You are on page 1of 14

Anila Asghar, Julie C.

Libarkin

Gravity, Magnetism, and “Down”:


Non-physics College Students’
Conceptions of Gravity
This study concentrates on exploring non-science majors’ conceptual
understanding of gravity and how they use this understanding while solving
problems involving gravity.

I. Introduction in entry-level geology, most of whom college students and in-service


This study investigates college would graduate from college without teachers, there is very little research
students’ ideas of gravity in the context university-level physics courses, on the conceptual understanding of
of an entry-level geology course in thought about and applied the concept gravity on the parts of adults (Gunstone
a North American university. The of gravity while solving problems & White, 1981; Hestenes, Wells,
concept of gravity is central to many concerning gravity. The repercussions & Swackhamer, 1992). In addition,
sciences, and level of understanding of students’ gravity concepts are the importance of physical concepts
of gravity will influence how people then considered in the context in understanding other sciences
apply knowledge from one domain to of non-physics courses, including suggests that investigation of gravity
another. For example, students who implications for reform efforts in ideas in related disciplines will have
believe that gravity only occurs on physics. Data were collected during implications for the teaching of physics
Earth may have difficulty applying the second week of an eleven-week in both high school and college.
geological principles to concepts term from two courses with an average Existing literature on younger
in planetary geology. Entry-level enrollment of just over 100 students. students provides a useful backdrop for
college geology instruction assumes Based on research literature (Claxton, framing our study of college students
an understanding of fundamental 1993; diSessa, 1983; Vosniadou, enrolled in entry-level geology
physical science concepts such as 1994) and one author’s experience of courses. Gravity-related studies focus
atoms, friction, gravity, and density. teaching this population of students, we on students’ ideas of the relationship
These basic concepts provide an hypothesized that students would have between gravity/gravitational force
essential foundation for building fragmented and loosely connected and other important concepts in
students’ sophisticated understanding ideas about gravity. physical science such as height,
of advanced geological concepts. Mass weight, or velocity. Additionally, some
wasting, for example, is a geologic II. Research Context have also examined how children
concept that describes the movement The investigation of children’s relate orientation (“up” and “down”)
of material under the influence of and adolescents’ ideas about various with reference to the Earth in space to
gravity alone. As a consequence, scientific models and the nature of weight and gravity (Driver, et al., 1994;
understanding fundamentals of gravity science is a long-standing and well- Sneider & Pulos, 1983). Studies have
can dramatically impact how students developed area of research (Driver, been conducted in K-college settings
understand and internalize mass 1985; Driver, Squires, Rushworth, & (Nussbaum, 1976; Trumper & Grosky,
wasting concepts taught in a typical Wood-Robinson, 1994). Although a 1997) and in a variety of countries
physical geology course. We set out few well-known studies have extended (Mali & Howe, 1979; Sneider & Pulos,
to investigate how students enrolled this work to include adults, including 1983; Za’Rour, 1975). The majority of

42 SCIENCE EDUCATOR
studies conducted in college settings of an object,” while gravity was Conceptions of the reasons that
incorporate the concept of gravity into perceived as a “property of space,” objects fall also provide insight into
the study of students’ force concepts, independent of the presence of an gravity understanding. Children
rather than explicitly studying gravity object (Ruggiero et al., 1985). In generally tend to think that objects
ideas themselves (Hestenes et al., 1992; a study of pre-service high school fall because nothing is holding them
Sadanand & Kess, 1990). Kavanaugh teachers enrolled in a college biology up, because the person dropping the
and Sneider (2006-2007) provide a course, Trumper and Gorsky (1997) object causes the fall, or because
thorough review of relevant literature found that 25-50% of these freshmen things just fall “naturally” (Gunstone
on gravity conceptions; we elaborate through senior college students rarely & White, 1981; Ruggiero et al., 1985;
on those studies of most importance or never recognized the relationship Selman, Krupa, Stone, & Jacuette,
here. between weight and gravity. Drawing 1982). Adolescents (12 to13 year-
Conceptual understanding of the on Piaget’s work (Piaget, 1972), olds) were found to have three main
source of gravity is an intriguing Galili (2001) points out two main non-scientific explanations for falling:
line of research and is of particular schemes involved in children’s naïve (a) an integrated view of gravity and
relevance to this paper. Gravity needs perceptions of weight: “weight is the weight where gravity is acting on
air to act appears to be a widespread pressing force featuring particular the weight of a falling object, (b) a
notion among children (Driver et al., objects—the sensed heaviness related clear distinction between gravity and
1994; Ruggiero, Cartielli, Dupre, & to a muscular effort”, and “weight is weight, where they both act separately
Vicentini-Missoni, 1985). Similarly, the amount of matter” in an object to cause the fall, and (c) an absence of
gravity is perceived to be caused by (Galili, 2001, p. 1085). Several gravity or weight conceptions where
“air pressure” by high school and studies report a considerable degree objects fall in the absence of support
college students (Hestenes et al., of confusion in children’s concepts due to “natural motion” (Ruggiero
1992). Elementary and secondary of weight and gravitation (Galili & et al., 1985; Selman et al., 1982). A
students in several studies also thought Bar, 1997; Galili & Kaplan, 1996; prevalent notion among people of
that gravity only operated on Earth Gunstone & White, 1980; Ruggiero et all ages is that heavier objects fall
(Bar, Zinn, Goldmuntz, & Sneider, al., 1985). The inability of secondary faster because they have a greater
1994; Stead & Osborne, 1980). and advanced placement students to acceleration due to gravity (Gunstone
Subjects generally supported this resolve their misconceptions about & White, 1981; Hestenes et al., 1992;
notion through the idea that gravity weight, gravitational force, and Osborne, 1984; Selman et al., 1982).
needs a medium, such as Earth’s weighing is a consequence of the For example, Gunstone and White
atmosphere, in which to operate. failure of physics instruction, argues (1981) found that approximately 10%
The concepts of the geomagnetic Galili (2001). of first year physics students hold this
field and the Earth’s rotation also seem belief. In addition, although several
to influence students’ ideas of gravity studies found that teenagers overall
in cross-age models (Stead & Osborne, think that gravity decreases with
1980). A number of investigations height, a considerable proportion of
focusing on children’s and adolescents’ Many of the students who students thought that gravity actually
understanding of gravity and weight held the idea that gravity increases with height above the Earth’s
report the prevalence of the idea that is a force of attraction had surface (Driver, 1985; Driver et al.,
gravity somehow only influences difficulty explaining why 1994; Ruggiero et al., 1985).
heavy things; some 15-year-olds gravity exists on Earth, often Research on children’s ideas about
believe that weight is independent relying on functional (i.e., gravity has also focused on their
of gravity such that objects can have notions of gravity with respect to
gravity’s effects) aspects
weight without gravity (Driver et spatial orientation. Several studies
al., 1994; Osborne, 1984; Stead &
of gravity on Earth, such as have reported that children’s ideas
Osborne, 1980). gravity “keeps things from about “down” with reference to the
Some students and even adults floating away,” and “holds Earth in space progress through
thought of “weight as a property people on earth.” different stages (Baxter, 1989; Driver

SPRING 2010 VOL. 19, NO. 1 43


et al., 1994; Nussbaum, 1985; Sneider In related work, Sneider and Ohadi
& Ohadi, 1998; Sneider & Pulos, (1998) investigated elementary and
1981). Children appear to develop an Through this and other middle school students’ conceptions
understanding of gravitational down future studies we hope to of shape and gravity in the context
as early as two years of age (Hood, engage in a conversation of evaluating the effectiveness of
1995). Children usually start off with about these issues across an instructional intervention. This
an “absolute view of down” that is the disciplinary boundaries study found that, prior to instruction,
independent of Earth, eventually that may exist between only 7-30% of 4th through 8th graders
developing an “Earth-referenced thought that objects fall towards the
view of down” at around the age of
physicists, other scientists, Earth’s center. After instruction, only
14 (Nussbaum, 1985). In a study with and science educators. 60% of 8th grade students acquired
15- and 16-year-olds, Baxter (1989) this concept. This suggests that some
found that about 80% of students had alternative ideas about the orientation
an earth-referenced view of down drawing depicting an object falling and of gravity are entrenched and may be
based on the Earth’s surface rather stopping immediately at the Earth’s prevalent even in older students.
than the center of the Earth. This Earth- center as “correct”, rather than the Although there is ample existing
referenced view usually translates more accurate model of an object literature on children’s and adolescents’
into an “up” position for the Earth. oscillating about the Earth’s center. (K-12 students) ideas about gravity,
Typically, the Northern Hemisphere Using a series of activities prompting there is a dearth of literature on
is “up” relative to the Southern elementary-aged students to depict college students’ and pre-serivce
Hemisphere, but we should caution the path taken by a rock, Nussbaum teachers’ understanding of gravity.
that this view may be dependant upon (1976) found that young students often This study specifically concentrates
the Northern Hemisphere populations have perspectives of “down” that are on exploring non-science majors’
studied. For example, in two studies in reference frame specific. Students (typical undergraduate students
England, Baxter (1989) and Arnold et drew rocks (a) falling towards the including pre-service teachers and
al. (1995) found that the majority of 7 bottom of the page, (b) falling towards child development majors) conceptual
to 16 year-olds viewed the Northern the Earth’s surface but not the center, understanding of gravity and how
Hemisphere of the Earth as “up.” As (c) falling away from the Earth, and they use this understanding while
a consequence, students believed rain (d) falling towards the Earth’s center. solving problems involving gravity.
fell away from the Earth, towards Nussbaum (1979) extended this More specifically, we examine the
space, in the Southern Hemisphere. work by providing students with four ways in which student ideas resonate
A few researchers have investigated drawings and asking them to choose the or conflict with the scientifically
pre-teenager (7 to 12 years-old) student one that they thought best represented accepted idea of gravity. Additionally,
conceptions of objects falling through what would happen to an object falling we seek to closely examine alternative
a tunnel in the Earth, as reproduced in through the tunnel. These results were frameworks about the nature of gravity
this study (Nussbaum 1976, 1979). similar to those of earlier work, and through a qualitative analysis of
These investigations are embedded in revealed that the gravity orientation of student written and pictorial responses.
larger studies of student understanding most young students is not in reference In particular, we wanted to answer two
of Earth’s shape and gravity, with a to the Earth’s center. The studies tend questions: (1) How do non-science
primary purpose of understanding to explain students’ responses to these major college students enrolled in
the general gravity orientation that problems, but do not attempt to probe a science course other than physics
students hold. Three conceptual the ways in which students understand understand gravity? (2) In what ways
orientations are considered in existing the problems. Students’ mental images do they apply their understanding of
studies: (a) objects falling towards the or perceptions of the tunnel are not gravity while solving problems related
center of the Earth, (b) objects falling clear from existing work; for example, to gravity? Additionally, we looked
towards the surface of the Earth, and do students think that the tunnel has at the relevant literature to compare
(c) objects falling down in space. air or water in it, or do they consider the participants’ understanding of
Generally, these studies consider a variables such as the Coriolis effect? gravity to models of gravity held by

44 SCIENCE EDUCATOR
K-12 students. For the purpose of this up 28%, 13%, and 9% of the study occurred in this course only after these
study, we defined gravity very simply population, respectively. At the start data were collected.
as the force of attraction between two of the term, 28% of the students were
B. Data Collection
or more objects that have mass. education or early childhood majors,
including one Earth Science Education The questionnaire utilized in this
III. Methods major, 20% were undecided, and six study included an open-ended question
The narrowly constrained research students had declared Geosciences and two gravity tasks. The open-ended
focus of this study allowed for as their major. Remaining students question asked students to explain
implementation of methodologies were enrolled in diverse majors, what gravity is and the cause of gravity
used previously by other researchers. including finance, sociology, English, on Earth (What is gravity? Why is there
In particular, we adopted gravity plant biology, forensic chemistry, gravity on Earth?). The gravity tasks
problems that Nussbaum (1976, mechanical engineering, and art. Of had previously been investigated with
1979), had originally used with these majors, 17% were declared as elementary-aged children (Nussbaum,
elementary students for use with our math, science (including geo-science), 1976, 1979). The gravity tasks asked
non-science major college population. computer science, or engineering students to pretend that a tunnel was
We specifically targeted students majors (Table 1). dug all of the way through the Earth

Table I: Course and participant demographics.

Female Male 1st-year Education or Child Undecided Geology


students Development majors majors
Winter 2004 60 46 57 36 20 0
Fall 2005 44 66 50 25 22 6
TOTAL 104 112 107 61 42 6
Participants 104 104 — — — —
% participation 100% 92.8% — — — —

enrolled in geology courses in which The response rate to the question- from the North to the South Pole,
an understanding of physics concepts naire from both courses was 96% imagine that a person standing at the
is generally assumed. (n=208). The institution where this surface dropped a rock, and draw a line
study was conducted is situated in from the person’s hand showing the
A. Participants the rural mid-west, although the ma- path taken by the rock. Students were
The study population (n=216) jority of students are from suburban also asked to explain their response. In
consisted of students enrolled in homes near small cities. The col- a second task, the frame-of-reference
two entry-level university geology lection of gravity conceptions data of the first task was modified to show
courses taught by one of the authors from students was conducted with a a tunnel oriented along the equatorial
during winter 2004 (n=96) and fall dual purpose: as part of instructional plane(Arnold, Sarge & Worrall, 1995)
2005 (n=104) at a mid-western pre-assessment and as conceptions in order to allow for investigation of
North American university. The two research. All students enrolled in the the reference dependence of gravity
courses participating in this study courses participated in the pre-assess- concepts. This allowed differentiation
were almost evenly divided between ment. Attendance rates for F2005 and between those students who believed
men and women, with a total of W2004 classes were 100% and 93%, objects fall towards the center of the
117 men and 99 women enrolled in respectively. 68% of students enrolled Earth and those who believe objects
the courses overall (48% female). in these courses reported previous fall towards the bottom of the page.
Students had mixed levels of college high school Earth Science or Physics The questionnaires were distributed
experience. 49.5% of the students courses, suggesting a significant level and collected before instruction and
were in their first year of college. 2nd, of exposure to gravity concepts in high had no impact on students’ grades.
3rd, and 4th/5th year students made school, although instruction on gravity In order to provide for anonymity,

SPRING 2010 VOL. 19, NO. 1 45


questionnaires were numbered before phenomena such as the Coriolis effect. data were divided into two sets for the
analysis. 96% of the 216 students We feel that future studies would purpose of analysis: (a) responses to the
enrolled in the courses completed benefit from probing during interviews open-ended gravity question and (b)
some portion of the questionnaire. to elicit a more accurate and richer drawings related to the gravity tasks.
Specifically, 208 students completed picture of students’ ideas. Students’ qualitative responses to the
the drawing tasks, and 197 completed open-ended question and explanations
the open-ended gravity question. C. Data Analysis of their gravity task drawings were
These data provide a unique look Each of the researchers analyzed tabulated electronically. The responses
into the gravity conceptions of typical one dataset completely, and conducted were coded thematically to capture
college students, as well as a large repeat analysis on the other dataset important ideas and misconceptions
subset of pre-service teachers and child to establish inter-rater reliability as expressed by the participants in relation
development majors. discussed below. We analyzed the to gravity. Codes were grouped into
This study did not probe students’ student responses and the drawings. broader categories and general themes
mental picture of the north-south and Questionnaire responses were analyzed as shown in Table II. Responses to
east-west tunnel, although, based on via thematic content analysis (Patton, each of the two gravity tasks were
the written responses, we have no 1990), wherein themes are allowed to also analyzed and coded thematically,
indication that students are considering emerge naturally from the data. The with dominant categories of responses
emerging from the data.
Table II: Common alternative conceptions of gravity identified in the study population.
Alternative conceptions Exemplar student responses (Gravity is…)
Gravity as an outside force • force acting on the objects of the universe in varying degrees
Gravity associated with the • nature’s force pulling down because of the atmosphere
atmosphere
Gravity associated with • a force created by objects in space, there is gravity on earth
celestial objects and/or because it is spinning in space
earth’s spin • There is gravity on earth because of rotation of the earth
• the pull or push of mass towards the earth. Because of Earth’s
mass and constant rotation and the pull of the moon
Gravity associated with the • a force that pulls everything down. There’s gravity on Earth
sun because of its relationship to the sun
Gravity as the pull or • the pull the earth’s core has on everything
attraction of Earth’s core • Force of attraction from the center of the planet that holds
things together. If there was no gravity, things would not be held
together and everything would just float into space and not stay
on Earth
Gravity associated with • the pressure that is forced onto an object at 9.8m/s^2
pressure
Gravity associated with size • a force that pulls on all objects. Depending on the size of the
and earth’s position planet, gravity’s pull is stronger. Caused by [earth’s] position in
solar system and closeness to sun
Gravity associated with • a force that holds everything on the earth, gravity is here because
magnetism the earth spins and revolves and maybe because its magnetic
• a magnetic force caused by the star or the sun.
• a force within the earth caused by metallic substances rotating in
the molten core, this also affects the magnetic field…
• There is gravity on Earth because of the magnetic field of the
solar system. Gravity is related to mass
Functional definition of • the force that holds everything in place on Earth to keep everyone
gravity on Earth in place and everything
• a force that pulls on objects. Gravity is here to keep everything
in its place on earth otherwise we would just float away with no
control

46 SCIENCE EDUCATOR
1. Validity and reliability on discussion of thematic codes. students hold only these distinct ideas.
The gravity tasks included in this The context of this study and the In fact, the data suggest that a multitude
questionnaire were adopted from assessment tool may have influenced of alternative ideas co-existed in
previous studies23 that have already student thinking; however, we noticed participants’ minds, albeit mostly
undergone validation for elementary- similar responses on both the written reflecting incoherent frameworks.
aged students. We view this prior explanation and the drawing task.
1. Gravity as a force of attraction
use and validation as a significant
IV. Findings And Discussion Students’ responses suggested
measure of the usability of these tasks
In this section, we discuss the salient that, surprisingly, only 21% of the
for eliciting gravity concepts. The
conceptions emerging from students’ participants had the correct scientific
application of materials developed
responses to the gravity problems. idea that gravity is a force of attraction.
for K-12 to the college population
More specifically, student notions of The specific idea that the force of
required further validation. Both the
gravity and causes are analyzed, as attraction exists between masses
drawing tasks and the open-ended
well as responses and explanations was ultimately not coded as very
gravity questions created for this study
for the gravity tasks. few students incorporated all three
were first used in a pilot study. This
concepts of attraction, mass, and force
piloting occurred in a similar entry-
A. Notions of Gravity in their explanations. Many of the
level geology course taught before
The open-ended questions were used students who held the idea that gravity
the courses included in this study.
to elicit students’ ideas about gravity. is a force of attraction had difficulty
Responses to the pilot questionnaire
Student responses (n=197) revealed a explaining why gravity exists on
indicated that students were able to
diverse and complex array of notions Earth, often relying on functional
understand and respond to the tasks
about both gravity and the relationship (i.e., gravity’s effects) aspects of
and questions. In addition, college
between gravity and the Earth gravity on Earth, such as gravity
students did not feel that these tasks
(Table II; Figure 1). It is particularly “keeps things from floating away,” and
were too simplistic for a college level
important to note that the majority of “holds people on earth.” Furthermore,
course.
the students’ responses exhibited a merely 12% of students held the
Additional aspects of qualitative
combination of misconceptions about correct conception that gravity is a
validity were addressed during this
gravity. We did not notice any truly force without simultaneously holding
study. Students were also consulted
coherent framework emerging from common alternative conceptions. The
through in-class discussions to
the responses, except for perhaps the 9% of the study population with dual
obtain their feedback on the analytic
scientifically accepted model held by scientifically accepted/non-scientific
categories to address the credibility
a very small proportion of students. models held misconceptions that were
of the interpretations made by the
Although we have identified some mostly in relation to why gravity
researchers (Patton, 1990). Students
salient themes arising out of students’ exists on Earth. A number of students
in both courses agreed that their
responses for the purpose of this thought that Earth’s “rotation,” “spin,”
personal models fell within one of the
analysis, we are not suggesting that “magnetism,” and “atmosphere” cause
described models. This process helped
a force of attraction between the Earth
in ascertaining the level to which the
and other objects. In fact, one student
study participants agreed with the
mentioned all of these concepts in
research findings. For the purpose of
her definition of gravity, explaining
creating inter-rater reliability, each Students tend to reason that gravity is a force of attraction
researcher also coded 20 responses from familiar concepts, and and that “… Gravity is a force due
from the alternate dataset. The scores when these concepts are to spinning of the earth around the
and codes were compared and will
non-scientific, conclusions sun, magnetism, and atmosphere”.
be discussed later in this article. The
can be far removed from the The following selected excerpts
inter-rater agreement was 100% for
concepts we think we are illuminate students’ reasoning for
drawings, and was 80% to 100%
various misconceptions:
for open-ended responses, based teaching.

SPRING 2010 VOL. 19, NO. 1 47


“Gravity exists on the earth, because the Earth specifically. As observed in movement of objects. A large number
of Earth’s mass and constant rotation other categories, responses reflected of those who thought of gravity as an
and the pull of the moon.” multiple alternative conceptions about outside force acting on objects also
gravity. Not only did students think of invoked several other concepts that
“Gravity is on earth to keep things
gravity as a force acting on the Earth, may or may not be connected to one
on earth.”
they also suggested that gravity exists another. The following responses
“There is gravity on earth because on Earth because of “magnetism,” reflect the diverse ideas these students
of the earth’s core.” “earth’s rotation,” “earth’s spin,” carried in relation to gravity:
“atmospheric pressure”, and Earth’s
2. Gravity as an outside force “Gravity is a force, acting on earth
“relative position to the sun”. About
keeping everything on the ground.
Approximately 46% of the 29% of students in this category also
There is gravity because of the
participants seemed to view gravity mentioned the function of gravity on
position in the solar system as well
as an outside force acting on an object, Earth (i.e., gravity prevents objects
as the rotation of Earth.”
which might (a) exist independently from “floating away”), either citing
of the object and (b) originate from it as the sole reason for the existence “Gravity pulls on everything
something other than the Earth plus of gravity on earth or coupling it with keeping it in place. There is gravity
the attracted object. For example, other phenomena, such as magnetism on Earth because of the way the
93% of those who perceived gravity and Earth’s rotation. A few students Earth rotates.”
as an outside force associated it with also associated gravity with the

Figure 1: College student conceptions of gravity and its causes. Alternative conceptions as illustrated in
Table II and additional ideas identified in the study population. Notice that over 90% of the students in this
study had an earth-centric perspective of gravity, and believed that gravity was a force acting on the Earth,
rather than something that was an inherent characteristic of Earth (and all objects with mass). A wide array of
other alternative conceptions were observed; see text for details.

100

90
Number of students (n=197)

80

70

60

50

40

30

20

10

0
e
e

e
s

in

on
n

ll

m
er

ur

rg
as

rc

tio
io

pu
re

sp
tis

iti

lu
ph

ne
es
fo
ct

ta
ne

fin
’s

vo
s
nt
a

pr

e
os
e

ro

h’
s

re
tr

ag
ce
h’

e
id

rt

or
at

ld
co

s
rt

ts

Ea
m

h’
h

at

ze
Ea

na
of

ou

rt

rt
’s

s
Ea

si
Ea
th

tio
e

ith

h’
an
rc

ith
rt

nc
Ea
w
fo

Ea

w
fu
ed

ed
at

at
ci

ci
so

so
as

as

48 SCIENCE EDUCATOR
“Gravity is the force put upon earth “energy that pulls.” She was applying object towards the center of the earth
due to earth’s rotation around its the concepts of force and energy to by the earth … the mass of earth in the
axis and the sun.” gravity in an incoherent way, which solar system creates the pull”.
suggests a faulty understanding of Again, several participants
“Gravity is the force pushing things
these concepts as well. Participants’ mentioned magnetism, Earth’s rotation,
down on the earth. Attraction of
responses suggest that they may be Earth’s spin, or a functional view of
one object to another. Energy that
using several technical terms, such as gravity to explain why gravity exists on
pulls. Large objects have stronger
“energy”, “magnetic force”, or “spin”, Earth. A few mentioned the Sun’s and
[gravity]. With out [sic] [gravity]
to explain their ideas of gravity without “moon’s pull” as reasons for Earth’s
things would float.”
a clear understanding of what these gravity. Surprisingly, some students
“It’s a magnetic like force that pulls terms mean. thought of gravity as “energy” “pulling
everything towards the
earth. If we didn’t have Figure 2: Exemplars of categories of student drawings for a N-S oriented tunnel. Category 1
it every thing [sic] would is the correct conception, depicting the rock oscillating about the Earth’s center. Category 2 is
float off the earth that the most commonly observed conception, where the rock follows a direct path and stops at the
wasn’t held down.” Earth’s center. Category 3 represents the concept that the rock will travel to the opposite side of
the Earth. Three variations of category 3 were observed: the rock falls out the opposite side of the
Although these five Earth, shown here, the rock will stop at the opposite side of the Earth, and the rock will fall out
participants’ responses the opposite side of the Earth, curve and hit the Earth’s surface. Category 4 and Category 5 were
related but distinct concepts. Category 4 is the idea that the rock will curve and come to rest at
seemed to imply that gravity the side of the tunnel very early in its journey down the tunnel. Category 5 blends Categories 2
is an external force “acting and 4, yielding the notion that the rock will fall to the center of the Earth and curve into the side of
on earth” that “pulls” or the tunnel.
”pushes” all objects on the
Earth towards the “center of
the earth” in order to “keep”
objects “in place”, they
tended to give divergent
explanations for why
gravity exists on Earth. Two
participants said that gravity
is caused by the “rotation” 1 2 3 4 5
of the Earth, and one of
them also cited the relative “position”
3. Earth-based notion of gravity on” objects. According to one student,
of the Earth in the solar system. One
About 50% of the students in this gravity is “energy that pulls you.”
student said that Earth’s spin and the
study seemed to think of gravity only Interestingly, some used the concept
“sun” caused gravity on the Earth. Two
in relation to the Earth, and most held of Earth’s “size” instead of mass
students mentioned only the function
other alternative ideas along with this while explaining their understanding
of gravity (i.e., without gravity
idea. As examples, students described of Earth’s gravitation.
things would “float away”) in their
gravity as “a force that the earth is Student application of their
explanations to account for gravity’s
exerting on us”, “a pull the earth’s core understanding of gravity to the two
existence on the Earth. One of these
has on everything”, “a force that earth’s simple gravity tasks also suggests
students also mentioned gravity as the
mass creates because of its size”, or a a limited understanding of gravity.
force of “attraction” between objects
“magnetic pull caused by the earth’s Overall, 73% of students drew the
while simultaneously viewing it as
core”. A number of participants also same pathway for both the N-S and
an external force “pushing” objects
correctly connected gravity on Earth to E-W tunnels, and the likelihood of
down on the Earth. Additionally, not
Earth’s mass in their responses. As one holding the same model regardless
only did this student define gravity
student put it, gravity is a “pull on an of tunnel orientation depended upon
as a “force,” she also described it as

SPRING 2010 VOL. 19, NO. 1 49


the student’s overall understanding coming to rest at the side of the tunnel. off the page). Finally, twelve students
of gravity. Conceptions were evenly Category 4 paths ended near the tunnel believed that the rock would fall off
distributed across the two studied entrance, while Category 5 paths ended of the page rather than move into the
course populations. at the Earth’s center. Nearly twice as E-W tunnel. Of these, two also held
Five categories of the conception that
rock paths dominated Figure 3: Exemplars of categories of student drawings for the E-W oriented the rock would move
the student responses tunnel. Categories as in Figure 2. away from or around
for both N-S and E-W the Earth for the N-S
tunnel orientations, tunnel orientation
and two additional (Category 6).
categories of response Student explana-
are worth discussion tions of their drawings
(Figure 2; Figure 3; 1 2 3 did not always indicate
Table III). Category deep understanding or
1 drawings depicted a firmly held beliefs, but
rock oscillating about they provided interest-
the Earth’s center, ing insight into student
which was the correct thought processes.
idea, and all students Although Category 1
(10%, n=21) who held 4 5 represented a correct
this idea for the N-S concept, students were
tunnel also held it for generally unable to
the E-W tunnel. One additional student many students believed the rock would provide a detailed explanation of this
held a Category 1 idea for the E-W curve towards the tunnel surface for phenomenon or demonstrate alterna-
tunnel and a Category 2 idea for the N- the E-W oriented tunnel (n=31) than tive ideas. For example, our Category 1
S tunnel. Category 2 drawings showed the N-S tunnel (n=18). student in Figure 2 explained his draw-
a rock path that stopped abruptly at the Category 6 and 7 ideas (Figure ing by stating “The center pulls it there
center, and these were the drawings 4) were held by fewer students than by 9.8 m/s and it rubberbands back +
most commonly observed. Overall, other models and were observed forth till [sic] it gets to the center”. This
49% (n=98) of students held this idea predominantly for specific tunnel student depicted an identical path for
for both tunnel orientations, six held it orientations. Four students indicated the E-W tunnel, stating simply, “Earth
for the E-W tunnel only, and thirteen that the rock in the N-S tunnel problem is round”. Another student explained
students expressed this idea for the would fall away from the Earth, “slide” this oscillating phenomenon in the
N-S tunnel only. These students were around the Earth, or orbit the Earth. An N-S tunnel with an alternative concep-
mixed between Categories 3, 4, and 5 additional student indicated that, while tion about the cause of the attraction
in their choice for the other orientation he believed most strongly that the rock between the rock and the Earth, stat-
or believed the rock in the E-W tunnel would fall to the opposite side of the ing “Gravity and earth’s rotation will
would simply fall off the page (Figure tunnel and stop (Category 3), the rock balance out”.
4). Category 3 drawings depicted a moving around the Earth instead of The dominant conception that the
rock that fell through the Earth. Once into the E-W tunnel was a possibility. rock will follow a straight path and
on the other side, the rock fell out of Another student preferred the model of come to rest at the Earth’s center
the tunnel, fell out and curved back the rock stopping at the center of the was often explained by a tug-of-war
down to the Earth’s surface, or simply N-S tunnel (Category 2), but suggested between the two ends of the tunnel
stopped moving. While 45 students that the rock might also “fall straight (“The rock will not fall the entire way
held this model for the N-S tunnel, down due to gravity”. This explanation through the center of the earth b/c the
only 20 drew similar paths for both was accompanied by a crossed-out gravity will pull the rock in the center
tunnels. Category 4 and 5 were related, path that showed the rock moving of the 2 ends”) or by the perceived
and both showed the rock curving and perpendicular to the N-S tunnel (that is, attractive nature of the core (“core pulls

50 SCIENCE EDUCATOR
things towards it”). The final motion at the center of the N-S tunnel and a student believed that the rock would
of the rock at the end of the tunnel, rock that curved away from the Earth fall into the tunnel and come right
whether continuing to move into or began to orbit the Earth (Figure 4; back to the surface for both tunnel
space, falling to the Earth’s surface, “Because of the earths rotation the rock orientations. This student did not have
or simply stopping, often changed as would slide away from the tunnel”). an explanation for this phenomenon
the tunnel changed orientation. A few Finally, students with Categories 4 and and simply stated that he “saw it in
students used their explanation for the 5 models believed that gravity pulls a magazine”. Four students believed
rock’s path in the N-S tunnel to justify objects down (“The rock would go into the rock would not move at all in the
a similar depiction for the E-W tunnel the Earth but then move downwards E-W tunnel, with one of these students
(“He is still standing next to a hole because of the gravity pulling it down”) holding the identical belief for the
and when he drops it it will still fall or towards a surface (“Gravity would N-S tunnel. The student with the “no
straight down through the tunnel”). eventually draw it to some sort of motion” conception for both tunnels
One student who believed the rock surface”). One student emphatically firmly stated that “the rock won’t
would fall straight through the N-S described her Category 4 conception move” without further explanation.
tunnel and out the other side (“The as shown in Figure 2, “The rock will The remaining three students with no
rock will fall until gravity is unable fall quickly to the Earth. It will not motion in the E-W tunnel all held the
to act upon it without another for [sic] go through the tunnel”. Interestingly, conception that the rock would follow
acting up on it) also believed the rock for most students the surface in the a direct path in the N-S tunnel and stop
would fall off of the page rather than tunnel to which the rock falls could at the Earth’s center. These students
into the E-W tunnel (“The rock will be orientated vertically or horizontally explained their ‘no motion’ model in
fall and have gravity enforced on it relative to the page. It appears that various ways. One student thought
until there is no more gravity”). Eight these students had an earth-referenced the rock would “fall to ground” but
of the twelve students with the “off conception of up/down relative to the not into the tunnel. Another student
of the page” conception believed the surface of the Earth. similarly explained that the rock would
rock would fall all of the way through A few students provided explana- “drop and stay in that area”, and a third
the N-S tunnel to the other side. The tions of the rock’s path that fell outside student indicated that the rock “would
remaining four students were evenly of the categories described here. One just stop”.
divided between a rock that stopped

Table III: Seven categories of student drawings.


Drawing Category N-S Tunnel E-W Tunnel
# of students # of students
(% of n=201) (% of n=201)
Category 1 drawings depicted a rock oscillating about the 21 (10%) 22 (11%)
Earth’s center, and this represents the correct idea.
Category 2 drawings depicted a rock that stopped abruptly at 111 (55%) 104 (52%)
the center.
Category 3 drawings depicted a rock that fell through the 45 (22%) 20 (10%)
tunnel to the opposite side of the Earth.
Category 4 drawings depicted a rock that fell into the tunnel 5 (2%) 15 (7%)
(not to the center) and curved to the tunnel’s surface.
Category 5 drawings depicted a rock that fell to the Earth’s 13 (6%) 16 (8%)
center and curved to the tunnel’s surface.
Category 6 drawings depicted a rock falling away from, or 3 (1%) 0
curving around, the Earth.
Category 7 drawings depicted a rock falling off the page. 0 12 (6%)
Total number of responses in these categories 196 189

SPRING 2010 VOL. 19, NO. 1 51


C. Undergraduate versus K-12 Overall, the solutions and young children documented almost
Students’ Conceptions of Gravity. explanations that college students three decades ago. For example,
This study was an attempt to provided for the gravity tasks were only 10% of our college population
explicitly link gravity conceptions generally more advanced than held the simplest scientifically
and problem-solving approaches of observations made of elementary accepted model of gravity without also
K-12 students with those of non- students in the 1970s (Nussbaum, simultaneously holding alternative
physics college students. Surprisingly, 1979). However, over 5% of the ideas, suggesting little change in
college students’ conceptions of gravity understanding from K-12
gravity seem remarkably similar Figure 4: Other common rock paths produced to college. The alternative ideas
to conceptions identified in by the study population were dependent upon observed here were quite similar to
tunnel orientation. Category 6: Four students
younger populations, particularly depicted the rock falling away from the Earth or those of elementary students, and
elementary-aged students. In curving around the Earth. This was only observed a high percentage of both groups
particular and as described earlier, as a primary model for the N-S oriented tunnel, of students equated gravity with
the research literature abounds with although one student suggested this as an magnetic or rotational forces (Table
non-scientific models of gravity Twelve students indicated that the rock would fall I; Figure 1). Ideas of gravitation that
alternative model for the E-W tunnel. Category 7:

held by children. For instance, off of the page for the E-W oriented tunnel only. have long been considered to be
children tend to think that gravity exclusive to young students, such as
either needs air to act or is caused the “off-the-page” model classified
by air pressure. Another prevalent here as Category 7 (Figure Z) and
misconception is that gravity documented by Nussbaum (1979),
operates on Earth only. Many K-12 were also observed in our adult
students tend to associate gravity population. These findings suggest
with magnetism or Earth’s rotation. that gravity models are either
An “Earth-referenced view of 6 7 resistant to change, are prone to
down” based on the Earth’s surface reversion from scientific models
rather than the center of the Earth is to original alternative conceptions,
also prevalent among children. While studied college population held the or are not being explicitly targeted by
solving the gravity task in a similar idea that gravity worked down relative instruction.
study, elementary and middle schools to a piece of paper, and 21% believed Student responses to the gravity
students also drew rocks (a) falling a ball would simply fall out of a N-S problems also revealed a host
towards the bottom of the page, (b) oriented tunnel and into space. Almost of alternative ideas about other
falling towards the Earth’s surface but 50% of the college students mentioned fundamental physical and geological
not the center, (c) falling away from in their responses that a dropped concepts. The connection of gravity
the Earth, and (d) falling towards the object will immediately stop at the with magnetism, Earth’s rotation,
Earth’s center (Nussbaum & Novak, Earth’s center; this was classified as a atmospheric pressure, and the core
1976; Nussbaum, 1979). Alarmingly, “correct” model by earlier researchers of the Earth suggests a limited
college student misconceptions are not investigating young children. Finally, understanding of these phenomena.
dramatically different from those of K- very few students were able to provide Other studies also suggest that
12 students, and a significant number coherent explanations for object many children and adults relate air,
of students in our study population held behavior, suggesting that college atmosphere, magnetism and Earth’s
ideas similar to those of elementary students hold incoherent ideas about spin to gravity (Ruggiero et al., 1985;
students. The notions that gravity gravity and related phenomena. Stead & Osborne, 1980). Additionally,
exists because of magnetism, air Despite more than a decade of half of the students in this study
pressure, the special nature of the diverse national efforts to modify and believed that a falling rock would reach
core, or the Earth’s rotation are all improve physics instruction in the the center of the Earth and stop, and
well-documented in pre-college K-college classroom, we observed many students explained this by saying
populations and were also observed little difference between current that there would be “no force” at the
in abundance here. college student models and those of center. While the students are correct

52 SCIENCE EDUCATOR
that there would be no net force at the magnetism, and Earth’s rotation confusions related to gravitation,
center of the earth, they are incorrect (Figure 1). Additionally, non-physics weight and other related concepts as
in their depiction of the rock stopping college student conceptions of gravity a core element of effective instruction.
at the center. This indicates that these are remarkably similar to ideas held For geoscientists, misunderstanding
college students may be confused in by elementary-aged students reported the fundamentals of gravity can have
their usage of the concepts of force, nearly three decades ago. This suggests serious implications for teaching any
velocity, and acceleration. It appears that physics instruction in pre-college number of larger ideas, including
that students need to develop a clear grades may need to explicitly address mass wasting, plate tectonics, and
understanding of force and motion gravitation ideas held by students, planetary geology. Furthermore,
to understand gravity as a force of rather than more common approaches many geology majors are required
attraction that is mediated by a field. that focus on the effects of gravity. to take geophysics classes, since
These data bring to mind a number The impact of alternative ideas on geophysics is a fundamental tool
of interesting questions about the reasoning in other domains, such as used in oil exploration, mining, and
relationships between physics reform Earth and Space science, cannot be other geologic industries. Some of the
efforts, pre-service preparation, understated. For example, the Earth- students in our study were enrolled
in-service continuing professional centric models of gravity held by many as geology majors, and at the very
development, and instruction in K-12 of the students in our study resulted in least, these majors should understand
classrooms. Through this and other some who believed that gravity only gravity in order to be able to reason
future studies we hope to engage in existed on Earth. Other students (in about gravity anomalies and planetary
a conversation about these issues related work with this same population systems. This study suggests that
across the disciplinary boundaries of students), felt that gravity did for faculty in interdisciplinary fields
that may exist between physicists, not exist on Mars, because Mars such as geology, it may be a good
other scientists, and science educators. does not have a magnetic field. The idea to probe about fundamental
What would it take to help students and likelihood that students will confuse ideas before engaging in a discussion
teachers to develop coherent models these foundational ideas with other of concepts that require an a priori
of foundational ideas in physics? conceptions suggests that curriculum understanding.
How do we supplement or follow up developers may need to identify One of the main limitations of
on “good teaching” practices so that and address students’ prevalent this study is that it was based on a
students retain content and develop a misconceptions in physical science relatively simple tool to elicit college
coherent and lasting understanding of by including multiple experiences students’ ideas about gravitation. We
scientifically accepted models? that help them to transform and employed this tool to replicate studies
develop their initial ideas into coherent originally conducted with children
V. Conclusions explanations of these basic scientific with the college level population.
and Implications ideas. We propose that future research and
We found that entry-level college Students tend to reason from familiar effort in teaching gravity concepts
students enrolled in geology courses concepts, and when these concepts are may need to focus on 1) Interview-
were unable to provide any coherent non-scientific, conclusions can be far based probing of high school and
scientific explanation of gravitationally removed from the concepts we think college students’ understanding of
related phenomena. Neither weak we are teaching. It then becomes the concepts of gravitational force,
explanations, such as the simple paramount that we explicitly target weight, the inertial mass, and the
regurgitation of the gravity definition, the most basic ideas in our instruction, gravitational mass (Gönen, 2008) to
nor strong explanations, including those rather than assuming, as certainly attain a deeper knowledge of their
that connect meaningfully to other many college faculty do, that students mental images and conceptions of
scientific ideas, were present in our have “already had that material” in an these ideas; 2) Conducting pre-post
population. Students’ own ideas about earlier class. The findings presented and experimental studies to examine
gravity reflect a lack of understanding here suggest that college faculty in all the effect of instruction on students’
about many fundamental ideas in sciences, not just physics, may want to prior knowledge of gravitation; and 3)
science, including pressure, gravity, identify, discuss, and clarify students’ Investigating how teachers translate

SPRING 2010 VOL. 19, NO. 1 53


concepts learned via reform efforts Claxton, G. (1993). Minitheories: a Le, V., Stecher, B. M., Lockwood,
to K-12 classrooms. K-12 teachers preliminary model for learning science. J. R., Hamilton, L. S., Robyn, A.,
are exposed to reform-based methods, In P. J. Black & A. M. Lucas (Eds.), Williams, V. L., et al. (2006). Improving
but are not always able to translate Children’s Informal Ideas in Science mathematics and science education:
(pp. 45-61). London: Routledge. A longitudinal investigation of the
these experiences to classroom
diSessa, A. A. (1983). Toward an relationship between reform-oriented
instruction. Certainly, we know that epistemology of physics. Cognition instruction and student achievement.
reform-based courses are effective and Instruction, 10(2 & 3), 105-225. Arlington: RAND Corporation.
at engendering conceptual change Driver, R. (1985). Children’s Ideas in Document Number)
(Shaffer & McDermott, 2005), but Science. Milton Keynes, UK: Open Mali, G. B., & Howe, A. (1979).
it is less certain if these approaches University Press. Development of earth and gravity
are being applied effectively (Le et Driver, R., Squires, A., Rushworth, concepts among Nepali children.
al., 2006). We also wonder if reform- P., & Wood-Robinson, V. (1994). Science Education, 63(5), 685-691.
based instruction targets the most Making sense of secondary science: Meyer, J. H. F., & Land, R. (2005).
common misconceptions about the Research into children’s ideas. London: Threshold concepts and troublesome
Rutledge. knowledge (2): epistemological
natural world (Vosniadou & Brewer,
Galili, I. (2001). Weight versus gravitational considerations and a conceptual
1992). Finally, it might be useful to force: Historical and educational framework for teaching and learning.
consider these fundamental concepts perspectives. International Journal Higher Education, 49, 373-388.
from the theoretical perspective of of Science Education, 23(10), 1073- Nussbaum, J. (1979). Children’s
threshold concepts (Meyer & Land, 1093. conceptions of the earth as a cosmic
2005). Threshold concepts are ideas Galili, I., & Bar, V. (1997). Children’s body: a cross age study. Science
that can act as barriers to a curriculum, operational knowledge about weight. Education, 63(1), 83-93.
wherein understanding a threshold International Journal of Science Nussbaum, J. (1985). The Earth as a
concept may provide a gateway to deep Education, 19(3), 317-340. cosmic body. In R. Driver, E. Guesne
understanding of related concepts. Galili, I., & Kaplan, D. (1996). Students’ & A. Tiberghien (Eds.), Children’s
operation with the concept of weight. ideas in science. Milton Keynes: Open
We as faculty may need to seriously
Science Education, 80(4), 457-487. University Press.
reconsider the assumptions we make Gönen, S. (2008). A study on student Nussbaum, J. (1976). An assessment
about students, in terms of both what teachers’ misconceptions and of children’s concepts of the earth
students are bringing to and taking scientifically acceptable conceptions utilizing structured interviews. Science
from the classroom. Ultimately, we about mass and gravity. Journal of Education, 60(4), 535-550.
as a community need to decide which Science Education and Technology, Osborne, R. (1984). Children’s dynamics.
concepts are absolutely necessary for 17, 70-81. The Physics Teacher, 22(8), 504-508.
deeper learning in a domain, and teach Gunstone, R., & White, R. T. (1981). Patton, M. Q. (1990). Qualitative
accordingly. Understanding of gravity. Science evaluation and research methods (2
Education, 65(3), 291-299. ed.). Newbury Park: Sage
References Gunstone, R. F., & White, R. T. (1980). A Pfundt, H., & Duit, R. (1994). Bibliography:
matter of gravity. Research in Science Students’Alternative Frameworks and
Arnold, P., Sarge, A., & Worrall, L. Education, 10, 35-44. Science Education. Kiel, Germany:
(1995). Children’s knowledge of the Hestenes, D., Wells, M., & Swackhamer, University of Kiel Institute for Science
earth’s shape and its gravitational G. (1992). Force concept inventory. The Education.
field. International Journal of Science Physics Teacher, 30, 141-158. Piaget, J. (1972). The Child’s Conception
Education, 17(5), 635-641. Hood, B. M. (1995). Gravity rules for 2-to of Physical Causality. Totowa, New
Bar, V., Zinn, B., Goldmuntz, R., & 4-year olds? Cognitive Development, Jersey: Littlefield, Adams.
Sneider, C. (1994). Children’s concepts 10, 577-598. Ruggiero, S., Cartielli, A., Dupre, F., &
about weight and free fall. Science Kavanagh, C., & Sneider, C. (2006-2007). Vicentini-Missoni, M. (1985). Weight,
Education, 78(2), 149-169. Learning about Gravity I. Free Fall: A gravity, and air pressure: Mental
Baxter, J. (1989). Children’s understanding Guide for Teachers and Curriculum representations by Italian middle
of familiar astronomical events. Developers. Astronomy Education school pupils. European Journal of
International Journal of Science Review, 5(2), 21-52. Science Education, 7, 181-194.
Education, 11(special issue), 502-
513.

54 SCIENCE EDUCATOR
Sadanand, N., & Kess, J. (1990). Concepts Stead, K., & Osborne, R. (1980). A working Anila Asghar is assistant professor in the
in force and motion. The Physics paper of the learning in science project. School of Education at Johns Hopkins
Teacher 530-533. Hamilton, New Zealand: University of University. Her work focuses on cognitive
Selman, R., Krupa, M., Stone, C., Waikato, Science Education Research and emotional development, science/STEM
& Jacuette, D. (1982). Concrete Unit. education reform; science, religion, and
operational thought and the emergence Trumper, R., & Grosky, P. (1997). A survey culture; and developing scaffolded contexts
of the concept of unseen force in of biology students’ conceptions of for faculty development. Correspondence
children’s theories of electromagnetism force in pre-service training for high concerning this article can be sent to <anila.
and gravity. Science Education, 66, school teachers. Research in science asghar@jhu.edu>.
181-194. and technological education, 15(2),
Julie Libarkin is assistant professor of
Shaffer, P. S., & McDermott, L. C. 133-147.
geocognition and tectonics, Department
(2005). A research-based approach Vosniadou, S. (1994). Capturing and
of Geological Sciences, Michigan State
to improving student understanding modeling the process of conceptual University, 206 Natural Science Building, East
of kinematical concepts. American change. Learning and Instruction, 4, Lansing, MI 48824. She can be contacted at
Journal of Physics, 73(10), 921-931. 45-69. <libarkin@msu.edu>.
Sneider, C., & Ohadi, M. (1998). Vosniadou, S., & Brewer, W. F. (1992).
Unraveling students’ misconceptions Mental models of the earth: A study Acknowledgements: We thank all students
about the earth’s shape and gravity. of conceptual change in childhood. who graciously participated in this research.
Science Education, 82, 265-284. Cognitive Psychology, 24, 535-585. Libarkin was partially funded by the National
Sneider, C., & Pulos, S. (1981). Children’s Za’Rour, G. I. (1975). Science Science Foundation through an Assessment
cosmographies: understanding the misconceptions among certain groups of Student Achievement in Undergraduate
earth’s shape and gravity. Science of students in Lebanon. Journal of Education grant during this project (DUE-
0127765). Any opinions, findings, and
Education, 67(2), 205-221. Research in Science Teaching, 12(4),
conclusions or recommendations expressed in
Sneider, C., & Pulos, S. (1983). Children’s 385-391.
this publication are those of the authors and do
cosmographies: Understanding the not necessarily reflect the views of the National
Earth’s shape and gravity. Science Science Foundation.
Education, 67, 205-222.

SPRING 2010 VOL. 19, NO. 1 55

You might also like