You are on page 1of 8

Research Policy 49 (2020) 103872

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Research Policy
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/respol

Gordon Tullock meets Phineas Gage: The political economy of lobotomies in T


the United States
Raymond J. Marcha, Vincent Gelosob,

a
North Dakota State University, Barry Hall Office 402, 811 2nd Ave. N, Fargo, ND 58102, United States
b
King's University College at Western University Canada, 266 Epworth Avenue, London, Ontario N6A 2M3, Canada

ARTICLE INFO ABSTRACT

Keywords: Incentives affect the ways in which scientific research is disseminated and translated into practice. From 1936 to
Lobotomies, Science 1972, approximately fifty thousand lobotomies were performed in the US, with the majority occurring during
Political economy the late 1940s and early 1950s. Curiously, the lobotomy's popularity coincided with a consensus within the
Mental health medical community that the procedure was ineffective. To explain this paradox, we follow the framework de-
Health economics
veloped by Tullock (2005) to examine how financial incentives within the scientific community affected how
scientific research is used in practice. We argue that government funding for public mental hospitals and asylums
JEL:
expanded and prolonged the use of the lobotomy, despite mounting scientific evidence. We demonstrate that the
I18
I11 lobotomy was used less in private mental hospitals and asylums. This paper provides an explanation for the use
H44 of scientifically discredited procedures due to the lack of responsiveness of government funding agencies. The
H4 results have implications for the dissemination and translation of scientific knowledge in practice.
H42

1. Introduction lobotomy to treat mental illness in the US. Beginning with the case of
Phineas Gage, whose personality was dramatically changed after a con-
There are numerous well-documented gaps between the academic struction accident left him without his frontal lobe, medical professionals
and the practical communities in fields such as nursing (Maben et al., hypothesized altering or removing portions of the brain could alleviate
2006; Huston et al., 2018), nutrition (Ciarli and Raflos (2017)), man- mentally illness (Pressman, 2002). From 1936 to 1972, approximately
agement practices (Rynes et al., 2018) and public health (Pierce and fifty thousand lobotomies were performed in the US. The majority of
Gilpin, 2001; McAteer et al., 2018). These gaps are challenges that af- them occurred during the late 1940s and 1950s during a period fre-
fect not only on the ability to bring scientific findings to professional quently called the “lobotomy boom” (Holden, 1973, p.1109). Although
practices but also the ability of the latter to inform the scientific com- the last lobotomy in the US was performed nearly fifty years ago, it still
munity of the needs that it faces so as to more efficiently guide research serves as a prominent example of the potential for widespread ineffective
(Bartunek and Rynes, 2014; Kealey and Ricketts, 2014). The under- medicine to permeate the medical community (Holden, 1973). Some
standing of the sources of the gaps (and its persistence) is crucial to have called it one of the most spectacular failures in the history of
avoid the prolonged use of erroneous or less effective practices medicine (Johnson, 2009, p.367) and more lunatic than the patients it
(March, 2017). This is especially true for medical discoveries, where was supposed to help (Havens, 2004, p.352). In addition to being
misaligned institutional factors can result in misuse of resources and medically ineffective, lobotomies were also psychologically devastating
negatively impact the health of patients. These factors also contribute for patients. Its side effects such as include severely impaired in-
to a public mistrust of scientists and expert opinion, which hinders the telligence, impulsivity, childlike dependency, mood fluctuations, incon-
application of science to address medical issues (Rynes et al., 2018). tinence, paranoia (Robinson, 1946; El-Hai, 2005). Many lobotomized
A well-known example of such a gap is the prolonged use of the patients also died due to surgical complications (Freeman, 1957). 1

Corresponding author.

E-mail addresses: raymond.j.march@ndsu.edu (R.J. March), vgeloso@uwo.ca (V. Geloso).


1
Families and loved ones of lobotomized patients also faced considerable hardships. In many cases, patients were unable to care for themselves and required
constant caretaking. The procedure's effect on patient's personalities often left them unable to maintain close relationships (El-Hai, 2005). In rare cases in which
lobotomized patients were able to leave the asylum, many were unable to care for themselves and eventually returned (Pressman, 2002). Patients who returned or did
not demonstrate improvement often underwent additional lobotomies (El-Hai, 2005).

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.respol.2019.103872
Received 13 August 2018; Received in revised form 9 October 2019; Accepted 12 October 2019
Available online 21 October 2019
0048-7333/ © 2019 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
R.J. March and V. Geloso Research Policy 49 (2020) 103872

Curiously, a noteworthy gap emerged early on between medical Because physicians frequently occupy a position requiring them to
research and medical practice. While lobotomies swiftly fell out of favor practice and contribute to medical science (Bartunek and Rynes, 2014),
when the Food and Drug Administration approved the first anti- understanding which institutional incentives weakened the relationship
psychotic drug in 1954 (even if it did continue on smaller but non- between medical practice and medical science has critical insights for
negligible numbers in public hospitals after that point), the procedure science and broader research policy. As the lobotomy boom took place
was reviled and scorned during the time it was used. Five years after the during the larger public health movement in US medical history
first US procedure, a consensus formed among medical professionals (Hamowy, 2008; Troesken, 2015), our case study also sheds light on the
across multiple specialties that there was little evidence the procedure impact of increased bureaucratization of science.
helped patients (Holden, 1973). The lobotomy was denounced by the To perform our analysis, we utilize the framework developed by
American Medical Association in 1941, several years before the “boom” Tullock (2005) in The Organization of Inquiry. In this book, Tullock uses
period. Indeed, the procedure faced criticism from physicians of various economic insight to examine the role of scientists within their peer
medical backgrounds for the entire duration of its use network (termed the scientific community) to advance the body of
(Valenstein, 1986). Given the magnitude of the harm caused and the scientific knowledge. Tullock's discussion of the various institutional
length of time that elapsed before the practice-academic gap began to factors that inhibit scientists’ ability to advance correct discoveries or
close, lobotomies are a prime example of the need to understand the stop false discoveries from gaining acceptance provides us a fruitful
source and persistence of gaps between the scientific discovery and framework to understand the incentive structure of physicians electing
adoption of these findings. Previous literature attempting to explain the to use lobotomies. To inform our analysis, we use historical government
lobotomy's longevity and popularity despite the persistent criticism documents containing data on mental hospital and asylum enrollment
from the medical community largely emphasizes the absence of over the relevant period. When historical documents are unable to
knowledge of how ineffective the procedure was or explains its popu- provide data, we rely on the work of Valenstein (1986),
larity by noting that few alternative treatment methods were present Pressman (2002), and El-Hai (2005) and findings from medical jour-
(Pressman, 2002; El-Hai, 2005). However, these explanations have nals.
limited value without referring to the role of economic incentives.2 This paper proceeds as follows. In Section 2, we briefly review the
In this paper, we propose that the lobotomy's popularity and long- history of the lobotomy in the US and further develop the puzzle of why
evity in the US was the result of the incentives generated by the in- the practice persisted for so long. In Section 3, we adapt Tullock's fra-
stitutional structure of mental healthcare provision. Primarily, we note mework to the incentive structures physicians faced when deciding to
that funding for public mental hospitals and asylums was provided by use or not use the lobotomy. Section 4 applies the framework through
state and federal governments at a very low level per capita by the time an institutional comparison of the two systems. Section 5 concludes and
the practice was introduced. This served to constrain revenues; and the provides implications for the current relationship between government
lobotomy was cheaper than other treatment methods (El-Hai, 2005). funding and scientific discovery as well as proposals for future research.
Further, lobotomized patients were easier to manage (their brain da-
mage often made them docile). These factors, in conjunction with the 2. History of lobotomies in the United States
fact that bureaucratic oversight provided little incentive to effectively
treat patients, motivated physicians to perform cost- and conflict- In the late 1890s and early 1900s, the psychiatry and neurology
minimizing treatment. In contrast, physicians operating in private disciplines underwent a large-scale lobbying effort calling for an in-
mental hospitals and asylums were funded by the patients, by their creased government role in promoting mental health and providing
caregivers, or through philanthropic donations. These funds could be care for mentally disturbed patients (Rothman, 1971, 2002;
reduced if physicians did not provide adequate care or used an in- Pressman, 2002; Burnham, 2015). The leader of the movement was
effective practice. The different institutional settings generated dra- Adolf Meyer, who worked to unify the fields of psychiatry, psychology,
matically different outcomes. and neurology under the banner of psychobiology (Pressman, 2002).
Analyzing how the incentives produced within different institu- Meyer's hope was to secure a larger role for these disciplines in the
tional environments where medical professionals performed lobotomies broader medical community by obtaining more political clout. As part
created a gap between medical research and practice also provides of his vision, Meyer sought to redefine the role of providing mental
important implications for the health economics literature and litera- healthcare as caring for patients and simultaneously engaging in public
ture examining scientific research policy (Lyall and Tait, 2019). Recent health efforts to prevent insanity (Rothman, 1971, 2002;
health economics literature finds that even small changes in incentives Pressman, 2002).
will affect the quality, amount, and type of care (Price and A consequence of this lobbying effort was to redirect physicians’
Simon, 2009; Clemens and Gottlieb, 2014; Godager et al., 2016; efforts into political rather than scientific or medical avenues. Through
Huck et al., 2016; Brekke et al., 2017) as well as technology adoption professional contacts within government, Meyer and his political sup-
(Freedman et al., 2015). Institutional structure also impacts the quan- porters secured financial support to establish a large network of state
tity and quality of scientific research (Walsh and Lee, 2015; hospitals (with psychiatric wards) and increased funding for state asy-
Demircioglu and Audertsch, 2017). More importantly, as it is relevant lums.3 Whereas most federal funds for scientific research in the United
to our case, these institutional structures also alter the tendency to States after the Civil War had been for agricultural purposes
engage in scientific misconduct (Berggren and Karabag, 2019). Ex- (Kealey, 1996, p.148), the 1930s witnessed greater federal involvement
amining the prolonged use of the lobotomy in US medical history in public health research. (Burnham, 2015). The result was a swift in-
contributes to these literatures by providing a case study of how det- crease in public hospitals and publicly funded asylums
rimental poorly constructed incentives can impact medical practices in
ways that are equally detrimental to patients. 3
Our analysis also provides important implications for the role of Organizing medical professionals to gain political clout was not unique to
psychiatry and mental healthcare providers. Over the same period, medical
institutions in effectively advancing scientific inquiry and discovery.
professionals organized to reduce tuberculosis related deaths. Although the
movement did little to reduce to reduce morality, it was able to help pass nu-
merous regulations and state-funded sanitoriums through political influence
2
The role of incentives, as we argue later, helps explain why widespread and (Anderson et al., 2019). Similarly, Leeson et al. (2019) find medical profes-
persistent warnings from many physicians against using the lobotomy had little sionals in 19th century England were able to gain political influence under the
impact on preventing its use. Incentives also help explain why the lobotomy banner of regulating unproven medical treatments (what was referred to as
outlasted other ineffective treatment methods. quack medicine).

2
R.J. March and V. Geloso Research Policy 49 (2020) 103872

(Pressman, 2002; Burnham, 2015).4 mental illness.6 His hypothesis formed from his review of psychiatric
Meyer's vision also inspired efforts at the state level to reform public literature that had found that intentionally damaging brain tissues
health policy. Rothman (2002) notes that many states reformed their provoked behavioral changes in chimpanzees (Pressman, 2002). Moniz
commitment laws while federal financial support increased. These re- performed several lobotomies in Portugal but largely abandoned the
forms decreased the difficulty (in rigor and number of steps) of having practice early in his career (Pressman, 2002).
patients involuntarily committed. New York, which consistently com- In the 1930s, Moniz's procedure was adopted by Walter Freeman
mitted the most patients, was a prime example (Pressman, 2002). In- and James Watts, who championed its use in the US. Ten years after
voluntarily committed patients typically resided in state hospitals. Moniz conducted his first lobotomy, Freeman reported having con-
However, to relieve the hospital staff of the overcrowding burden, ducted 400 lobotomies (Freeman and Watts, 1946, p.293). To cut down
many patients were sent to state asylums for observation on procedure time and avoid some surgical risks, Freeman developed
(Pressman, 2002). Increased numbers of committed patients, stemming the transorbital lobotomy procedure in 1945. This version of the lo-
from increased government funding for mental healthcare and easier botomy involved inserting an icepick into the patient's nostril or eyelid
commitment laws, promoted a sharp increase in demand for the ser- to destroy brain tissue rather than opening the skull to remove tissue.7
vices of mental institutions (Rothman, 1971). As two reports from the By 1957, Freeman claimed to have performed some 3000 lobotomies
Census Bureau (cited in Noll, 1995, p.5) make clear, the number of with 600 being pre-frontal and 2400 transorbital (Freeman, 1957). The
institutionalized individuals relative to the general population in- procedure's comparative ease of use combined with overcrowding in
creased substantially between 1904 and 1923 (from 17.3 per 100,000 mental hospitals and asylums likely contributed to the rapid adoption
to 46.7 in 1923). From 1920 to 1940, the population of patients in and use of the lobotomy during the boom period of the late 1940s and
public mental hospitals and asylums committed because of retardation early 1950s.
increased nearly 167%, from 50,000 to 113,000 (Nelson and However, there was a strong reaction against lobotomies by medical
Crocker, 1978, p.1039). However, when we include other patients, the professionals throughout this period. In 1941, the American Medical
population increases to 481,000 (an over 850% increase) in 1940 Association published an editorial addressing five important criticisms
(NIMH, 1952, p.14). Overall, from 1900 to 1950, the inpatient popu- of the procedure, concluding no one could assert the lobotomy was a
lation increased nearly fourfold (Bassuk and Gerson, 1978).5 worthwhile procedure (Anonymous, 1941).8 In 1949, Charles Burlin-
Mental hospitals and asylums were ill equipped to handle the rapid game noted that worldwide scrutiny had developed over the effec-
influx of patients (Valenstein, 1986; El-Hai, 2005). In some cases, tiveness of the procedure (as cited in Pressman, 2002, p.141). Ac-
hospital directors and physician staff had no experience treating mental cording to the available data, the lobotomy's use grew exponentially
illnesses (Rothman, 2002). Overcrowding in mental healthcare facilities despite widespread criticism (Valenstein, 1986). The procedure's use
was commonplace as the national patient overpopulation rate increased also continued well after it fell out of favor with the medical profession
from 7.1% above capacity in 1926, to 9.4% in 1938 (NIHM, 1941, at large (Valenstein, 1986; Pressman, 2002).
p.75), to 12.5% in 1945 (NIMH, 1948, p.26), to 18.1% in 1949 Most explanations for why the lobotomy became popular despite
(NIMH, 1952, p.68). Despite widespread complaints of a lack of public the criticism emphasize that the mental health community did not
funds, public expenditures for mental healthcare were increasing sub- know it was ineffective or that alternatives to treat mental illness were
stantially. From 1946 to 1960, average expenditures per patient (ad- not available (Worthing et al., 1949; Valenstein, 1986;
justed for inflation) increased 153.5%. Between 1946 and 1950 (during Pressman, 2002). However, these explanations have noteworthy lim-
the lobotomy boom), expenditures per patient increased 43.4% itations. The lack-of-knowledge hypothesis seems unlikely given the
(Grob, 2014, p.164). widespread criticism the lobotomy faced through its use in the US
It is also necessary to mention the political impetus for devoting (Pressman, 2002, El-Hai, 2005). Further, before the results of the first
greater resources to dealing with mental illnesses. As historian Steven lobotomies were conclusive, the procedure was heavily criticized for its
Noll points out, this increase did not result from a greater concern for theoretical foundations (Valenstein, 1986). In fact, even Moniz and
the mentally ill. Rather, the actors involved, including legislators and Freeman both acknowledged the procedure was theoretically unsound
governors, considered “protection for society from retarded people, and (Pressman, 2002) even though the latter proceeded with the practice.
not the reverse, as their major priority” (Noll, 1995, p.5). Accordingly, The explanation pointing to lack of alternative treatments is best
the political environment was one in which elected officials were in- illustrated by Worthing et al. (1949, p.647), who, although critical of
centivized to increase the institutionalized population to isolate it from the procedure's effectiveness after evaluating some 350 case reports,
society. This did not mean that public funding matched the cost of the still concluded, “[i]n spite of its limitations, the operation seems to be
increased level of institutionalization. As this occurred during the therapeutically active and fills a real need while we await a better
Progressive and New Deal eras, when governments at all levels were treatment.” Although many of the treatment methods for mental illness
growing, public-funding requests for mental institutions competed with at the time were unsound, psychoanalysis and electroshock therapy
public-funding requests for social welfare programs such as workers’ were available. Both methods are still used today (Valenstein, 1986).
compensation and old-age pensions. Accordingly, the institutional su- Further, in 1890, Swiss physician and asylum director Gottlieb
perintendents were “beset with political pressures” that were accom- Burkhardt had lobotomized six of his patients.9 Burkhardt's efforts were
panied by “monetary constraints” (Noll, 1995, p.9). severely criticized, and he was eventually ostracized from the medical
It is within this context that lobotomies entered the stage. The first community. These efforts occurred when fewer treatment options for
lobotomies performed on humans consisted of injecting alcohol into the mental illness were available (El-Hai, 2005). The Swiss medical
prefrontal cortex to destroy tissues (Pool, 1954; Gross and
Schäfer, 2011). The procedure was later adapted to treat mental illness
6
(mental illnesses were also called such terms as mania, mental dis- In the medical literature, mental illness is also called mania, mental dis-
turbance, feeblemindedness, retardation, moronism, and insanity). The turbance, and insanity.
7
Other devices were used in different hospitals. However, Freeman typically
genesis of this movement was in Portugal, where physician Egas Moniz
used an icepick (El-Hai, 2005).
hypothesized that removing brain tissue in humans could alleviate 8
Freeman and Watts also faced considerable criticism before the 1940s as
well. Although the procedure faced considerable ethical criticism (El-Hai 2005),
we focus on the scientific criticism in this paper.
4 9
Although, historically, public asylums were common (Rothman, 1971), they Although the term did not exist at the time, Burkhardt attempted to remove
became significantly more common after the 1900s (Pressman, 2002). parts of the brain to alleviate mental illness, effectively serving the same pur-
5
Over the same period, the US population increased by less than 30 percent. pose as a lobotomy.

3
R.J. March and V. Geloso Research Policy 49 (2020) 103872

community was unwilling to condone suspect medical practice despite market economy (see also Hayek, 1945). Scientists rely on different
the absence of treatment for asylum patients (El-Hai, 2005). Thus, a institutional mechanisms to distinguish productive from unproductive
lack of accepted treatment seems insufficient to explain why the lo- uses of research resources and guide research efforts toward more-
botomy enjoyed widespread popularity in the US over forty years later. fruitful research projects. The key mechanism Tullock specifically em-
We now turn to an alternative framework emphasizing physician phasizes is the ability of the scientific community to verify discoveries.
incentives within the institutional structure of US mental healthcare The verification process is largely performed by scientists who are
(from the early 1900s to the mid-1950s). By adopting Tullock's (2005) considered experts in their fields of study. These experts earn their
framework, we are better able to explain the divergence of medical reputation from their peers and perform their verification role by ser-
consensus from medical practice than previous hypotheses. ving in leadership positions for academic journals and research orga-
nizations and serving in other professional capacities. If a scientist's
discovery is verified by his or her peers, it gains acceptance from the
3. Incentives and scientific inquiry broader scientific community. Similar to how the market process pun-
ishes transactional misconduct (see also Mises, 2007), when a scientist
Incentives matter in scientific research, discovery, and the dis- is caught engaging in fraudulent research activity, they suffer reputa-
tribution of research findings (Tullock 2005). In The Organization of tional damage within their peer network (Azoulay, Bonatti, and
Inquiry, Tullock (2005) considers scientists’ primary objective to be to Kreiger, 2017). Even co-researchers have strong incentives to police
utilize scientific discovery for personal gain. While Tullock develops each other to avoid the reputational costs of scientific misconduct as
two ideal-type scientists—the pure scientist and the applied scientist prior collaborators tend to suffer from a researcher's misconduct
(terms which are reminiscent of those used to refer to the academic- (Hussinger and Pellens, 2019). Others examine the dynamics of the
practice gap). It is the applied scientist's role that interests us because scientific community outside of the market-process framework but
the lobotomy primarily concerns that role. The personal gains include which remain complementary to it. Merton (1938) maintains that the
influencing peers in the broader scientific community (Tullock, 2005, relationship between a scientist's work and the impact other scientists
p.24) and earning financial rewards for pursuing research of interest to have in evaluating his work depends on the social structure of the
those outside the scientific community. By seeking the latter kind of broader scientific community. Kitcher (1990) contends mismatches
gain and attempting to assess the value of a scientific discovery for the between the group rationality of accepted scientific theories and the
public, scientists serve an entrepreneurial role. individual rationality of scientists working to change them ultimately
Although scientists’ broad research objectives differ, the scientists advance the body of scientific knowledge. Although the scientific
serve complementary roles in advancing the application and theoretical community must channel various research efforts to fruitful ends, di-
foundations of their disciplines (Tullock, 2005, p.33). As Tullock de- versity of scientific opinion promotes change of prevailing theories.11
scribes it, “The scientist's curiosity is subject to social guidance. The D'Agostino (2009) notes that the interconnectedness of various scien-
information inputs from other scientists are important in shaping the tific disciplines, in addition to the broader scientific division of labor,
problems which he will investigate. Similarly, he is normally interested also affects whether the body of scientific knowledge advances.
in the approval of his peers and hence will usually consciously shape his The sociological underpinnings of the scientific community may
research into a project which will pique other scientists’ curiosity as also influence how theories are accepted or discredited. Bloor (1976)
well as his own” (2005, p.25). and other scholars within the research program of science and tech-
In this regard, Tullock likens the process of scientific research and nological studies hold that scientifically accepted theories exist within
inquiry to the unhampered market process in economics. In his own particular social contexts that affect the influence they have on the
words, “The most effective way of ‘organizing’ science seems to be the broader scientific community. Individual interest guided within scien-
most perfect laissez faire” (Tullock, 2005, p.5). The market process tific social structures influences what positions scientists take on new
allows consumers and producers to exchange with each other in a theories and existing scientific knowledge (Mackenzie, 1981;
manner that coordinates effective (socially desirable) uses of resources Sismondo, 2008). Fuller (1993) notes that these social influences also
(Kirzner, 1974). impact scientists’ activism outside of their community. Even when
Although scientists pursue separate goals and lines of research and scientific consensus is reached, public perceptions and social structure
differ on a variety of other margins, their efforts are coordinated toward also impact how science is utilized (Turner, 2001). Further, as
advancing the broader scientific body of knowledge. Similarly, the Foucault (1965) points out, some social structures may allow for mis-
market process channels the actions of different actors pursuing their treatment of marginalized groups, including the insane. Foucault ar-
self-interest toward cooperative efforts to extend the nexus of exchange. gued (see Dillon and Foucault, 1980) that his work explained the ra-
Tullock is not the only one to notice these similarities. Polanyi (1951, tionalization that emerged for the type of treatment reserved for the
1962), Kealey (1996), Butos and Boettke (2002), and insane (i.e., confinement) after the eighteenth century. For the purposes
March et al. (2016) also compare the coordinating mechanisms of the of this paper, this argument of Foucault can be interpreted as an ex-
market process to the institutional structure of scientific research. Si- pression of how institutions shape incentives of actors and how, in turn,
milarly, Morlacchi and Nelson (2011), find medical practice advances the actors rationalize the incentivized practices.
through the interdependent workings of scientific understanding, the Another institutional mechanism occurs when scientific discoveries
applied practice of medicine, and the introduction of new medical are applied by other scientists in their research. This dissemination
technology.10 As such, we can categorize successful coordination as process serves to bolster current research lines and to uncover short-
reducing the gap between academic knowledge and application of this comings or limitations of discoveries in applied research. For example,
knowledge into professional practice. Contrarily, institutions which Subramanian, Lim, and Soh (2013) find that even when scientists’ re-
hinder coordination can widen such a gap. search draws contradictory conclusions, their findings can still be co-
As Tullock notes, the success of coordination within the scientific ordinated to advance R&D practices within the bio-technology industry.
community does not have access to a price system to coordinate dis- To Tullock (2005), the verification and the dissemination mechanisms
parate actions, and so they do not coordinate as successfully as the

10
It is important to note societal demands and scientific research often, but
do not necessarily always overlap. For example, Cairli and Rafols (2019) find
11
when examining rice research, find scientific discovery and societal demands Kitcher (1990) refers to the organization of the scientific community as the
overlap, “to a limited extent” (p. 949). “cognitive division of labor.”

4
R.J. March and V. Geloso Research Policy 49 (2020) 103872

imperfectly perform in the scientific community the role prices perform beyond the point when medical professionals (scientists) deemed it
in the market.12 ineffective indicates there were distortions in the verification and dis-
Much like the mechanisms of the market process, the scientific semination mechanisms. As noted below, dissemination-mechanism
community's mechanisms are less effective if they become distorted. breakdowns include favorable and misleading coverage from popular
Primarily, when the institutional structure changes the mechanisms press outlets as well as numerous adaptations of the procedure
that reward or punish scientific research, scientists’ incentives change. (Diefenbach et al., 1999; El-Hai, 2005). Further, when examining his-
These institutional changes can result in promoting avenues of scientific torical documents provided by the National Institute for Mental Hy-
research before they are validated by scientific peers (Taubes, 2007). giene (NIMH), there is a clear difference in popularity and longevity
They can also limit the corrective ability of knowledge producers to regarding the lobotomy's use in private mental hospitals and asylums
weed-out (or retract) incorrect statements (Berggren and compared to public ones. This provides further reason to suspect that
Karabag, 2019). Consequently, the verification mechanism's role of institutions played a prominent role structuring incentives to loboto-
preventing the advancement of erroneous scientific discoveries be- mize patients.
comes less effective (Kealey, 1996). Also, erroneous discoveries are
more likely to permeate other research areas, which distorts the dis- 4. The institutional structure of mental hospitals and asylums
semination mechanism (Tullock, 2005). 13
This is not to say that less effective institutions allow less effective Pressman (2002) notes that the kinds of evaluations made as to
science to continue indefinitely. Scientific errors will be found whether psychosurgery (i.e., lobotomies) worked were very different in
(Tullock, 2005) and cumulative research (and costs from foregoing the the public institutional context than they were in the private-practice
use of the resulting knowledge) may eventually force a correction context (p.145). Primarily, the way public mental hospitals and asylums
(Furman and Stern, 2011). However, when new theories are distanced were funded provided stronger incentives to use the lobotomy in
from the institutional mechanisms within the scientific community, it comparison to private establishments. We now review the institutional
delays the verification of and prolongs the dissemination of erroneous framework for public and private medical establishments deciding
discoveries. whether to use the lobotomy.
Previous literature examining distortions in scientific research and
their impact on the advancement of science bolsters Tullock's frame- 4.1. Public institutional structures
work. Iaria, Schwarz, and Waldinger (2018) find policies limiting
communication between scientists within different nations after World In the United States, starting in the late nineteenth century, asylums
War I significantly curtailed, “the production of basic science and its and special institutions for the feebleminded began to house larger and
application in new technology” (p. 927). Biasi and Moser (2018) find larger numbers (relative to population) of inmates (Sutton, 1991). This
that copyright laws worked in the same manner by limiting access to increase coincided with an increased desire of state governments to get
cutting-edge research and curtailing the number of newly-minted PhDs. involved because of the unease that insanity and feeblemindedness
Taubes (2007) finds financial incentives tied to researching dietary generated within the electorate (Noll, 1995), so these asylums were
advice advanced by the US Department of Agriculture likely prolonged largely state funded and were complemented with numerous manda-
incorrect nutritional hypotheses associating fat consumption with heart tory-sterilization laws, which were meant to dent the perceived increase
disease. Similarly, Scheall et al. (2018) find external influences af- in insanity during the era (Ladd-Taylor, 2017).
fecting the publication-citation-reputation process in dietary and nu- According to the NIMH, the staff-to-patient ratio increased from
tritional science that resulted in erroneous and unhealthy dietary 16:1 in 1938 (National Institute of Mental Health, 1941, pp.5, 77) to
guidelines provided by government agencies. Kealey (1996) notes 21:1 in 1949 (National Institute of Mental Health, 1952, pp.14, 66).
universities and scientists can serve in a powerful lobbying capacity, Because funding formulas for state hospitals and asylums were, for the
working to steer resources away from scientific inquiry and into se- most part, fixed at the state level on a per diem basis, low-cost methods
curing state favors and funding. These are the same distortions that of treatment were preferable from a financial standpoint. Further, be-
affect scientists in the private sector when they are trying to secure cause state budget allocations were insufficient to cover expenses in the
public research funds. As a result, Kealey found faster-decreasing re- postwar era (Grob, 2014, pp.66, 78), low-cost procedures were highly
turns to public research funding and less research diversity, which desired to keep hospitals and asylums solvent. With limited resources
hinders discovery. More closely related to this paper, Szasz (1963, and state-based funding tied to occupancy, physicians within public
2008, 2009) notes that unquestioned underlying assumptions in psy- hospitals and asylums faced strong incentives to minimize treatment
chiatry persist because they financially benefit mental healthcare pro- costs and time spent with patients comparatively more difficult to treat
fessionals (despite uncertainty regarding the benefits to patients).14 (or manage).
Extending Tullock's (2005) framework to examine the lobotomy is The lobotomy helped advance these objectives. First, it was a
fruitful for several reasons. First, the fact that the lobotomy was used cheaper alternative to many other available treatment methods. This is
especially true of the transorbital lobotomy, introduced by Freeman in
1946 as a quicker and less difficult way to lobotomize patients (El-
12
Tullock (2005) is not the only one who emphasizes this flawed process. Hai, 2005). Unlike forms of treatment that were administered over
Mantere and Ketokivi (2013) as well as Kapeller and Steinberger (2016) also multiple rounds, the lobotomy was, in theory, a onetime procedure. It
note the dissemination of discoveries process is limited and can be inefficient. was also common for lobotomized patients to become more docile and
13
In fact, scientists may have an incentive to distort the process if it protects easier to manage (Freeman, 1957). Freeman (1957) also notes that
their reputation. Once established, a scientist may try to erect barriers to entry patients were more likely to be discharged after a lobotomy, which
in the field or use political influence to redirect research funding so as to pre- could ease the burden of an overcrowded facility.
vent competition against his research. This would generate the process of cu- Physicians in public asylums and hospitals faced few repercussions
mulative advantage in science, which Robert Merton (1968) bemoaned. Be-
for their actions because their funding was largely based on the number
cause distorting the process limits competition, it cumulates the advantage of
of patients rather than whether the patients received adequate medical
“well established” scholars.
14
In Coercion as Cure (Szasz, 2009, pp.96–97), Szasz also tried tying the use of care. Further, public asylums and hospitals in some states held the right
coercion in psychiatry to government funding. However, he did not phrase this to perform lobotomies without obtaining the consent of patients (or
explicitly in terms of incentives. He merely noted that government financial legal caretakers) and could also refuse visitation rights (Rothman, 1971;
involvement would unavoidably increase the desire to rely on coercion on the Pressman, 2002). As Pressman notes, professionals working within the
part of psychiatrists. He also did not elaborate on this point. state hospital system were conflicted in their roles as medical

5
R.J. March and V. Geloso Research Policy 49 (2020) 103872

professionals and as hospital administrators (Pressman, 2002, p.209). (Pressman, 2002, p.162).
In short, there was a conflict between the scientific standards set by Further, an article in the Stanford Law Review in Anonymous (1949)
their medical peers and their financial support. insisted that if public hospitals and asylums did not face legal action,
Minimum-cost treatment techniques were also desired by super- their conflict of interest would result in the procedure's being used for
intendents and government officials. Both parties monitored distribu- physicians’ best interests and not their patients’. For instance, the op-
tion of treatment and were much less concerned with the effectiveness eration could be a satisfactory means of controlling obstreperous in-
of treatment (Rothman, 1980, 293–97). In some instances, public hos- mates even when unsuccessful at rehabilitating the patient.
pitals would document the number of lobotomies performed to indicate Overcrowding of facilities in conjunction with a lack of funds to
that patients received treatment (Pressman, 2002). Further, because administer care and treatment, as is characteristic of many state in-
federal funding financially rewarded physicians in public hospitals and stitutions, supplies a strong incentive for providing minimal care (1949,
asylums to use less costly and less effective treatments, criticisms from p.471). Physicians facing these limitations had incentives to use the
the medical community became less important for professional re- lobotomy, even if inappropriately, to save on costs. Indeed, Steven Noll
cognition. As physicians within public medical establishments acted as notes that the institutionalization of large numbers of individuals re-
applied scientists implementing potentially erroneous methods of sulted from political pressures but was not matched by adequate
treatment, their source of funding likely clouded the way they de- funding: “Beset by political pressures and monetary constraints, in-
termined scientific use and validity (Tullock, 2005, p.29). Publicly stitutional superintendents also struggled to control diverse populations
owned asylums, whose patients are involuntarily admitted, were simi- of individuals labelled as feeble-minded” (Noll, 1995, p.9). Noll (1995,
larly less receptive to scientific scrutiny of their treatment methods p.125) also notes that state legislatures and governors did not specify
because their profits were also obtained through minimizing costs ra- clear objectives, so there was a “poorly delineated sense of purpose” for
ther than finding effective treatments. mental institutions, which made it hard to consistently prioritize certain
objectives. In the 1950s, several prominent physicians, some of whom
4.2. Private institutional structures previously supported the limited use of the lobotomy, would come to
agree the procedure was overused (Pressman, 2002).
In contrast to physicians in public medical establishments, physi- There was a disconnect between the verification and dissemination
cians in private settings faced strong incentives to utilize treatment mechanisms during and after the lobotomy boom. Freeman's work to
methods that demonstrated success and to inquire into the treatments’ promote and use the lobotomy exemplifies this institutional failure.
soundness or limitations. As physicians financially benefited by deci- Freeman was opposed by medical professionals from numerous dis-
phering which methods were likely to help their patients, engaging ciplines who were skeptical at first (advocating limited use) and later
with their medical peers was advantageous. Similarly, physicians risked openly hostile during the boom (Valenstein, 1986; El-Hai, 2005). Many
financial losses and reputational damage for using ineffective treatment of his earliest detractors criticized the anatomical soundness of the lo-
methods. These institutional mechanisms served to motivate physicians botomy (El-Hai, 2005). Freeman's original presentation of his lobotomy
to exercise caution when selecting a treatment method they felt was research, at the Southern Medical Association in 1936, was met with
unproven or potentially harmful. severe criticism. He was on more than one occasion shouted down
Although physicians either directing or working for private mental during other presentations (El-Hai, 2005 pp.114–19). His book Psy-
asylums faced slightly different incentives from those in private prac- chosurgery was rejected by two academic publishers before being pub-
tice, they too faced steep financial and reputational costs by taking lished by a minor editing house. The publisher also received letters
excessive risks when treating patients. Reputational damage to the from medical professionals urging it not to publish the book
asylum could result in donors’ withdrawing their financial support, (Pressman, 2002). When it was published, few medical journals re-
decreases in voluntary enrollment (typically a large percentage of pa- viewed the book (El-Hai, 2005). In one rare review, prominent neuro-
tients), or caregivers’ removing committed patients. Second, many of surgeon Loyal Davis harshly remarked that the offhand way the surgical
those physicians were disciplined by participating in medical societies procedure is described and discussed is no credit to the essayist as a
in which their own research was discussed. surgeon, a pathologist, or one who is searching for scientific truth
This is not to say physicians in the private sector never made errors. (Pressman, 2002, p.81). In 1949, Psychiatric Quarterly depicted the use
Erroneous treatment methods were used by physicians in all settings of psychosurgery as little more than a shot in the dark (Pressman, 2002,
(Pressman, 2002). However, incentives faced by physicians in private p.319). Freeman's other academic publications made little impact
settings were aligned to exercise more caution than physicians in public (ibid.).15
asylums. However, because Freeman was serving the purpose of controlling
bureaucratic expenditures, he was shielded in part from the costs of
5. The lobotomy in practice continuing with lobotomies. The superintendents of hospitals and other
hospital administrators still spoke highly of him and hired him to per-
The tying of federal funds to patient populations combined with form lobotomies. Although the larger medical profession was highly
incentives to minimize treatment expenditures per patient motivated critical of the lobotomy procedure on theoretical grounds, the longevity
the use of the lobotomy. Physician Mesrop Tarumianz, a superintendent and popularity of its use can be explained by institutional changes in
of Delaware State Hospital, provides evidence that public hospitals the market for caring for the mentally disturbed. Where the incentives
responded to these incentives when, during a 1941 panel discussion at of those applying the lobotomy are misaligned, techniques change and
the American Medical Association, he notes: are less bound by the critical feedback of the scientific community.
From an Economic point of view, I should like to give some figures In contrast, private-practice physicians faced incentives to exercise
as to what this may mean to the public. We have to the following greater caution when administering treatment. Several medical histor-
conclusions with regard to our own cases: In our hospital there are 1250 ians note physicians in private practices were often hesitant to accept,
cases and of these about 18 could be operated on for $250 per case. and were even critical of, new mental-illness treatment methods.
That will constitute a sum of 5000 for 180 patients. Of these we will Valenstein (1986) notes the quick dismissal of infection theory in the
consider that 10%, or 18, will die, and a minimum of 50% of the re- 1920s. Infection theory, developed by Henry Cotton, medical director
maining, or 81 patients will become well enough to go home or be
discharged. The remaining 81 will be much better and more easily
cared for in hospital. Thus the hospital will be relieved of the care of 99 15
Others expressed similar skepticism towards the practice of the lobotomy at
patients. That will mean a savings $351,000 in ten years the beginning of the boom (Goldstein, 1950).

6
R.J. March and V. Geloso Research Policy 49 (2020) 103872

for a state hospital with a psychiatric ward, held that mental illness was While this paper speaks to an extreme case of a delay in the dis-
caused by infected body parts including teeth, tonsils, pituitary glands, semination of knowledge between medical discovery and medical
and several sexual organs. Cotton's theory faced severe criticism from practice, it speaks to wider audiences – especially those interested in
numerous physicians who challenged the theory on its biological health economics and management. The institutional incentives de-
soundness as well as the results he reported. Consequently, the theory scribed in this paper are observed in similar forms with regards to the
was quickly discredited. Freeman struggled to develop a successful quantity, quality and type of other medical services (Price and Simon
private practice early in his career (El-Hai, 2005, p.72). Shortly after 2009; Clemens and Gottlieb, 2014; Godager et al., 2016; Huck et al.,
developing the transorbital lobotomy, Watts (his business partner) re- 2016; Brekke et al., 2017). Our contribution compliments these findings
fused on moral and scientific grounds to work with Freeman and the research policy literature in that it examines how state-based
(Valenstein, 1986, pp.227–28; El-Hai, 2005, pp.189–92). Freeman funding alters the institutional structures to provide medical care.
would spend the rest of his medical career traveling the country giving Given our findings, the advancement of scientific inquiry and discovery
lectures and performing lobotomies outside his practice. His largest (including falsifying erroneous discoveries) may be better attained with
supporters in these endeavors were leading figures in public mental less state-led funding. In the case of the lobotomy, the manner in which
health establishments (Pressman, 2002). state financing was provided created distortions in the ability of med-
Pressman (2002, pp.236–42) provides further evidence of great ical professionals, acting as scientists, to dismiss the lobotomy as an
caution in private hospitals and asylums and evidence for the merits of ineffective treatment and deter its frequent use in mental healthcare.
their incentive structure when he examines the use of the lobotomy at Our analysis also contributes to the emerging literature on the bu-
McLane Hospital, a private mental hospital in Massachusetts. He finds reaucratization of science (Walsh and Lee, 2015; Demircioglu and
the medical staff engaged in a rigorous selection method and decision- Audertsch, 2017). Because institutional environments which fund re-
making process whenever the procedure was considered. Physicians search can make scientific research comparatively more bureaucratic,
organized committees to discuss whether the patients’ condition war- bureaucratic structures can affect the quantity and the quality of sci-
ranted undergoing surgery and whether all other treatment methods entific output by changing researcher's incentives. Tullock also pub-
had been truly exhausted. The committee also sought approval from the lished a prominent work on the economics of the bureaucracy
patients’ families and the patients if possible. If the committee agreed, (Tullock, 1965). To our knowledge, the literature examining the impact
and it often did not, that the lobotomy was worth the risk, the proce- of bureaucracy in utilizing and generating medical knowledge largely
dure took place in a separate hospital with a handpicked surgeon. Ex- emphasizes a managerial framework rather than economics
tensive procedures such as these were rarely observed in public asy- (Battilana 2011; Kellogg 2019).
lums, where treatment discretion was in the hands of the Future research may find Tullock's work on bureaucracy to be
superintendent (Rothman, 1971, p.136). Although Pressman provides fruitful in understanding how the incentives of a bureaucracy affect the
one example, his findings can be applied to private hospitals and asy- scientific.
lums generally. In 1950 (the height of the lobotomy boom), only 6% of
all lobotomies were performed in private settings (Valenstein, 1986, Declaration of Competing Interest
p.173).
None.
6. Conclusion
References
The advancement of scientific inquiry and discovery, like the ad-
vancement of the market, relies heavily on an institutional structure. Anderson, D.M., Charles, K.K., Las Hervas Olivares, C., Rees, D.I., 2019. Was the first
When effective, institutional mechanisms in science guide efforts to- public health campaign successful? Am. Econ. J. 11, 143–175.
Anonymous, 1941. Frontal lobotomy. J. Am. Med. Assoc. 117, 534–535.
ward productive lines of research and away from erroneous ones. When Anonymous, 1949. Lobotomy: surgery for the insane. Stanf. Law Rev. 1, 463–474.
these mechanisms are distorted, they are less able to perform these Azoulay, P., Bonatti, A., Krieger, J.L., 2017. The career effects of scandal: evidence from
roles. As a result, important gaps between practice and research may scientific retractions. Res. Policy 46, 1552–1569.
Bartunck, J.M., Rynes, S.L., 2014. Academics and practitioners are alike and unalike: the
emerge (and persist) in ways that are detrimental to the general public. paradoxes of academic-practitioner relationships. J. Manag. 40, 1181–1201.
The lobotomy (and the “lobotomy boom” period) in the United Bassuk, E.L., Gerson, S., 1978. Deinstitutionalization and mental health services. Sci. Am.
States provides such an example. Using the framework Tullock (2005) 238 (2), 46–53.
Battilana, J., 2011. The enabling role of social position in diverting from the institutional
developed to understand the institutional structure of the scientific status quo: evidence from the UK National Health Service. Org. Sci. 22, 817–834.
community, we argue that the prolonged use and popularity of the Berggren, C., Karabag, S.F., 2019. Scientific misconduct at an elite medical institute: the
lobotomy stemmed from the incentives created by perverse institutions role of competing institutional logics and fragmented control. Res. Policy 48,
428–443.
created by government financing of mental healthcare. This contrasts
Biasi, B., Moser, P., 2018. Effects of Copyrights on Science-Evidence from the US Book
with previous explanations, which largely held that the use of the lo- Republication Program. National Bureau of Economic Research , No. w24255.
botomy in the United States was due to a lack of knowledge and al- Bloor, D., 1976. Knowledge and Social Imaging. University of Chicago Press.
ternative treatment methods. By emphasizing incentives stemming Burnham, J.C., 2015. Health Care in America: A History. Johns Hopkins University Press.
Butos, W., Boettke, P., 2002. Kirznerian entrepreneurship and the economics of science. J.
from institutional constraints in public and private mental healthcare, Écon. Études Hum. 12, 1–14.
our hypothesis is better able to explain this puzzling episode in medical Brekke, K.R., Holmas, T.H., Monstad, K., Straume, O.R., 2017. Do treatment decisions
history. depend on physician financial incentives? J. Public Econ. 155, 74–92.
Ciarli, T., Raflos, I., 2017. The relationship between research priorities and societal de-
We bolster our hypothesis by noting the historical trend for public mand: the case of rice. Res. Policy 2019, 949–967.
hospitals and asylums to use the lobotomy despite widespread agree- Clemens, J., Gottlieb, J.D., 2014. Do physician's financial incentives affect medical
ment by the medical community that the procedure was ineffective. treatment and patient health? Am. Econ. Rev. 104, 1320–1349.
D'Agostino, F., 2009. From the organization to the division of cognitive labor. Polit.
Physician incentives within public settings were closely aligned to Philos. Econ. 8, 101–129.
minimize treatment costs resulting from the financial incentives set up Demircioglu, M.A., Audretsch, D.B., 2017. Conditions for innovation in public sector
by public funding. In contrast, physicians in private settings were less organizations. Res. Policy 46, 1681–1691.
Diefenbach, G.J., Diefenbach, D., Baumeister, A., West, M., 1999. Portrayal of lobotomy
likely to use the lobotomy because they faced greater financial and in the popular press: 1935–1960. J. Hist. Neurosci. 8, 60–69.
professional costs when adopting ineffective treatment options. Using Dillon, M., Foucault, M., 1980. Conversation with Michel Foucault. Threepenny Rev.
historical records and the findings of medical historians, we find evi- 1, 4–5.
El-Hai, J., 2005. The Lobotomist: A Maverick Medical Genius and His Tragic Quest to Rid
dence supporting our institutions- and incentives-based explanation for
the World of Mental Illness. J. Wiley.
the use of the lobotomy in the United States.

7
R.J. March and V. Geloso Research Policy 49 (2020) 103872

Foucault, M., 1965. Madness and Civilization: A History of Insanity in the Age of Reason. practice/policy gap in public health: perspectives from Scotland and Canada. J.
Pantheon Books. Public Health.
Freedman, S., Lin, H., Simon, K., 2015. Public health insurance expansions and hospital Merton, R.K., 1938. Science and the social order. Philos. Sci. 5, 321–337.
technology adoption. J. Public Econ. 121, 117–131. Merton, R.K., 1968. The Matthew effect in science: The reward and communication
Freeman, W., 1957. Frontal lobotomy 1936–1956: a follow-up study of 3000 patients systems of science are considered. Science 159, 56–63.
from one to twenty years. Am. J. Psychiatry 113, 877–886. Mises, L., 2007. Human Action: A Treatise on Economics. Liberty Fund.
Freeman, W., Watts, J., 1946. Pain of organic disease relieved by prefrontal lobotomy. Morlacchi, P., Nelson, R.R., 2011. How medical practice evolves: learning to treat failing
Lancet 247, 953–955. hearts with an implantable device. Res. Policy 40, 511–525.
Furman, J.L., Scott Stern, S., 2011. Climbing atop the shoulders of giants: the impact of National Institute of Mental Health, 1941. Patients in Mental Institutions. 1938. US
institutions on cumulative research. Am. Econ. Rev. 101, 1933–1963. Government Printing Office.
Godager, G., Hennig-Schmidt, H., Iversen, T., 2016. Does performance disclosure influ- National Institute of Mental Health, 1948. Patients in Mental Institutions. 1945. US
ence physicians’ medical decisions? An experimental study. J. Econ. Behav. Organ. Government Printing Office.
131, 36–46. National Institute of Mental Health, 1952. Patients in Mental Institutions. 1949. US
Goldstein, K., 1950. Prefrontal lobotomy. Sci. Am. 182 (2), 44–47. Government Printing Office.
Grob, G.N., 2014. From Asylum to Community: Mental Health Policy in Modern America. Nelson, R.P., Crocker, A.C., 1978. The medical care of mentally retarded persons in public
Princeton University Press. residential facilities. N. Engl. J. Med. 299, 1039–1044.
Gross, D., Schäfer, G., 2011. Egas Moniz (1874–1955) and the invention of modern Noll, S., 1995. Feeble-Minded in Our Midst: Institutions for the Mentally Retarded in the
psychosurgery: A historical and ethical reanalysis under special consideration of South, 1900–1940. University of North Carolina Press.
Portuguese original sources. Neurosurg. Focus 30, E8. Pierce, J.P., Gilpin, E.A., 2001. News media coverage of smoking and health is associated
Havens, L., 2004. Psychiatric Movements: From Sects to Science. Transaction Publishers. with changes in population rates of smoking cessation but not initiation. Tob. Control
Hayek, F.A., 1945. The use of knowledge in society. Am. Econ. Rev. 35, 519–530. 10, 145–153.
Holden, C., 1973. Psychosurgery: legitimate therapy or laundered lobotomy? Science Polanyi, M., 1951. The Logic of Liberty. Routledge & Kegan Paul.
179, 1109–1112. Polanyi, M., 1962. The republic of science: its political and economic theory. Minerva 1,
Hamowy, R., 2008. Government and Public Health in America. Edward Elgar Publishing. 54–74.
Huck, S., Lusner, G., Spitzer, F., Tyran, J.R., 2016. Medical insurance and free choice of Pool, J., 1954. Psychosurgery in older people. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 2, 456–466.
physician shape patient overtreatment: a laboratory experiment. J. Econ. Behav. Org. Pressman, J.D., 2002. Last Resort: Psychosurgery and the Limits of Medicine. Cambridge
131, 78–105. University Press.
Huston, C.L., Phillips, B., Jeffries, P., Todero, C., Rich, J., Knecht, P., Sommer, S., Lewis, Price, J., Simon, K., 2009. Patient education and the impact of new medical research. J.
M.P., 2018. The academic‐practice gap: strategies for an enduring problem. Nurs. Health Econ. 28, 1166–1174.
Forum 53, 27–34. Robinson, M.F., 1946. What price lobotomy? J. Abnorm. Soc. Psychol. 41, 421.
Hussinger, K., Pellens, M., 2019. Guilt by association: how scientific misconduct harms Rothman, D.J., 1971. The Discovery of the Asylum: Social Order and Disorder in the New
prior collaborators. Res. Policy 48, 516–530. Republic. Aldine de Gruyter.
Iaria, A., Schwarz, C., Waldinger, F., 2018. Frontier knowledge and scientific production: Rothman, D.J., 2002. Conscience and Convenience: The Asylum and Its Alternatives in
evidence from the collapse of international science. Q. J. Econ. 927–991 n.d. Progressive America. Routledge.
Johnson, J., 2009. A dark history: Memories of lobotomy in the new era of psychosurgery. Rynes, S.L., Colbert, A.E., Boyle, E.H., 2018. When the “best available evidence” doesn't
Med. Stud. 1, 367–378. win: how doubts about science and scientists threaten the future of evidence-based
Kapeller, J., Steinerberger, S., 2016. Emergent phenomena in scientific publishing: a si- management. J. Manag. 44, 2995–3010.
mulation exercise. Res. Policy 45, 1945–1952. Scheall, S., Butos, W., McQuade, T., 2018. Social and scientific disorder as epistemic
Kealey, T., 1996. The Economic Laws of Scientific Research. Macmillan, London. phenomena, or the consequences of government dietary guidelines. J. Inst. Econ.
Kealey, T., Ricketts, M., 2014. Modelling science as a contribution good. Res. Policy 43, 1–17 n.d.
1014–1024. Sismondo, S., 2008. Science and Technology Studies and an Engaged Program. MIT Press.
Kellogg, K.C., 2019. Making the cut: using status-based countertactics to block social Subramanian, A., Lim, K., Soh, P., 2013. When birds of a feather don't flock together:
movement implementation and microconstitutional change in surgery. Organ. Sci. different scientists and the roles they play in biotech R&D alliances. Res. Policy 42,
23, 1546–1570. 595–612.
Kirzner, I., 1974. Competition and Entrepreneurship. University Chicago Press. Sutton, J.R., 1991. The political economy of madness: The expansion of the asylum in
Kitcher, P., 1990. The division of cognitive labor. J. Philos. 87, 5–22. progressive America. Am. Sociol. Rev. 56, 665–678.
Ladd-Taylor, M., 2017. Fixing the Poor: Eugenic Sterilization and Child Welfare in the Szasz, T., 1963. Law, Liberty and Psychiatry: An Inquiry into the Social Uses of Mental
Twentieth Century. Johns Hopkins University Press. Health Practices. Syracuse University Press.
Leeson, P.T., King, M.S., Fegley, T.J., 2019. Regulating quack medicine. Public Szasz, T., 2008. Psychiatry: The Science of Lies. Syracuse University Press.
Choice n.d. Szasz, T., 2009. Coercion as Cure: A Critical History of Psychiatry. Transaction Publishers.
Lyall, C., Tait, J., 2019. Beyond the limits to governance: new rules of engagement for the Taubes, G., 2007. Good Calories, Bad Calories. Anchor.
tentative governance of the life sciences. Res. Policy 48 (5), 1128–1137. Troesken, W., 2015. The Pox of Liberty: How the Constitution Left Americans Rich, Free,
Maben, J., Latter, S., Clark, J.M., 2006. The theory–practice gap: impact of professional – and Prone to Infection. University of Chicago Press.
bureaucratic work conflict on newly‐qualified nurses. J. Adv. Nurs. 55, 465–477. Tullock, G., 1965. The Politics of Bureaucracy. Public Affairs Press.
MacKenzie, D.A., 1981. Statistics in Britain, 1865–1930: The Social Construction of Tullock, G., 2005. The organization of inquiry. Vol. 3 of The Selected Works of Gordon
Scientific Knowledge. Edinburgh University Press. Tullock. Liberty Fund.
Mantere, S., Ketokivi, M., 2013. Reasoning in organization science. Acad. Manag. Rev. 36, Turner, S., 2001. What is the problem with experts? Soc. Stud. Sci. 31, 123–149.
70–89. Valenstein, E.S., 1986. Great and Desperate Cures: The Rise and Decline of Psychosurgery
March, R.J., 2017. Entrepreneurship in off-label drug prescription: Just what the doctor and Other Radical Treatments for Mental Illness. Basic Books.
ordered!. J. Priv. Enterp. 32, 75–92. Walsh, J.P., Lee, Y., 2015. The bureaucratization of science. Res. Policy 44, 1584–1600.
March, R.J., Martin, A.G., Redford, A., 2016. The substance of entrepreneurship and the Worthing, H.J., Brill, H., Wigderson, H., 1949. 350 cases of prefrontal lobotomy.
entrepreneurship of substances. J. Entrep. Public Policy 5, 201–220. Psychiatr. Q. 23 (4), 617–656.
McAteer, J., Ruggiero, E., Fraser, A., Frank, J.W., 2018. Bridging the academic and

You might also like