You are on page 1of 13

931915

research-article2020
JPEXXX10.1177/0739456X20931915Journal of Planning Education and ResearchDumbaugh et al.

Planning Research

Journal of Planning Education and Research

Toward Safe Systems: Traffic


1­–13
© The Author(s) 2020
Article reuse guidelines:
Safety, Cognition, and the Built sagepub.com/journals-permissions
DOI: 10.1177/0739456X20931915
https://doi.org/10.1177/0739456X20931915

Environment journals.sagepub.com/home/jpe

Eric Dumbaugh1 , Dibakar Saha1, and Louis Merlin1

Abstract
Conventional transportation practice attributes traffic crashes to human error, leading to the prevailing assumption that
crash prevention is principally an outcome of driver education and law enforcement programs. But what if planning and urban
design decisions induce human errors? In this study, we examine the literature in organizational systems safety, cognitive
psychology, and behavioral economics to detail how cognitive interpretations of the built environment may produce the
errors that result in traffic crashes. We proceed to examine crash incidence in Charlotte-Mecklenberg County in light of this
cognitive framework and discuss its implications for research and practice.

Keywords
design, transportation, urban form

The basic principle of Safe Systems starts with the insight that injury through the 1990s, the safety improvements attribut-
human error should no longer be seen as the primary cause of able to these strategies appears to have tapered off, and traf-
crashes. Instead, road crashes are seen as a consequence of fic fatalities have begun to increase in the United States over
latent failures created by decisions and actions within the the last decade, particularly for pedestrians (NHTSA 2017).
broader organizational, social or political system which Much of the problem is the design of our transportation
establishes the context in which road users act. infrastructure; after controlling for safety improvements
—International Transport Forum (ITF) 2016, 27. attributable to increased seat belt use, reductions in drunk
driving, and improvements to emergency services, the
changes to our transportation infrastructure have been found
Introduction not to reduce fatal crashes, but to instead increase them
Traffic safety, and particularly a concern for the safety of (Noland 2003; Noland and Oh 2004). Over the last three
pedestrians and cyclists, has long been a concern of the plan- decades, seat belt usage rates have climbed to 90 percent,
ning profession. Nonetheless, the prevailing model for while fatalities attributable to drunk driving have been
addressing traffic safety is one that provides little opportu- reduced by nearly 50 percent (NHTSA 2019). Collectively,
nity for planners to meaningfully influence safety outcomes this suggests that new approaches are needed if we are to
(see Figure 1). Under this model, crashes are regarded prin- make meaningful advancements to traffic safety.
cipally as a product of human failures (National Highway Safe Systems (also referred to as “Sustainable Safety” in
Traffic Safety Administration [NHTSA] 2015), leading to the Netherlands), has emerged as a response to these trends.
the view that crash prevention is best achieved through pro- As an approach to road safety, Safe Systems is centered on
grams aimed at altering driver behavior, such as driver edu- the kinetic energy model, based on the recognition that
cation programs and advertising campaigns that seek to crash-related trauma is a direct function of the human body’s
discourage people from driving under the influence of drugs ability to withstand the forces that occur during a crash
or alcohol or texting while driving, among others. Design is impact (Corben et al. 2010). This has led to a four-pronged
viewed not as a means of crash prevention, but instead as a
mechanism for reducing the consequences of these otherwise Initial submission, March 2019; revised submissions, June and September
unpreventable human errors. Typically, this entails the appli- 2019; final acceptance, April 2020
cation of engineering countermeasures intended to “forgive” 1
Florida Atlantic University, Boca Raton, FL, USA
these errors, such as wide travel lanes and roadside clear
Corresponding Author:
zones (Dumbaugh 2005). Eric Dumbaugh, Florida Atlantic University, 777 Glades Road, SO 284C,
While education, enforcement, and vehicle safety systems Boca Raton, FL 33431, USA.
have been successful in reducing traffic-related death and Email: edumbaug@fau.edu
2 Journal of Planning Education and Research 00(0)

penetrated road safety research, practice, or policy in any


meaningful manner” (p. 1837).
The purpose of this article is to begin to define such an
approach, particularly as it relates to the planning and design
professions. Addressing traffic safety in urban environments
requires a better understanding of the manner in which the
built environment influences human cognition, and how cog-
Figure 1.  The current model for addressing traffic safety.
nition, in turn, influences the behaviors that result in traffic-
related crashes, deaths, and injuries. To do so, we examine
the literature in the domains of organizational systems safety,
cognitive psychology, and behavioral economics to detail
how the cognitive interpretation of the built environment
may influence the behaviors that lead to crash incidence. We
then proceed to examine crash incidence in Charlotte-
Mecklenberg County in light of the cognitive framework we
have proposed and conclude by discussing its applications
for future research and practice.

Error, Crash Causation, and the Built


Environment
The conventional wisdom, regularly encountered by plan-
ners and urban designers that engage in urban street design,
holds that “90% or more all crashes are attributable to driver
error” (NHTSA 2015). This factoid underpins the largely
unsupported assertion1 that automated vehicles will enhance
safety, which is based on the rationale that if we eliminate the
driver, we will therefore eliminate the errors they produce,
which would in turn lead to elimination of 90 percent of all
crashes.
Is it reasonable to assert that drivers are to blame for the
Figure 2.  A safe systems approach to traffic safety (International 90 percent of all crashes that occur? This idea first emerged
Transport Forum 2016).
in 1979 from a study published by Treat et al. and remains
the primary means for understanding crash causation through
approach to addressing safety: providing safe roads, ensur- the present (NHTSA 2015). To arrive at this assertion, Treat
ing safe speeds, designing safe vehicles, and encouraging and his coauthors examined data contained in police accident
safe behavior (see Figure 2). reports for Monroe County, Indiana, identifying the behav-
Of these four areas, two are of particular relevance to iors in which the participants were reported to have engaged
planners and urban designers: safe speeds and safe roads. immediately prior to the crash. Their assumption is that, had
Safe speeds is a relevantly straightforward concept that is these behaviors been altered, the crash would have been
already familiar to most planners and designers. Based on prevented.2 The logical exercise used in this analysis, known
the understanding that crash severity increases signifi- by behavioral economists as the simulation heuristic, entails
cantly and exponentially as a function of impact speeds the construction of an “if only” proposition (Kahneman and
(Rosen and Sander 2009; Zegeer et al. 2002), Safe Systems Tversky 1998; Roese 1997). If only the driver had not
asserts that “speed policy should seek to minimise the exceeded the speed limit, he or she would not have run off
kinetic energy released in a road traffic collision” the road. If only a pedestrian had used protected crosswalk,
(International Transport Forum [ITF] 2016). Less clear, he or she would not have been struck by the oncoming vehi-
but of no less importance, is the call for the creation of cle. Following this line of reasoning, which certainly has a
safer roads. The ITF states that “road safety needs to be a high degree of face validity, if we can encourage people to
central focus when wider urban planning decisions are reverse their pre-crash behaviors, we can reverse the crash
being made” (ITF 2016, 136). Absent from the literature, outcomes as well.
however, is any substantive discussion of what such a From a systems-safety perspective, however, such expla-
focus should entail, or how it is to be achieved. As stated nations are regarded as naïve (Leveson 2004). Contemporary
by Salmon, McClure, and Stanton (2012), “although many city streets, like other industrial systems, entail human inter-
have made the call for a systems approach, it has not actions with complex environments. While it is true that
Dumbaugh et al. 3

people may err in the use of these systems, these errors can- appropriate for their environment, given their prior knowl-
not be solely attributed to human fallibility; many of these edge and experience (Abelson 1981; Schank and Abelson
errors may instead be the result of a misalignment between 1977).
the system’s design and manner in which it is likely to be
used (Reason 1997). For planners and urban designers, this
The Role of Schema and Scripts in Common
suggests that the need for a greater understanding of the
manner in which people interpret and make behavioral deci- Crash Phenomenon
sions regarding their use of urban streets. Schema and scripts explain phenomena that are regularly
observed in the incidence of crashes involving pedestrians
Understanding Driver Behavior: Scripts and and cyclists. The first is popularly known as the “safety in
numbers” hypothesis, which finds that as pedestrians and
Schema cyclists become more prevalent in a particular environment,
Historically, safety researchers have applied utility models to their risk of being involved in a traffic crash declines
understand driving behavior, based on the idea that drivers (Jacobsen 2002; Leden 2002; Robinson 2005). From the per-
seek to maximize their speed or mobility in exchange for spective of human cognition, the increased presence of
accepting some level of situated risk (Blomquist 1986; Fuller pedestrians and cyclists in a particular environment would be
2011; Noland 2013; Peltzman 1975; Wilde 1982b). Yet driv- expected to lead to the production of refined schema and
ing behavior is often not the result of intentional decisions to scripts that prepare motorists to anticipate their presence,
maximize utility, but instead a mundane activity typically thus making them better able to respond to a conflicting
accomplished by intuitive and largely preconscious pro- pedestrian or bicyclist movement.
cesses (Perez et al. 2015). Individuals gleam information Yet environments can also produce the opposite effect;
from their environment by relating it into existing cognitive namely, that their design fails to trigger schema regarding
categories, which allow them to quickly and efficiently pro- hazards that are, in fact, present, thereby rendering them
cess large amounts of sensory information and instanta- cognitively “invisible.” This phenomenon, well documented
neously apply it to the situation at hand (Rosch 1978; Van in the psychology literature, is known as “inattentional
Elslande and Faucher-Alberton 1997). blindness,” which is the failure to observe something that is,
With respect to urban streets, this means that drivers infer in fact, present and readily visible (Chabris and Simons
an overall sense of a roadway based on their existing knowl- 2011; Mack and Rock 1998). In the case of traffic safety,
edge of, and experience with, similar “types” of roadways. inattentional blindness results in a crash type categorized
A simulation study has found that driver’s infer a roadway’s as “looked-but-failed-to-see,” a crash type that typically
“type” through the presence of a limited number of environ- involves pedestrians, bicyclists, and motorcyclists, and
mental cues, such as lane widths or the massing of roadside which has been estimated to account for 10 percent of all
development (Theeuwes 2012). This cognitive categoriza- fatal crashes (Brown 2002). In the case of inattentional
tion in turn establishes expectations regarding the features blindness, the roadway’s environment fails to prime schema
that are likely to be present in the environment, referred to relating to the presence of pedestrians or bicyclists, result-
by psychologists as schema. Schema are relevant because ing in the driver’s failure to “see” these users prior to crash-
humans are incapable of mentally processing the wide array ing into them, even when they were located in the driver’s
of information contained in their environment (Gigerenzer immediate cone of vision prior to the collision.
and Brighton 2009; Kahneman 2011), leading them to cog- That consistent and predictable patterns in the cognitive
nitively simplify the interpretive task by intuiting the ele- interpretation of the built environment lead to correspond-
ments that are likely to be present and useful, and then ingly predictable increases or reductions in crash incidence
filtering out most other (presumably irrelevant) sensory suggests we can no longer attribute crash-producing errors
information3 (Theeuwes 2002, 2012). In the language of purely, or even largely, to individualized patterns of human
behavioral economics, the characteristics of the built envi- error.
ronment have the ability to “prime” expectations about the
possible presence of specific crash hazards and thus prepare Sources of Error: Active Failures and Latent
drivers to respond to their emergence (Höger and
Seidenstücker 2007; Tversky and Kahneman 1982).
Conditions
These expectations in turn lead to the production of In the domain of organizational systems safety, human error
behavioral scripts, which entail the organization of schema is categorized as belonging to one of two types: active fail-
into behavioral routines. The production of scripts is why ures and latent conditions. Active failures are the type of
basic operational decisions, such as speed selection and lane error that serves as the focus of conventional safety practice
placement, typically require little cognitive effort. They are and naturally occurs at the interface between humans and
automated behavioral routines that an individual intuits is complex systems. Active failures are the result of a person
4 Journal of Planning Education and Research 00(0)

incorrectly executing a “correct” intended action (known as is influenced, in large part, by the design of the built environ-
a slip), or failing to undertake a “correct” intended action ment. In a fascinating study by Recarte and Nunes (2002),
(called a lapse) and are the result of individual characteris- drivers proved incapable of traveling at reduced speeds when
tics, such as distraction, fatigue, or impairment, rather than their attention was directed to tasks other than monitoring the
the result of design deficiencies. As such, they should not be speedometer for compliance. When engaged in cognitive
concentrated at specific geographic locations, but instead tasks requiring mental recall, drivers naturally increased their
distributed across the system as a function of trip generation operating speed, a finding that suggests a reversion to intui-
and traffic volumes. tive operating scripts. Stated another way, when drivers are
Yet organizational systems safety also recognizes a sec- not performing the unnatural task of directing the entirety of
ond source of error, one which is of far greater relevance to their attention to monitoring the speedometer, they revert to
planners and urban designers. These are the errors that the application of the behavioral scripts that they intuit are
emerge out of latent conditions embedded into the design of appropriate for their environment.
the built environment, conditions that induce behaviors that Similarly, the design of the built environment has been
increase one’s potential exposure to a crash event. As observed to reduce operating speeds beyond that which
described by Reason (1997), latent conditions can be would be expected to be produced by street geometry alone.
regarded as resident pathogens designed into complex sys- Features such as sidewalks, on-street-parking, street-oriented
tems, which lie dormant until activated by predictable com- buildings, and roadside trees on urban streets have all been
binations of local circumstances. observed to produce statistically significant reductions in
The errors produced by latent conditions—henceforth operating speeds (Dumbaugh 2006; Gargoum, El-Basyouny,
referred to as latent errors—are not the product of the ordi- and Kim 2016; Harvey and Aultman-Hall 2015; Ivan,
nary slips and lapses that one can expect to occur across a Garrick, and Hanson 2009; Smith and Appleyard 1981). In
given population. Instead, latent conditions are transformed these cases, the design and configuration of the built envi-
into errors through design-induced mistakes,4 which occur ronment result in the adoption of lower speed scripts, which,
when road users execute a behavioral routine (i.e., apply a set in the majority of these studies, are associated with reduc-
of schema and scripts) that they intuit is appropriate for their tions in crash incidence
environment, but which instead places them at increased The second type of mistake is a knowledge-based mis-
risk. Unlike active failure, mistakes are directly and intrinsi- take, which occurs when a driver or other road user encoun-
cally associated with the design of the built environment and ters an unfamiliar situation, and where the correct behavior
occur when the built environment fails to prime appropriate cannot be easily intuited from prior experience. In this situa-
scripts and schema, as evidenced by the phenomenon of inat- tion, the individual is forced to make a conscious decision
tentional blindness. The presence of environmentally induced about how to proceed in the absence of adequate informa-
mistakes is already evident in the findings of research con- tion. As detailed by Daniel Kahneman (2011), conscious
ducted in the domains of urban planning and design, which decisions in complex situations are cognitively intensive and
has consistently found that crashes are not evenly distrib- require deliberation. Mistakes can be expected to occur when
uted, but are instead geographically clustered in specific the time needed to make a correct operating decision exceeds
environments, most notably along arterials and, especially, the time available to do so.
along arterials with adjacent commercial uses (Dumbaugh Knowledge-based mistakes can be observed at complex
and Rae 2009; Hadayeghi, Shalaby, and Persaud 2003; intersections, most notably at interchanges between free-
Levine, Kim, and Nitz 1995a, 1995b; Lovegrove and Sayed ways and arterials, where drivers are unsure about how to
2006; Yu and Xu 2018). These environments result in the appropriately navigate an intersection. These crashes, known
production of errors, and thus the incidence of crashes, that as “wrong-way driving,” are the result of an individual’s lack
greatly exceed that which would be expected from slips and of knowledge about how to safely maneuver along complex
lapses alone. interchanges and have been found to produce roughly 360
deaths in the United States each year (Pour-Rouholamin
et al. 2014). To date, there has been little examination of the
Rule-Based Mistakes and Knowledge-Based
role of knowledge-based mistakes on crash incidence, though
Mistakes they are likely responsible for a significant share of crashes
Mistakes can be further categorized as belonging to one of that occur at complex, multi-phase intersections, particularly
two primary types. The first are rule-based mistakes, which those that employ permitted left-turning maneuvers or which
occur when a road user applies an incorrect set of schema and apply lead-lag signal phasing.
scripts in their specific environment, resulting in behaviors
that increase a person’s crash exposure. Rule-based mistakes Examining Active Failure and Latent
are readily evidenced when drivers adopt operating speeds in
excess of a roadway’s posted speed limit. While it is tempting
Errors in Urban Environments
to simply attribute this to reckless behavior, research in the To date, there has been little, if any, examination into the
domain of traffic psychology indicates that speed selection relative role that active failures and latent errors may play on
Dumbaugh et al. 5

human fallibility and can be understood as a product of the


number of people in an area and the amount of travel that
they do. To account for the influence of active failure, we
used two variables: million vehicle miles traveled (MVMT)
and block group population, which is a proxy for trip
generation.

•• MVMT: MVMT was calculated by obtaining average


annual daily traffic volumes (AADT) from the North
Carolina State Department of Transportation. AADT
is reported for point locations. To convert AADT to
VMT, we assigned AADT to the street segments con-
tained in the block groups and then multiplied AADT
by the segment length. At locations with more than
one AADT count (i.e., two point locations), we
assigned the street segment to the closest available
count, splitting street segments at the midpoint where
necessary. This process gave us daily VMT for the
street segments. These were summed to get daily
VMT for each block group. To convert this to MVMT
for the study period, we multiplied the daily VMT by
Figure 3.  Charlotte-Mecklenburg county.
365 (days) and 3 (years) and then divided by one mil-
lion. Note that the values pertain to the total VMT for
both directions of traffic on each street segment.
urban crash incidence. We conducted a literature search on •• Population: Information on population at the block
the Transportation Research Information Database (TRID), group level was acquired from the U.S. Census. More
provided by the National Academies of Science, using the people would be expected to generate more travel,
terms “safe systems,” “active failure,” “latent conditions,” regardless of mode, and thus create more crash-pro-
“latent error,” “latent failure,” and “mistakes,” among others, ducing slips and lapses.
and were unable to find a single empirical study relating
these concepts to traffic safety; in the cases where these
search terms were present, they pertained almost exclusively Operationalizing Latent Error
to aviation and marine transport, or the movement of hazard- Latent errors occur when an environment fails to cogni-
ous materials, areas that have long adopted an organizational tively prime schema and scripts that prepare road users to
systems-based approach to addressing safety. respond to potential traffic hazards. While conventional
As such, we sought to examine whether this theoretical crash data do not provide us with information on cogni-
framework might be similarly useful for understanding crash tion, we can nonetheless infer the presence of misapplied
incidence on urban streets. To do so, we conducted a geo- scripts and schema as a product of the crashes that occur in
spatial analysis of crash frequency and severity at the block relation to specific environmental features, over and
group level for Charlotte-Mecklenburg County, North beyond those crashes already accounted by measures of
Carolina (see Figure 3). Because there are no readily avail- active failure.
able data sources on behavioral adaptations to different envi- For the purposes of this analysis, we defined latent con-
ronmental conditions, we sought to construct operational ditions as those environmental features that create conflict-
definitions from available data sources that would allow us ing travel movements between multiple road users. If
to examine whether the distinction between active failures crashes are principally a function of active failure, as is cur-
and latent errors may be a useful theoretical frame for under- rently presumed in the United States (American Association
standing urban crash frequency and severity. While this is an of State Highway and Transportation Officials [AASHTO]
examination of a single metropolitan area, it is nevertheless 2010, 2011), these environmental features, which include
instructive for examining the theoretical relevance of these intersections, commercial parcels, and bus stops, should
concepts. have little, if any, influence on crash incidence. Nonetheless,
if road users are applying an incorrect set of schema and
scripts, then these elements would be expected to result
Operationalizing Active Failure
in a measurable increase in traffic-related crashes, inju-
Active failures are the routine slips and lapses that can be ries, and deaths. We operationalized these variables as
expected to occur across a population as a result of ordinary follows:
6 Journal of Planning Education and Research 00(0)

•• Four-or-more-leg intersections: Four-or-more leg total crash incidence. To determine the validity of these
intersections are locations where opposing streams of assertions, we included two additional variables in the model:
traffic cross, thus creating locations where conflicts
between opposing streams of traffic, and thus traffic •• Percentage of streets with four-or-fewer through
crashes, are more likely to occur (Ben-Joseph 1995; lanes: This variable is the mileage of non-freeway
Dumbaugh and Rae 2009; Ladrón de Guevara, streets with four-or-fewer through lanes, divided by
Washington, and Oh 2004; Lovegrove and Sayed the total number of non-freeway streets in the block
2006; Marks 1957). To calculate this measure, we group.
used street network data provided by Mecklenberg •• Percentage of streets with speeds less than thirty-five
County and identified every intersection point loca- MPH: This variable is the mileage of non-freeway
tion with four-or-more legs. These were then summed streets with posted speeds of thirty-five MPH or less,
to the block group level. divided by the total mileage of non-freeway streets in
•• Commercial parcels: Commercial parcels likewise the block group.
create traffic conflicts through their use of driveways
for street network access, which function as de facto
intersections. Using parcel-level data provided by the Dependent Variables: Total and KAB Crashes
Mecklenberg County tax assessor’s office, this vari- To understand the influence of these variables on crash inci-
able is the sum of the number of unique commercial dence, we used three years of crash data (2012–2015) for this
parcels contained in each block group. analysis to account for regression-to-the-mean effects that
•• Bus stops: Bus stops can create traffic conflicts by may influence our estimates (Hauer 1997; Hauer and Persaud
attracting and distributing pedestrians at points along 1983). Because we were interested in the role of the built
the street network, creating concentrations of activity environment on the creation of latent conditions, we exam-
that may lead to conflicts between vehicles and pedes- ined crashes occurring on surface streets exclusively. Crashes
trians. They may further create traffic conflicts at loca- occurring on interstates and freeways, where access is gov-
tions where the bus stops in the outside travel lane (i.e., erned by on- and off-ramps, and which principally serve
online locations), which may in turn encourage drivers through-moving motorists, are excluded from this analysis.
to attempt to maneuver around the stopped bus, result- A total of 50,387 crashes occurred on surface streets in the
ing in sideswipe and rear-end collisions with adjacent study area between 2011 and 2013. This includes 135 fatal
vehicles (Amoh-Gyimah, Saberi, and Sarvi 2017; Chin crashes, 254 incapacitating injury crashes, 3,724 non-inca-
and Quddus 2003; Rhee et al. 2016). Our bus stop vari- pacitating injury crashes, 14,276 possible injury crashes,
able is the sum of the number of individual bus stops 29,761 property damage only crashes, and 2,327 non-traffic
located within each block group. injury crashes. These crashes were aggregated into two
dependent variables: total crashes, which are the sum of all
Moderating Latent Conditions: Safe Systems crashes listed above, and “KAB” crashes, which includes
fatal (K), incapacitating injury (A), and non-incapacitating
Elements injury (B) crashes.
If the built environment can establish latent conditions that To account for the influence of the built environment, we
induce mistakes, it is equally plausible that the design of the aggregated census data, street network data, and parcel-level
built environment can reduce their occurrence. Safe Systems land use data to the block group level. A major issue with
and Vision Zero proponents have called for reducing vehicle aggregating data to the block group level is that census geog-
speeds on urban surface streets to thirty-five miles per hour raphies are typically bounded by major roadways, such as
(MPH) or less (Vision Zero Network 2018), as well as arterials, which do not align perfectly with block group
reducing the number of through-lanes along urban streets to boundaries. Furthermore, variations in geo-location can result
four-or-fewer (National Association of City Transportation in crashes, which are point features, not aligning with streets,
Officials [NACTO] 2013, 2016). Neither assertion is backed which are line features. We addressed this issue using the
by direct empirical evidence of their effects on crash fre- methodology detailed in Dumbaugh, Rae, and Wunneburger
quency and severity, but instead on the rationale that more (2011), running a 200-ft buffer (roughly the width of a fully
constrained environments encourage drivers to exercise designed principal arterial) around each block group, with the
additional care. Restated in terms of the cognitive framework goal of accounting for errors in geo-location.
described above, these elements encourage the adoption of Nonetheless, this approach may bias our model results
environmentally appropriate scripts and schema. If this is based on the crash history of a handful of high-crash streets
true, then one would expect these elements to lead to observ- or intersections. To remedy this problem, we conducted a
able reductions in not only the incidence of injurious and random assignment process for assigning border crashes to
fatal crashes (which are a function of impact speed) but also block groups using a two-step process. First, an array of
Dumbaugh et al. 7

Table 1.  Summary of Crash Statistics.

Description Total M SD Minimum Maximum


Number of total crashes 50,387 90.79 88.97 0 611
Number of fatal (K) crashes 135 0.24 0.53 0 3
Number of incapacitating (A) crashes 254 0.46 0.80 0 6
Number of non-incapacitating (B) crashes 3,724 6.71 5.91 0 36
Number of K + A + B crashes 4,113 7.41 6.53 0 43

numbers was generated such that duplicate crashes had iden- million additional vehicle miles traveled in each block group.
tical numbers. We randomly assigned these crashes to block Each bus stop is associated with 2.5 percent increase in total
groups, retaining only the first observation and deleting all crashes, and 2.0 percent increase in KAB crashes, roughly
duplicate counts. Table 1 provides descriptive statistics for the equivalent of adding an additional four million VMT to
the block groups in this study. these block groups. Stated another way, environmental con-
ditions that create conflicts between different street users, a
proxy for latent conditions, are associated with large and sta-
Model Specification tistically significant increases in total and KAB crashes.
Crashes are discrete, non-negative, and independent events. The two “safe systems” variables included in the model
Poisson and negative binomial error distributions are typi- proved moderate effects. Because the independent variables
cally adopted for modeling crash count data. However, the were modeled as percentages, the model results can be inter-
assumption of the Poisson distribution is that the mean equals preted as elasticities, and the results are elastic. A 1 percent
the variance. Crash data are oftentimes found to exhibit extra increase in the percentage of streets with posted speeds of
variation, or overdispersion (i.e., variance mean). Negative thirty-five MPH or less is associated with a 1.9 percent
binomial models are better able to account for overdisper- reduction in both total crashes and KAB crashes; a 1 percent
sion in crash data and are therefore applied here. The coeffi- increase in the lane mileage comprising four-or-fewer lanes
cients for negative binomial models report the percent is associated with a 3.3 percent reduction in total crashes and
change in the dependent variable associated with each unit 1.8 percent reduction in KAB crashes.
increase of the independent variables (Hilbe 2007).
Toward Safe Systems: A Cognition-
Model Results Centered Approach to Traffic Safety
Tables 2 and 3 present our model results. As expected, popu- At present, crash prevention is based on the presumption
lation and VMT, the variables that serve as a proxy for slips that traffic crashes are a product of innate human fallibility,
and lapses, were associated with the increased incidence of fallibility to be addressed principally through driver educa-
total and KAB crashes, though population did not prove to be tion and law enforcement programs. The recent emergence
a significant variable at the 0.05 level of confidence. of Safe Systems has nonetheless encouraged safety profes-
Nonetheless, their influence on total and KAB crash inci- sionals to move beyond such simplistic perspectives on
dence is not great. Adding a million miles of additional VMT crash causation and to begin examining and addressing the
to a block group would only be expected to produce a 0.39 nature of the behaviors that result in traffic crashes (ITF
percent increase in total crashes and 0.48 percent increase in 2016; ITF 2008).
KAB crashes. To put this in perspective, the average block In this study, we have sought to understand the nature of
group accommodated roughly 48 million miles of VMT dur- crash causation on urban streets, paying particular attention
ing the three-year study period. Doubling the typical block to the cognitive mechanisms that may induce—or prevent—
group’s VMT to 96 million miles would have only a slight the behaviors that transform latent conditions into a crash
effect on the incidence of KAB crashes, which would be event. Research in the areas of organizational systems safety,
expected to increase from 2.46 to 3.10 over the three-year cognitive psychology, and behavioral economics provide
study period. important insights into the central role of cognition in crash
The variables associated with latent conditions have a far incidence, though these insights have not, to date, been
greater influence on crash incidence. Each four-or-more leg meaningfully integrated into safety practice.
intersection is associated with a 2.6 percent increase in total Doing so requires the development of a new framework
crashes, and a 2.2 percent increase in KAB crashes. Each for understanding crash causation, one which places human
commercial parcel is associated with a 0.44 percent increase cognition at its center. As illustrated in Figure 4, the built
in total and 0.28 percent increase in KAB crashes, which environment shapes the schema and scripts applied by road
is roughly comparable to the safety effects of adding one users, which in turn influences their exposure to crash risks
8 Journal of Planning Education and Research 00(0)

Table 2.  Total Crashes.

Total crashes Coefficient SE z p(z) [95 percent confidence interval]


Active failure
 Population 0.00002 0.0000 0.460 .643 [−0.0001, 0.0001]
  VMT (millions) 0.0048 0.0011 4.210 .000 [0.0026, 0.0070]
Latent failure
  # four of more-leg intersections 0.0262 0.0058 4.560 .000 [0.0149, 0.0375]
  # commercial parcels 0.0044 0.0012 3.690 .000 [0.0021, 0.0068]
  # bus stops 0.0246 0.0040 6.210 .000 [0.0169, 0.0324]
System characteristics
  % streets thirty-five MPH or less −0.0187 0.0038 −4.920 .000 [−0.0261, −0.0112]
  % streets with four or fewer lanes −0.0334 0.0072 −4.660 .000 [−0.0475, −0.0194]
Constant 8.3029 0.7780 10.670 .000 [6.7784, 9.8273]
Log likelihood = −2,887
N = 555

Note: VMT = vehicle miles traveled; MPH = miles per hour.

Table 3.  KAB Crashes.

KAB Crashes Coefficient SE z p(z) [95 percent confidence interval]


Active failure
 Population 0.0001 0.0000 1.720 .085 [0.0000, 0.0001]
  VMT (millions) 0.0039 0.0010 4.100 .000 [0.0020, 0.0058]
Latent failure
  # four of more-leg intersections 0.0217 0.0051 4.260 .000 [0.0117, 0.0317]
  # commercial parcels 0.0028 0.0010 2.780 .005 [0.0008, 0.0047]
  # bus stops 0.0197 0.0033 5.960 .000 [0.0132, 0.0262]
System characteristics
  % streets thirty-five MPH or less −0.0194 0.0034 −5.730 .000 [−0.0260, −0.0128]
  % streets with four or fewer lanes −0.0185 0.0061 −3.040 .002 [−0.0304, −0.0066]
Constant 4.6049 0.6492 7.090 .000 [3.3326, 5.8773]
Log likelihood = −1,535
N = 555

Note: VMT = vehicle miles traveled; MPH = miles per hour.

during the course of their travel. Latent conditions emerge they are incapable of detailing these processes themselves.
when the design of the built environment induces schema This will require moving beyond the statistical analysis of
and scripts that are inappropriate for a specific operating large datasets, and to begin examining how design influences
environment. These entail more than simply the choice of the behaviors and understandings of real-world human
higher-than-desirable operating speeds; they can prime a subjects.
motorist to identify and adequately respond to the presence Moving forward, we need to develop our understanding
of hazards in their environment, or they can render these haz- of how the built environment may prime specific scripts and
ards cognitively invisible. schema in human drivers, and how those scripts and schema
We understand some basic components of the outcomes may, in turn, influence crash frequency and severity. These
of these cognitive processes, such that dense development may entail psychological studies that task subjects with road
adjacent to the right-of-way leads is associated with lower scene interpretation, which can in turn be used to identify the
operating speeds (Harvey and Aultman-Hall 2015; Smith salient environmental features that govern the adoption of
and Appleyard 1981; Theeuwes 2012). Yet the further devel- specific schema and scrips, or the use of driving simulators
opment of our understanding of the role of cognition will to capture human behavioral responses to different design
require new research methods. Conventional sources of scenarios.
safety data, such as those used in this study, are useful for We believe that latter approach, which permits research-
identifying the outcomes of these cognitive processes—that ers to develop and test specific environmental stimuli,
is, environmentally based variations in crash incidence—but will provide to be a particularly fruitful approach. In an
Dumbaugh et al. 9

Figure 4.  A cognition-centered approach to crash prevention.

interesting study of driver cognition and behavior, van der Similarly, transportation performance measures such as
Horst and Kaptein (1998) used a simulator to demonstrate vehicle delay or level-of-service, which are the primary mea-
that differing the marking configurations for lanes, center- sures of transportation system performance (Shaw 2003),
lines, and shoulders influences speed choice, as well as encourage the advancement of transportation projects that
driver expectations governing the selection of a “safe” add lanes, increase speeds, of both, often without regard to a
speed. Such an approach, when combined with variations in street’s developmental context. Indeed, conventional safety
the roadway’s developmental context and the simulation of guidance, such as AASHTO’s (2010) Highway Safety
common urban crash scenarios during the simulation rou- Manual, does not regard number of lanes, developmental
tine, such as an abrupt vehicle or pedestrian crossing, would context, or its road user mix as having any influence on
likely yield highly useful results for understanding how cog- safety outcomes, ostensibly from the belief that road users
nitive expectancy may establish, or prevent, latent error. are individual responsible for the commission of these errors,
Conventional simulators allow researchers to capture driver and would do so regardless of the characteristics of the trans-
reaction times and braking response following the introduc- portation system. The result is that capacity-expanding proj-
tion of a traffic conflict, while readily available biometric ects are able to advance through planning and design
tools, such as eye-tracking monitors, can be further added to processes with little substantive regard for their influence on
capture visual fixations in order to identify the specific fea- road user behavior and the resulting safety consequences.
tures within the simulation that drivers are using to establish The NACTO (2013) and the Institute of Transportation
their behavioral routines. Engineers (2010) have sought to remedy the resulting safety
problems at the design level through the development and
promotion of new “context-sensitive” street design guid-
Implications for Practice ance. Nonetheless, there would be little need for post hoc
If latent errors are indeed a primary determinant of the inci- design interventions if safety considerations were embedded
dence of traffic-related death and injury, then planning and into the earliest phases of the planning and project develop-
policy decisions regarding the location and configuration of ment process. Correspondingly, we believe Figure 5 is as
future development may have a profound long-term impacts useful as a prescriptive model for advancing safe systems.
on traffic safety. Development prescription contains regional By illustrating the pathway through which the conditions for
development plans, zoning ordinances, and subdivision reg- latent error are established, it also identifies opportunities for
ulations which can lead to the creation of environments that proactively identifying planning-level decisions than be
establish specific—and predictable—patterns of road user expected to led to negative safety consequences, such as the
behavior. The decision to zone an arterial for commercial or adoption of land use codes encouraging future developmen-
retail use, for example, is not a safety-neutral proposition. tal growth in areas with concentrations of commercial uses
This future development will require access and egress, and higher speed arterial systems.
change the user mix of the thoroughfare, and result in patterns Indeed, practitioners seeking to proactively identify how
of behavior that may make crash events more—or less— future development scenarios may affect annual crash inci-
likely to occur. Figure 5 depicts causal pathway through dence can simply insert block group-level forecast values
which latent error may be embedded into the transportation into the regression equations emerging from the models pre-
system through the planning and design processes. sented in Tables 2 and 3:
10 Journal of Planning Education and Research 00(0)

Figure 5.  Planning, design, and the path of latent error.

influence traffic safety outcomes and it is our hope that the


Total crashes per year = [EXP (8.3 + 0.0002x1
results of this study provide a useful means for doing do.
+ 0.0048x 2 + 0.0262x3 + 0.0044x 4
+ 0.0246x5 − 0.0187x 6 − 0.0334x 7 ) / 3] Declaration of Conflicting Interests
The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect
1 to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.
KAB crashes per year = [EXP (4.6 + 0.0001x
+ 0.0039x 2 + 0.0217x3 + 0.0028x 4 Funding
5 6 7
+ 0.0197x − 0.0194x − 0.0185x ) / 3] The author(s) disclosed receipt of the following financial support
for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article: This
where x1 = forecast population; x2 = forecast VMT (mil- research was supported, in part, by a grant from the United States
lions); x3 = forecast # of intersections; x4 = forecast # of Department of Transportation through the Collaborative Sciences
Center for Road Safety.
commercial parcels; x5 = forecast # of bus stops; x6 = fore-
cast % of streets with limits of 35 MPH or less; x7 = forecast
% of streets with four or fewer lanes. ORCID iDs
While the resulting predictions will be necessarily inexact Eric Dumbaugh https://orcid.org/0000-0002-5254-9711
and pertain specifically to Charlotte-Mecklenberg County, Louis Merlin https://orcid.org/0000-0002-9267-5712
previous research suggests that the risk factors observed for
Charlotte-Mecklenberg County are not dissimilar to those Notes
found in other regions throughout the United States. As such, 1. Such assertions are refuted by the fact that automated vehicles
we believe these models are an enormously useful mecha- (AVs) have been found to be involved in crashes at higher
nism for identifying—and thus preventing—the emergence rates, per mile traveled, than conventionally driven vehicles
of future safety problems that may occur as a result of broader (Schoettle and Sivak 2015). As we argue in this article, crash
development policies. incidence is a function of the cognitive processes through
We thus conclude on an optimistic note. The planning which humans interpret and respond to their environments.
profession is currently in the midst of a widespread re- AVs, as non-human systems, do not apply these processes.
assessment of its role in advancing traffic safety. In the last It nonetheless remains unclear whether advancements in
five years, more than thirty cities in the United States have machine learning will enable AVs to become more effective
than mental heuristics in negotiating the complex social envi-
adopted Vision Zero as a policy goal, which calls for the
ronments found on urban streets. All that can be confidently
complete elimination of traffic-related deaths and injuries asserted is that AVs apply new technologies that will change
(Vision Zero Network 2018). While the success of Vision the operating dynamics of the transportation system, which
Zero policies remains to be seen, it nonetheless heralds a new may reduce certain types of human error, but which, as part of
commitment to advancing traffic safety as a central planning a complex system, will introduce new forms of latent condi-
concern. This is an opportune time for the profession to tions, conditions which are present, unknown, and unexam-
revisit the mechanisms through which planning decisions ined (Koopman, Kane, and Black 2019). This assertion further
Dumbaugh et al. 11

ignores the almost certain emergence of unanticipated hard- Ben-Joseph, Eran. 1995. Livability and Safety of Suburban Street
ware and software problems. One need only to recall the trou- Patterns: A Comparative Study. Working Paper No. 642.
bled history of Windows Vista, or the crashes resulting from Berkeley: Institute of Urban and Regional Development,
the flight stabilization software used in the Boeing 737 MAX, University of California.
for example of why the safety assertions of AV proponents Blomquist, Glenn. 1986. “A Utility Maximization Model of Driver
should be viewed with a healthy dose of skepticism. Traffic Safety Behavior.” Accident Analysis and Prevention 18
2. The categories that Treat et al. (1979) defined, and which are (5): 371–75.
still used by National Highway Traffic Safety Administration Brown, D. 2002. “Looked but Did Not See’ Accidents: A Review.”
(2015), fall into one of three categories: In Behavioural Research in Road Safety: 11th Seminar, edited
by G. B. Grayson, 116–124. Department for Transport, Local
• Recognition error, which may include driver inatten- Government and the Regions.
tion or distraction, as well as inadequate surveillance for Carhart-Harris, Robin L., David Erritzoe, Tim Williams, James M.
oncoming hazards before entering an intersection of mak- Stone, Laurence J. Reed, R. Alessandro Colasanti, Robin J.
ing a lane change. Tyacke, et al. 2012. “Neural Correlates of the Psychedelic State
• Decision error, such as driving too fast for conditions or as Determined by fMRI Studies with Psilocybin.” Proceedings
misjudging gaps in oncoming traffic. of the National Academy of Science 109 (6): 2138–43.
• Performance error, such as poor directional control over Chabris, Christopher F., and Daniel J. Simons. 2011. The Invisible
the vehicle prior to a crash, a factor most often attributable Gorilla: How Our Intuitions Deceive Us. New York: Random
to drowsy driving. House.
Chin, Hoong Chor., and Mohammed Abdul Quddus. 2003.
3. This effect is further evidenced by the effect of hallucinogenic “Applying the Random Effect Negative Binomial Model
drugs, such as LSD and psilocybin mushrooms, which have to Examine Traffic Accident Occurrence at Signalized
been shown to reduce activity in the medial prefrontal cortex Intersections.” Accident Analysis & Prevention 35 (2): 253–59.
(mPFC), the area responsible for processing sensory informa- Corben, Bruce F., David B. Logan, Lisa Fanciulli, Roger Farley,
tion and organizing it into specific actions (Carhart-Harris and Iain Cameron. 2010. “Strengthening Road Safety Strategy
et al. 2012; Euston, Gruber, and McNaughton 2012). This, Development ‘towards Zero’ 2009–2020—Western Australia’s
in turn, appears to be responsible for the heightened sensory Experience Scientific Research on Road Safety Management
experiences associated with the use of hallucinogenic drugs. SWOV Workshop 16 and 17 November 2009.” Safety Science
4. In interests of brevity, we have simplified this discussion. 48:1085–97.
Mistakes can be divided into three types. The first is a rule- Dumbaugh, Eric. 2005. “Safe Streets, Livable Streets.” Journal of
based mistake, which we focus on in the body of the article. the American Planning Association 71 (3): 283–98.
The second is a knowledge-based mistake, which occurs when Dumbaugh, Eric. 2006. “The Design of Safe Urban Roadsides:
a road user lack prior knowledge on the correct behavioral An Empirical Analysis.” Transportation Research Record:
rules to apply in their current situation, which may be observed Journal of the Transportation Research Board 1961:74–82.
at complex interchanges, and which result in phenomenon Dumbaugh, Eric, and Michael King. 2018. “Engineering Livable
such as wrong-way driving up a freeway off-ramp. The third Streets: A Thematic Review of Advancement in Urban Street
is one we have defined as an incentivized violation, which Design.” Journal of Planning Literature 33:451–65.
occurs when an individual is aware of the behaviors that are Dumbaugh, Eric, and Robert Rae. 2009. “Safe Urban form:
socially expected, but nevertheless make a conscious decision Revisiting the Relationship between Community Design and
to deviate from them under the expectation that the violation of Traffic Safety.” Journal of the American Planning Association
norms of conduct are permissible. Incentivized violations are 75 (3): 309–29.
evidenced by jaywalking and illegal turning maneuvers. Dumbaugh, Eric, Robert Rae, and Douglas Wunneburger. 2011.
“Using GIS to Develop a Performance-Based Framework for
Evaluating Urban Design and Crash Incidence.” Urban Design
References International 16 (1): 63–71.
Abelson, Robert P. 1981. “Psychological Status of the Script Euston, David R., Aaron J Gruber, and Bruce L McNaughton.
Concept.” American Psychologist 36 (7): 715–29. 2012. “The Role of Medial Prefrontal Cortex in Memory and
American Association of State Highway and Transportation Decision Making.” Neuron 76 (6): 1057–70.
Officials. 2010. Highway Safety Manual. Washington, DC: Fuller, Ray. 2011. “Driver Control Theory: From Difficulty
American Association of State Highway and Transportation Homeostasis to Risk Allostasis.” In Handbook of Driver
Officials. Psychology, edited by Bryan Porter, 13–26. New York:
American Association of State Highway and Transportation Academic Press.
Officials. 2011. A Policy on the Geometric Design of Highways Gargoum, Suliman A., Karim El-Basyouny, and Amy Kim. 2016.
and Streets. 6th ed. Washington, DC: American Association of “Towards Setting Credible Speed Limits: Identifying Factors
State Highway and Transportation Officials. That Affect Driver Compliance on Urban Roads.” Accident
Amoh-Gyimah, Richard, Meead Saberi, and Majid Sarvi. 2017. Analysis & Prevention 95:138–48.
“The Effect of Variations in Spatial Units on Unobserved Gigerenzer, Gerd, and Henry Brighton. 2009. “Homo Heuristicus:
Heterogeneity in Macroscopic Crash Models.” Analytic Why Biased Minds Make Better Inferences.” Topics in
Methods in Accident Research 13:28–51. Cognitive Science 1:107–143.
12 Journal of Planning Education and Research 00(0)

Hadayeghi, Alireza, Amer Said Shalaby, and Bhagwant N. Persaud. Zonal Generators.” Accident Analysis & Prevention 27 (5):
2003. “Macrolevel Accident Prediction Models for Evaluating 675–85.
Safety of Urban Transportation Systems.” Transportation Lovegrove, Gordon R., and Tarek Sayed. 2006. “Macro-Level
Research Record: Journal of the Transportation Research Collision Prediction Models for Evaluating Neighbourhood
Board 1840:87–95. Traffic Safety.” Canadian Journal of Civil Engineering 33 (5):
Harvey, Chester, and Lisa Aultman-Hall. 2015. “Urban Streetscape 609–21.
Design and Crash Severity.” Transportation Research Record: Mack, Arien, and Irvin Rock. 1998. Inattentional Blindness.
Journal of the Transportation Research Board 2500:1–8. Cambridge: MIT Press.
Hauer, Ezra. 1997. Observational before-after Studies in Road Marks, Harold. 1957. “Subdividing for Traffic Safety.” Traffic
Safety: Estimating the Effect of Highway and Traffic Quarterly 11:308–25.
Engineering Measures on Road Safety. Tarrytown: Pergamon. National Association of City Transportation Officials. 2013. Urban
Hauer, Ezra, and Bhagwant N. Persaud. 1983. “A Common Street Design Guide. Washington, DC: Island Press.
Bias in before and after Comparisons and Its Elimination.” National Association of City Transportation Officials. 2016. Global
Transportation Research Record 905:164–74. Street Design Guide. Washington, DC: Island Press.
Hilbe, Joseph M. 2007. Negative Binomial Regression. Cambridge: National Highway Traffic Safety Administration. 2015. “Critical
Cambridge University Press. Reasons for Crashes Investigated in the National Motor
Höger, Rainer, and Jessica Seidenstücker. 2007. “Driving and the Vehicle Crash Causation Survey.” DOT HS 812 115, February.
Activation of Mental Concepts.” Special issue 2007, Advances Washington, DC: US Department of Transportation.
in Transportation Studies 91–96. National Highway Traffic Safety Administration. 2017. “Traffic
International Transport Forum [ITF]. 2016. “Zero Road Deaths Safety Facts: Annual Report.” Report # DOT HS 812 580.
and Serious Injuries: Leading a Paradigm Shift to a Safe Washington, DC: United States Department of Transportation,
System. Paris: Organisation for Economoic Co-operation and September.
Development [OECD]. National Highway Traffic Safety Administration. 2019. “Seat Belt
International Transport Forum [ITF]. 2008. “Towards Zero: Use in the United States—Overall Results.” Report # DOT HS
Ambitious Road Safety Targets and the Safe System 812 662, January. Washington, DC: United States Department
Approach.” Paris: Organisation for Economoic Co-operation of Transportation.
and Development [OECD]. Noland, Robert. 2003. “Traffic Fatalities and Injuries: The Effect
Institute of Transportation Engineers. 2010. Designing Walkable of Changes in Infrastructure and Other Trends.” Accident
Urban Thoroughfares: A Context Sensitive Approach. Analysis & Prevention 35:599–611.
Washington, DC: Institute of Transportation Engineers. Noland, Robert. 2013. “From Theory to Practice in Road Safety
Ivan, John N., Norman W. Garrick, and Gilbert Hanson. 2009. Policy: Understanding Risk versus Mobility.” Research in
Designing Roads That Guide Drivers to Choose Safer Speeds. Transportation Economics 43:71–84.
Rocky Hill: Connecticut Department of Transportation. Noland, Robert, and Lyoong Oh. 2004. “The Effect of Infrastructure
Jacobsen, Peter Lyndon. 2002. “Safety in Numbers: More and Demographic Change on Traffic-Related Fatalities
Walkers and Bicyclists, Safer Walking and Bicycling.” Injury and Crashes: A Case Study of Illinois County-Level Data.”
Prevention 9 (3): 205–209. Accident Analysis & Prevention 36:525–32.
Kahneman, Daniel. 2011. Thinking Fast and Slow. New York: Peltzman, Sam. 1975. “The Effects of Automobile Safety
Farrar, Straus and Giroux. Regulation.” Journal of Political Economy 83:677–725.
Kahneman, Daniel, and Amos Tversky. 1998. “The Simulation Perez, Omri., Roy Mukamel, Ariel Tankus, Jonathan D. Rosenblatt,
Heuristic.” In Judgment under Uncertainty: Heuristics and Yehezkel Yeshurun, and Itzhak Fried. 2015. “Preconscious
Biases, edited by Daniel Kahneman, Paul Slovic, and Amos Prediction of a Driver’s Decision Using Intracranial Recordings.”
Tversky, 201–208. New York: Cambridge University Press. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 27 (8): 1492–1502.
Koopman, Philip, Aaron Kane, and Jen Black. 2019. “Credible Pour-Rouholamin, Mahdi, Huaguo Zhou, Jeffrey Shaw, and
Autonomy Safety Argumentation.” Safety-Critical Systems Priscilla Tobias. 2014. “Overview of Safety Countermeasures
Symposium, Bristol, England, February. https://edge-case- for Wrong-Way Driving Crashes.” ITE Journal 84 (12): 31–38.
research.com/project/credible-autonomy-safety-argumentation/. Reason, James. 1997. Managing the Risks of Organizational
Ladrón de Guevara, Simon P. Washington, and Jutaek Oh. Accidents. England: Ashgate.
2004. “Forecasting Travel Crashes at the Planning Levels: Recarte, Miguel Angel, and Luis Nunes. 2002. “Mental Load and
Simultaneous Negative Binomial Crash Model Applied in Loss of Control over Speed in Real Driving. Towards a Theory
Tucson, Arizona.” Transportation Research Record: Journal of Attentional Speed Control.” Transportation Research Part
of the Transportation Research Board 1897:191–99. F 5:111–22.
Leden, Lars. 2002. “Pedestrian Risk Decrease with Pedestrian Flow. Rhee, Kyoung-Ah, Joon-Ki Kim, Young-ihn Lee, and Gudmundur
A Case Study Based on Data from Signalized Intersections in F. Ulfarsson. 2016. “Spatial Regression Analysis of Traffic
Hamilton, Ontario.” Accident Analysis and Prevention 34 (4): Crashes in Seoul.” Accident Analysis & Prevention 91:190–99.
457–64. Robinson, Dorothy L. 2005. “Safety in Numbers in Australia: More
Leveson, Nancy. 2004. “A New Accident Model for Engineering Walkers and Bicyclists, Safer Walking and Bicycling.” Health
Safer Systems.” Safety Science 42:237–70. Promotion Journal of Australia 16:47–51.
Levine, Ned., Karl E. Kim, and Lawrence H. Nitz. 1995a. “Spatial Roese, N. J. 1997. “Counterfactual Thinking.” Psychological
Analysis of Honolulu Motor Vehicle Crashes: I. Spatial Bulletin 121 (1): 133–48.
Analysis.” Accident Analysis & Prevention 27 (5): 663–74. Rosch, Ernst. 1978. “Principles of Categorization.” In Cognition
Levine, Ned., Karl E. Kim, and Lawrence H. Nitz. 1995b. and Categorization, edited by Ernst Risch and B. B. Lloyd,
“Spatial Analysis of Honolulu Motor Vehicle Crashes: II. 27–48. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Dumbaugh et al. 13

Rosen, Erik, and Ulrich Sander. 2009. “Pedestrian Fatality Risk Slovic and Amos Tversky, 117–128. Cambridge: Cambridge
as a Function of Car Impact Speed.” Accident Analysis & University Press. doi:10.1017/CBO9780511809477.009
Prevention 41:536–42. van der Horst, Richard, and Nico Kaptein. 1998. “Self-Explaining
Salmon, Paul M., Roderick McClure, and Neville Anthony Stanton. Roads.” In Proceedings of the 11th ICTCT Workshop, 15–32.
2012. “Road Transport in Drift? Applying Contemporary Systems Budapest, Hungary: International Conference on Traffic
Thinking to Road Safety.” Safety Science 50 (9): 1829–38. and Transport Psychology [ICTCT]. https://www.ictct.net/
Schank, Roger C., and Robert P. Abelson. 1977. Scripts, Plans, conferences/11-budapest-1998/
Goals and Understanding: An Inquiry into Human Knowledge Van Elslande, Pierre, and L. Faucher-Alberton. 1997. “When
Structures. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum. Expectancies become Certainties: A Potential Adverse Effect
Schoettle, Brandon, and Michael Sivak. 2015. “A Preliminary Analysis of Experience.” In Traffic and Transport Psychology: Theory
of Real-world Crashes Involving Self-driving Vehicles.” Report and Application, edited by Talib Rothengatter and Enrique
# UMTRI-2015-34. Ann Arbor: The University of Michigan Carbonell Vaya, 147–59. Amsterdam: Pergamon.
Transportation Research Institute. Vision Zero Network. 2018. “Vision Zero Cities.” https://vision
Shaw, T. 2003. “NCHRP 311: Performance Measures of Opera- zeronetwork.org/wp-content/uploads/2018/01/2018_Jan_VZ
tional Effectiveness for Highway Segments and Systems.” -map.pdf.
Washington, DC: Transportation Research Board. Wilde, Gerald J. S. 1982. “The Theory of Risk Homeostasis:
Smith, Daniel T., and Donald Appleyard. 1981. “Improving Implications for Safety and Health.” Risk Analysis 2:209–25.
the Residential Street Environment, Final Report.” Report Yu, Chia-Yuan, and Minjie Xu. 2018. “Local Variations in the
FHWA/RD-81/031. Washington DC: Federal Highway Impacts of Built Environments on Traffic Safety.” Journal of
Administration. Planning Education and Research 38:314–28.
Theeuwes, Jan. 2002. “Sampling Information from the Road Zegeer, Charles V., Cara Seiderman, Pete Lagerwey, Mike Cynecki,
Environment.” In Human Factors for Highway Engineers, Michael Ronkin, and R. Schnieder. 2002. “Pedestrian Facilities
edited by Ray Fuller and Jorge A. Santos, 131–46. New York: Users Guide—Providing Safety and Mobility.” Report No.
Pergamon. FHWA-RD-102-01. Washington, DC: Federal Highway
Theeuwes, Jan. 2012. “Self-Explaining Roads and Traffic Administration.
System.” In Designing Safe Road Systems: A Human Factors
Perspective, edited by Jan Theeuwes, Richard van der Horst, Author Biographies
and Maria Kuiken, 11–26. New York: CRC Press.
Treat, John R., Nicholas S. Tumbas, Stephen T. McDonald, Eric Dumbaugh is a professor of Urban and Regional Planning at
David Shinar, Rex D. Hume, R. E. Meyer, R. L. Stansifer, Florida Atlantic University and Associate Director of the Collabor-
and N. John Castellan. 1979. “Tri-Level Study of the Causes ative Sciences Center for Road Safety.
of Traffic Accidents.” Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Dibakar Saha, PhD, is a research associate with the Collaborative
Transportation. Sciences Center for Road Safety.
Tversky, Amos and Daniel Kahneman. 1982. “Causal Schemas in
Judgments under Uncertainty”. In Judgment under Uncertainty: Louis Merlin is an assistant professor in the School of Urban and
Heuristics and Biases, edited by Daniel Kahneman, Paul Regional Planning at Florida Atlantic University.

You might also like