You are on page 1of 11

Journal of Gerontology: PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCES Copyright 2001 by The Gerontological Society of America

2001, Vol. 56B, No. 3, P176-P186

Gender Differences in the Relationship Between


Marital Status Transitions and Life Satisfaction in
Later Life
Judith G. Chipperfield1 and Betty Havens2

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/psychsocgerontology/article-abstract/56/3/P176/2965076 by guest on 11 April 2020


Health Leisure and Human Performance Research Institute and 2Community Health Sciences,
University of Manitoba, Winnipeg, Manitoba, Canada.

This study examined life satisfaction among individuals who had undergone a transition in marital status and
those whose marital status remained stable over a 7-year period. In particular, using data from a large-scale, lon-
gitudinal study we assessed life satisfaction as measured in 1983 and 1990 among 2,180 men and women between
the ages of 67 and 102. Groups of individuals were identified on the basis of whether a spouse was present or ab-
sent at the two measurement points. This allowed for a classification of groups who experienced stability or tran-
sitions in marital status. Among those individuals whose marital status remained stable over the 7 years,
women's life satisfaction declined and men's remained constant. Among those who experienced a transition—in
particular, the loss of a spouse—a decline in life satisfaction was found for both men and women, decline being
more predominant for men. In addition, men's life satisfaction increased over the 7-year period if they gained a
spouse, whereas the same was not true for women. Generally, these findings imply that the relationship between
marital status transitions or stability differs for men and women.

A S people age, many face the traumatic experience of


losing their spouses, either through death or institu-
tionalization. Evidence suggests that individuals who lose a
ety of indicators including morale, depression, happiness,
life satisfaction, and so forth (e.g., Allen, Ciambrone, &
Welch, 2000; Holicky & Charlifue, 1999; Hong & Duff,
spouse are more likely to die than their counterparts who do 1997; Kehn, 1995; Morris, 1997; Ryff & Keyes, 1995).
not experience this stressful event (e.g., Bowling, 1987). An Overwhelmingly, the literature points to a positive associa-
equally important issue is the quality of life following the tion between having a marriage partner and being satisfied
loss of a spouse. Some individuals adjust well to the loss; with life (e.g., Hong & Duff, 1997; Kehn, 1995). Much of
others do not. Attempting to understand the experience, re- the past work has focused exclusively on women (Bennett
searchers have studied everything from bereavement (e.g., & Morgan, 1992; Day & Day, 1993; Kousha & Mohseni,
Herriott & Kiyak, 1981; Lund, 1989) and depression (e.g., 1997; Mookherjee, 1997), in part because they generally
Osterweis, Solomon, & Green, 1984; Stroebe & Stroebe, outlive men and have a greater likelihood of being wid-
1983) following the loss of a spouse to use of health ser- owed. Reinhardt and Fisher (1989), for example, compared
vices (e.g., Wolinsky & Johnson, 1992). This emphasis on reports of life satisfaction among women who were married
widowhood, however, has overshadowed the need to study and those who were widowed and found significantly
stability in marital status or marital status transitions other higher life satisfaction among those who were married.
than the loss of a spouse. For example, marriage in later life, Other researchers have directly examined gender differ-
whether for the first time or following the loss of a spouse, ences, comparing married and widowed men and women.
has important implications for quality of life. Connidis and McMullin (1993), for example, found that di-
The goal of the present study was to explore life satisfac- vorced men and women were less satisfied than their mar-
tion among seniors who had undergone both types of transi- ried counterparts. Lubben's (1989) results indicated that
tions (i.e., those who gained spouses and those who lost married men were more satisfied with life than either mar-
spouses) and seniors who retained marital status stability. In ried or widowed women, and among men, those who were
particular, we examined life satisfaction among various married had higher life satisfaction than those who were
groups of seniors differing in marital status, thereby adding not. Moreover, in their extensive search of the literature,
to the literature on life satisfaction among those who are Stroebe and Stroebe (1983) reported that "married females
single (Chappell & Badger, 1989), divorced (Farnsworth, were more depressed than married males and widowed
Pett, & Lund, 1989), remarried (Bulcroft, Bulcroft, Hatch, males were more depressed than widowed females" (p.
& Borgatta, 1989; Burks, Lund, Gregg, & Bluhm, 1988), or 284).
married and widowed (e.g., D'Amato, 1987; Fengler, Dani- Taken together, these findings suggest that the presence
gelis, & Little, 1983; Geis & Klein, 1990). of a marriage partner has positive implications for life satis-
faction. As Dykstra (1995) pointed out, "Many activities are
Marital Status and Well-Being couple-companionate, undertaken as a couple, with other
A large literature has emerged on the relationship be- couples" (p. 321). Thus, to the extent that social interaction
tween marital status and well-being, as measured by a vari- facilitates psychological well-being, the availability of a

P176
CHANGES IN LIFE SATISFACTION Pill

spouse may offer a "protective" function. Interestingly, the presence or absence of a spouse, the groups who experi-
some of the existing research has pointed to the possibility enced stability expressed similar levels of morale. In other
that the presence of a spouse affords a relatively greater ad- words, morale appeared to be more affected by a transition
vantage for men than for women. For example, in a study of than by the mere availability or presence of a spouse.
retirement satisfaction, the beneficial impact of marriage
on satisfaction with retirement was greater for men than The Present Study
for women (Calasanti, 1996). Moreover, researchers have Using a longitudinal design, we directly addressed the
shown an increased risk of dying among widowers, but not question of whether patterns of life satisfaction differed for
among widows, following their spouses' deaths (e.g., Bi- seniors who underwent a transition in marital status (e.g.,

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/psychsocgerontology/article-abstract/56/3/P176/2965076 by guest on 11 April 2020


ondi & Picardi, 1996; Bowling & Windsor, 1995; Marti- death of spouse, remarriage) compared with those who ex-
kainen & Valkonen, 1996). This implies that the implica- perienced stability (i.e., remained married, remained never
tions of marriage and widowhood are different for men and married, remained divorced or widowed). First, we pre-
women. dicted that, over time, life satisfaction would remain rela-
Although numerous studies of aging have explored the tively unchanged among those who experienced stability in
"loss-of-spouse" transition (i.e., death, divorce, institution- marital status. This prediction was based on Bennet and
alization), fewer studies have examined the remarriage tran- Morgan's (1992) finding that those who experienced mari-
sition. This is, in part, because small numbers of people tend tal status stability expressed similar levels of morale over
to remarry late in life (Zick & Smith, 1988). Likewise, with time and on the findings from several large-scale longitudi-
a few exceptions (e.g., Barrett, 1999; Bennett & Morgan, nal studies of well-being, one involving a 9-year follow-up
1992; Dykstra, 1995; Strain & Payne, 1992), never-married (Costa et al., 1987) and another involving a 14-year follow-
seniors have been largely ignored when examining the rela- up (Stacey & Gatz, 1991).
tionship between marital status and well-being. Second, we predicted that life satisfaction would change
among those who underwent a marital status transition. In
Stability versus transitions in marital status.—The im- particular, within the marital status transition groups, we ex-
portant distinction between marital status stability versus pected that those who lost spouses would experience a de-
marital status transition has received little attention in past cline in life satisfaction, whereas those who gained spouses
research. A potentially useful approach would allow for a would enjoy heightened satisfaction. More important, we
two-dimensional classification that distinguishes between expected that, relative to women, for men the magnitude of
the presence of a spouse (present vs absent) and transitions change in life satisfaction would be larger. Women's life
in marital status (yes vs no). Within this framework, never- satisfaction, on the other hand, was expected to be relatively
married and still-married individuals are similar on the tran- more stable than men's, despite changes in marital status.
sition dimension (i.e., they have not experienced a transition These predictions are in keeping with research that has
in marital status) and different on the presence dimension shown an increased risk of dying among widowers but not
(i.e., never-married individuals do not have spouses present, among widows following the death of a spouse (e.g., Bowl-
and still-married individuals do have spouses present). This ing & Windsor, 1995).
classification would allow for a consideration of whether Of course, an evaluation of changes in life satisfaction
undergoing a transition in marital status has more serious would be inadequate without attention to other factors. For
consequences for life satisfaction than the mere presence or the present purpose, potentially important variables were
absence of a spouse. identified by drawing from the literature on the predictors of
Longitudinal designs are essential in order to directly life satisfaction (Davis, 1991; D'Amato, 1987; Fengler et al.,
study the role of marital status transitions. However, in the 1983; Girzadas, Counte, Glandon, & Tancredi, 1993; Rid-
few longitudinal studies that exist, it is difficult to summa- dick, 1985; Spreitzer & Snyder 1974). We included mea-
rize the impact of marital status transitions on changes in sures of health, which is consistently shown to be the
life satisfaction because the comparison groups differ across strongest predictor of life satisfaction (D'Amato, 1987;
studies (e.g., married vs widowed; remarried vs widowed). Farnsworth et al., 1989; Girzadas et al., 1993; Lubben, 1989;
In one study of seniors who had lost spouses, those who re- Morris, 1997). Also included were age (e.g., Geis & Klein,
married displayed higher levels of life satisfaction after 2 1990; Lund, Caserta, & Dimond, 1986), education and in-
years than those who did not (Burks et al., 1988). In another come (e.g., Farnsworth et al., 1989), social support (e.g.,
10-year study, those who remarried were relatively happier Fengler et al., 1983; Strain & Chappell, 1982), and activities
with their lives than those who did not, but this happiness of daily living (Girzadas et al., 1993), variables that have
diminished with time (Bulcroft et al., 1989). been shown to predict life satisfaction.
Finally, Bennett and Morgan (1992) found that patterns The majority of the existing work in this area has used
of psychological change over 4 years differed for women cross-sectional designs, obtaining only posttransition mea-
who had been widowed, had never married, or were still sures of well-being. Unlike these past studies, which do not
married. Overall, morale decreased; however, the magni- allow for a direct analysis of marital status transitions and
tude of this decrease was strongest among widowed women. accompanying changes in well-being, the present study sys-
Also, those who experienced a transition (i.e., the widowed tematically assessed these changes across 7 years, from
women) had significantly lower levels of morale after 4 1983 to 1990. Participants were part of the longitudinal Ag-
years than those whose status remained stable (i.e., still- ing in Manitoba (AIM) Project, and because of the represen-
married and never-married women). Despite differences in tative nature of the study sample, it was possible to track
P178 CHIPPERFIELD AND HAVENS

those individuals who experienced both types of marital sta- steps were taken to determine the status of a small percent-
tus transitions (e.g., widowed, remarried) as well as those age of individuals (n = 170, or 5%) who could not be inter-
who retained stability (e.g, married, never married, re- viewed in 1990 for a variety of reasons and to compare
mained widowed or divorced). As such, our assessment ex- these nonrespondents with respondents. No significant dif-
pands on the previous work, which has only indirectly ad- ferences were found on a variety of demographic variables,
dressed marital status transitions versus stability primarily indicating that the nonrespondents and respondents were
by comparing groups that are married versus widowed. As similar (Chipperfield et al., 1997).
such, it provides an opportunity to extend the existing de-
scription of the relationship between marital status and life Present Study Sample

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/psychsocgerontology/article-abstract/56/3/P176/2965076 by guest on 11 April 2020


satisfaction that has arisen from those studies that focus on To consider the relationship between marital status and
specific comparison groups. life satisfaction, only a subset of the 1990 AIM survivors
were included. The sample was restricted to those individu-
METHODS als who, in 1983 and 1990, completed Neugarten, Ha-
vighurst, and Tobin's (1961) Life Satisfaction Index A
AIM Database (LSIA), thus excluding individuals who were not inter-
The AIM database contains data from interviews con- viewed at both times, those who required assistance from a
ducted with older people in Manitoba at five points in time: proxy, and those who were cognitively incapable of re-
1971, 1976, 1983, 1990, and 1996. The individuals who sponding to these items. The participants retained for analy-
agreed to participate (or in some cases proxies) were inter- ses (n = 2,180) consisted of 1,321 women and 859 men
viewed in their home, in their language of choice (e.g., ranging in age from 67 to 102 (M = 77.7). One half (50%)
Ukrainian, French, Italian, Icelandic, Yiddish, Portuguese, were married, 40% were widowed, 3% were divorced, and
Polish, German, Chinese, Sioux, Chippewa, and Cree). The 7% were single. Formal education ranged from a low of 0
interviews lasted approximately 1 to 1.5 hr and assessed a (2%) to a high of 16 or more years (2%). The majority lived
variety of variables, including demographics, social net- in a house (68%) with at least one other person (56%) and
works, perceptions, health, well-being, and so forth. described their ethnic descent as British (41%).
To obtain sufficient numbers of individuals who experi-
enced marital status transitions and large enough numbers Measures
to examine each of the stability-transition groupings (e.g.,
see subsequent description of stability-transition groups), Demographic variables.—Age was calculated from year
the present analysis focused on a subset of individuals who of birth as indicated on the Manitoba Health records (M =
were interviewed over a 7-year period. This included AIM 11.1 years, SD = 6.9). Other demographic variables for the
participants who were interviewed in 1983 and again in analyses were based on information obtained during the
1990. Thus, the following description is restricted to these 1983 interview. Education was assessed by asking the par-
two measurement points. A full description of the method- ticipants, "How many grades did you complete in school?"
ology of the AIM studies can be found elsewhere (Chipper- The answers were coded into one of seven categories: 1 = 0
field, Havens, & Doig, 1997). years (no school), 2 — 1 to 4 years, 3 = 5 to 8 years, 4 = 9
In 1983, a total of 5,273 individuals were interviewed; to 10 years, 5 = 11 to 12 years, 6 = 13 to 16 years, and 7 =
some of these (n = 2,780) were first-time participants. Us- 16 years or more. The median level of education was be-
ing an age-gender stratified area-probability sampling tech- tween 9 to 10 years. Income in 1983 was calculated by add-
nique, these first-time participants were randomly selected ing all the sources of reported income (i.e., pensions, sala-
from Manitoba Health's computerized registry, which con- ries, rents, interest, insurance annuities, Old Age Security,
tains information on all Manitoba residents (e.g., age, gen- Guaranteed Income Supplement, allowances, welfare, un-
der, and area of residence) who are covered under the uni- employment insurance, Old Age Assistance, Shelter Assis-
versal medical insurance program. Because this registry tance for Elderly Renters, Manitoba Supplement for Pen-
represents the most comprehensive listing of residents sioners, or cash from other sources). The mean level of
available in the province of Manitoba, samples drawn from monthly income in 1983 was $595.33 (SD = $552.54) in
it can be considered representative of the overall provincial Canadian dollars.
population. Other 1983 participants were-part of a longitudi-
nal follow-up panel (n = 2,403), having begun the AIM Social support.—On the basis of Gumming and Henry's
study at an earlier time (i.e., in 1971 or 1976). Again, the (1961) Social Lifespace Measure, we created two variables
initial 1971 and 1976 samples were selected from Manitoba to reflect monthly social interaction in 1983. These vari-
Health's registry using the same age-gender stratified area- ables were constructed from respondents' reports of the
probability sampling technique. Interviews were completed numbers of people they saw every day, once a week, a few
by 95% of the potential surviving participants (Chipperfield times a month, once a month, and less than once a month.
et al., 1997). First, interaction with relatives was assessed by responses
In 1990, a second follow-up study was conducted, rein- to the question "Of the relatives (including any in house-
terviewing all of the individuals who had participated in hold) you feel closest to, how many do you see and how of-
1983, excluding those who had died. At this time, inter- ten?" As suggested by Gumming and Henry, the number of
views were again conducted with 95% (n = 3,218) of po- people seen was multiplied by a weighting factor so that a
tential participants (Chipperfield et al., 1997). Rigorous large score would mean more social contact. The mean
CHANGES IN LIFE SATISFACTION P179

score for interaction with relatives in 1983 was 44.3 (SD = ing independence and lower scores, decreasing indepen-
47.18). dence (M = -0.16, SD = 0.89).
Second, interactions with friends was assessed by re-
sponses to the following question: "Of the friends (includ- Stability-transition groups.—The major independent vari-
ing any in household) you feel closest to, about how often able, marital stability-transition, was determined by asking
do you get together with any of them?" Again, the number respondents about who lived in their households in 1983
of people seen was multiplied by a weighting factor. The and again in 1990. For the purposes of this study, four
mean score for interaction with friends in 1983 was 48.6 groups were identified on the basis of presence (P) or ab-
(SD = 88.3). sence (A) of a spouse. Spousal presence obviously referred

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/psychsocgerontology/article-abstract/56/3/P176/2965076 by guest on 11 April 2020


Finally, an interaction with neighbors variable was as- to the presence of a spouse who lived in the home. Spousal
sessed by responses to "How often do you get together with absence from the household could be due to having never
the neighbour which you see most frequently?" The respon- been married, the death or institutionalization of a spouse,
dents could choose between every day (1), at least once a or divorce or separation. The P or A groups were further
week (2), a few times a month (3), about once a month (4), classified as having stability in marital status or as having
and anything less than once a month (5). Scores on this experienced a transition.
4-point scale were reverse coded such that high scores re- Table 1 shows the four groups identified in the present
flected more interaction. The mean interaction-with-neigh- study, separately for women and men. The present—present
bor score was 3.8 (SD = 1.4) in 1983. (PP) stability group included those who reported living
with a spouse in both 1983 and 1990. These individuals ex-
Change in health status variables.—On the basis of par- perienced stability in the sense that they remained married
ticipants' responses to the interview questions, three mea- over the 7-year period of this study. As shown in Table 1, a
sures were created to assess changes that occurred in health greater proportion of men (27.34) relative to women (19.40)
status over 7 years. Perceived health was assessed both in were classified into the PP stability group.
1983 and in 1990 by asking, "For your age, would you say, Respondents who were not living with a spouse in either
in general, your health is good, fair, or poor?" The re- 1983 or 1990 were classified into the absent-absent (AA)
sponses were provided on a 5-point scale ranging from 1 stability group. The absence of a spouse was for a variety of
(excellent) to 5 (bad for your age). These scores were subse- reasons (e.g., never married, divorced or widowed before
quently reverse coded such that high scores reflected excel- 1983; see Appendix, Note 1). Although some of these indi-
lent health and low scores, poor health. By subtracting the viduals had undergone a transition before 1983, they had all
1983 score from the 1990 score, a variable was created to retained at least recent stability in marital status.
measure change in perceived health (M = —0.1, SD = The present-absent (PA) transition group included indi-
0.88). Scores ranged from —3.0 to 4.0, with higher (i.e., pos- viduals who were living with a spouse in 1983 but not in
itive) scores reflecting improvements in perceived health. 1990. The loss of a spouse was most often due to death
Morbidity was assessed in 1983 and again in 1990 by (87%). However, for a small percentage of the participants,
summing the number of health problems. Individuals identi- it was due to divorce (3%) or other reasons (11%) such as
fied these problems from a list of problems or diseases en- institutionalization of a spouse.
countered within the past year: arthritis or rheumatism; The final, absent-present (AP) transition group consisted
palsy; problems with eyes, ears, teeth, stomach, feet, skin, of those respondents who were not living with a spouse in
and other areas; heart-related problems (e.g., hardening of 1983 but were in 1990. In the majority of cases (83%), the
arteries, hypertension, heart attack); lung problems (e.g., presence of a spouse in 1990 was due to remarriage or being
emphysema, tuberculosis, breathing problems); stroke; kid- reunited with a separated spouse, although for 5 individuals
ney problems; and diabetes. A low score reflected low mor- (17%) the presence of a spouse in 1990 was indicative of a
bidity and a high score, high morbidity. Again, the 1983 first marriage. It is worth noting that fewer than 1% of both
score was subtracted from the 1990 score to create a change men and women in this study gained a spouse in late life.
in morbidity score. Higher scores indicated increased mor-
bidity; lower scores, decreased morbidity (M = 1.01, SD = Life satisfaction.—The dependent variable, life satisfac-
2.24). tion, was measured in 1983 and 1990 using Neugarten and
Functional independence was determined by asking the associates' (1961) LSIA. The LSIA was administered to
participants about their ability to perform basic activities of
daily living as identified in other standardized measures
(Branch, Katz, Kniepman, & Papsidero, 1984; Branch & Table 1. Marital Status Group Sizes (Frequences and Percentages)
Myers, 1987; Katz, Ford, Moskowitz, Jackson, & Jaffee, Men Women
1963): going up and down the stairs; getting about the
house; getting in and out of bed; washing or bathing or Status
grooming; dressing and putting shoes on; and eating. A Stability
functional independence score was created by summing the Present-present 596 27.34 423 19.40
activities that respondents were able to perform indepen- Absent-absent 153 7.02 657 30.14
dently. Thus, higher scores reflected greater independence. Transition
Present-absent 93 4.27 229 10.50
By subtracting the 1983 score from the 1990 score, a change
Absent-present 17 0.78 12 0.55
score was created such that higher scores reflected increas-
P180 CHIPPERFIELD AND HAVENS

study participants who were capable of responding to 20 1. For example, in examining the correlations between po-
statements (i.e., proxy responses were not obtained). These tential covariates and the LSIA change score for women
statements included such items as "I have gotten more of within the AP group, correlations were based on as few as
the breaks in life than most of the people I know" and "As I 15 individuals, whereas Table 1 shows 17 individuals in the
grow older, things seem better than I thought they would AP group.
be." Respondents were asked to tell the interviewer whether Table 2 shows that none of the demographic or social
they agreed, disagreed, or were not sure whether each state- support variables correlated significantly with change in life
ment was true for them. Only a small percentage (under 9%) satisfaction. Some large correlations emerged for women in
of individuals had missing values on more than 10 of the 20 the AP group; however, due to the very small sample (n =

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/psychsocgerontology/article-abstract/56/3/P176/2965076 by guest on 11 April 2020


LSIA items (see Appendix, Note 2). A mean LSIA score 15), these are nonsignificant and likely to be quite unreli-
was created for each individual, ranging from 0 to 1. In able. In contrast, there was some evidence for associations
1983, the mean LSIA index score was .71 (SD = .18), with between changes in health and in life satisfaction. Table 2
a Cronbach's alpha of .77. In 1990, the mean LSIA index shows that this relationship was most apparent for the per-
score was .67 (SD = .20), with a Cronbach's alpha of .74. ceived health variable: Those who perceived their health to
be deteriorating over the 7 years of the study showed more
RESULTS evidence of declining satisfaction with life. This was espe-
cially true for women, with three of the correlations exceed-
Preliminary Analyses: Identifying Covariates ing .20. Changes in independence on activities of daily liv-
Before addressing the major question of whether changes ing, although significantly correlated with LSIA changes for
in life satisfaction varied depending on the stability of or women in the PP and AA groups, were low (.127 to .154).
transition in marital status, preliminary correlational analy-
ses were undertaken to identify potential covariates. Poten- Repeated Measures
tial variables included demographics (age, education, and We conducted repeated measures analyses with Proc
income), social support (interaction with relatives, neigh- Mixed (SAS Institute, Inc., 1992, p. 229) to examine differ-
bors, and friends), and heath status (perceived health, mor- ences in patterns of life satisfaction as they related to gender
bidity, and functional capacity). Each of these variables was and marital status stability-transition. The design was a split
assessed in relation to the change in LSIA scores over time, plot, involving a gender (male or female) X stability-transi-
which was measured by subtracting a respondent's LSIA tion group (PP, A A, PA, or AP) X year (1983 or 1990) 2 X
score in 1990 from his or her LSIA score in 1983. A posi- 4 X 2 analysis of covariance model with repeated measures
tive difference score indicated an increase and a negative on life satisfaction. Change in perceived health and inde-
difference score indicated a decline in life satisfaction over pendence were initially retained as covariates because of
time. their correlations (p < .05) with change in life satisfaction
Because the relationships between each potential covari- (Table 2). However, the subsequently reported analysis ex-
ate and the LSIA change might differ by gender and stabil- cludes these covariates because, when included, they did
ity-transition group, we examined correlations separately not alter the overall effects of other independent variables
for men and women in the four groups (i.e., PP, PA, AP, (see Appendix, Note 3).
AA). It should be noted that, owing to missing values on the A significant main effect emerged for the between factor,
potential covariates, the sample sizes for the calculation of a Stability-Transition Group, F(3,2172) = 30.79, p < .0001
given coefficient may differ from those presented in Table (Table 3). However, the main effect for Stability-Transition

Table 2. Correlations Between Changes in Life Satisfaction Scores and Selected Variables for Women and Men
Women Men
Variable PP AA PA AP PP AA PA AP
Demographics
Age -.076 -.092 -.041 -.138 -.082 -.090 .007 .100
Education -.092 -.003 -.005 -.077 -.045 .023 .211 .004
Income .011 -.041 -.086 -.103 -.030 .027 -.085 -.081
Social Support
Relatives .067 -.015 .073 -.037 -.006 .068 .085 -.247
Neighbors .027 -.045 -.083 -.419 -.026 -.067 -.017 -.028
Friends -.076 -.014 .143 -.160 -.028 .007 .107 -.191
Health Status3
Perceived health .270* .245* .181* -.387 .215* .189 .100 -.296
Morbidity -.127 .093 .100 -.150 .059 -.205 .051 -.217
Independence ADL .127* .154* -.003 .611 .068 -.089 .255 -.086

Note: PP = present-present; AA = absent-absent; PA = present-absent; AP = absent-present.


a
Health status measures reflect changes in health status over the 7 years of the study. Positive correlations indicate that increasing life satisfaction over time was as-
sociated with improved perceived health, independence, and morbidity.
*p < .01.
CHANGES IN LIFE SATISFACTION P181

Table 3. Summary of Repeated Measures of Analysis: Effects of the within-subject error term, the degrees of freedom for
Gender and Stability/Transition (ST) Group Over 7 Years on each comparison was 2172. Finally, effect sizes (ES) were
Reported Life Satisfaction calculated using Hedge's g standardized mean differences
Source SS F P procedure (R. Rosenthal, 1994) for each significant compar-
df
ison.
Between Subject
Gender 1 0.072 1.48 .224
4.495 30.79 .000 Stability in martial status.—Figure 1 illustrates the lon-
ST group 3
3 0.161 1.11 .345 gitudinal patterns of life satisfaction for seniors who experi-
Gender X ST Group
105.682 enced stability in marital status. Simple main effect tests re-

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/psychsocgerontology/article-abstract/56/3/P176/2965076 by guest on 11 April 2020


Error 2,172
Within Subject vealed a significant decline in life satisfaction for women,
Year 1 0.005 0.23 .631 both those women in the PP category (i.e., those women
1 0.016 0.76 .383
Gender X Year who, over time, remained married), t = 4.30, p < .0001, ES =
ST Group X Year 3 1.075 16.70 .000
0.464 7.20 .000 .209, and those in the AA category (i.e., those who re-
ST Group X Gender X Year 3
Error 2,172 46.61 mained widowed or single), t — 4.96, p < .0001, ES =
.194. In contrast, for men, no significant declines were
found in the AA group, t = 0.37, p = .714, or in the PP
group, t = 3.07, p — .002, ES = .126. Rather, as shown in
(ST) Group is qualified by its interaction with Year, Figure 1, men's pattern of life satisfaction was one of con-
F(3,2172) = 16.70, p < .0001, and by a three-way interac- stancy. Thus, significant declines in life satisfaction among
tion between ST Group, Gender, and Year, F(3,2172) = those who experienced stability in marital status were re-
7.20, p < .0001. This three-way interaction, as illustrated in stricted to women. Moreover, the treatment-contrast inter-
Figures 1 and 2, indicates that changes in life satisfaction action for the AA group was significant, t = —2.49, p =
over time were not equivalent for women and men within .01, ES = -.224, SEs = .007 for women, .015 for men, in-
specific stability-transition groups. dicating that the slopes of the lines were significantly differ-
To consider the nature of the interaction, further tests ent for men and women. This was not true in the case of the
were carried out to assess the significance of treatment-con- PP group, where the slopes did not differ significantly, t =
trast interactions and simple main effects (Kirk, 1982). The -1.31, p = .19, SEs = .009 for women, .008 for men.
treatment-contrast interactions involved a comparison of
the mean LSIA scores over time for men and women, sepa- Transitions in marital status.—Figure 2 shows the lon-
rately within each of the four stability-transition groups. In gitudinal patterns of life satisfaction for those who experi-
other words, these tests examined gender differences in the enced a transition in marital status. Simple main effects of
slopes of LSIA scores. Tests of simple main effects con- the decline in life satisfaction were conducted first for the
sisted of comparisons of LSIA changes over time, sepa- PA group (i.e., the group with a spouse present in 1983 but
rately within each of the four stability-transition groups and absent in 1990; see Figure 2a). The results indicated that the
separately for women and men. Because there were eight decline was significant for both men, t = 6.91, p < .0001,
simple main effect comparisons, a corrected/? value of .05/8 ES = .717 and women, t = 4.11, p < .0001, ES = .272.
(p = .006) was used, and because each of these tests used Furthermore, the magnitude of the effects differed for men
and women, as suggested by the significant test of the treat-
ment-contrast interaction, t — 3.62, p = .0003, ES = .445,
SEs = .012 for women, .019 for men. Thus, for those who
Marital Status Stability lost their spouses the decline in LSIA over time was signifi-
Men cantly greater for men than women.
Women For the AP group (Figure 2b), the treatment-contrast in-
0.78
teraction was not significant, implying that the increase in
life satisfaction for men was not significantly greater than
0.76
the increase in life satisfaction for women, t = —1.32, p =
0.74
Present
.187, SEs = .054 for women, .045 for men. However, tests
0.72
of the simple main effects in the AP group showed that the
LSIA 0.70 increase in life satisfaction was significant for men, t =
Scores 0.68 -3.68, p = .0002, ES = -.893, but not for women, t =
0.66 Absent — 1.37, p = .17. Taken together, these findings suggest that
0.64 gaining a spouse has positive consequences for men's life
0.62 satisfaction but not for women's.
0.60
0.58
Subsequent Analyses
0.56
One explanation for the relatively stronger decline in life
1983 satisfaction that emerged for men (Figure 2a) following the
Year loss of a spouse is that the loss had occurred more recently
Figure 1. Patterns of life satisfaction over time for those who ex- (Barer, 1994), thereby leaving men less time than women to
perienced stability in marital status. LSIA = Life Satisfaction Index A. have adapted. This possibility was addressed in a subse-
P182 CHIPPERFIELD AND HAVENS

Marital Status Transitions

A Spouse Present (1983) - Spouse Absent (1990) B Spouse Absent (1983) - Spouse Present (1990)

0.78 0.78
0.76 0.76
0.74 0.74
0.72 0.72
0.70 0.70

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/psychsocgerontology/article-abstract/56/3/P176/2965076 by guest on 11 April 2020


LSIA 0.68 LSIA 0.68
Scores 0.66 Scores 0.66
0.64 0.64
0.62 0.62
0.60 0.60
0.58 0.58
0.56 0.56
0.54 0.54
0.52 0.52
0.50 0.50

1983 1990 1983 1990


Year Year

Figure 2. Patterns of life satisfaction over time for those who lost a spouse, A, and for those who gained a spouse, B. LSIA = Life Satisfaction
Index A; cT = men; ^ = women.

quent analysis of those who lost a spouse between 1983 and DISCUSSION
1990 (i.e., those in the PA group). Men, on average, had not The results from the present study add to the growing re-
lost their spouses more recently than women, ?(318) = search on the relationship between marital status and well-
—1.80, p > .05. Thus, recency of loss was not a viable ex- being (e.g., Lubben, 1989; Lund and colleagues, 1986).
planation for the relatively stronger decreases in life satis- Some of our results suggest that transitions and stability in
faction for men compared with women. marital status have different implications for older men and
An analysis was also conducted for the subset of individu- women. Among men, life satisfaction remained constant for
als (the AP group) who gained a spouse between 1983 and those whose marital status remained stable over 7 years, and
1990. This permitted an evaluation of whether an increase in it changed for those who experienced marital status transi-
life satisfaction for men was due to a "honeymoon effect" re- tions. The pattern was not as simple for women.
sulting from their having been married more recently than
women. No significant gender differences emerged in the Transitions in Marital Status
length of time since marriage, ?(16) = —1.73,;? > .05. Thus, The overall examination of life satisfaction among those
the increase in men's life satisfaction following marriage who lost a spouse and those who gained a spouse produced
does not appear to be explained by recency of marriage. a nearly mirror-image pattern (Figure 2a and 2b), suggest-
A final issue that we addressed involved a more refined ing an overall decline in life satisfaction for those who lost a
analysis to consider possible differences between the "always spouse and an increase for those who gained a spouse. How-
single" (AS) and those who were only "recently single" (RS), ever, further exploration of the patterns provided some evi-
that is, those who had lost a spouse before 1983. In our basic dence for gender differences. The decline in life satisfaction
group classification, all of these individuals were classified as following the loss of a spouse was found to be significantly
AA because they had no spouse in 1983 or 1990. However, more pronounced for men than for women, with a corre-
recent research has suggested the importance of studying al- spondingly large effect size (.445). This may suggest that
ways single or never married individuals as a special group of the loss of a spouse is relatively more detrimental to men
interest (e.g., Barrett, 1999; Ferguson, 2000). than women. Although men typically experience widow-
We compared these two groups (AS, RS) by using an hood at a later age than women, leaving them relatively less
analysis similar to that shown in Table 3; however, the four- time to adjust to their loss (Barer, 1994), the recency of loss
level stability-transition group variable was replaced with in the present study could not account for men's more pro-
the two-level AS-RS group variable. The results revealed a nounced decline in life satisfaction following the loss of
significant effect for the AS-RS comparison, F( 1,806) 6.47, their wives.
p < .05; AS seniors reported significantly higher levels of This gender difference in declining well-being following
life satisfaction than their RS counterparts. However, no the loss of a spouse is entirely consistent with empirical evi-
differences between the groups emerged in their patterns of dence from the field of social support. Unlike women, men
LSIA change over time, F(l,806) = 1.04,p = .301, nor did often view their spouse as their only close confidant (Chap-
LSIA changes over time between the groups interact with pell, 1989; Strain & Chappell, 1982) or the first in their
gender, F(l,806) = 2.38,p = .124. companion network (Connidis & Davies, 1990). In other
CHANGES IN LIFE SATISFACTION P183

words, women's spouses may be one among several rather partner pool. This implies that men have more opportunity
than one among few confidants. If men's emotional well- than women to select a compatible mate, thereby enhancing
being is partly determined by access to a confidant relation- their satisfaction with life.
ship, widowhood may result in the loss of their only confi- Still another possible reason for men's gains in life satis-
dant. In addition, men's social interactions with other family faction following late-life marriage is that a wife may have
members could be undermined by the loss of a wife who has a health-promoting influence, in turn leading to greater life
been the kin-keeper, "the one who attempts to keep family satisfaction. This is consistent with the findings of a recent
members in touch with one another" (C. J. Rosenthal, 1985, study (Steinberg Schone & Weinick, 1998) in which mar-
p. 965). Without a "kin-keeper" to foster family interac- riage had a relatively larger positive impact on health be-

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/psychsocgerontology/article-abstract/56/3/P176/2965076 by guest on 11 April 2020


tions, widowed men may be prone to greater degrees of iso- haviors for elderly men than for elderly women. In short,
lation and loneliness than women who have undergone a there are numerous potential explanations for men's gains
similar loss. Finally, social support at a broad level appears in life satisfaction following late-life marriage that are
to buffer the negative impact of widowhood (Lin & Ensel, worth examining in future research.
1989), and as women typically have more extensive support
networks, social support may protect them from some of the Stability in Marital Status
negative consequences of widowhood. The present findings also showed that among those indi-
Although men may suffer more psychological conse- viduals whose marital status remained stable, men's life sat-
quences than women following the loss of a spouse, our re- isfaction remained consistent over time, whereas women's
sults suggest that, unlike women, they derive benefit when declined. The stability in men's life satisfaction is congruent
marrying late in life. It must, however, be noted that the with the results from several large-scale longitudinal studies
small cell size of this group who gained a spouse in late life of well-being, one involving a 9-year follow-up (Costa and
limits our ability to draw conclusions. Nonetheless, the colleagues, 1987) and another, a 14-year follow-up (Stacey
large effect size (—.893) suggests the importance of the & Gatz, 1991). The declining life satisfaction for women in
finding that men experience an increase in life satisfaction the present study is congruent with some findings, such as
with remarriage late in life. In addition, it is interesting that the declining life satisfaction over a 12-year period (1971 to
the magnitude of increases in men's life satisfaction after 1983) in our earlier analyses of very old women in the AIM
gaining a spouse (AP group) appeared to be approximately study. On the other hand, the declining satisfaction is incon-
equal to the magnitude of their declines in satisfaction after gruent with findings from Costa and colleagues' and Stacey
losing a spouse (PA group), as is illustrated in Figure 2 by and Gatz's studies. Age variation in samples may partly ex-
the mirror-image patterns of changing life satisfaction for plain the inconsistency in findings across these studies; the
men who experienced opposing transitions in marital status. present study focused on older individuals compared with
The gain in life satisfaction for men following the attain- Costa and associates' study of 25- to 74-year-olds and Sta-
ment of a spouse may also be due to changes in the social cey and Gatz's study of 15- to 100-year-olds. In fact, Bar-
relationships that accompany the presence of a spouse. rett's (1999) analysis of never-married adults showed that
Whether the role of a wife is direct (e.g., access to a confi- the effects of never being married were greatest among el-
dant) or indirect (promotion of interactions outside the derly individuals, suggesting inconsistent findings may well
spousal relationship), the enhanced exposure to social net- occur across studies of different age groups.
works and increased social activity may foster a more ful- Clearly, our findings suggest that life satisfaction deterio-
filling social experience for men. The potential for psycho- rates for older women who remain in a stable marital condi-
logical gain is consistent with the fact that men are more tion (i.e., never married, always married). What would ex-
likely than women to remarry (Burch, 1990) and they re- plain their deterioration in life satisfaction over the 7 years
marry more quickly than women (Wu, 1995), implying, per- of this study? One explanation is based on women's roles as
haps, a greater motivation to do so. caregivers and the burdens associated with caregiving. Pro-
Although the positive benefit in life satisfaction for men viding care in later life is associated with severe psycholog-
is likely partly due to the social facilitation effect of a wife, ical burdens (Jones & Peters, 1992; Wijeratne & Lovestone,
other factors are surely involved. For example, although we 1996; Zanetti, Magni, Sandri, Frisoni, & Bianchetti, 1996),
did not find age to relate significantly to life satisfaction and and women are more likely than men to be in caregiver
we did statistically adjust for health, we cannot entirely rule roles (e.g., Davis, 1991) and to suffer from the burden
out the possibility that our results partly reflect a selection (Grafstrom, Fratiglioni, & Winblad, 1994). Thus, women's
bias in which younger and healthier men may be more declining reports of life satisfaction could be a reaction to
likely to remarry. Another interesting possibility is that men the psychological costs of providing care. A related possible
marry more compatible mates than women and therefore are explanation for women's declining satisfaction is that they
happier and more content in their late-life marriages. This may have increasing unmet needs. This is consistent with
"compatibility" hypothesis is derived from the work of Wu the finding that women report more unmet needs for help
(1995) and others who have pointed out that, due to the cul- than men (Chipperfield, 1996).
tural norms that dictate that men marry younger women
who are often less educated, there is an oversupply of avail- Cautions
able mates from which men can select. In contrast, for For several reasons, caution is required in the interpreta-
women, who tend to marry older, more educated men, there tion of our finding. First, our interpretation of the social fa-
is an undersupply of men in the potential late-life marriage- cilitation effect of women on their spouses' life satisfaction,
P184 CHIPPERFIELD AND HAVENS

which is based on a substantial literature in this field (e.g., tempts to acquire representative samples, the present AIM
see Lubben, 1989, for a review), was not totally supported sample of survivors remains representative of survivors in
by subsequent analyses of our social interaction variables. the overall provincial and national population (Chipperfield
Among men, those who were married did have more inter- et al., 1997). Thus, the patterns of life satisfaction that
actions with relatives (M = 52.52) compared with their un- emerged in this study are likely to generalize to the broader
married counterparts (M = 39.85); however, this difference population of survivors.
was not significant. Moreover, married men did not have Finally, our analysis is subject to the same criticism made
higher levels of interaction with neighbors or friends than about studies that examine predictors of living arrangement.
their unmarried counterparts. Perhaps more surprising, over- In particular, Wilmoth (1998) has argued that multiple tran-

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/psychsocgerontology/article-abstract/56/3/P176/2965076 by guest on 11 April 2020


all comparisons of men and women revealed that men had sitions may be obscured when living arrangements are as-
significantly more interactions with family (M = 49.05 vs sessed at two interview dates. In other words, "respondents
43.37), f(1910) = 2.23, p < .05, and with friends (M = could have experienced several transitions during the period
144.61 vs 120.00), r(1749) = 1.99, p < .05. Although these but only the difference between the beginning state and end-
supplemental preliminary analyses did not support the inter- ing state is modeled" (Wilmoth, 1998, p. 434). To consider
pretations outlined earlier, it may be that different findings this possibility of multiple transitions, we examined the re-
would emerge from further detailed analyses of the relation- ported length of time married for the group of individuals
ships between marital status stability-transitions and social who reported having a spouse in 1983 and also in 1990 (i.e.,
support. Analyses of measures other than frequency of in- the PP group). In fact, there were individuals who reported
teractions (e.g., perceived support, confidant support) that being married fewer than 3 years, suggesting that a transi-
are beyond the scope of this study might reveal findings tion was obscured in our PP classification. However, the
more consistent with the past literature. percentage of misclassification was extremely small (2%),
Second, as in all quasi-experimental and field research, indicating that this was a minor concern in the present
we were unable to rule out potentially important third vari- study. Moreover, given the low rate of remarriage in later
ables that may be responsible for a spurious relationship be- life, this does not become a particularly compelling con-
tween marital status stability-transitions and life satisfac- cern.
tion. For example, the stress of coping with a spouse's
debilitating arthritis might lead to divorce, and the docu- Conclusions
mented decline in life satisfaction may be due to the stress Our study demonstrated different patterns of life satisfac-
rather than the divorce (i.e., the marital status transition). In tion for men and women, for both those who experienced
short, we cannot rule out a variety of third-variable explana- marital status transitions and those who retained stability in
tions for the reported relationships in our study. their marital status. These results can be interpreted pessi-
Third, as previously pointed out, the group who remar- mistically or optimistically. With respect to women, the
ried consisted of a very small number of individuals. This pessimistic view is that life satisfaction generally declined
may, in part, explain why we did not find the predicted over time, the declines occurring for those women who lost
greater increase in life satisfaction for men who remarried their husbands but also for those whose marital status re-
than for women. On the other hand, the increase in life satis- mained stable over the study. Thus, for the majority of
faction after remarriage was restricted to men. Women did women, life satisfaction may be at risk in old age. The opti-
not experience a similar increase in life satisfaction, which mistic view of our findings is that life satisfaction did not
is consistent with the view that men's life satisfaction may decline among older women who marry, suggesting that
be beneficially affected by late-life marriage, but women's later life marriage may offset the expected decline in life
may not. satisfaction. For men, the pessimistic view is that they ap-
Fourth, our study did not allow for an in-depth analysis of pear to be at a relatively greater risk than women for suffer-
the many possible explanations for gender differences in ing psychological consequences following the loss of a
patterns of life satisfaction. To understand how marital sta- spouse. This finding is consistent with the reported gender
tus stability and transitions relate to life satisfaction in later differences in survival following the death of a spouse (e.g.,
life, it is essential to consider the complexity of the spousal Biondi & Picardi, 1996; Bowling & Windsor, 1995). The
relationship. A variety of factors are important, from basic need to assist men in their adjustment to the loss of a spouse
marital compatibility (Wu, 1995) to perceptions of relation- is underscored by the elevated rate of widowhood among
ship standards and opportunities for future improvements men that has occurred in the past decade (Martin Matthews,
(see Dykstra, 1995). Although the present study did not al- 1994) and that will likely continue in the future. Ultimately,
low for an analysis of the complexity of spousal relation- men's ability to adapt to widowhood will likely also influ-
ships, it documented patterns of life satisfaction in a unique ence their physical health in later life and perhaps their lon-
way, considering both spousal presence versus absence and gevity. On a more optimistic note, with the exception of
stability versus transitions in marital status. those who lost spouses, men generally experienced either
Fifth, selective mortality complicates our understanding stability or increased life satisfaction over the 7 years of our
of how life satisfaction changes over time, just as it does in study. This implies that, with the exception of those men
any research that examines survivors. Because measures of who lost spouses, men are likely to maintain their psycho-
1990 life satisfaction were obviously unavailable for those logical health in later life.
AIM participants who died before 1990, they were excluded Taken together, these findings are consistent with the no-
from the present analysis. Nonetheless, due to rigorous at- tion that the presence of a spouse buffers men against de-
CHANGES IN LIFE SATISFACTION P185

clining life satisfaction in late life, highlighting the impor- Chipperfield, J. G. (1996). Perceived adequacy of instrumental assistance:
tance of a spouse for men in late life. However, for women, Implications for well being in later life. Journal of Aging and Health, 8,
72-95.
satisfaction with life appears to deteriorate if they remain Chipperfield, J. G., Havens, B., & Doig, W. (1997). Methods and descrip-
married as well as if they lose their spouses. Despite tion of the Aging in Manitoba Project: A 20-year longitudinal study.
women's relatively greater social interaction than men, all Canadian Journal on Aging, 76(4), 606-625.
social interactions may be decreasing at older ages, thus Connidis, I. A., & Davies, L. (1990). Confidants and companions in later
life: The place of family and friends. Journal of Gerontology: Social
contributing to declining satisfaction with life. It may also Sciences, 45, S141-S149.
be that women's dissatisfaction with life is less firmly con- Connidis, I. A., & McMullin, J. (1993). To have or have not: Parents status
nected to their marital status and that their declining satis-

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/psychsocgerontology/article-abstract/56/3/P176/2965076 by guest on 11 April 2020


and subjective well-being of older men and women. The Gerontologist,
faction is influenced by a broader range of factors associ- 33, 630-636.
ated with growing old in contemporary society. A careful Costa, P. T., Zonderman, A. B., McCrae, R. R., Cornoni-Hyntley, J., Locke,
B. Z., & Barbano, H. E. (1987). Longitudinal analyses of psychological
analysis of these issues is required before a more precise un- well-being in a national sample: Stability of mean levels. Journal of
derstanding of the relationship between marital status tran- Gerontology, 42, 50-55.
sitions and life satisfaction will emerge. What is clear from Gumming, E., & Henry, W. E. (1961). Growing old: The process of disen-
the present study is that the implications of marital status gagement. New York: Basic Books.
D'Amato, T. J. (1987). Factors causing variation in the life satisfaction of
stability-transitions are likely to differ for men and women. the elderly. National Journal of Sociology, 1, 54-72.
Davis, K. (1991). Life satisfaction and older adults (Background Paper Se-
ries No. 6, 36). New York: Commonwealth Fund, Americans Over 55
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS at Work Program.
This research was supported by a Canadian Institutes of Health Research Day, A., & Day, L. (1993). Living arrangements and "successful" ageing
Career Award to Judith G. Chipperfield. We appreciate the cooperation of among ever-married American White women 77-87 years of age. Age-
Manitoba Health in the construction of the samples for our study. Thanks ing and Society, 13, 365-387.
also go to Wendy Doig and Darren Campbell for their assistance with data Dykstra, P. A. (1995). Loneliness among the never and formerly married:
analyses. The importance of supportive friendships and a desire for indepen-
dence. Journal of Gerontology: Social Sciences, SOB, S321—S329.
Address correspondence to Judith G. Chipperfield, Health, Leisure &
Farnsworth, J., Pett, M. A., & Lund, D. A. (1989). Predictors of loss man-
Human Performance Research Institute, 305 Max Bell Centre, Winnipeg,
agement and well-being in later life widowhood and divorce. Journal
Manitoba R3T 2N2, Canada. E-mail: chipper@ms.umanitoba.ca
of Family Issues, 10, 102-121.
Fengler, A. P., Danigelis, N., & Little, V. C. (1983). Later life satisfaction
and household structure: Living with others and living alone. Ageing
REFERENCES and Society, 3, 357-377.
Allen, S. M., Ciambrone, D., & Welch, L. C. (2000). Stage of life course Ferguson, S. J. (2000). Challenging traditional marriage: Never married
and social support as a mediator of mood state among persons with dis- Chinese American and Japanese women. Gender and Society, 14(1),
abilities. Journal of Aging and Health, 12, 318-341. 136-159.
Barer, B. M. (1994). Men and women aging differently. International Geis, J. A., & Klein, H. A. (1990). The relationship of life satisfaction to
Journal of Aging and Human Development, 38, 29-40. life change among the elderly. The Journal of Genetic Psychology,
Barrett, A. (1999). Social support and life satisfaction among the never 757(2), 269-271.
married. Research on Aging, 27(1), 46-72. Girzadas, P. M., Counte, M. A., Glandon, G. L., & Tancredi, D. (1993). An
Bennett, K. M., & Morgan, K. (1992). Health, social functioning, and mar- analysis of elderly health and life satisfaction. Behavior, Health, and
ital status: Stability and change among elderly recently widowed Aging, 3(2), 103-117.
women. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 7, 813-817. Grafstrom, M., Fratiglioni, L., & Winblad, B. (1994). Caring for an elderly
Biondi, M., & Picardi, A. (1996). Clinical and biological aspects of be- person: Predictors of burden in dementia care. International Journal of
reavement and loss-induced depression: A reappraisal. Psychotherapy Geriatric Psychiatry, 9, 373-379.
and Psychosomatics, 65(5), 229-245. Herriott, M., & Kiyak, H. A. (1981). Bereavement in old age: Implications
Bowling, A. (1987). Mortality after bereavement: A review of the literature for therapy and research. Journal of Gerontological Social Work, .?(3),
on survival periods and factors affecting survival. Social Science Medi- 15-43.
cine,24, 117-124. Holicky, R., & Charlifue, S. (1999). Ageing and spinal cord injury: The
Bowling, A., & Windsor, J. (1995). Death after widow(er)hood: An analy- impact of spousal support. Disability and Rehabilitation: An Interna-
sis of mortality rates up to 13 years after bereavement. Omega: Journal tional Multidisciplinary Journal, 21, 5-6.
of Death and Dying, 31(1), 35^9. Hong, L. K., & Duff, R. W. (1997). Relative importance of spouses, chil-
Branch, L. G., Katz, S., Kniepman, K., & Papsidero, J. A. (1984). A pro- dren, and friends in the life satisfaction of retirement community
spective study of functional status among community elders. American residents. Journal of Clinical Geropsychology, 3(4), 275-282.
Journal of Public Health, 74, 266-268. Jones, D. A., & Peters, T. J. (1992). Caring for elderly dependents: Effects
Branch, L. G., & Myers, A. R. (1987). Assessing physical function in the on the carer's quality of life. Age and Ageing, 21, 421-428.
elderly. Clinics in Geriatric Medicine, 3, 29-51. Katz, S., Ford, A. B., Moskowitz, R. W., Jackson, B. A., & Jaffee, M. W.
Bulcroft, K., Bulcroft, R., Hatch, L., & Borgatta, E. F. (1989). Antecedents (1963). Studies of illness in the aged. The index of ADL: A standard-
and consequences of remarriage in later life. Research on Aging, 11, ized measure of biological and psychological function. Journal of the
82-106. American Medical Association, 185, 914—919.
Burch, T. K. (1990). Remarriage of older Canadians: Description and inter- Kehn, D. J. (1995). Predictors of elderly happiness. Activities, Adaptation,
pretation. Research on Aging, 12(4), 546-559. and Aging, 19(3), 11-30.
Burks, V. K., Lund, D. A., Gregg, C. H., & Bluhm, H. P. (1988). Bereave- Kirk, R. E. (1982). Experimental design: Procedures for the behavioral sci-
ment and remarriage for older adults. Death Studies, 12, 51-60. ences (2nd ed.). Belmont, CA: Brooks/Cole.
Calasanti, T. M. (1996). Gender and life satisfaction in retirement: An as- Kousha, M., & Mohseni, N. (1997). Predictors of life satisfaction among
sessment of the male model. Journal of Gerontology: Social Sciences, urban Iranian women: An exploratory analysis. Social Indicators Re-
5IB, S18-S29. search, 40(3), 329-357.
Chappell, N. L. (1989). Health and helping among the elderly. Journal of Lin, N., & Ensel, W. M. (1989). Life stressors and health: Stressors and re-
Aging and Health, 1, 102-120. sources. American Sociological Review, 54, 382-399.
Chappell, N. L., & Badger, M. (1989). Social isolation and well-being. Lubben, J. E. (1989). Gender differences in the relationship of widowhood
Journal of Gerontology: Social Sciences, 44, S169-S176. and psychological well-being among low income elderly. In L. Grau
P186 CHIPPERFIELD AND HAVENS

(Ed.), Women in the later years: Health, social, and cultural perspec- Wijeratne, C., & Lovestone, S. (1996). Pilot study comparing psychologi-
tives (pp. 161-189). New York: Harrington Park Press. cal and physical morbidity in carers of elderly people with dementia
Lund, D. A. (1989). Conclusions about bereavement in later life and impli- and those with depression. International Journal of Geriatric Psychia-
cations for interventions and future research. In D. A. Lund (Ed.), try, 11, 741-744.
Older bereaved spouses: Research with practical applications (pp. Wilmoth, J. M. (1998). Living arrangement transitions among America's
217-231). New York: Hemisphere. older adults. The Gerontologist, 38, 434-444.
Lund, D. A., Caserta, M. S., & Dimond, M. F. (1986). Gender differences Wolinsky, F. D., & Johnson, R. J. (1992). Widowhood, health status, and
through two years of bereavement among the elderly. The Gerontolo- the use of health services by older adults: A cross-sectional and pro-
gist, 26, 314-320. spective approach. Journal of Gerontology, 47, S8-S16.
Martikainen, P., & Valkonen, T. (1996). Mortality after death of a spouse: Wu, Z. (1995). Remarriage after widowhood: A marital history study of

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/psychsocgerontology/article-abstract/56/3/P176/2965076 by guest on 11 April 2020


Rates and causes of death in a large Finnish cohort. American Journal older Canadians. Canadian Journal on Aging, 14(4), 719-736.
of Public Health, 86, 1087-1093. Zanetti, O., Magni, E., Sandri, C., Frisoni, G. B., & Bianchetti, A. (1996).
Martin Matthews, A. (1994). Widowhood no longer an expectable life Determinants of burden in an Italian sample of Alzheimer's patient care-
event. Linkage: The Canadian Aging Research Network, 4(2), 2. givers. Journal of Cross Cultural Gerontology, 11(1), 17-27.
Mookherjee, H. N. (1997). Marital status, gender, and perception of well- Zick, C. D., & Smith, K. R. (1988). Recent widowhood, remarriage, and
being. Journal of Social Psychology, 137(1), 95-105. changes in economic well-being. Journal of Marriage and the Family,
Morris, D. C. (1997). Health, finances, religious involvement, and life sat- 50, 233-244.
isfaction of older adults. Journal of Religious Gerontology, 10(2), 3-17.
Neugarten, B. L., Havighurst, R. J., & Tobin, S. S. (1961). The measure-
ment of life satisfaction. Journal of Gerontology, 16, 134-143.
Osterweis, M., Solomon, F., & Green, M. (1984). Bereavement: Reactions, Received March 29, 2000
consequences, and care. Washington, DC: National Academy Press. Accepted November 6, 2000
Reinhardt, J. P., & Fisher, C. B. (1989). Kinship versus friendship: Social Decision Editor: Margie E. Lachman, PhD
adaptation in married and widowed elderly women. In L. Grau (Ed.),
Women in the later years: Health, social, and cultural perspectives (pp.
161-189). New York: Harrington Park Press. Appendix
Riddick, C. C. (1985). Life satisfaction determinants of older males and fe-
males. Leisure Sciences, 7, 47-63.
Rosenthal, C. J. (1985). Kinkeeping in the familial division of labor. Jour-
Notes
nal of Marriage and the Family, 47(4), 965-974. 1. Within the AA group, 20% (n = 161) had never mar-
Rosenthal, R. (1994). Parametric measures of effect sizes. In H. Cooper & ried and 79% (n = 636) had either been widowed or di-
L. V. Hedges (Eds.), The handbook of research synthesis (pp. 231- vorced before 1983. Fewer than 1% (n = 8) indicated that
243). New York: Russell Sage Foundation. they were married in 1990 but that they had not lived with
Ryff, C. D., & Keyes, C. L. M. (1995). The structure of psychological
well-being revisited. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, their spouse in 1983 or 1990. In the latter case, it is assumed
719-727. that the spouse was still alive but living elsewhere, perhaps
SAS Institute, Inc. (1992). SAS Technical Report. In SAS/STAT Software: in a long-term care facility.
Changes and Enhancements (Release 6.07, p. 229) Gary, NC: Author. 2. The major analyses were also conducted excluding the
Spreitzer, E., & Snyder, E. E. (1974). Correlates of life satisfaction among
the aged. Journal of Gerontology, 29, 454-458.
individuals who had responded to fewer than 10 of the 20
Stacey, C. A., & Gatz, M. (1991). Cross-sectional age differences and lon- LSI A items. The results did not differ from those reported in
gitudinal change on the Bradburn Affect Balance Scale. Journal of Table 3.
Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 46, P76-P78. 3. In the expanded analysis, the effect was not significant
Steinberg Schone, B., & Weinick, R. M. (1998). Health-related behaviors for the covariate, perceived health, F( 1,2146) = 0.31, p =
and the benefits of marriage for elderly persons. The Gerontologist, 38,
618-627. .58, but was significant for independence, F( 1,2146) =
Strain, L. A., & Chappell, N. L. (1982). Confidants: Do they make a differ- 14.30, p < .001. Consistent with the reported findings in Ta-
ence in quality of life? Research on Aging, 4, 479-502. ble 3, a significant main effect did emerge for Stability-Tran-
Strain, L. A., & Payne, B. J. (1992). Social networks and patterns of social sition Group, F(3,2146) = 27.90, p < .0001, and significant
interaction among ever-single and separated/divorced elderly Canadi-
ans. Canadian Journal on Aging, 11(1), 31-53.
interactions emerged for Stability-Transition Group X Year,
Stroebe, M. S., & Stroebe, W. (1983). Who suffers more? Gender differences F = 15.32, p = .0001, and for Stability-Transition Group X
in health risks of the widowed. Psychological Bulletin, 93, 279-301. Year X Gender, F(3,2146) = 5.31, p < .005.

You might also like