You are on page 1of 7

Psychological Inquiry Copyright © 2005 by

2005, Vol. 16, No. 4, 162–167 Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

The Nonobvious Social Psychology of Happiness


Ed Diener
University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, and The Gallup Organization
Shigehiro Oishi
University of Virginia

There are several classic findings in social psychol- mous article of Brickman, Coates, and Janoff-Bulman
ogy that behavioral scientists recognize as having ex- (1978) in which they maintain that people adapt
erted a profound influence on people’s understanding hedonically to both positive and negative conditions.
of the world. The stars of our field, such as Milgram, The idea of adaptation has arguably been the most in-
Zimbardo, Darley, and Latane, have demonstrated the fluential concept guiding conceptions of well-being.
power of social situations to influence behavior, for ex- We discuss the current state of our knowledge about
ample in obedience, bystander intervention, confor- whether the hedonic treadmill (Brickman & Camp-
mity, and altruism. Another classic set of findings of bell, 1971) of adaptation is adequate. The second area
social psychology describes in-group biases, and how we review is the importance of social relationships to
group membership profoundly influences people’s adequate human functioning. Social psychologists
perceptions of events. The “They Saw a Game” study such as Baumeister and Leary (1995) reviewed the
by Hastorf and Cantril (1954)—which showed how negative impact of ostracism and a lack of social sup-
Dartmouth and Princeton fans recalled two very differ- port on people’s functioning. Evidence has shown
ent football games, although they viewed the same that close social relationships might also be necessary
game—is a wonderful example in this area. John Bargh to the subjective experience of well-being, not simply
and others have begun a research tradition on beneficial to it.
nonconscious influences that has had a startling impact The third topic we cover relates to the second, and it
on our understanding of motivation. Yet another area concerns the fact that negative stimuli are more power-
where social psychology has made true progress is in ful than positive stimuli (e.g., Rozin & Royzman,
our conception of stereotyping and prejudice. Al- 2001). In social relationships, as with other stimuli,
though all of these areas deserve mention as major ad- negative episodes seem to be weighted more heavily
vances in social psychology, we wanted to avoid re- than positive ones. This helps explain why good social
peating the studies mentioned by others in this issue. relationships are essential to well-being. Our fourth
Thus, we have chosen to describe the social psycholog- topic is concerned with the power of culture. Social
ical findings that have had a large influence on our own psychologists have demonstrated the dramatic influ-
ence of culture on thinking and behavior (e.g., Markus
field, the understanding of well-being, because we sus-
& Kitayama, 1991; Nisbett, 2003; Triandis, 1989). We
pect that the ties of social psychology to well-being re-
describe how culture can also influence the definition,
search might be less self-evident to most readers, ex-
experience, and causes of happiness. Finally, we de-
cept for the important program of research on
scribe evidence showing that memory is reconstructive
well-being judgments of Norbert Schwarz and Fritz
rather than an exact replica of the original experience
Strack (1999). Because the link between their research
(e.g., Ross, 1989). In the domain of well-being this has
and well-being research is well known to social psy-
implications not only for how people recall their
chologists, we do not review it here (see Schwarz &
moods and emotions, but also for how memory influ-
Strack, 1999, for review). One motive behind our
ences people’s future choices. The findings from each
building the bridge from social psychology to well-be-
of these five lines of social psychological research have
ing research is to extend the field of social psychology strongly influenced our understanding of well-being
beyond the traditional bounds. and ill-being, extending our scientific conceptions far
We will briefly touch on five sets of findings in so- beyond self-evident popular notions of happiness.
cial psychology, discuss the implications of these
findings to well-being, and describe studies con-
ducted by well-being researchers that follow directly The Hedonic Treadmill Revisited:
from the social psychological studies. Although lay Adaptation to Circumstances
people might guess some aspects of these findings
correctly, we believe the conclusions are nonobvious Brickman and Campbell (1971) advanced the idea
to most nonpsychologists. First, we discuss the fa- that we all live on a hedonic treadmill because good
HAPPINESS

things make us only temporarily happy and bad Implications


things make us only temporarily unhappy. In the long
Although the idea of the hedonic treadmill has not
run, we are fixed at hedonic neutrality, and our efforts
been supported in all of its particulars (see Diener,
to make ourselves happier by gaining good life cir-
Lucas, & Scollon, 2005), it still provides a fundamen-
cumstances are only short-term solutions. Brickman
tal insight—over time good things and bad things usu-
and Campbell asserted that efforts to be happier than
ally lose their power to strongly affect us. Although
neutral in the long run are doomed to failure. A slave
there are some extreme conditions that can lower our
with chronic emphysema living alone in a hovel next
well-being, many of the good and bad events provide
to his master’s mansion ought to be no less happy
only short boosts and downdrafts. Receiving a raise at
than the healthy and wealthy master. Although this
work, buying a new car, or winning an award are not
conclusion is shocking, it has informed our thinking
usually the road to long-term happiness. Instead, fresh
about what causes happiness.
involvement in activities, relationships, and goals can
In 1978 Brickman et al. presented evidence from
be a continuous source of happiness. In this sense, the
paraplegics and lottery winners to offer empirical
aphorism, “Happiness is a process, not a place,” seems
support for the idea that adaptation brings us all back
to be accurate. This does not mean that our circum-
down to hedonic neutrality, irrespective of how good
stances have no influence whatsoever on our happi-
or bad the event was originally experienced. It should
ness; they do. Rather it means that we should not rely
be noted, however, that in a closer inspection, the evi-
on circumstances alone to give us long-term feelings of
dence of Brickman et al. for adaptation was mixed
well-being. Continued involvement in new goals,
(i.e., paraplegics were not as happy as others). Our
meaningful social interactions, and interesting activi-
recent studies offer stronger support for adaptation, as
ties is required to maintain a flourishing sense of hap-
well as the modifications that must be made to the
piness (Lyubomirsky, Sheldon, & Schkade, in press).
original theory. First, it appears that people adapt
Another important implication of the findings on
over time, but not always completely back to the
adaptation is that we often mispredict what will make
point where they started. For instance, we found that
us happy and unhappy (Gilbert, Pinel, Wilson,
both widowhood (Lucas, Clark, Georgellis, & Diener,
Blumberg, & Wheatley, 1998). We believe that if we
2003) and unemployment (Lucas, Clark, Georgellis,
become wealthy we will be happier than we will in fact
& Diener, 2004) led to lower levels of life satisfaction
be, and we believe that if we do not obtain tenure we
even many years after the event. Although people
will be more miserable than we will in fact be. These
showed adaptation over time after the event occurred,
mispredictions demonstrate that we probably count too
they had not adapted completely back to their former
much on conditions to make us happy. It follows, for
levels of life satisfaction even after 5 years. Despite
example, that it would be a mistake to sacrifice close
the fact that people may not adapt to all conditions,
relationships or interesting work to pursue a job that
we have found that they adapt to the smaller rewards
was uninteresting but lucrative. The mispredictions
and setbacks of everyday life (Suh, Diener, & Fujita,
people make reveal that the full effects of adaptation
1996).
are not understood by laypeople.
Given that adaptation occurs but seems incom-
plete, are people living in harsher conditions less
happy than others? The answer seems to be that
sometimes they are, and sometimes they are not. For Close Social Relationships Are
example, we found that the African Maasai are rela- Essential to Well-Being
tively happy, although they live in dung huts without
indoor plumbing or electricity, and the Inuit of Bradburn (1969), in his classic seminal work on
Northern Greenland are relatively satisfied with well-being, found that social relationships were one of
their lives despite living in a very harsh climate the strongest correlates of positive emotions. In the
(Biswas-Diener, Vittersø, & Diener, 2004). How- past decades evidence has accumulated showing that
ever, we have found that street prostitutes, the home- lack of social support and close social relationships can
less, and people in mental hospitals are unhappy, far have far-reaching effects. Baumeister and Leary
below neutral, even when their conditions have per- (1995) reviewed the evidence showing that people
sisted over some period of time (e.g., Biswas-Diener seem to have a fundamental need for close social rela-
& Diener, 2001). Perhaps such social variables as tionships. Social connectedness and support are asso-
lack of respect and lack of trusted friends make ciated with better levels of autonomic activity, better
these conditions more persistently difficult than pov- immunosurveillance, and lower basal levels of stress
erty. This idea can be substantiated by the fact that hormones (Uchino, Cacioppo, & Kiecolt-Glazer,
impoverished individuals in the slums of Calcutta, 1996). Other recent work by Williams (2001) reveals
who live in shacks with their families, score in the impaired cognitive functioning in people who have
positive zone on life satisfaction. been ostracized from a group.

163
DIENER & OISHI

Newer evidence now suggests that close social re- although our system is designed to shut down these
lationships are not simply correlates of well-being, negative reactions relatively quickly. Thus, we react
but may have causal force. For example, research on strongly to negative events, according to Taylor, but we
widows (Lucas et al., 2003; Stroebe, Stroebe, also adapt to them.
Abakoumkin, & Schut, 1996) and divorced people Gottman (1994) extended to social relationships the
(Clark, Diener, Georgellis, & Lucas, 2004) shows idea that negative is stronger than positive. In his re-
substantial declines in well-being right before and af- search on marriage, Gottman found that marital part-
ter the loss of a significant other. When we examined ners who have an equal number of positive and nega-
the characteristics of the happiest individuals, we tive interactions perceive their marriage in negative
found without exception that they reported strongly terms. Indeed, the Gottman ratio suggests a ratio of five
positive social relationships (Diener & Seligman, or six positive interactions for every negative interac-
2002). Thus, Bradburn’s (1969) early findings on the tion for a marriage to be considered satisfying. Obvi-
importance of social relationships to well-being have ously, the specific ratio will change, depending on the
now been extensively supported. Although laypeople nature of the interactions, but the point is the same—
probably understand that close friends and family are that one usually needs more positive interactions than
correlated with happiness, they may not realize that negative ones to experience a relationship as desirable.
they are necessary for happiness, as well as for health Fredrickson and Losada (2004) recently extended the
and optimal cognitive functioning. Gottman ratio to positive and negative emotions as
they relate to global well-being, and found that a ratio
of 2.9 or above was needed for flourishing.
Implications
Many people focus on wealth when they pursue
Implications
happiness, but research on social relationships sug-
gests that they can be more important than material Much more research is needed on the Gottman ratio
prosperity to subjective well-being. The word needs to in other types of social relationships. However, for the
be spread—it is important to work on social skills, time being it would be wise in one’s relationships to re-
close interpersonal ties, and social support to be happy. member the dictum that negative is stronger than posi-
It is a mistake to value money over social relationships. tive. If one criticizes or corrects a friend or family
For instance, we found that students who value money member, one must work to have many positive interac-
more than love are dissatisfied with their lives (Diener tions to make up for that. The findings in this area sug-
& Oishi, 2000). gest that with our friends and family members, giving
compliments, helping them at tasks, interacting in in-
teresting conversations with them, and expressing af-
The Power of the Negative fection are necessary to keep the relationships in the
positive zone. Occasionally, of course, we need to offer
Close personal relationships are essential to criticisms, advice, and corrections because a behavior
well-being, but we are made miserable by tyrannical of a friend, colleague, or family member should be
supervisors, abusive spouses, and vindictive friends. changed. But we then need to work on our Gottman ra-
Without social relationships, it may be very difficult or tio to keep the relationship in the positive zone.
impossible to reach high levels of happiness. However,
because negative events affect our well-being more
than positive events, relationships can also be a power- Culture and Well-Being
ful force for unhappiness. An important insight of so-
cial and experimental psychologists is that we are built Anthropologists such as Mead, Benedict, and
to react more strongly to negative events than to posi- Malinowski made famous the dramatic differences that
tive ones. Thus, social relationships are a necessary exist between cultures. It was left to social and person-
cause for happiness, but they must contain a prepon- ality psychologists to measure and quantify the differ-
derance of positive interactions to serve this function. ences in a much more rigorous way than had occurred
In “Prospect Theory,” Kahneman and Tversky before. Furthermore, pioneers such as Hofstede,
(1979) hypothesized that losses loom larger than gains. Triandis, Markus, Kitayama, and Nisbett showed that
In later work, losses and gains were reframed in a even when people live in similar ways in industrialized
broader way to include all bad and good events nations, there are clear differences in the ways they
(Baumeister, Bratslavsky, Finkenauer, & Vohs, 2001; think, feel, and behave.
Rozin & Royzman, 2001). Summing up this view, Recently cultural differences have been found for
Baumeister et al. (2001) wrote that “bad is stronger the form and shape of happiness, as well as what
than good.” In a related vein, Taylor (1991) maintained causes it. Kitayama and Markus (2000) emphasized
that we react quickly and strongly to negative events, that what it means “to be well” differs across cultures.

164
HAPPINESS

For instance, in eastern Asia, happiness results from Memory for Everyday Life
taking a disciplined and critical view of the self, and in
so doing, exchanging sympathy for each other. Pioneering social and experimental psychologists
Kim-Prieto, Fujita, and Diener (2004) have found that have shown that our memories do not completely map
certain emotions, such as pride and worry, cluster dif- onto the original experience. Instead, they are transfor-
ferently across cultures. Although pride clusters with mations of the experience that are reconstructed based
the positive emotions in the United States, it clusters on current motives and beliefs, as well as other factors
with the negative emotions in many cultures. Although (e.g., Bartlett, 1932; Loftus, 1992; Roediger & Gallo,
we found a universal patterning of certain core emo- 2002). The practical implications of this work have
tions across all cultures, we found that other, second- been far reaching in the legal arena, where eyewitness
ary, emotions changed positions across cultures (see testimony is now known to be less reliable than for-
also Eid & Diener, 2001). Tsai, Knutson, and Fung merly believed (e.g., Wells & Olson, 2003).
(2004) found that Americans valued high activation Michael Ross (1989) reviewed a diverse array of
positive states, whereas Chinese valued low activation findings that suggest that people often reconstruct their
positive states. Thus, certain emotions are considered memory to fit their implicit theories or current views.
valuable and appropriate in some cultures, and less so For instance, women often reported having experi-
in other cultures. This reinforces the contention of enced more pain during their menstrual periods, al-
Markus and Kitayama (1991) that what emotions feel though their daily reports proved otherwise. More re-
good depends to some extent on culture. cent work (e.g., Wilson & Ross, 2001) indicates that
Besides the nature and patterning of well-being, we people degrade their personality in the past so as to
also have found that the causes and correlates of happi- view themselves as having improved over time. Robin-
ness vary across cultures. For example, Diener and son and Clore (2002) concluded that retrospective
Diener (1995) found that self-esteem was a much judgments of emotions are influenced by beliefs and
stronger predictor of the life satisfaction of women in self-concepts, and are based on different kinds of evi-
individualistic than in collectivistic societies. On the dence than are “online” judgments. Levine’s (1997)
other hand, financial satisfaction was a stronger predic- study on the memories of Ross Perot supporters in the
tor of life satisfaction in poor than in wealthy nations. 1992 presidential election is a classic in this field.
Similarly, Suh (2002) found that personal consistency When Perot withdrew from the election, respondents
was a stronger predictor of both self-rated well-being reported their emotional reactions to this event. When
and peer-rated likability in the United States than in they later recalled their reactions, their memories were
Korea (see Diener, Oishi, & Lucas, 2003; Diener & tainted by their current views of Perot.
Tov, in press; for review). In the area of well-being, the experience sampling
What we do not yet know is whether the outcomes method (ESM) has improved researchers’ ability to ex-
of happiness differ across cultures. We find that happy amine the accuracy of self-reported experiences of
people are more successful in many areas of life; but al- emotions. For instance, the ESM studies revealed that
most all of the relevant research has been conducted in people overestimate the intensity of emotions, whereas
modern Westernized nations (Lyubomirsky, King & they underestimate the absolute frequency of emotions
Diener, in press). We suspect that the relation of happi- (e.g., Schimmack, 1997; Thomas & Diener, 1990).
ness to success might differ in other societies. Retrospective judgments of emotions are also biased
toward peak and end experiences (Fredrickson &
Kahneman, 1993). European Americans’ retrospective
judgments of well-being move in a more positive direc-
Implications
tion than do Asians’ (e.g., Oishi, 2002). Of importance,
An essential insight of social psychology is that although retrospective judgments of affective experi-
what it means to be happy is not identical around the ences deviate from actual experiences, they seem to
globe. Certain feelings can be valued in one culture predict future choices better than the aggregate of on-
and not in another. Furthermore, although there are line experiences (Wirtz, Kruger, Scollon, & Diener,
undoubtedly universals across cultures in what causes 2003).
happiness, there are also correlates of happiness that
are culture specific. This knowledge should help us
Implications
decenter from our own worldview, and realize that
not all peoples are motivated by identical concerns Memory for emotional experiences is not static, but
and that specific moods and emotions might serve as dynamic (Kim-Prieto, Diener, Tamir, Scollon, &
differential guides to behavior in various societies. Diener, in press). It sometimes goes through a dramatic
Thus, people’s judgments about what feelings are de- transformation (e.g., a first kiss to a boyfriend might be
sirable can vary, as well as can the causes of pleasant recalled as happy rather than fearful now). Online and
feelings. recalled experiences are both “real” and capture impor-

165
DIENER & OISHI

tant aspects of emotion, and therefore there are two aptation level theory: A symposium (pp. 287–302). New
separable aspects of happiness—that which we feel York: Academic.
Brickman, P., Coates, D., & Janoff-Bulman, R. (1978). Lottery win-
online and that which we recall feeling. Memory re- ners and accident victims: Is happiness relative? Journal of Per-
search suggests that people can feel happy in several sonality and Social Psychology, 36, 917–927.
ways: (a) by seeing themselves grow through negative Clark, A., Diener, E., Georgellis, Y., & Lucas, R. E. (2004). Lags and
experiences, (b) by positively reappraising past nega- leads in life satisfaction: A test of the baseline hypothesis.
tive experiences, and/or (c) by downplaying the Manuscript submitted for publication.
Diener, E., & Diener, M. (1995). Cross-cultural correlates of life sat-
positivity of past positive events relative to current isfaction and self-esteem. Journal of Personality and Social
ones. Psychology, 68, 653–663.
Diener, E., Lucas, R., & Scollon, C. N. (2005). Beyond the hedonic
treadmill: Revisions to the adaptation theory of well-being. Un-
published manuscript.
Concluding Remarks Diener, E., & Oishi, S. (2000). Money and happiness: Income and
subjective well-being across nations. In E. Diener, & E. M. Suh
Social psychology has had an enormous impact on (Eds.), Culture and subjective well-being (pp.185–218). Cam-
subjective well-being research over the years. Now bridge, MA: MIT Press.
Diener, E., Oishi, S., & Lucas, R. E. (2003). Culture, personality, and
we know a substantial amount about various aspects
well-being. Annual Review of Psychology, 54, 403–425.
of happiness such as the course of adaptation, errone- Diener, E., & Seligman, M. E. P. (2002). Very happy people. Psycho-
ous predictions about future happiness, memory ac- logical Science, 13, 80–83.
curacy for happiness, different forms of happiness, Diener, E., Tamir, M., Kim-Prieto, C., Scollon, C. N., & Diener, M.
and cultural differences in what correlates with hap- (in press). A time sequential theory of subjective well-being
and ill-being. Journal of Happiness Studies.
piness. One emerging agenda is how subjective
Diener, E., & Tov, W. (in press). Well-being and culture. In
well-being, including moods and emotions, influ- S. Kitayama & D. Cohen (Eds.), Handbook of cultural
ences objective life outcomes (e.g., longevity, health, psychology.
job performance, income, and marriage), prosocial Eid, M., & Diener, E. (2001). Norms for experiencing emotions in
behaviors (e.g., donating to charity and volunteering), different cultures: Inter- and within-nation differences. Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 869–885.
and interpersonal and intergroup behaviors (e.g., inti-
Fredrickson, B. L., & Kahneman, D. (1993). Duration neglect in ret-
macy and cooperation). Social psychology can be rospective evaluations of affective episodes. Journal of Person-
proud of the large and rapid advances made in under- ality and Social Psychology, 65, 45–55.
standing well-being, but there is still much exciting Fredrickson, B. L., & Losada, M. (2004). Positive affect and the
work ahead. complex dynamics of human functioning. Unpublished manu-
script, University of Michigan.
Gilbert, D. T., Pinel, E. C., Wilson, T. D., Blumberg, S. J., &
Wheatley, T. P. (1998). Immune neglect: A source of durability
Note bias in affective forecasting. Journal of Personality & Social
Psychology, 75, 617–638.
Gottman, J. M. (1994). What predicts divorce? The relationship be-
Correspondence should be sent to Ed Diener, De- tween marital processes and marital outcomes. Hillsdale, NJ:
partment of Psychology, University of Illinois, Cham- Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
paign, IL 61820. E-mail: ediener@uiuc.edu Hastorf, A. H., & Cantril, H. (1954). They saw a game: A case study.
Journal of Abnormal & Social Psychology, 49, 129–134.
Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1979). Prospect theory: An analysis
of decision under risk. Econometrica, 47, 263–291.
References Kim-Prieto, C., Diener, E., & Fujita, F. (2004). Culture and patterns
of emotional experience. Unpublished manuscript.
Bartlett, F. C. (1932). Remembering: A study in experimental and so- Kim-Prieto, C., Diener, E., Tamir, M., Scollon, C. N., & Diener, M.
cial psychology. London: Cambridge University Press. (in press). Integrating the diverse definitions of happiness: A
Baumeister, R. F., Bratslavsky, E., Finkenauer, C., & Vohs, K. D. time-sequential framework of subjective well-being. Journal of
(2001). Bad is stronger than good. Review of General Psychol- Happiness Studies.
ogy, 5, 323–370. Kitayama, S., & Markus, H. R. (2000). The pursuit of happiness and
Baumeister, R. F., & Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong: Desire the realization of sympathy: Cultural patterns of self, social rela-
for interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human motiva- tions, and well-being. In E. Diener & E. M. Suh, (Eds.) Culture
tion. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 497–529. and subjective well-being (pp. 113–164). Cambridge, MA: MIT
Biswas-Diener, R., & Diener, E. (2001). Making the best of a bad sit- Press.
uation: Satisfaction in the slums of Calcutta. Social Indicators Levine, L. J. (1997). Reconstructing memory for emotions. Journal
Research, 55, 329–352. of Experimental Psychology, 126, 165–177.
Biswas-Diener, R., Vittersø, J., & Diener, E. (in press). Most people Loftus, E. F. (1992). When a lie becomes memory’s truth: Memory
are pretty happy, but there is cultural variation: The Inughuit, distortion after exposure to misinformation. Current Directions
the Amish, and the Maasai. Journal of Happiness Studies. in Psychological Science, 1, 121–123.
Bradburn, N. M. (1969). The structure of psychological well-being. Lucas, R. E., Clark, A. E., Georgellis, Y., & Diener, E. (2003).
Chicago: Aldine. Re-examining adaptation and the setpoint model of happiness:
Brickman, P., & Campbell, D. T. (1971). Hedonic relativism Reactions to changes in marital status. Journal of Personality
and planning the good society. In M. H. Appley (Ed.), Ad- and Social Psychology, 84, 527–539.

166
HAPPINESS

Lucas, R. E., Clark, A. E., Georgellis, Y., & Diener, E. (2004). Un- Strobe, W., Strobe, M., Abakoumkin, G., & Schut, H. (1996). The
employment alters the set-point for life satisfaction. Psycholog- role of loneliness and social support in adjustment to loss: A test
ical Science, 15, 8–13. of attachment versus stress theory. Journal of Personality and
Lyubomirsky, S., King, L., & Diener, E. (in press). Happiness as a Social Psychology, 70, 1241–1249.
strength: A theory of the benefits of positive affect. Psychologi- Suh, E. M. (2002). Culture, identity, consistency, and subjective
cal Bulletin. well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83,
Lyubomirsky, S., Sheldon, K. M., & Schkade, D. (in press). Pursuing 1378–1391.
happiness: The architecture of sustainable change. Review of Suh, E., Diener, E., & Fujita, F. (1996). Events and subjective
General Psychology. well-being: Only recent events matter. Journal of Personality
Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: Implica- and Social Psychology, 70, 1091–1102.
tions for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychological Re- Taylor, S. E. (1991). Asymmetrical effects of positive and negative
view, 98, 224–253. events: The mobilization minimization hypothesis. Psychologi-
Nisbett, R. E. (2003). The geography of thought: How Asians and cal Bulletin, 110, 67–85.
Westerners think differently … and why. New York: Free Press. Thomas, D. L., & Diener, E. (1990). Memory accuracy in the recall
Oishi, S. (2002). The experiencing and remembering of well-being: of emotions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59,
A cross-cultural analysis. Personality and Social Psychology 291–297.
Bulletin, 28, 1398–1406. Triandis, H. C. (1989). The self and social behavior in differing cul-
Robinson, M. D., & Clore, G. L. (2002). Belief and feeling: Evi- tural contexts. Psychological Review, 96, 506–520.
dence for an accessibility model of emotional self-report. Psy- Tsai, J. L., Knutson, B., & Fung, H. H. (2004). Cultural variation in
chological Bulletin, 128, 934–960. affect valuation. Unpublished manuscript, Stanford University,
Roediger, H. L., & Gallo, D. A. (2002). Processes affecting accuracy California.
and distortion in memory: An overview. In M. L. Eisen (Ed.), Uchino, B. N., Cacioppo, J. T., & Kiecold-Glazer, J. K. (1996).
Memory and suggestibility in the forensic interview. (pp. The relationship between social support and physiological
3–28). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. processes: A review with emphasis on underlying mecha-
Ross, M. (1989). Relation of implicit theories to the construction nisms and implications for health. Psychological Bulletin,
of personal histories. Psychological Review, 96, 341–357. 119, 488–531.
Rozin, P., & Royzman, E. B. (2001). Negativity bias, negativity dom- Wells, G. L., & Olson, E. A. (2003). Eyewitness testimony. Annual
inance, and contagion. Personality and Social Psychology Re- Review of Psychology, 54, 277–295.
view, 5, 296 – 320. Williams, K. D. (2001). Ostracism: The power of silence. New York:
Schimmack, U. (1997). Frequency judgments of emotions: How ac- Guilford.
curate are they, and how are they made? Unpublished disserta- Wilson, A. E., & Ross, M. (2001). From chump to champ: People’s
tion, Free University of Berlin, Germany. appraisals of their earlier and current selves. Journal of Person-
Schwarz, N., & Strack, F. (1999). Reports of subjective well-being: ality and Social Psychology, 80, 572–584.
Judgmental processes and their methodological implications. Wirtz, D., Kruger, J., Scollon, C. N., & Diener, E. (2003). What to do
In D. Kahneman, E. Diener, & N. Schwarz (Eds.), Well-being: on spring break? The role of predicted, online, and remembered
The foundations of hedonic psychology (pp. 61–84). New York: experience in future choice. Psychological Science, 14,
Russell Sage Foundation. 520–524.

167

You might also like