new
information to previous knowledge, and recognizing rela- For the purpose of this review, the term fraction refers to
tionships and commonalities among different pieces of in- common fractions, and not related concepts in working with
LEARNING DISABILITIES PRACTICE 111

decimals, percents, ratio, or proportion. Instruction includes struggling learners as well as the general population
interventions that require students to manipulate fractions (e.g., Jordan, Miller, & Mercer, 1999). This decision
(i.e., add, subtract, multiply, divide), to examine the equiva- was made on the basis of the purpose statement, ra-
lence, or to compare the magnitudes of fractions. tionale, and literature review.
5. The intervention focused on common fractions (see
operational definition) and was conducted in a school
Struggling Learners setting including the general education classroom,
special education classroom, or resource room.
The term struggling learners are an umbrella term including 6. Only studies conducted in the United States were
students with LD, MLD, low-achievement in mathematics, included to facilitate comparison across studies and
and at-risk for failure in mathematics as identified by the context.
study. Both groups, students with LD and those with low
achievement in mathematics were selected because (1) re- First, a computer search of two databases, ERIC and
searchers have reported that although the problems experi- PsychINFO, was conducted, to locate studies published be-
enced by students with LD tend to be more severe than those tween 1990 and June 2008, using the search terms learning
of low-achieving students (Geary et al., 2007), instructional disab∗ , learning difficult∗ , mild disab∗ , math disab∗ , slow
practices to improve performance for students in both cat- learner, low-achiever, at-risk, struggling learner, mathemat-
egories are similar and the two groups are often combined ics, arithmetic, and fractions, in various combinations. This
(NMAP, 2008); (2) given the small body of literature ad- electronic search resulted in a total of 1,838 abstracts of
dressing fraction instruction, it seemed important to capture which 30 studies, whose abstracts met the criteria for inclu-
all relevant literature in fractions. sion, were selected for further review. The participant de-
scription, methods, and result sections of these studies were
then further examined to determine eligibility for this review.
Next, bibliographies of syntheses in mathematics identi-
METHODOLOGY fied through the electronic search, (see Baker, Gersten, &
Lee, 2002; Kroesbergen & Van Luit, 2003; Kunsch, Jiten-
For this review, intervention studies addressing performance dra, & Sood, 2007; Maccini & Hughes, 1997; Maccini et
in fractions for struggling learners in mathematics and pub- al., 2007; Montague, 2007; Xin & Jitendra, 1999) as well
lished in peer-reviewed journals were reviewed. The search as reference sections of articles that met the search crite-
for relevant literature was conducted in three steps (1) a ria were examined, to identify studies that had might have
search of electronic databases, (2) examination of previously been missed through the electronic search. Finally, a hand
published related syntheses and references to related articles, search of major journals in the field of special education
and (3) a hand search of selected relevant journals. Studies (namely Education and Treatment of Children, Exceptional
that met the following criteria were included in the review: Children, Learning Disability Quarterly, Learning Disabili-
ties Research and Practice, Journal of Learning Disabilities,
1. Only studies published in peer-reviewed journals af-
Journal of Special Education, Remedial and Special Edu-
ter 1990 were selected to distinguish research reflect-
cation, Review of Educational Research) and mathematics
ing current NCTM (1989, 2000) standards. The peer-
(Journal for Research in Mathematics Education) was con-
reviewed criterion was set as an initial gateway to help
ducted to ensure all relevant articles had been included. Ten
identify studies of higher methodological quality. In
studies met the final inclusion criteria for this review. The
addition, the methodology employed in the studies
majority of the 30 studies were excluded for the following
was also reviewed, the specific details of which are
reasons: they did not specifically target fractions and oper-
included in the results section.
ations in fractions (see Manalo, Bunnell, & Stillman, 2000;
2. Empirical studies investigating the efficacy of frac-
Owen & Fuchs, 2002; Rittle-Johnson et al., 2001; Woodward
tion instruction with struggling learners employing
et al., 1999); did not disaggregate results for fractions (see
experimental, quasi-experimental, single-subject, and
Bottge, Rueda, Serlin, Hung, & Kwon, 2007); or did not in-
qualitative methods were included.
clude struggling learners as defined by this study (see Hiebert
3. Only outcome measures assessing performance in
et al., 1991; Mack, 1990).
fractions (problem solving or computation) were in-
cluded. Measures that assessed related skills such
as decimals (see Hiebert, Wearne, & Taber, 1991;
Woodward et al., 1999), or those that did not examine Data Analysis
performance in fractions as an outcome measure (see
Baker, Young, & Martin, 1990) were excluded. Coding Procedures
4. Studies included struggling learners (see operational
definition) in the sample. Only disaggregated results Once studies had been identified, they were coded for partic-
for struggling learners were included wherever possi- ipant information (socioeconomic status of students, sample
ble. Studies in which the number of struggling learn- size, age, grade, gender, and disability), design description
ers constituted less than 50 percent of the total sam- (purpose, design method, assignment of students for experi-
ple, and studies that did not disaggregate data were mental and quasi-experimental designs, and fidelity checks),
included if the interventions were designed to target quality indicators of the methodology and design (such as
112 RADHIKA MISQUITTA: FRACTION INSTRUCTION FOR STRUGGLING LEARNERS

internal validity, fidelity of treatment, description of out- appear to be better estimates of effect sizes and are easier to
come measures), intervention characteristics (fraction skills interpret than regression-based methods (Campbell, 2004).
targeted, procedural, or conceptual knowledge), appropriate-
ness of analysis procedures, and reported findings. Studies
were coded as primarily addressing conceptual knowledge if RESULTS
they included activities to compare magnitudes of fractions,
fraction equivalence, or application of skills to real-world Ten studies met the inclusion criteria for this review, five em-
problems. Studies were coded as primarily addressing proce- ploying treatment/ comparison designs, three single-group
dural knowledge if they employed algorithms and strategies designs, and two single-subject designs. A total of 195 par-
to aid problem solving. The code sheet was modeled on one ticipants identified as struggling learners (108 LD; 24 other
developed and used in previous syntheses (see Kim, Vaughn, disabilities; 64 low-achievers and/or students at-risk for fail-
Wanzek, & Wei, 2004; Vaughn, Kim, Sloan, Hughes, El- ure) were included in these studies. With the exception of
baum, & Sridhar, 2003) and on the basis of recommenda- one study (see Jordan et al., 1999), all interventions targeted
tions by Odom, Brantlinger, Gersten, Horner, Thompson, & students in middle and high school.
Harris (2005).
Quality of Studies
Interrater Reliability The robustness of a study significantly influences the level
of confidence that can be placed in the results (Press-
A graduate student independently coded 30 percent of the ley & Harris, 1994). As a first step, the quality of the
studies (n = 3). An interrater reliability of 96 percent was studies was examined. Quality was evaluated on the ba-
established (agreements ÷ [agreements + disagreements]) sis of recommendations by Gersten et al. (2005), Horner,
(Cohen & Swerdlik, 2005). Carr, Halle, McGee, Odom, and Wolery (2005), and Odom
et al. (2005). Information on the quality of the stud-
ies served to guide the conclusions drawn in this article
Computation of Effect Sizes as well as to make recommendations for future research.
Table 1 presents a description of intervention and study char-
Effect sizes in this review were calculated to enhance de- acteristics. No study included in this review met all quality
scription and not as a means of comparison across studies. indicators. Four of the 10 studies met three of the four qual-
Comparative ability of effect sizes across studies was limited ity indicators (Bottge, 1999; Butler et al., 2003; Joseph &
given the small corpus of studies, and the varying sample Hunter, 2001; Kelly, Gersten, & Carnine, 1990). When dis-
sizes and methodologies employed. cussing interventions therefore, results from the four studies
Effect sizes were calculated for both experimental and of higher quality are emphasized. Readers should consider
single-subject studies. Three studies (Bottge & Hasselbring, the study findings with caution based on the limited number
1993; Flores & Kaylor, 2007; Jordan et al., 1999) did not of studies included, and the even smaller subset of studies
provide adequate information to calculate effect sizes. For meeting quality methodology indicators.
these studies, descriptive findings are reported instead.
Cohen’s d was calculated when researchers reported
means and standard deviations for intervention and control Internal Validity
groups as the difference between the mean posttest score of
the intervention group and mean posttest score of the con- When discussing the internal validity, Gersten and colleagues
trol group, divided by the pooled standard deviation (Cooper (2005) indicate randomization as the gold standard to achieve
& Reach, 2004). For studies that reported more than one to make confident causal relationships. Two of the 10 studies
outcome measure, the mean effect size (E.SM ) was calcu- (Bottge, 1999; Kelly et al., 1990) in this review used random
lated (see Kroesbergen & Luit, 2003). The adjusted mean, assignment of students to treatment and control conditions.
as reported in the study, was used to calculate effect sizes When random assignment was not possible, Gersten,
for studies that used pretest scores as a covariate. For one Baker, and Lloyd (2000) suggest measures such as adequate
study (see Butler, Miller, & Mercer, 2003), d could not be pretesting, the use of pretests as covariates, and controlling
calculated as the authors employed multivariate statistics. extraneous variables such as teacher influence. Two of the
Eta squared (η2 ), as reported by the authors, was therefore 10 studies (see Table 1; Bottge, Heinrichs, Mehta, & Hung,
included as an effect size. 2002; Butler et al., 2003) employed measures to improve
Lastly, the percent of nonoverlapping data (PND; Scruggs, comparability of groups. Butler and colleagues (Butler et al.,
Mastropieri, & Casto, 1987) was computed for single-subject 2003) randomly assigned classes to conditions and Bottge
studies as the number of data points in the treatment and and colleagues (Bottge et al., 2002) first matched students
maintenance phases that exceeded the highest baseline data within each class and randomly assigned pairs to groups
point in an expected direction, divided by the total number within each class. Furthermore, both studies counterbalanced
of data points in the treatment and maintenance phase, and teachers across conditions, as well as included pretest scores
multiplied by 100. While there is no consensus on the type of as a covariate in the analyses, to better equate groups.
effect size that adequately reflects results from single-subject Single-subject studies establish internal validity by re-
studies, (Swanson & Lee, 2000), nonparametric measures porting at least three demonstrations of experimental control
LEARNING DISABILITIES PRACTICE 113

TABLE 1
Intervention and Study Characteristics

Intervention
Study Participants Grade Design Dependent Variable Focus Results

Bottge (1999) 3 LD; three other 8 Experimental: Score on computation (rα =.94) Problem solving No significant difference
disabilities; 11 Pretest–posttest and word problem test between groups;
struggling control group design (rα =.90) assessing addition E.S. M = −.28
learners in and subtraction of fractions
math
T1: Anchored
Instruction
(fraction of the
cost video)
CI: Word problem
instruction
(textbook
instruction)
Bottge & 17 with 9 Quasi-experimental: Score on computation (rα =.98) Computation of Significant increase from
Hasselbring disabilities Pretest–posttest and word problem test fractions pretest to posttest (p < .01)
(1993) single-group design (rα =.95) assessing addition
and subtraction of fractions
T1: Direct instruction
(videodisc of
mastering
fractions)
Bottge, Heinrichs, 7 LD, 1 ED, 34 7 Quasi-experimental: Score on computation (rα =.91) Problem solving No significant difference
Mehta, & Hung without Pretest–posttest and word problem test between groups;
(2002) disabilities control group design (rα =.92) assessing addition E.S. M = −.25
and subtraction of fractions
T1: Enhanced
anchored
instruction
(fraction of the
cost video)
Butler, Miller, 42 LD, eight
Crehan, Battitt, & other
Pierce (2003) disabilities;
6, 7, 8 Quasi- Score on three subsets Concepts in fractions (fraction Significant
experimental: of CIBS-R and two equivalence) difference
Pre–post experimenter- using
control group developed subtests MANOVA in
design assessing favor of CRA
representing (p < .05), E.S.
fractions, computing = .26; higher
equivalent fractions, means for
and word problems CRA group
on fraction on all subtests
equivalence
T1: Graduated
sequence (CRA)
C1: Graduated
sequence (RA)
Flores & Kaylor 30 at-risk for 7 Quasi-experimental: Percent correct on computation Computation of Significant improvement from
(2007) failure in math Pretest–posttest test assessing addition, fractions pretest to posttest (p < .01),
single-group design subtraction, multiplication of E.S. not reported
fractions
T1: Direct
instruction
(corrective
mathematics,
basic fractions)
Gersten and Kelly 26 LD Secondary Pretest–posttest Percent correct on 30-item test Computation of Average percent change from
(1992) single-group design (rα = .98;r x x = .96) fractions pretest to posttest = 51.5%
measuring fraction analysis,
adding and subtracting
fractions and mixed numbers,
computing equivalent fractions,
and comparing fractions
Continued
114 RADHIKA MISQUITTA: FRACTION INSTRUCTION FOR STRUGGLING LEARNERS

TABLE 1
Continued.

Intervention
Study Participants Grade Design Dependent Variable Focus Results

T1: Direct
instruction
(videodisc of
mastering
fractions)
Jordan et al. (1999) 5 LD, 1 ED, 6 4 Experimental: Score on 80-item test assessing Concepts and Significant increase in
OHI Pretest–posttest identification skills, computation performance in favor of
control group design comparison skills, equivalence of fractions treatment group over
skills, addition skills and control group from pretest
subtraction skills to posttest (p < .001),
posttest 1 to posttest 2 (p <
.05), and posttest 2 to
posttest 3 (p < .05)
T1: Graduated
sequence
(concrete-
semiconcrete-
abstract or
CRA)
C1: Traditional
instruction
Joseph & Hunter 3 LD 8 Single subject: Score on 10 item fraction Computation of Improvement for all students
(2001) Multiple baseline computation test addition and fractions from baseline to
across participants subtraction of fractions maintenance. PND =
design. 96.6%
T1: Strategy
instruction (cue
card strategy)
Kelly et al. (1990) 17 LD, 11 LA 9 Percent correct on 12-item test Computation of Positive results in favor of
(rα =.98) measuring writing fractions treatment group (p < .01)
fractions from pictures, E.S. = 1.24
distinguishing numerator from
denominator, adding and
subtracting fractions with like
denominators, multiplying
fractions
T1: Direct instruction
(videodisc of
mastering
fractions)
C1: Basal curriculum
(textbook
instruction)
Test & Ellis (2005) 3 LD, 3 ID 8 Single subject: Delayed Percent correct on LAP fractions Computation of Improvement for all students
multiprobe multiple strategy steps; percent correct fractions from baseline to
baseline across pairs on application of LAP strategy maintenance. PND =
of participants on fractions test 100%
design
T1: Strategy
instruction (LAP
fractions strategy)
Note: LD: learning disabilities; ID: intellectual disabilities: LA low achievers; ED: emotional disturbance; OHI: other health impairment; NR: not reported; T: treatment;
C: comparison condition; E.S.: effect size; rα : internal consistency reliability; r x x : parallel forms reliability; CIBS-R: Brigance Comprehensive Inventory of Basic Skills-Revised;
CRA: concrete-representational-abstract; RA: representational-abstract; LAP: (L)ook at the denominator and sign to determine if fractions were like or unlike, and what operation was
required; (A)sk themselves if the denominators would divide evenly into each other; and (P)ick a fraction type Radhika Misquitta is currently a doctoral candidate at the University
of Texas at Austin. She completed her Masters in Special Education from SNDT University, Mumbai. Her research interests are in mathematical instructional strategies for students
with disabilities.

(Horner et al., 2005). Only one of the two single-subject stud- Fidelity of Treatment
ies (see Table 1; Joseph & Hunter, 2001) met the criterion for
internal validity. Experimental control in the Test and Ellis Fidelity of treatment or treatment integrity is another es-
(2005) study was compromised as the authors had to change sential element in understanding the effectiveness of the
from multiple-baseline to delayed multiple-baseline design intervention (Gersten et al., 2005). Treatment integrity
due to attrition. describes whether the intervention was implemented as
LEARNING DISABILITIES PRACTICE 115

planned (Gresham, MacMillan, Beebe-Frankenberger, & ceptual knowledge (Bottge, 1999; Bottge et al., 2002; Butler
Bocian, 2000) and provides information necessary for repli- et al., 2003; Jordan et al., 1999) and two, strategy instruc-
cation. Seven of the 10 studies (see Table 1) reported that they tion and direct instruction, focused on procedural knowledge
measured fidelity of implementation as well as described how (Bottge & Hasselbring, 1993; Flores & Kaylor, 2007; Gersten
fidelity was established (Bottge, 1999; Bottge et al., 2002; & Kelly, 1992; Joseph & Hunter, 2001; Kelly et al., 1990;
Bottge & Hasselbring, 1993; Butler et al., 2003; Flores & Test & Ellis, 2005). Dependent measures primarily focused
Kaylor, 2007; Kelly et al., 1990; Test & Ellis, 2005). on fraction computation skills. Four studies (Bottge, 1999;
Bottge et al., 2002; Bottge & Hasselbring, 1993; Butler et al.,
2003) also included measures assessing word problem solv-
Outcome Measure ing, and two studies (Butler et al., 2003; Jordan et al., 1999)
assessed fraction concepts including equivalence, and com-
When employing outcome measures, Gersten and colleagues paring fractions. While describing the instructional practices
(2005) encourage the use of multiple measures to cap- in this section, the emphasis is on interventions that met the
ture effects of the intervention. Experimenter-developed standards described in the quality indicators section.
tools and intervention-specific outcome measures can re-
flect performance specific to skills taught but tend to ele-
vate effect sizes when compared to standardized measures Graduated Sequence
(Swanson & Hoskyn, 1998). Gersten and colleagues recom-
mend, therefore, that studies incorporate both standardized Butler and colleagues (2003) and Jordan et al. (1999) in-
and experimenter-developed measures and report reliability vestigated a graduated sequence, concrete-representational-
and validity information for both. abstract (CRA), to teach fractions, with positive results in
Only one study (see Table 1; Butler et al., 2003) incor- favor of CRA sequence (see Table 1). Results indicated that
porated subtests from a standardized measure as well as re- students in the CRA group outperformed the control group
searcher developed subtests specific to the intervention. Six on outcomes measures (Butler et al., 2003) and were able to
of the 10 studies (Bottge, 1999; Bottge et al., 2002; Bottge maintain performance levels at least 2 weeks after the con-
& Hasselbring, 1993; Butler et al., 2003; Gersten & Kelly, clusion of the study (Jordan et al., 1999). CRA was noted to
1992; Kelly et al., 1990) reported test validity and reliability be effective in teaching fraction computations (Jordan et al.,
information for dependent measures. 1999) and the concept of fraction equivalence (E.S. = .26;
Butler et al., 2003).
A particularly well-designed study, Butler and colleagues
Social Validity (Butler et al., 2003) employed the CRA sequence to teach
fraction equivalence to students with LD in grades 6, 7, and
Social validity is estimating the “importance, effectiveness, 8. The authors developed 10 lessons where students were
appropriateness and /or satisfaction” (Kennedy, 2005, p. 219) first introduced to fraction equivalence with concrete objects
of an intervention to the different stakeholders. Establish- such as commercially available fraction circles, construc-
ing the social validity of an intervention is essential in the tion paper, and white beans. In subsequent lessons, students
field of special education as it separates clinical research graduated to working with representational drawings of frac-
from applied, and speaks to the extent an intervention can be tional quantities such as circles and polygons, and finally, to
adopted and used effectively in classroom settings (Horner abstract algorithms. Students also practiced word problems
et al., 2005). Social validity can be measured in different daily using the different manipulatives and abstractions. It is
ways such as estimating need prior to the study, using typical important to note that as students moved through the grad-
agents to deliver instruction, and measuring satisfaction and uated sequence, the topics for the lessons also differed. The
needs met at the conclusion of the study. overarching objective remained fraction equivalence, but stu-
Eight of the 10 studies (see Table 1) established social va- dents were systematically introduced to working with proper,
lidity of the intervention (Bottge, 1999; Bottge et al., 2002; improper, and mixed fractions.
Bottge & Hasselbring, 1993; Butler et al., 2003; Gersten &
Kelly, 1992; Joseph & Hunter, 2001; Kelly et al., 1990; Test
& Ellis, 2005) by employing a typical agent as the primary Anchored Instruction
interventionist. Two studies (see Butler et al., 2003; Test &
Ellis, 2005) enhanced social validity evidence by interview- Bottge (1999) and Bottge and colleagues (Bottge et al., 2002)
ing students at the conclusion of the study. compared the effectiveness of anchoring mathematics in-
struction using videodiscs of real-world problems, to typi-
cal instruction, for improving the problem-solving ability of
Results by Intervention Type struggling learners (see Table 1). The authors assessed stu-
dent performance on fractions alone, as well as real-world
A total of four interventions (graduated sequence, anchored problem solving. Students in the anchored instruction con-
instruction, strategy instruction, and direct instruction) for dition in both studies outperformed students taught using
teaching fractions to struggling learners emerged (E.S. typical instruction on measures assessing real-world prob-
range = −.28 to 1.24; PND = 96.6, 100). Two interventions, lem solving, but not on measures of fraction performance
graduated sequence and anchored instruction, targeted con- alone. The results related to real-word problem solving,
116 RADHIKA MISQUITTA: FRACTION INSTRUCTION FOR STRUGGLING LEARNERS

however, were not included in this review because students ing fraction computations to struggling learners. The stud-
were expected to use multiple mathematical operations such ies yielded positive results in favor of direct instruction (see
as fractions and measurement, and results for fractions alone Table 1). Three studies (Bottge & Hasselbring, 1993; Gersten
could not be disaggregated. For measures assessing frac- & Kelly, 1992; Kelly et al., 1990) examined the effectiveness
tion performance alone, neither study yielded significant ef- of a videodisc instructional package, Mastering Fractions.
fects in favor of the intervention group (E.S. range = −.25 Master Fractions are based on the direct instruction ap-
to −.28). proach and Flores and Kaylor (2007) employed a published
The primary purposes of the studies by Bottge (1999) Direct Instruction curriculum, Corrective mathematics, ba-
and Bottge and colleagues (2002) were to develop students’ sic fractions. The direct instruction approach was seen to be
problem-solving abilities. In both studies, learning was pri- more effective than traditional instruction (Flores & Kaylor,
marily student-directed, and the teacher served as a facilitator, 2007; Kelly et al., 1990) and improved students’ performance
guiding students’ discussions, and providing feedback when over time (Bottge & Hasselbring, 1993; Gersten & Kelly,
necessary. Neither study taught fractions explicitly to stu- 1992).
dents. Bottge (1999) and Bottge and colleagues (Bottge et al., Kelly et al. (1990) employed a treatment/comparison de-
2002) used instructional videos that presented students with sign comparing performance of students taught using the
real-world problems, such as building a skateboard ramp, or videodisc curriculum, Mastering Fractions, with traditional
a bird’s cage, to improve students’ problem-solving perfor- instruction (E.S. = 1.24). The videodisc curriculum included
mance. Students worked in groups to solve problems, using three key curriculum design variables: discriminating among
fractions, measurement, and money computations. problem types, discriminating important concepts and termi-
nology, and using a wide range of examples. The curriculum
introduced step-by-step instructional strategies for different
Strategy Instruction problem types and provided opportunities for guided and
independent practice. Students moved to the next step only
The two single-subject studies (Joseph & Hunter, 2001; Test once mastery was achieved. While the primary intervention
& Ellis, 2005) employed multiple baseline designs to evaluate was the videodisc, the teacher played an important role, con-
the effectiveness of strategy instruction on the fraction per- trolling the pace of the lesson, and modifying the sequence of
formance of struggling learners (see Table 1). Both studies lessons in the videodisc curriculum based on students’ needs
yielded positive effect sizes (PND = 96.6 and 100, respec- and progress.
tively) in favor of the strategy, and results were maintained
over time (3 and 6 weeks postintervention, respectively).
Joseph and Hunter (2001) also discussed how students with DISCUSSION
differential planning abilities might apply the strategy differ-
ently. The performance of the student with lower than average Understanding and computing fractions is challenging for
planning abilities compared to the students with average or struggling learners in mathematics (Groebecker, 1999). Iden-
above average planning was scattered and he needed frequent tifying what interventions are available to teach fractions to
prompting to use the strategy consistently. The authors there- struggling learners and their effectiveness is critical because
fore suggested that students with low planning abilities may fractions are considered an essential foundational skill for
require additional teacher mediation when using strategy in- future academic success (NMAP, 2008). The purpose of this
struction. article was to summarize and describe the literature on in-
Interventions involved in the studies included a three-step terventions for teaching fractions to struggling learners in
strategy with Joseph and Hunter (2001) using a cue card, and mathematics. As a part of the review, the quality of the stud-
Test and Ellis (2005) developing a mnemonic (L-ook at the ies were examined. Four significant patterns emerged from
denominator and sign; A-sk yourself if the denominators will this review:
divide evenly; P-ick a fraction type), to teach computation First, three interventions were noted to improve students’
of proper fractions to struggling learners. In both studies, performance in fractions: graduated sequence, direct instruc-
strategies required students to first identify whether the frac- tion, and strategy instruction. These three interventions have
tions to be added or subtracted were like or unlike, and if repeatedly been identified by the literature as effective prac-
not, then to implement steps to make both fractions like. tices for teaching mathematics to struggling learners (Gersten
Next, students were instructed to add or subtract fractions, et al., 2009; Swanson & Hoskyn, 1998). Miller and Hud-
and finally to reduce to the lowest number. In addition to fol- son (2007) examined approaches in mathematics to enhance
lowing the strategy, students in the Joseph and Hunter (2001) conceptual knowledge and discussed the CRA or graduated
study also monitored their progress daily by graphing their sequence as one method associated with positive results.
performance. Most recently, the WWC guide to RtI instruction (Gersten
et al., 2009) recommended the use of visual representations
and manipulatives to enhance mathematics instruction for
Direct Instruction students struggling in mathematics. Swanson and Hoskyn
(1998) identified direct instruction and strategy instruction
Four studies (Bottge & Hasselbring, 1993; Flores & Kaylor, as validated practices to teach struggling learners in their
2007; Gersten & Kelly, 1992; Kelly et al., 1990) investigated exhaustive meta-analysis of intervention practices spanning
the effectiveness of a direct instructional approach to teach- 25 years. From the body of literature in this review, we see
LEARNING DISABILITIES PRACTICE 117

that some interventions for improving overall performance includes operations with fractions such as adding, subtract-
in mathematics are also effective for teaching fractions. ing, multiplying, and dividing, as well as algorithms to solve
Second, in the studies reviewed here, implicit or incidental problems. Using the three approaches, the graduated se-
teaching of fractions, did not improve performance on frac- quence approach, strategy instruction, and direct instruction
tion computation tasks, as demonstrated in the Bottge (1999) seen to improve performance in this review, educators could
and Bottge et al, (2002) studies. Anchored instruction as em- develop an intervention program that taps both conceptual
ployed by Bottge (1999) and his colleagues (Bottge et al., and procedural knowledge. When selecting skills to teach,
2002) was designed to improve students’ problem-solving practitioners could make use of the benchmarks outlined by
skills using real-life problems. The authors did not provide the NMAP (2008) as a guide, emphasizing representation of
any explicit instruction in fraction computation skills. Results fractions in grade 4, comparing fractions, fraction equiva-
were not effective to improve the performance in fractions lence, and addition and subtraction of fractions in grade 5,
of struggling learners. However, in both studies, students in multiplication and division of fractions in grade 6, and finally
the anchored instruction group improved their performance all operations with negative and positive fractions by grade 7.
in real-world problem solving when compared to the typi- Second, this review emphasizes the need for explicit teach-
cal instruction group. The results of the Bottge (1999) and ing of fractions skills. Educators can still meet the NCTM
Bottge et al. (2002) studies are consistent with the find- (NCTM, 1989, 2000) focus on conceptual understanding by
ings of the NMAP (2008) that indicated that exposure to using explicit teaching methods to teach concepts as demon-
real-world problems tended to improve performance in real- strated by Butler and her colleagues (Butler et al., 2003). The
world problems, but not in computational skills. This review authors targeted conceptual understanding in mathematics,
indicates the need for a more direct and explicit approach making use of manipulatives and visual representations, and
when teaching fractions, such as the use of direct instruction structured each lesson around an explicit teaching frame-
(see Bottge & Hasselbring, 1993; Flores & Kaylor, 2007; work by reviewing previous skills taught, modeling, guided
Gersten & Kelly, 1992; Kelly et al., 1990). The WWC RtI practice of skills, opportunities for independent practice, and
guide (Gersten et al., 2009) also strongly recommends the use providing corrective feedback. Researchers (Hudson, Miller,
of explicit instructional practices such as think alouds, mod- & Butler, 2006) encourage teachers to combine the CRA
eling, guided practice, corrective feedback, and cumulative sequence with explicit instruction when teaching students
reviews when teaching mathematics to struggling learners. struggling in mathematics.
Third, this review reflects an emphasis on both procedu-
ral and conceptual knowledge in fractions. Interventions (see
Table 1) in this review emphasized both procedural (n = 6) Limitations and Future Directions
and conceptual knowledge (n = 4). Both are necessary. Con-
ceptual knowledge enables a learner to apply skills to differ- The primary limitation of this review is the small corpus of
ent situations and is “an essential component for dealing with studies included in the analysis. While the purpose of this ar-
novel problems in a variety of settings” (Miller & Hudson, ticle was only to examine instructional practices for common
2007, p. 49; NCTM, 2000). Procedural knowledge equips fractions, broadening the parameters to include interventions
a learner with an algorithm or steps to solve a mathemati- with rational numbers such as decimals and percentages,
cal problem (Miller & Hudson, 2007). The development of might indicate additional strategies that are effective. Fur-
conceptual and procedural knowledge go hand-in-hand and thermore, as designs employed by the studies were so varied,
increase in one type of knowledge lead to gains in the other comparing across effect sizes was not meaningful. Future re-
(Rittle-Johnson et al., 2001; Rittle-Johnson & Koedinger, search could consider examining only treatment/comparison
2009). designs to identify effective practices.
Finally, this review highlighted the relatively small body Another limitation is that this review included a di-
of literature addressing instructional practices in fractions for verse population of students with disabilities as well as
struggling learners (n = 10), as well as the need for higher low achievement. Limiting parameters to only students with
quality research. Of the 10 studies reviewed, no study met LD/MLD or only to low-achievers might identify interven-
all the quality indicators discussed in this review. Overall, tions that are differentially effective for these populations.
more research of high quality is needed in order to determine This review highlights the need for studies to incorporate
the impact of an instructional strategy in fractions and place maintenance and generalization data when assessing the im-
confidence in the results of the study. pact of an intervention. The difficulties struggling learners
face with retaining information and skills, as well as trans-
ferring to novel situations has been repeated documented
Implications for Practice in special education (Gersten & Baker, 1998; Kolligian &
Sternberg, 1987; NMAP, 2008). However, most studies in
This review on fraction instruction has two primary instruc- this review examined the effectiveness of interventions using
tional implications for educators: First, the NMAP (2008) only pre- and posttest data. No study reported generalization
emphasizes the need for both conceptual and procedural data and only four studies (Butler et al., 2003; Jordan et al.,
knowledge in fractions. Conceptual knowledge in fractions 1999; Joseph & Hunter, 2001; Test & Ellis, 2005) incorpo-
involves understanding the different representations of frac- rated data on maintenance.
tions, comparing magnitudes and ordering, and determining Finally, while this study focused on outcome mea-
fraction equivalence (NMAP, 2008). Procedural knowledge sures to assess the impact of interventions, future research
118 RADHIKA MISQUITTA: FRACTION INSTRUCTION FOR STRUGGLING LEARNERS

could also establish how interventions meet current NCTM Cohen, R. J., & Swerdlik, M. E. (2005). Psychological testing and as-
standards (NCTM, 1989, 2000) in the field. Researchers sessment: An introduction to tests and measurement. New York, NY:
McGraw-Hill.
(Ginsburg-Block & Fantuzzo, 1998; Woodward & Montague, Cooper, H., & Reach, K. (2004). What is a meta-analysis and how do we
2002) contend that areas emphasized in the NCTM stan- know we can trust it? In P. McCardle & V. Chhabra (Eds.), The voice of
dards, such as problem-solving skills, are areas of concern evidence in reading research (pp. 103–126). Baltimore, MD: Paul H.
for struggling learners too, and aligning interventions with Brookes Publishing Co.
the standards is beneficial for students who have difficulties Fletcher, J. M., Lyon, G. R., Fuchs, L. S., & Barnes, M. A. (2007).
Learning disabilities: From identification to intervention. New York:
in mathematics. Future research could examine intervention The Guilford Press.

programs that integrate methods seen to be effective to teach Flores, M. M., & Kaylor, M. (2007). The effects of a direct instruction
struggling learners (such as explicit, direct instruction) along program on the fraction performance of middle school students at-risk
with a more inquiry-based approaches so interventions for for failure in mathematics. Journal of Instructional Psychology, 34(2),
84–94.
this population are also aligned with the NCTM (2000) stan- Gallistel, C. R., & Gelman, R. (1992). Preverbal and verbal counting and
dards. computation. Cognition, 44(1–2), 43–74.
This review presents an overview of current instructional Geary, D. C., Howard, M. K., Byrd-Craven, J., Nugent, L., & Numtee,
practices to teach fractions to students struggling in mathe- C. (2007). Cognitive mechanisms underlying achievement deficits in
matics. While the corpus of studies reviewed is small, pat- children with mathematical learning disability. Child Development,
78(4), 1343–1359.
terns that emerge from this review serve an initial guide Gersten, R., & Baker, S. (1998). Real world use of scientific concepts:
to practitioners on effective instructional practices in frac- Integrating situated cognition with explicit instruction. Exceptional
tions, such as using the graduated sequence, strategy in- Children, 65(1), 23–35.
struction, and direct instruction. Given the importance of Gersten, R., Baker, S., & Lloyd, J. W. (2000). Designing high-quality re-
search in special education: Group experimental design. Journal of
fractions in determining students’ future academic success Special Education, 34(1), 2–18.
(NMAP, 2008), more research is needed to identify effective Gersten, R., Beckmann, S., Clarke, B., Foegen, A., Marsh, L., Star, J. R.,
instruction practices to teach this critical foundational skill et al. (2009). Assisting students struggling with mathematics: Response
in mathematics. to intervention (RtI) for elementary and middle schools. NCEE 2009–
4060. Washington, DC: National Center for Education Evaluation and
Regional Assistance, Institute of Education Sciences, U.S. Department
of Education.
REFERENCES Gersten, R., Fuchs, L. S., Compton, D. L., Coyne, M., Greenwood, C.,
& Innocenti, M. S. (2005). Quality indicators for group experimen-
Baker, S., Gersten, R., & Lee, D. S. (2002). A synthesis of empirical research tal and quasi-experimental research in special education. Exceptional
on teaching mathematics to low-achieving students. The Elementary ∗
Children, 71(2), 149–164.
School Journal, 103(1), 51–73. Gersten, R., & Kelly, B. (1992). Coaching secondary special education
Baker, J., Young, M., & Martin, M. (1990). The effectiveness of small-group teachers in implementation of an innovative videodisc mathematics
versus one-to-one remedial instruction for six students with learning curriculum. Remedial & Special Education, 13(4), 40–51.
difficulties. The Elementary School Journal, 91(1), 65–76. Ginsburg-Block, M. D., & Fantuzzo, J. W. (1998). An evaluation of the
Behr, M., Lesh, R., Post, T., & Silver, E. (1983). Rational number concepts. relative effectiveness of NCTM standards-based interventions for low-
In R. Lesh & M. Landau (Eds.), Acquisition of mathematical concepts achieving urban elementary students. Journal of Educational Psychol-
and processes (pp. 91–125). New York: Academic Press. ogy, 90(3), 560–569.
Behr, M. J., Wachsmuth, I., Post, T. R., & Lesh, R. (1984). Order and equiv- Gresham, F. M., MacMillan, D. L., Beebe-Frankenberger, M. E., & Bocian,
alence of rational numbers: A clinical teaching experiment. Journal for K. M. (2000). Treatment integrity in learning disabilities intervention
Research in Mathematics Education, 15(5), 323–341. research: Do we really know how treatments are implemented? Learn-
Blank, R. K., & Dalkilic, M. (1992). State policies on science and mathemat- ing Disabilities Research & Practice, 15(4), 198–205.
ics education 1992. Washington, DC: Council of Chief State School Grobecker, B. (1999). The evolution of proportional structures in children
Officers. with and without learning differences. Learning Disability Quarterly,

Bottge, B. A. (1999). Effects of contextualized math instruction on problem 22(3), 191–211.
solving of average and below-average achieving students. Journal of Hecht, S. A., Close, L., & Santisi, M. (2003). Sources of individual dif-
Special Education, 33(2), 81–92. ferences in fraction skills. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology,

Bottge, B. A., & Hasselbring, T. S. (1993). A comparison of two ap- 86(4), 277–302.
proaches for teaching complex, authentic mathematics problems to ado- Hiebert, J. (1985). Children’s knowledge of common and decimal fractions.
lescents in remedial math classes. Exceptional Children, 59(6), 556– Education and Urban Society, 17(4), 427–437.
511. Hiebert, J., & Tonnessen, L. H. (1978). Development of the fraction concept

Bottge, B. A., Heinrichs, M., Mehta, Z. D., & Hung, Y.-H. (2002). Weighing in two physical contexts: An exploratory investigation. Journal for
the benefits of anchored math instruction for students with disabilities Research in Mathematics Education, 9(5), 374–378.
in general education classes. Journal of Special Education, 35(4), 186– Hiebert, J., Wearne, D., & Taber, S. (1991). Fourth graders’ gradual con-
200. struction of decimal fractions during instruction using different physi-
Bottge, B. A., Rueda, E., Serlin, R. C., Hung, Y.-H., & Kwon, J. M. (2007). cal representations. The Elementary School Journal, 91(4), 321–341.
Shrinking achievement differences with achored math problems: Chal- Horner, R., Carr, E., Halle, J., McGee, G., Odom, S., & Wolery, M. (2005).
lenges and possibilities. The Journal of Special Education, 41(1), 31– The use of single-subject research to identify evidence-based practice
49. in special education. Exceptional Children, 71, 165–179.

Butler, F. M., Miller, S. P., Crehan, K., Babbitt, B., & Pierce, T. (2003). Frac- Hudson, P. J., Miller, S. P., & Butler, D. L. (2006). Adapting and merg-
tion instruction for students with mathematics disabilities: Comparing ing explicit instruction within reform based mathematics classrooms.
two teaching sequences. Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, ∗
American Secondary Education, 35(1), 19–32.
18(2), 99–111. Jordan, L., Miller, D., & Mercer, C. D. (1999). The effects of concrete to
Campbell, J. M. (2004). Statistical computation of four effect sizes for semiconcrete to abstract instruction in the acquisition and retention of
single-subject designs. Behavior Modification, 28(2), 234–246. fraction concepts and skills. Learning Disabilities: A Multidisciplinary
Charalambous, C., & Pitta-Pantazi, D. (2007). Drawing on a theoretical ∗
Journal, 9(3), 115–122.
model to study students’ understandings of fractions. Educational Stud- Joseph, L. M., & Hunter, A. D. (2001). Differential application of a cue
ies in Mathematics, 64, 293–316. card strategy for solving fraction problems: Exploring instructional
LEARNING DISABILITIES PRACTICE 119

utility of the cognitive assessment system. Child Study Journal, 31(2), numbers: The origins and implications of whole number bias. Educa-

123–136. tional Psychologist, 40(1), 27–52.
Kelly, B., Gersten, R., & Carnine, D. (1990). Student error patterns as Odom, S. L., Brantlinger, E., Gersten, R., Horner, R. D., Thompson, B.,
a function of curriculum design: Teaching fractions to remedial high & Harris, K. (2005). Quality indicators for research in special educa-
school students and high school students with learning disabilities. tion and guidelines for evidence-based practices: Executive summary.
Journal of Learning Disabilities, 23(1), 23–29. Division for Research: Council for Exceptional Children, 71, 137–148.
Kennedy, C. H. (2005). Single-case designs for educational research. Owen, R. L., & Fuchs, L. S. (2002). Mathematical problem-solving strategy
Boston, MA: Pearson. instruction for third-grade students with learning disabilities. Remedial
Kim, A.-H., Vaughn, S., Wanzek, J., & Wei, S. (2004). Graphic organizers & Special Education, 23(5), 268–278.
and their effects on the reading comprehension of students with learning Pitkethly, A., & Hunting, R. (1996). A review of recent research in the area
disabilities: A synthesis of research. Journal of Learning Disabilities, of initial fraction concepts. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 30(1),
37(2), 105–118. 5–38.
Kolligian Jr., J., & Sternberg, R. J. (1987). Intelligence, information process- Pothier, Y., & Sawada, D. (1983). Partitioning: The emergence of rational
ing, and specific learning disabilities: A triarchic synthesis. Journal of number ideas in young children. Journal for Research in Mathematics
Learning Disabilities, 20(1), 8–17. Education, 14(5), 307–317.
Kroesbergen, E. H., & Van Luit, J. E. H. (2003). Mathematical interventions Pressley, M., & Harris, K. R. (1994). Increasing the quality of educational
for children with special education needs: A meta-analysis. Remedial intervention research. Educational Psychology Review, 6(3), 191–208.
& Special Education, 24(2), 97–114. Reyna, V. F., & Brainerd, C. J. (2008). Numeracy, ratio bias, and denominator
Kunsch, C. A., Jitendra, A. K., & Sood, S. (2007). The effects of peer- neglect in judgments of risk and probability. Learning and Individual
mediated instruction in mathematics for students with learning prob- Differences, 18(1), 89–107.
lems: A research synthesis. Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, Rittle-Johnson, B., Siegler, R. S., & Alibali, M. W. (2001). Developing con-
22(1), 1–12. ceptual understanding and procedural skill in mathematics: An iterative
Maccini, P., & Hughes, C. A. (1997). Mathematics interventions for ado- process. Journal of Educational Psychology, 93(2), 346–362.
lescents with learning disabilities. Learning Disabilities Research & Rittle-Johnson, B., & Koedinger, K. (2009). Iterating between lessons on
Practice, 12(3), 168–176. concepts and procedures can improve mathematics knowledge. British
Maccini, P., Mulcahy, C. A., & Wilson, M. G. (2007). A follow-up of math- Journal of Educational Psychology, 79, 483–500.
ematics interventions for secondary students with learning disabilities. Scruggs, T. E., Mastropieri, M. A., & Casto, G. (1987). The quantitative
Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, 22(1), 58–74. synthesis of single-subject research: Methodology and validation. Re-
Mack, N. K. (1990). Learning fractions with understanding: Building on medial & Special Education, 8(2), 24–33.
informal knowledge. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, Subramaniam, J., & Verma, B. (2009, January). Introducing fractions using
21(1), 16–32. share and measure interpretations: A report from classroom trials.
Mack, N. K. (1995). Confounding whole-number and fraction concepts Paper presented at the epiSTEME 3, Mumbai.
when building on informal knowledge. Journal for Research in Math- Swanson, H. L., & Hoskyn, M. (1998). Experimental intervention research
ematics Education, 26(5), 422–441. on students with learning disabilities: A meta-analysis of treatment
Manalo, E., Bunnell, J. K., & Stillman, J. A. (2000). The use of process outcomes. Review of Educational Research, 68(3), 277–321.
mnemonics in teaching students with mathematics learning disabilities. Swanson, H. L., & Sachse-Lee, C. (2000). A meta-analysis of single-subject-
Learning Disability Quarterly, 23(2), 137–156. design intervention research for students with learning disabilities.
McLeod, T. M., & Armstrong, S. W. (1982). Learning disabilities in ∗
Journal of Learning Disabilities, 33(2), 114–136.
mathematics: Skill deficits and remedial approaches at the inter- Test, D. W., & Ellis, M. F. (2005). The effects of LAP fractions on addi-
mediate and secondary level. Learning Disability Quarterly, 5, tion and subtraction of fractions with students with mild disabilities.
305–310. Education & Treatment of Children, 28(1), 11–24.
Miller, S. P., Butler, D. L., & Lee, K.-h. (1998). Validated practices for The National Mathematics Advisory Panel. (2008). Reports of the task
teaching mathematics to students with learning disabilities: A review groups and Subcommittees. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Ed-
of literature. Focus on Exceptional Children, 31(1), 1–24. ucation
Miller, S. P., & Hudson, P. J. (2007). Using evidence-based practices to build Vaughn, S., Kim, A-H., Sloan, C., Hughes, M., Elbaum, B., & Sridhar, D.
mathematics competence related to conceptual, procedural, and declar- (2003). Social skills interventions for young children with disabilities:
ative knowledge. Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, 22(1), A synthesis of group design studies. Remedial and Special Education,
47–57. 24, 2–15.
Mix, K. S., Levine, S. C., & Huttenlocher, J. (1999). Early fraction calcula- Wagner, M. M., & Blackorby, J. (1996). Transition from high school to
tion ability. Developmental Psychology, 33(5), 164–174. work or college: How special education students fare. The Future of
Montague, M. (2007). Self-regulation and mathematics instruction. Learn- Children, 6(1), 103–120.
ing Disabilities Research & Practice, 22(1), 75–83. Woodward, J., Baxter, J., & Robinson, R. (1999). Rules and reasons: Dec-
National Council of Teachers of Mathematics. (1989). Curriculum and eval- imal instruction for academically low achieving students. Learning
uation standards for school mathematics. Reston, VA: Author. Disabilities Research & Practice, 14(1), 15–24.
National Council of Teachers of Mathematics. (2000). Principles and stan- Woodward, J., & Montague, M. (2002). Meeting the challenge of mathe-
dards for school mathematics. Reston, VA: Author. matics reform for students with learning disabilities. The Journal of
National Council of Teachers of Mathematics. (2006). Curriculum focal Special Education, 36(2), 89–101.
points for prekindergarten through grade 8 mathematics: A quest for Wu, H. (2008). Fractions, decimals, and rational numbers. Retrieved Au-
coherence. Virginia: Reston. gust 20, 2008, from University of California, Berkeley, Department of
Ni, Y. (2001). Semantic domains of rational numbers and the acquisition Mathematics Web site: http://math.berkeley.edu/∼wu/
of fraction equivalence. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 26, Xin, Y. P., & Jitendra, A. K. (1999). The effects of instruction in solving
400–417. mathematical word problems for students with learning problems: A
Ni, Y., & Zhou, Y.-D. (2005). Teaching and learning fraction and rational meta-analysis. The Journal of Special Education, 32(4), 207–225.
Copyright of Learning Disabilities Research & Practice (Blackwell Publishing Limited) is the property of
Wiley-Blackwell and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without
the copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for
individual use.

View publication stats

You might also like