You are on page 1of 24

950208

research-article2020
ASRXXX10.1177/0003122420950208American Sociological ReviewDaminger

American Sociological Review

De-gendered Processes, 2020, Vol. 85(5) 806­–829


© American Sociological
Association 2020
Gendered Outcomes: How DOI: 10.1177/0003122420950208
https://doi.org/10.1177/0003122420950208
journals.sagepub.com/home/asr

Egalitarian Couples Make


Sense of Non-egalitarian
Household Practices

Allison Damingera

Abstract
Despite widespread support for gender-egalitarianism, men’s and women’s household labor
contributions remain strikingly unequal. This article extends prior research on barriers to
equality by closely examining how couples negotiate contradictions between their egalitarian
ideals and admittedly non-egalitarian practices. Data from 64 in-depth interviews with
members of 32 different-sex, college-educated couples show that respondents distinguish
between labor allocation processes and outcomes. When they understand the processes as
gender-neutral, they can write off gendered outcomes as the incidental result of necessary
compromises made among competing values. Respondents “de-gender” their allocation
process, or decouple it from gender ideology and gendered social forces, by narrowing their
temporal horizon to the present moment and deploying an adaptable understanding of
constraint that obscures alternative paths. This de-gendering helps prevent spousal conflict,
but it may also facilitate behavioral stasis by directing attention away from the inequalities
that continue to shape domestic life.

Keywords
gender, family, household labor, egalitarianism

Heather and Jeremy, married parents of two the average married mother still performs
toddlers, are a proudly egalitarian couple. In nearly two hours of housework for every hour
separate interviews, Jeremy reported that her male counterpart contributes (Bianchi
“[we] don’t believe in a lot of traditional gen- et al. 2012), it is also unsurprising that Heather
der norms. . . . We think a lot of that stuff is and Jeremy admitted they fall short of their
nonsense,” and Heather argued that “obvi- stated ideals. “It turns out when you talk
ously ideally [our division of household
labor] is 50/50.” The couple’s endorsement of
gender equality was unsurprising based on a
Harvard University
their demographic characteristics: they are
young (mid-30s), upper-middle-class, college- Corresponding Author:
Allison Daminger, Harvard University, 440
educated, and live in a progressive Northeast- William James Hall, 33 Kirkland Street,
ern city (Bolzendahl and Myers 2004; Davis Cambridge, MA 02138
and Greenstein 2009). However, given that Email: daminger@g.harvard.edu
Daminger 807

through it that we adhere to very traditional their own experiences. Specifically, how do
norms,” said Jeremy. “We’re sort of loath to people living their own version of the stalled
admit this, but we do fall into the very typical revolution—that is, couples who combine
gender stereotypes,” concurred Heather. progressive ideals with admittedly traditional
Despite the acknowledged gap between their practices—make sense of their domestic
ideal and their reality, however, neither activities? What mechanisms enable aspiring
Heather nor Jeremy reported efforts to real- egalitarians to tolerate a persistent mismatch
locate labor or conflicts over who does what between their stated ideals and acknowledged
in their household. practices vis-à-vis household labor? Answers
In this article, I examine the experience of to these questions could illuminate how
cisgender, college-educated, different-sex micro-level processes help maintain gender
couples like Heather and Jeremy who aspire to inequality in the face of ideological change.
equality, admit they have not achieved it, yet Based on 64 in-depth interviews with
coexist in a state of apparent equilibrium. members of 32 different-sex, college-
How do such couples maintain the status quo educated couples, I argue that egalitarian cou-
in their households despite acknowledged dis- ples practice a form of “de-gendering” that
crepancies between their beliefs and behaviors obscures the gendered forces shaping their
vis-à-vis household labor? This micro-level household labor practices. In contrast to Lor-
puzzle mirrors macro-level questions about ber’s (2000) vision of a “feminist degender-
why and how the gender revolution has ing movement” in which gender differences
“stalled” (England 2010). In recent decades, cease to operate as a heuristic for allocating
support for gender egalitarianism rose consid- labor, this shallow version of de-gendering
erably (Scarborough, Sin, and Risman 2019); recasts gender inequality as personal inequal-
today, younger generations endorse women’s ity without disturbing the underlying gender
advancement in the public sphere and equality hierarchy. Couples accomplish this by distin-
in romantic relationships at high rates (Gerson guishing household labor allocation pro-
2011). Behaviors, too, have changed, as cesses, which they see as gender-neutral,
women have entered the workforce in greater from allocation outcomes, which they see as
numbers and now equal or exceed men in their only incidentally gender-traditional.
educational attainment (Aud et al. 2010; Juhn De-gendered accounts are often plausible at
and Potter 2006). Despite these gains, full- the level of an individual household operating
time female workers earn only 79 percent as on a daily basis. However, when the data are
much as comparable men on an annual basis viewed in the aggregate and choices over a
(Blau and Kahn 2017). On the domestic front, longer timespan are taken into account, gender
the ratio of women’s to men’s household labor is harder to ignore. Despite considerable varia-
hours is well below mid-twentieth-century tion in life circumstances, most respondents
levels but plateaued several decades ago and acknowledge the female partner completes
still hovers around 2:1 (Bianchi et al. 2012). more household labor. Simultaneously, they
Prior scholarship documents a constella- deny or downplay any connection between this
tion of structural and cultural factors hinder- unequal labor division and gender ideology.
ing further progress toward gender equality in Instead, they understand their behavior in indi-
household labor, including occupational seg- vidualistic terms. While this de-gendered
regation, a culture of overwork, inadequate understanding may reduce cognitive disso-
family leave and childcare policies, and gen- nance and improve marital harmony, it also
dered socialization patterns (Fuwa 2004; allows gender to operate as an unseen and
Horne et al. 2018; Lachance-Grzela and uncontested force shaping household labor
Bouchard 2010). However, we know less patterns outside couples’ conscious awareness.
about how individuals and couples under- Further movement toward household labor
stand these societal forces in the context of equality—and, likely, gender equality more
808 American Sociological Review 85(5)

broadly—will require greater acknowledg- shopping, childcare, and other household


ment of gender’s role in structuring apparently domains, narrowed considerably in the second
personal circumstances. half of the twentieth century (Sayer 2005). Yet
men still spend approximately half as many
weekly hours on physical housework as
Household Labor in The women—a gap that has remained relatively
Stalled Revolution stable since the mid-1990s (Bianchi et al.
Many sociologists understand gender as a 2012).1 Furthermore, men’s housework hours
social structure, institution, or system inscribed are disproportionately allocated toward rela-
in individual, interactional, and institutional tively infrequent and flexible tasks (e.g., home
dynamics (Martin 2004; Ridgeway and Correll repairs or yard work), while women shoulder
2004; Risman 2004). The past half-century many of the recurring daily tasks (e.g., cook-
brought extensive change to this structure, ing and childcare) that cannot be put off to a
particularly in the realm of beliefs and atti- convenient time (Bianchi et al. 2000).
tudes. In 1977 in the United States, for Physical work remains the focus of most
instance, 68 percent of men and 62 percent of research on household labor, but sociologists
women agreed it was better that men work for have also drawn attention to “hidden” or
pay and women handle domestic matters “invisible” forms of labor, including cogni-
(Smith et al. 2018). Four decades later, only tive and emotional labor (Daminger 2019;
27 and 22 percent of men and women, respec- Daniels 1987; DeVault 1999). Cognitive
tively, endorsed this gender-traditional divi- labor, sometimes described as mental labor
sion (Smith et al. 2018). Over the same period, (Lee and Waite 2005; Offer 2014), comprises
individuals with traditional or strongly tradi- the work of anticipating household needs,
tional gender attitudes went from comprising identifying options for filling them, choosing
the bulk of General Social Survey respondents among these options, and monitoring the
(59 percent) to all but disappearing (7 percent) results (Daminger 2019). Although the inter-
(Scarborough et al. 2019). nal nature of such tasks makes cognitive labor
Behavioral change is also evident, although difficult to measure, the available indicators
it has been uneven and stalled well short of suggest it, too, is highly gendered, with
gender equality (England 2010). Different- women often serving as household managers
sex romantic relationships have proven par- who delegate tasks to their male “helpers”
ticularly resistant to change. Young adults (Coltrane 1996; Daminger 2019).
idealize long-term partnerships where paid Social scientists explain the persistence of
work and caregiving responsibilities are gendered family labor patterns using some
shared (Gerson 2011). They believe gender combination of three major theories: time
should not dictate familial roles, although availability, relative resources, and gender
they may modify their position in light of (Bianchi et al. 2000; Coltrane 2000; Lachance-
perceived institutional constraints (Pedulla Grzela and Bouchard 2010). In combination
and Thébaud 2015). Nevertheless, gender- with key features of the current gender struc-
traditional patterns of dating, parenting, and ture, all three proposed mechanisms facilitate
breadwinning remain widespread (Bass 2015; a division of labor in which women take on
Lamont 2014; Milkie et al. 2002). more unpaid labor and men more paid labor.
Gender remains a key predictor of the Yet the question of why, in a causal sense,
amount and kind of labor an individual com- certain family labor patterns persist is distinct
pletes on the family’s behalf, although by from questions about how couples experience
some measures the association has weakened and understand those patterns. Couples whose
(Bianchi et al. 2012; Sayer 2005). The gender conscious beliefs about gender contradict the
gap in physical housework, which encom- persistence of gendered labor patterns in their
passes chores related to cleaning, cooking, household face “moral dilemmas” as they
Daminger 809

attempt to reconcile ideology and behavior in the fact that from an outsider’s vantage point,
the absence of more widespread changes to couples’ narratives often seem closer to fiction
the gender structure (Gerson 2002). Deeper than fact. Their respondents Nancy and Evan
understanding of this reconciliation process Holt exemplify the myth-making phenome-
may suggest a path toward restarting the non: although Nancy completes the majority
stalled gender revolution. Assuming struc- of housework and childcare for the family, she
tural conditions shape but do not determine reframes her responsibilities as encompassing
individual actions, individuals and couples only the “upstairs” activities (Hochschild and
can make choices that challenge existing Machung 2012). In the researchers’ estima-
norms (Coleman 1990). Figuring out why tion, Nancy’s responsibilities clearly exceed
cisgender, different-sex couples more often her husband’s, yet Nancy frames his work as
reproduce than subvert such norms requires parallel to her own. Evan, she says, completes
deeper understanding of the meanings cou- the “downstairs” tasks of pet care, car mainte-
ples ascribe to their actions and the processes nance, and lawn work. Creative reframing of
by which they jointly create such meanings. each spouse’s contributions as distinct but
equivalent effectively resolves Nancy’s cogni-
tive dissonance: she retains her feminist com-
De-Gendering Household mitment to sharing the household load but
Labor avoids conflict with her husband, who resisted
The persistence of gendered labor patterns her prior efforts to change his behavior.
despite widespread ideological change raises The precise details of such myths may be
questions about how egalitarian couples idiosyncratic, but many fall into one of two
experience the disjuncture between their categories: erasure of unpaid labor inequality
beliefs and behaviors.2 This disjuncture likely or justification of that inequality. Nancy Holt’s
generates cognitive dissonance, a state of ten- upstairs/downstairs myth exemplifies the for-
sion resolved by changing beliefs, changing mer strategy. Tired of fighting with her husband
behaviors, or reframing the situation to about his minimal contributions but unwilling
remove the appearance of conflict between to end the relationship, Nancy opts to “see” the
the two (Festinger 1957; Harmon-Jones, situation differently, recasting it as equal. Simi-
Harmon-Jones, and Levy 2015). Prior larly, sociologists attempting to recruit egalitar-
research demonstrates all three tactics are at ian couples report that many self-identified
work in the realm of household labor. For egalitarians fail to meet even lenient criteria for
instance, the transition to parenthood is some- equality (Deutsch 2000; Risman and Johnson-
times associated with a change in gender Sumerford 1998). Their identity as egalitarians
beliefs, as new parents respond to their altered may lead such couples to interpret their house-
circumstances (Katz-Wise, Priess, and Hyde hold activities through a particular lens.
2010). Deliberate efforts to change behaviors More commonly, however, couples
are also evident: in response to dissatisfaction acknowledge inequality in their household
with or conflict over household labor alloca- labor contributions but subsequently justify it.
tion, for example, individuals or couples may Consistent with time availability and speciali-
actively pursue a more equal division of labor zation theories, for instance, some couples
(Hochschild and Machung 2012; Mannino justify an unequal allocation of unpaid labor
and Deutsch 2007). as fair by arguing that each partner’s overall
The third option, reframing family labor contributions to the household are similar
patterns as consistent with egalitarian princi- (Becker 1993; Deutsch 2000). For instance, if
ples, is most commonly discussed in the exist- one partner works longer hours outside the
ing literature. Hochschild and Machung home, a couple may consider it appropriate for
(2012) coined the term “family myths” to the other partner to complete the majority of
describe this reframing process, nodding to the domestic labor (van Hooff 2011). Yet
810 American Sociological Review 85(5)

commensurating contributions made across Also unclear is how justifications for phys-
multiple domains (physical housework, cogni- ical or cognitive labor patterns work to resolve
tive housework, paid work) and measured in or prevent interpersonal conflict and feelings
multiple forms (time, money, effort) is a dif- of cognitive dissonance regarding household
ficult and subjective process. Prior research labor. What “work” do these justifications
demonstrates that fairness assessments hinge accomplish for couples who have an ideologi-
on factors well beyond the actual contribu- cal commitment to gender equality but recog-
tions each partner makes, including compari- nize their failure to fully enact that ideology?
son referents, overall relationship satisfaction, I argue that successful justifications “de-
and the portion of appropriately gender-typed gender” labor allocation processes: they
tasks each partner completes (DeMaris and obscure the gendered component of a funda-
Longmore 1996; Gager 2008; Smith, Gager, mentally gendered process, often by replac-
and Morgan 1998). ing gendered explanations with appeals to
Couples may also justify their unpaid labor other agreed-upon values.3
inequality as the result of forces outside their Sociologists describe a social process as
conscious control (van Hooff 2011; Nyman, “gendered” when the gender of the individu-
Reinikainen, and Eriksson 2018). Even if part- als involved shapes their outcomes. In quanti-
ners’ paid hours are similar, couples may cite tative terms, this means gender acts as an
the inflexible or stressful nature of those hours explanatory variable that patterns behavior at
as an obstacle to one partner’s greater partici- a supra-individual level. Many of the pro-
pation in household affairs (Beagan et al. cesses sociologists view as gendered, how-
2008). Similarly, couples may offer their per- ever, are understood in different terms by the
sonal preferences or skills—for instance, one individuals experiencing them. For instance,
partner’s higher tolerance for disorder or lim- occupational gender segregation persists in
ited facility with a mop—as the reason one part because at an aggregate level men and
partner performs certain chores (Lamont women make different decisions about which
2020; Nyman et al. 2018). Whether they point college majors and jobs to pursue (Charles
to differences in skills, preferences, or employ- and Bradley 2009; Charles and Grusky 2004).
ment circumstances, couples who recognize a Similarly, many more women than men leave
disjuncture between their ideological commit- the paid workforce to care for children (Stone
ments and daily practices clearly have a broad 2007). The individuals making such deci-
repertoire of justifications from which to draw. sions, however, experience their career
Less clear is the extent to which the justifi- choices as personal. They attribute their out-
cations given for physical labor inequalities— comes to individual preferences, traits, or
the main focus of prior research—apply to the circumstances without acknowledging that
cognitive dimension of housework. For these apparently individual-level factors are
instance, work outside the home clearly inter- strongly gendered (Cech 2013; Stone 2007).
feres with one’s ability to cook, clean, and In what follows, I demonstrate a similar
care for a child, as these activities require the process at work in the context of unpaid
laborer to be in the home or in the presence of household labor. By narrowing their temporal
children. The same cannot necessarily be said horizon to the present moment and deploying
of cognitive tasks such as meal planning, an adaptable understanding of constraint,
emailing a child’s teacher about a recent issue, respondents de-gender their labor allocation
or booking flights for an upcoming family processes even as they acknowledge the gen-
vacation. Such labor is less tied to location dered nature of their allocation outcomes.
and can often be performed in parallel with Such de-gendering appears to facilitate mari-
other tasks (e.g., commuting can be combined tal harmony: when an egalitarian couple devi-
with dinner planning) (Bittman and Wajcman ates from the narrative of gender-neutral
2000; Daminger 2019; Emens 2019). allocation processes, the threat of conflict
Daminger 811

looms. However, this superficial form of within a small sample, although they neces-
de-gendering—relabeling rather than reform- sarily limit the generalizability of the findings
ing—likely entrenches the gender inequality presented here.
that has long characterized different-sex cou- Table 1 summarizes the demographic char-
ples’ unpaid labor practices. acteristics of the resulting sample. The aver-
age participant was in their mid-30s (35 for
women and 36 for men), had 1.5 children, and
Data and Methods had been married for 6.6 years. Participants
Data come from 64 semi-structured, in-depth had considerably more education and income
individual interviews conducted between than the average American: 69 percent of men
June and December 2017 with members of 32 and 91 percent of women held an advanced
cisgender, different-sex couples living in the degree, and median annual household income
Boston area. I advertised the study on list- was $170,000. Six women and four men were
servs and Facebook groups directed at par- out of the workforce altogether at the time of
ents, most of which targeted mothers or our interview, although a few of the women
caregivers in a particular geographic area planned to return to work after maternity
(e.g., “Somerville Moms”). Seventy-five per- leave. Among the employed respondents,
cent of couples (N = 24) in the sample women worked a median of 40 hours and
entered via this channel, and the remainder earned $70,000 per year; men worked a
were referred by prior study participants or by median of 46 hours and earned $107,500
contacts in my extended social network. To annually. Although no racial exclusion criteria
reduce the likelihood that only people with were advertised, the majority of participants
strong opinions or unusual practices regard- (81 percent of men and 78 percent of women)
ing gender and household labor would volun- identified as white. I also refrained from spec-
teer, I avoided gender- and housework-related ifying any sexual orientation criteria; how-
language in all recruitment materials. Instead, ever, the vast majority of couples consisted of
I advertised a study about “how parents make a cisgender man and woman. I interviewed
decisions,” which was appropriate given that three couples with different gender configura-
my primary research questions when the tions, but I exclude them from the analysis
study began centered on couples’ division of presented here due to the small sample size.
cognitive tasks (including decision-making). In most cases, an individual responded to
Respondents were required to be married,4 the advertisement and then, at my request,
age 25 to 50, hold a bachelor’s degree,5 and recruited their partner to participate. I inter-
live with at least one child under age 5. viewed each participant individually at their
Toward the end of the study period, I deliber- home, workplace, or a local café. Interviews
ately recruited couples with a female partner lasted approximately 60 to 80 minutes and
working full-time and a male partner working were audio-recorded with respondents’ per-
part-time or not at all, to achieve rough parity mission. All partners were interviewed within
in the employment status of male and female a few days of one another—the majority on
respondents. Because the transition to parent- the same day—and asked not to discuss the
hood has been associated with an increase in study with their spouse until they had also
gendered labor patterns (Katz-Wise et al. been interviewed.
2010), I was particularly interested in how Approximately two days prior to each
members of a highly-educated, relatively pro- interview, I asked each respondent to track
gressive demographic would experience this family- and household-related decisions they
shift (Davis and Greenstein 2009; Scarbor- made or contemplated over the course of a
ough et al. 2019). These eligibility criteria 24-hour period. I provided a simple spread-
were intended to ensure comparability on the sheet template with fields for the nature of the
dimensions of class and family structure issue or decision in question, the outcome, the
812 American Sociological Review 85(5)

Table 1.  Demographic Characteristics of the Sample

Individual-Level
Men Women  
Mean age 36 Mean age 35
Race Race  
 White 81%  White 78%
 Asian 9%  Asian 16%
 Other 9%  Other 6%
Median incomea $107,500 Median incomea $70,000
Median work hoursa 46 Median work hoursa 40
Work status at time of intvw. Work status at time of intvw.  
  Full-time (34+ hrs.) 78%   Full-time (34+ hrs.) 66%
 Part-time 9%  Part-time 16%
 Unemployed 13%  Unemployed 18%
Education level Education level  
  Some college 3%   Some college 0%
 Bachelor’s 28%  Bachelor’s 9%
  Graduate degree 69%   Graduate degree 91%

Couple-Level
Number of children 1.5 Years married 6.6
Youngest child’s age 2.0 Median income $170,000
Relative income Relative paid work hours  
  Wife $5k+ more 22%   Wife 4+ more 28%
  Equal earnings 13%  Equal 13%
  Husband $5k+ more 65%   Husband 4+ more 60%

Note: Data come from participants’ responses to a series of demographic questions posed at the end of
their interviews. N = 64 (32 women, 32 men).
a
Conditional on paid employment.

setting, whether others were consulted or practices. As such, these findings largely
involved, and the start and end time. These draw on data from the final component of the
“decision logs” structured the first portion of interview, in which I asked participants a
each interview, in which I asked participants series of more general questions about physi-
to provide more detail about a subset of cal and cognitive labor. After defining cogni-
logged decisions and to contextualize the tive labor as the set of research, planning,
recorded day’s events in terms of what typi- decision-making, and coordination tasks
cally happens in their household. I also asked involved in running a household, I offered
each respondent to describe the most recent respondents two examples to illustrate the
occurrence of a standard set of irregular activ- distinction between cognitive and physical
ities (e.g., planning a vacation or making a labor, including making a weekly meal plan
home repair) and to tell me about a recent (cognitive) and chopping vegetables (physi-
spousal conflict or disagreement sparked by a cal). I then asked respondents to characterize
household matter. their ideal and actual allocation of cognitive
The decision logs and related interview and physical labor (i.e., “I (would) do X per-
questions were important for assessing each cent and my partner Y percent”). Often,
couple’s division of labor (see Daminger respondents spontaneously reflected on the
2019), but this article centers on couples’ reasons they allocated labor as they did. I
perceptions of and explanations for their labor asked interviewees who were not
Daminger 813

forthcoming with this information to explain “horizontal” rather than vertical split: “[My
what prevented them from achieving their partner should] take half the things in our life
desired allocation (if applicable), describe and think about them, and I’ll take half the
past or ongoing efforts to adjust their division things in our life and think about them, but I
of labor, and imagine what would happen if don’t want to think about half the finances
they stopped performing their portion of and [my partner] think about half the
household tasks. finances.” Similarly, Kendra was okay with
Each interview was transcribed verbatim task-by-task variation so long as it “washes
and paired with ethnographic notes taken out to a good 50/50.”
shortly following the conversation. Initially, Among respondents reporting an ideal
data analysis centered on the research ques- other than 50/50, most nevertheless aspired to
tions that inspired the study: what is cognitive a more equal division than they currently had.
labor, and how does gender shape the amount Nina estimated that she completes 90 percent
and kind of this labor individuals in different- of the physical labor in her household but
sex couples complete? In the process of would ideally complete only 70 percent: “I
reviewing and open-coding transcripts, how- guess probably like realistically, I would hope
ever, I was increasingly puzzled by what was that [my husband] could do 30 percent of [the
not mentioned. Despite acknowledging their physical labor]. You know, idealistically, I
unequal labor allocations, few respondents would say 50/50, but that’s just not the reality
reported conflict over or attempts to alter on the ground.” Like other respondents, Nina
their division of labor. In subsequent rounds endorsed an equal division of labor as a gen-
of reading, memo-writing, and coding, I eral principle but seemed unable even to
focused on respondents’ efforts to justify dis- imagine a world in which such equality would
crepancies between their beliefs and behav- be attainable.
iors, their (relatively rare) descriptions of Widespread egalitarian or near-egalitarian
conflict or dissatisfaction regarding house- aspirations coincided with widespread admis-
hold labor, and, more broadly, their explana- sion of current inequality. No spouses agreed
tions of why they behaved as they did. This they have a 50/50 allocation (or the qualita-
data analysis generated themes such as “effi- tive equivalent) in both the physical and cog-
ciency” and “self-expression,” which I nitive labor dimensions, although two
explore in depth in subsequent sections. indicated that imbalances in these dimensions
were perfectly offset (e.g., a 75/25 split in one
dimension and a 25/75 split in the other), and
Findings in another two couples the female partner
Egalitarian Ideal, Non-egalitarian alone reported equality. Although spouses
Reality rarely concurred on the precise magnitude of
the imbalance, the majority (27 of 32) agreed
Consistent with prior research documenting on the direction (i.e., which partner com-
widespread egalitarianism among the demo- pleted a greater proportion of the labor).
graphic studied here (Bolzendahl and Myers Gender-traditionalism was the norm: 22 men
2004; Davis and Greenstein 2009), most and 23 women indicated that the female part-
respondents described their “ideal world” ner does more overall household labor. Some
division of labor as something close to a couples described extreme gaps, with one
50/50 split.6 “I want it to be an equal partner- partner completing 90 percent or more of the
ship,” said Kristen, echoing many respon- labor, but a slight or moderate skew toward
dents’ sentiments. Other respondents added one partner was more commonly reported.
more nuance to their answers, but the under- Despite widespread acknowledgment of a
lying ideal remained consistent. Miranda, for discrepancy between the labor allocation they
instance, argued that she wanted a desired and the one they experienced, the
814 American Sociological Review 85(5)

majority of couples (28 of 32) referenced only William’s household contributions, she pur-
infrequent and mild, if any, disagreements sued change somewhat halfheartedly, via
regarding labor allocation, either in response non-combative channels. Similarly, Jackie
to a question about a recent disagreement or and Matthew independently reported seeking
when asked to compare their actual labor allo- to transfer some of Jackie’s cognitive labor
cation to their ideal. To account for the unex- load to Matthew, thus far with limited suc-
pected absence of such conflict in the sample, cess. “I know that [Jackie] carries a lot of load
I first examined the possibility that partici- that I don’t,” Matthew told me, “so I’ve been
pants experienced labor-related conflict but trying to get more involved in her process. . . .
were unwilling to disclose relationship imper- I feel a duty to try to rectify this sort of unspo-
fections to an outsider. However, couples ken disparity that we have.”
described numerous relationship challenges Yet Stacey, Jackie, and Matthew were in
unrelated to household labor. These disagree- the minority in describing deliberate attempts
ments largely centered on parenting (e.g., how at reallocation. Instead, most couples pre-
best to discipline a child), finances (e.g., sented as stably unequal: one or both partners
whether to make a big purchase), and leisure acknowledged that, contrary to their ideals,
time (e.g., where to go on vacation). Some their household labor allocation was
used blunt language and revealed potentially imbalanced—typically in the direction of
embarrassing facts as they recollected recent gender-traditionalism—but relatively fixed,
conflict. Garrett, for instance, reported that his sparking neither serious conflict nor con-
family is $250,000 in debt to his parents, certed efforts to change.7 Understanding this
although his wife “doesn’t deal with” their stability requires closer examination of the
debt because it threatens “her fragile self- meanings respondents ascribed to their labor
image,” and Chelsea described a “constant practices. In the following sections, I first
battle” over how often to visit her husband show that couples understand their current
Phil’s family, who she does not enjoy spend- practices as the result of compromises made
ing time with. Unwillingness to disclose disa- among competing objectives, and second,
greement, it seems, was not the primary reason that although respondents depict these trade-
for the absence of labor-related disputes from offs as gender-neutral, they are better under-
the data. stood as de-gendered: gender’s influence is
Alternatively, individual respondents elided but not absent.
might internally experience dissatisfaction
with their division of labor without regularly
Labor Allocation as Compromise
channeling it into interpersonal conflict. The
data support this hypothesis for a subset of Like Heather, who was “loath to admit”—but
couples who reported a desire to reallocate still admitted—that she and her husband fall
labor in their household. Stacey, for example, into traditional roles, respondents often
seemed torn between bemusement and exas- acknowledged the resemblance between their
peration as she recounted her attempts to labor allocation and the gender norms of an
increase her husband William’s involvement earlier era. However, they denied any connec-
in caring for their toddler. She recently asked tion between their ideology and behavior.
him to take responsibility for restocking their Instead, respondents described their labor
daughter’s diaper bag: “The day after I said allocation as the product of gender-neutral
that, he stuck an extra five diapers in the bag, trade-offs made among their competing
but he didn’t check to see if there were desires for household efficiency, personal
already diapers in the bag. So I had to go in expression, and spousal equality. In their
and take them out, and I just didn’t tell him ideal world, these three imperatives would
about it because I was glad that he made the perfectly align. In their actual world, respon-
effort.” Although Stacey was dissatisfied with dents described them as routinely at odds.
Daminger 815

The majority demonstrated a commitment I’m uniquely qualified for,” she told me, “but
to domestic efficiency: they sought to opti- it will just be much faster if I do it.” Jenna has
mize household operations by minimizing the a knack for finding deals on plane tickets, so
time, effort, or money expended in pursuit of she books most travel for the family. Gina is
a desired outcome. Todd noted that both he better at anticipating upcoming needs and
and his wife are “very good at identifying if tracking household inventory, so she monitors
[we’re] capable of doing something that will the children’s clothing supply and manages
make a process more efficient,” and Troy the family’s ongoing shopping list.
recalled a conversation about how he and his Alongside their concern for efficiency,
wife could “maximize” their limited childfree most respondents referenced a commitment to
hours to unpack and settle into their new self-expression, citing personality traits and
home. Natalie and Jay independently refer- ingrained preferences that shaped partners’
enced their “non-overlapping skill sets”: hers distinct approaches to household activities.
in cooking and childcare, his in finances and They framed household labor—both what
logistics. Jay elaborated that the couple deter- they did or did not do, and how—as not only
mined which partner had a “comparative a set of tasks but also a reflection of one’s
advantage” in a given domain and, where nature. Inequalities were tolerable if they were
possible, endeavored to “specialize” in driven by partners’ distinct personalities. Such
accordance with this assessment. Craig used expressive concerns were primarily cited in
his skills as an IT professional to design a relation to cognitive activities. The partners
dynamic household inventory system he acknowledged to do a greater share of such
hoped would minimize waste in the form of work were labeled (or labeled themselves)
spoiled food and unnecessary grocery trips. “type-A,” “detail-oriented,” or “planful”—or,
Couples perceived equality- and efficiency- less charitably, “uptight,” “OCD,” and “anx-
maximizing options to be routinely at odds. ious.” The secondary cognitive laborers, in
Most often, the relative timing, location, and contrast, were understood (or understood
flexibility of each partner’s paid work shaped themselves) to be “laid-back,” “relaxed,” and
the couple’s perceptions of the most efficient “really present in the moment,” particularly in
allocation of labor. Holly’s workday typically comparison to their spouse. Put in a more
ends earlier than her husband’s, so she is negative light, these partners were depicted as
responsible for starting dinner. Jason, a finan- the more “passive,” “disorganized,” or prone
cial services analyst, can work remotely, to “make stuff up on the fly” of the two.
whereas his wife, a teacher, cannot; thus, Notably, respondents expressed their com-
Jason is more likely to pick up a sick child mitment to self-expression in individual rather
from school. Because Joanna is taking an than gendered terms, focusing on personal
extended maternity leave and her husband is qualities rather than masculine or feminine
working from an office full-time, Joanna han- traits. Jeremy attributed his wife’s greater
dles all preschool pick-ups and drop-offs. share of the family’s cognitive load to the
Secondarily, respondents cited partners’ partners’ differing “personality styles”:
different skill levels as drivers of conflicts “Heather’s a planner and is just constantly
between efficiency and equality: if one per- thinking about this stuff. And I—I wouldn’t
son could complete a household task faster say I’m a doer, necessarily, but I’m just more
without compromising the outcome, that per- disorganized.” Heather agreed that although a
son should do so—even if that meant the 50/50 split would be ideal, “That just doesn’t
“skilled” partner completed more housework lend itself to our personalities. I’m so much
overall. Both Bridget and her husband Jimmy more uptight than he is.” When asked to imag-
acknowledged Bridget’s superior internet ine what would need to change for her partner
research skills: “There’s nothing about to take on more cognitive labor, Bridget
[researching extracurricular activities] that laughed: “I think his brain would have to be
816 American Sociological Review 85(5)

different. Which is not a thing I’m committed Gendered outcomes were acceptable to the
to trying to change.” Carla speculated that a extent they facilitated greater efficiency or
similarly dramatic transformation would be accommodated partners’ personality differ-
required for her to reach equality with her ences, rather than as an end in themselves.
husband. When it came to tasks like managing However, as the following section shows, gen-
their kindergartener’s wardrobe, she said, “I der’s role was obscured but not eliminated.
don’t think he’s capable of thinking about
things like [that]—I just don’t think that he
De-gendering the Compromise
works that way.” None of these respondents
generalized about men’s brains or cited a mas- Although respondents presented the tradeoffs
culine inability to plan; instead, they spoke they made among efficiency, expression, and
about the characteristics of a specific man and equality as gender-neutral, these compro-
how these compared to his partner’s. mises drove the majority of couples toward a
Even couples who described an atypically gender-traditional division of household
gendered labor allocation (i.e., with the male labor. Female respondents were more often
partner dominant in the cognitive or physical described, by both men and women, as the
dimensions) cited expressive concerns. “Type-A” partner who naturally gravitated
Antoni, for instance, described a year-long toward cognitive labor, whereas male respon-
quest to establish new routines for his house- dents were more often labeled disorganized
hold after his daughter’s birth, admitting he or laid-back. Men’s job requirements more
could be “a little OCD” about certain activi- often interfered with their domestic contribu-
ties: “Being the planful person that I am, it’s tions, and women’s paid work was more
just nature. It just comes naturally to me.” Jay, amenable to household needs. Yet respon-
who shared the overall cognitive load roughly dents insisted any resemblance between their
equally with his wife but conducted the bulk behaviors and traditional norms was coinci-
of scheduling and logistical work, also attrib- dental. “I know the large gender blah blah
uted the couple’s dynamic to temperamental blah structure,” said Levi dismissively as he
differences: “I would love for [planning and described his wife’s long-standing tendency
logistics] to be more 50/50, frankly. I just to do more of the housework and childcare,
doubt we’ll ever get there just because of the “but I don’t know why we specifically do this
way we’re both wired.” Although Antoni and the way we [do].” Likewise, Jay acknowl-
Jay cited a commitment to self-expression to edged he and his wife “fall into traditional
justify a nontraditional division of labor, the gender roles” before clarifying that it was
result was similar. By framing household “not because we subscribe to them.”
labor in terms of “who we are” rather than Respondents relied on syllogistic logic to
merely “what we do,” they implied a belief explain their allocation compromises: given
that reallocation toward equality would be partners’ job characteristics, skills, or person-
costly at best and impossible at worst. ality, only one labor outcome was reasonable.
Respondents accounted for the conflict From this perspective, beliefs about gender
between their egalitarian ideals and non- were irrelevant; gendered outcomes were the
egalitarian actions by contextualizing equality as coincidental result of unrelated allocation
one among several goals. For many, the ideal processes. Yet closer examination suggests
division of labor was not only equal between gender was in fact baked into respondents’
partners but also maximized the couple’s col- conceptions of what was optimal or even pos-
lective efficiency and allowed each partner to sible and that gendered life trajectories shaped
act in accordance with their individual nature. the conditions respondents faced, largely out-
These goals were often at odds, however, side their conscious awareness. Respondents,
prompting couples to make tradeoffs that, from however, “de-gendered” their allocation pro-
their perspective, had little to do with gender. cesses by adopting a flexible understanding
Daminger 817

of constraint and narrowing their temporal what I need to do in my job, is get into the
horizon to the present. Their elision of gender nitty-gritty.” Describing his wife’s tendency to
as a causal force facilitated their acceptance plan the family’s weekends, Steve pointed out
of gender-traditional labor outcomes. a similar irony: “Oddly, I’m a project manager
by trade . . . [but] sometimes I don’t want to
An adaptable understanding of con- plan for the weekend.” These men, and their
straint.  The language of constraint domi- wives, saw themselves as constrained by their
nated respondents’ accounts of their labor “personality,” “nature,” or “character.” Yet
allocation: certain behaviors “made more these allegedly fixed traits appeared situa-
sense” or “just came more naturally” or were tional, with men in particular varying in pro-
“a function of how we are wired as people.” activity and attention to detail between work
For instance, when respondents offered and home. Few respondents were willing to
personality-based explanations for their labor dwell on such contradictions, however, per-
allocation, they emphasized the immutability haps because doing so might call the gender-
of these traits. “It’s just the way [my husband] neutrality of their expressive considerations
works, it’s just not the same as me,” mused into question.
Jackie when asked what she thought pre- Similarly, although respondents often
vented the couple from sharing the cognitive described their prioritization of efficiency as
load more equally. “[Our inequality] is sort of obvious or unavoidable when explaining why
a function of how we are wired as people,” their labor practices fell short of equality,
Sharon explained. Yet comparisons within examples abounded of inefficiencies tolerated
and across couples suggest apparent con- in service of goals other than equality. Several
straints were in fact context-dependent and couples, for instance, reported that the family
selectively overcome. calendar lived in one (typically female) part-
For example, the same male respondents ner’s head. Desiree, for instance, explained
noted for their reactivity, passivity, or laid- that “[my husband] comes to me all the time
back nature at home held demanding jobs as about making plans . . . to make sure nothing
management consultants, project managers, else is going on.” Repeated conversations
and physicians, among other occupations about the schedule seemed less efficient than
requiring high levels of executive function maintaining a shared external calendar, but
and proactive leadership. The traits that likely Desiree tolerated this suboptimal system
facilitate their professional success were because she believed her husband was forget-
somehow invisible—or not deployed—after- ful by nature. Other couples acknowledged
hours. Several respondents seemed to stumble behaving inefficiently in the service of
on this irony in the course of our conversation. strengthening the spousal relationship, better
Julian, a surgeon, noted that he can “go a very supporting a child’s needs, or even managing
long time before it hits me that now is the time their own emotions. Jackie reported she
to deal with [a problem],” such as a lightbulb “feel[s] weird even spending a couple bucks”
in need of replacing. Then he immediately without first checking with her husband, even
clarified his statement: “I mean, in the home though he would rather she “just make the
life—not, like, work.” Alan noted a similar decision myself,” because getting a second
paradox in his own relationship. “I’m the opinion helps calm her financial anxiety.
ideas guy,” he told me, and his wife is the Isaac admitted he and his wife “probably con-
“project manager” and the one to ask “what sult each other more than we need to,” which
are we going to do and how [would] that actu- “definitely takes more time,” because he felt
ally work, and like, the nitty-gritty.” Their joint decision-making was key to building a
arrangement is “funny,” he admitted, because shared life with a partner. Their ability to
Alan works full-time as a project manager for selectively override the efficiency or expres-
a large insurance company: “That’s partly sion constraints suggests other forces, likely
818 American Sociological Review 85(5)

including gender, shaped couples’ percep- doing, “just to give [my husband] that break.”
tions of their own agency. She also pointed out that being home less than
In addition to a given couple’s selective her husband was not an excuse for slacking:
adherence to constraints, comparison across “There’s a lot of that [cognitive labor] I can do
couples revealed varied perceptions of what while I’m not at home. . . . I can think about
counted as a constraint in the first place. stuff while I’m [commuting] on my bike, and
These perceptions differed based on the gen- during my lunch break or whatever.” Kelli, a
der of the party in question. For instance, software engineer, was similarly reluctant to
women who were out of the workforce, force her husband, who cares full-time for their
worked fewer paid labor hours than their part- children, to do tasks he does not enjoy just
ner, or worked from home often described because he is around to do them. “Roger hates
their greater domestic burden as an inevitable cooking, and I don’t mind it,” she told me. “So
consequence of their increased time in the when we end up at home, I usually cook. . . .
home. If the female partner’s job was more When I’m not home and Roger won’t cook,
demanding than her husband’s on one or [our children] learned to cook for themselves,
more dimensions, however, respondents did so they will make pasta or whatever.” Roger’s
not necessarily reach the same conclusions. greater availability during the pre-dinner hours
Joanna, for instance, was on an extended did not constrain the couple to one allocation
maternity leave, caring for her two children of labor; instead, everyone in the family
while her husband Isaac worked 40 hours per adapted.
week in marketing. Joanna argued that many In some couples, one partner worked
childcare responsibilities fell to her “by default, entirely or partially from home while the
because I’ve taken on the role of being with the other partner cared full-time for the couple’s
kids” and that the correlation between time at children. Here, too, the gender configuration
home and domestic responsibility was “natural shaped perceptions of constraint. For instance,
. . . and it shouldn’t be any other way.” Isaac Frank, a financial consultant, works from a
implied a similar perspective when he com- home office. Although he has considerable
plained about Joanna’s tendency to delay din- ability to shape his schedule—“If I want to
ner preparations until he returned from work: [get a haircut during the traditional workday],
“That’s frustrating. It’s not that I mind cooking I just have to clear my calendar at work and
dinner, it’s more that because dinner is not get it”—he argues he must maintain a strict
underway, then I feel like I should start helping barrier between job and family to keep his
with it. . . . Then I’m not going to spend much work-from-home setup viable. He cited a
time with the kids in the evening because I’m recent morning as an example:
making dinner.” Lisa, a stay-at-home mom,
shared Isaac’s sense that the primary bread- I said goodbye to everyone and went up to
winner’s limited time at home should not be work. Then I could hear the kids raising hell
wasted on routine chores: “It seems ridiculous downstairs and [my wife] trying to clean the
to me to have [my husband] put in a 10- or house because we have a cleaning lady coming
12-hour [work] day, and then say, ‘Hey, please, over. . . . So I knew it’s not going well. I just
now make the dinner.’” felt like I made this decision, and I’m just
However, in keeping with prior research on going to have to talk to [my wife] about it later.
female breadwinner households, women mar- I’m not going to stop working and go down
ried to stay-at-home fathers feared putting too there. I have a noise machine, I put it on, and I
much on their partner’s plate (Tichenor 2005). put some music on and just ignored [the noise].
Meg, who works full-time from an office while
her husband cares for their children at home, Gina works as a postdoc at an academic
reported that whenever she is present she tries research center, where she also has considera-
to do more than 50 percent of whatever needs ble latitude in determining where and when
Daminger 819

she works. Unlike Frank, who feels obligated requires me to do more” household labor,
to detach himself from domestic life while he Kristen explained. She handles the majority of
is on the clock as the primary breadwinner, daycare drop-offs and pick-ups, cares for their
Gina believes she must remain connected to toddler when he is sick and needs to come
family life, or chaos descends: “I tend to do the home early, and folds loads of laundry on con-
planning when [all three kids] are at home, and ference calls. Alan told a similar story: “Kris-
I tend to try to be around when they are all at ten does almost all the housework. . . . Partly
home. . . . Now that there are many more mov- because, 99 percent because, she’s home [and]
ing parts, I tend to take over a lot of the plan- she just does it.” The couple did not intend to
ning, because it is complicated.” She looked pursue a gender-traditional division of labor:
forward to leaving academia and starting a both Kristen and Alan said their ideal alloca-
“required 9:00 to 5:00 office position” with tion would be closer to 50/50. Given their
some trepidation: how would her husband respective commutes and time at home, how-
manage without her continual oversight? ever, they both saw Kristen’s greater domestic
The idea that circumstances or personality workload as all but inevitable.
differences dictated labor allocations facili- Yet the couple had considerable agency in
tated respondents’ belief that gendered out- shaping their circumstances. They moved to
comes could result from gender-neutral the suburbs to be closer to family, knowing
processes. Yet couples typically had more dis- Alan’s commute time would increase; now,
cretion than they recognized or acknowledged. they were in the process of buying a home
Whether and how they exercised that discre- even further from his office. Alan’s company
tion, however, depended in part on whether allows employees to work part-time from
their circumstances were nudging them in a home, but he did not report regularly taking
gender-traditional or nontraditional direction. advantage of this option. The couple had not
Constraints were more or less constraining forgotten their history; indeed, Kristen remi-
across contexts (home or work?), goals (equality nisced about a time when “things were more
or efficiency?), and employment configurations equal around the house . . . because we com-
(was the female or male partner’s job more muted together” and “took turns with every-
demanding?). Reframing gendered choices as thing.” Rather, they dissociated that history
constraints facilitated the status quo by absolv- from their current circumstances, as if the
ing couples of full responsibility for their non- couple making choices about where to live
egalitarian labor practices. and work was separate from the couple opti-
mizing for a particular set of constraints.
Narrowing temporal horizons. Much Other respondents showed a similar decou-
as gender shaped the options respondents per- pling when they asserted that one partner’s
ceived as viable in light of their current cir- more demanding profession limited his or her
cumstances, gender shaped the circumstances ability to contribute at home. Although true in
in which they found themselves in the first some sense, this telling obscured the reality
place. Respondents, however, treated their cir- that respondents were highly-educated,
cumstances as given, de-gendering their labor middle- to upper-middle-class, and privileged
patterns by obscuring the relationship between with some degree of occupational choice.
present conditions and prior choices. In most Furthermore, it overlooked the fact that
cases, those prior choices—where to purchase women often adapted their career trajectories
a home, which job to take, whether to invest in to accommodate their husband’s needs, ramp-
learning a particular skill—nudged couples ing their own work down to allow their hus-
down gender-traditional paths. band’s to ramp up. Kara, for instance,
Kristen, for instance, works from home, consistently works 36 hours per week outside
whereas her husband Alan commutes over an the home, while her husband Joel works long
hour each way. Working from home “sort of and unpredictable hours as a management
820 American Sociological Review 85(5)

consultant. “Realistically,” Joel argued, best practices or available options. Matteo


Kara’s shorter hours and greater flexibility marveled at his wife’s ability to track their
required her to manage tasks like their child- 1-year-old’s evolving diet: “I know the
care search and, later, communication with basics,” he told me, “but in terms of progress,
the nanny they selected. Joel’s narrative, like each month [our daughter] kind of
however, deemphasizes the fact that Kara changes in terms of what she can eat and can’t
requested and was granted a reduction in her eat and how you prepare it—I think [my wife]
work hours following their son’s birth. Leah has all the knowledge. I’m always sur-
Similarly, Julian attributed his limited prised with what she came up with . . .
household contributions to his long hours at because I would have never thought about it.”
the hospital where he works as a physician. At More broadly, Mateo believed Leah “will
the end of each day, he’s “fairly intellectually always be better at sensing what our daughter
exhausted—I don’t come home and think needs as a next developmental step. . . . She
about what needs to be arranged for child- will always be ahead of me.”
care.” Julian’s wife Nina left a job in finance Phil explained that his wife was in charge
to pursue a PhD, in part because she wanted of identifying formal childcare and extracur-
children and anticipated the challenges that ricular activities for their son because she was
would come with having two parents working more plugged into the Facebook groups and
demanding jobs. Now, she told me, her work community listservs where parenting issues
is much more “bendy” (i.e., flexible) than her were discussed. Asked what he would do if
husband’s: she can set her own hours and his wife stopped performing this work, Phil
stack her meetings and teaching commit- joked that he would “steal her iPad” to gain
ments to work from home several days per access to her social media contacts. Such
week. Although Nina sometimes gets frus- explanations focused on an existing knowl-
trated with their unequal workload, “The real- edge gap without considering its origin; often,
ity is his job requires more hours than mine, one partner invested in reading parenting
and less flexible hours.” books and joining online communities where
Joel’s and Julian’s professional demands information about local events circulated.
indeed outstripped their wives’ on some What began as a small discrepancy snow-
dimensions, but the two men presumably balled over time as one partner took on a
selected the grueling fields of management specialist role and the other increasingly
consulting and medicine with at least some deferred to their judgment.
knowledge of their demands. For their part, Women were more commonly depicted as
Kara and Nina returned to work only part-time skilled in the feminized realm of childcare,
after giving birth (Kara) and switched profes- but couples used similar logic to explain why
sions (Nina) with some expectation that they the male partner handled car maintenance,
would need greater flexibility to compensate long-term investing, or home repairs.
for their husbands’ sporadic availability. Both “Mechanically,” said Steve, “I think I have a
couples’ accounts emphasized making the best better sense of what needs to be done”; thus,
of the hand they were dealt, but they had in he is the one to fix whatever breaks and to
part dealt that hand to themselves, through a operate machinery such as lawn mowers and
series of decisions that implicitly treated the snow blowers. Heather described her husband
male partner’s career as a fixed point. Jeremy as “the fixer”: when their microwave
More subtly, some “given” circumstances malfunctions or car battery dies, Jeremy
were produced via a series of small invest- either fixes it or finds someone else to do the
ments that accumulated over time. For exam- job. “I typically take the lead on [repair]
ple, respondents commonly asserted that the things,” he told me. “I guess it’s [because] I
female partner managed elements of child- know more?” Respondents’ accounts focused
care because she had greater knowledge of on knowledge or skill gaps in the present
Daminger 821

moment, but those gaps were likely driven by a couple months, then decided to try until [the
years of investment—or lack thereof—in twins turned] 2 years old, and then decided to
learning about and practicing basic mainte- keep going. . . . . If [a career opportunity]
nance tasks. comes back up again [for Jonathan], then
The labor implications of certain choices we’ll just rearrange stuff.”
often appear predictable in hindsight: when a Gina, a social scientist whose husband Gar-
couple selected a childcare center across the rett stays at home with their three children,
street from one partner’s office, for instance, was especially clear about the foresight and
it could not be a surprise when this partner determination required to stick to the couple’s
subsequently managed pick-ups and drop- atypical path. When Gina entered a PhD pro-
offs. If one partner read every parenting book gram years earlier, she and Garrett agreed that
and blog she could find or spent her free time her success in academia would depend on her
watching DIY home maintenance videos, that ability to be “all-in” as a graduate student,
partner’s greater expertise—and likely greater rather than constantly pulled between research
responsibility for related household work— and caring for their toddler. Garrett’s part-
was almost a foregone conclusion. Respond- time, work-from-home job enabled him to
ents seldom referenced such foresight, serve as primary parent, but finances were
however. Instead, many described their labor tight. When he received a full-time job offer,
allocation as unplanned. Describing the tasks the economically prudent choice was obvious.
she and her husband gravitate toward, Brooke Yet Gina remembered anticipating the hidden
told me, “It’s not like we’ve said, ‘Oh, you costs of this extra income:
have to do this, and you have to do that.’ It’s
just naturally, over time . . . that’s how it’s He would have to leave [for the office] at 7
worked. . . . I just happen to be more with the in the morning, because this was LA, and he
kids while he does the lawn.” Referring to a would be home at 7pm—7:00-7:00. A nanny
recent shift in his and his wife’s respective would cost more than his salary. So what’s
childcare responsibilities, Douglas argued, “It going to happen then? What’s going to hap-
wasn’t anything explicit. You just go through pen then is my time is going to be consid-
a relationship and in life with some ebbs and ered more flexible. I’m going to be the one
flows. . . . I don’t think we talk about it as who has to be at home when my son is sick.
much.” “Once we became parents, it just was I’m going to be the one who has to pick him
natural to us, like responsibilities,” explained up when he needs to be picked up early.
Antoni. “We never talked about it; [our labor
allocation] just happened.” Although Garrett’s earnings would have out-
However, among the handful of couples stripped Gina’s graduate school stipend, he ulti-
with a nontraditional labor allocation in which mately declined the job to avoid shifting more of
the woman’s career took precedence, tempo- the childcare burden onto his wife. However,
ral horizons were often broader, and respond- Gina’s and Rebecca’s clear-eyed assessment of
ents’ personal agency was foregrounded. how their prior choices contributed to current
These couples more often described their circumstances, and how future choices might
current situation in contingent terms and create new circumstances, set them apart from
recalled critical junctures when they could the majority of the sample in much the same
have made a different choice. Soon after their way their employment configurations did.
twins were born, for instance, Jonathan left
his job as a lawyer to care for the children
Narrative Failure
full-time. His wife Rebecca, a surgeon,
described this arrangement as an experiment The combination of narrow temporal hori-
subject to ongoing evaluation: “We decided zons and a flexible understanding of con-
during [my maternity leave] that Jonathan straint facilitated the de-gendering process: it
would try [staying] at home. And so, he tried allowed couples to acknowledge inequalities
822 American Sociological Review 85(5)

in their labor arrangements without attribut- pickups and drop-offs—but suggested limits
ing them to the gender-traditional ideology to how far he would go. Getting to 50/50 on
they disavowed. Because their inequality was this particular task would “require a signifi-
not “about” gender, and thus not in conflict cant overhaul” of their lives, he argued, and
with their egalitarian identities, de-gendering potentially hurt them financially. For these
helped preserve marital harmony. This rela- two couples, the problem was not the magni-
tionship is perhaps best illustrated by its tude of their perceived inequality—indeed,
absence: when one partner veered off-script they were far from the most extreme in the
and insisted on connecting inequality to gen- sample on that dimension. Rather, the sugges-
der, conflict loomed. tion that the inequality they did experience
Stephanie, for example, told me she’d was gendered, or would be if Carla and Steph-
recently read about “how, in the U.S., even if anie dropped their guard, was a larger threat to
both husband and wife work equal time, the marital harmony.
wife still does more than 50 percent of the
household work. I just think that’s really
unfair. And so I definitely want our family to Discussion and
be—to not contribute to that statistic. I’ve Conclusions
worked really hard to make sure that we’re Summary of the Argument
equitable.” Even though, as a graduate stu- and Findings
dent, Stephanie’s schedule was more flexible
than her software engineer husband’s, she This article examined the stalled and uneven
insisted they split pickups and drop-offs gender revolution from the vantage point of
equally and that Carl handle most of the individual couples. It asked how a couple
cooking. Stephanie was proud of this arrange- might acknowledge that their unequal house-
ment: “We’re equitable,” she argued, “because hold labor practices fall short of their egalitar-
I put in all this work to get that way.” Yet Carl ian ideals yet experience little conflict over
seemed frustrated by his wife’s vigilance, their labor allocation. I argue that egalitarian
which he implied was a waste of time. Steph- couples come to terms with gendered alloca-
anie “will often accuse me of not doing as tion outcomes by “de-gendering” allocation
much,” he said. “If I neglect my portion [of processes. They explain their labor allocation
the chores], or kind of either on purpose or as the result of inevitable, gender-neutral
inadvertently neglect starting laundry . . . then compromises made among competing imper-
she’ll either do it or she often reminds me to atives. For cognitive labor tasks, equality
do it.” Stephanie’s insistence on gendering largely competes with self-expression, as
the couple’s actions despite her husband’s many respondents argue that the cognitive
contrasting perspective set them up for ongo- work they do and the way they do it is inex-
ing tension, and occasional conflict, over tricably tied to who they are as individuals. In
their labor allocation. the realm of physical labor, equality is in
Similarly, Robert described his wife Carla competition with both expression and effi-
as “particularly conscious of [the division of ciency, as current circumstances are said to
labor], in not wanting to sort of fall into the render a particular division of labor the most
stereotyped gender roles.” She did not want to practical. Regardless of the specific explana-
be solely responsible for transporting their tion offered, the net effect is to reassure cou-
daughter to and from daycare “even though ples that they are not necessarily enacting
she could do that,” he said, because she wanted societal gender norms or revealing their own
it to be “a shared responsibility.” Robert hidden sexism if they happen to behave in
seemed willing to humor his wife’s request— gender-traditional ways.
he managed to configure his work schedule to Viewed narrowly, at the level of an indi-
allow him to do 30 to 40 percent of the weekly vidual couple operating at a single point in
Daminger 823

time, these compromise narratives are com- effectively constrained to act in accordance
pelling: it may well be more efficient for the with these tendencies.
partner whose office is closer to the daycare My data suggest that what is seen as
center to do most pick-ups, or for the more choice and as constraint depends in part on
organized partner to manage the family calen- the interaction of context and gender, consist-
dar, regardless of their gender. However, ent with prior research suggesting men’s paid
when we expand the aperture to include each work is often perceived as non-negotiable and
couple’s past alongside their present and to women’s as supplementary or voluntary—
compare the experiences of couples facing and vice versa for their unpaid labor (Dam-
similar circumstances, gender comes back aske 2011; Killewald and García-Manglano
into view. Respondents treat the broad out- 2016). The demands of paid work, for
lines of their life as given, but their current instance, appear to constrain male and female
circumstances are the product of a long line of respondents to different degrees: at-home
choices about what to study, where to live, mothers are expected to do more domestic
what work to do, and which childcare option work than comparable at-home fathers. And
to pursue. Intentionally or not, these prior men whose organizational skills are deemed
choices tend to render gender-traditional rou- subpar in the domestic setting nevertheless
tines the most comfortable and efficient. manage large teams and oversee sizable
budgets at the office. When pressed, a few of
these men admit they turn off or tone down
Implications
the relevant aspects of their personality and
Respondents often use the language of con- skill set when they arrive home. Women, it
straint to account for their behavior, at first seems, do not have—or do not perceive they
glance contradicting findings from earlier have—such a choice.
research. Stone (2007), for instance, demon- Adaptable understandings of choice and
strates that high-achieving women rely heav- constraint prevent respondents from viewing
ily on “choice rhetoric” to explain why they their experiences through a gender lens. They
left the workforce to care for children full- may acknowledge their unequal labor out-
time. Similarly, Lamont (2020) finds that comes, but they erase any connection to gender
heterosexual men and women accept some ideology. This “de-gendering” likely contrib-
degree of gender-traditionalism when they utes to the noted intractability of gender ine-
can recast it as a matter of personal prefer- quality in different-sex romantic relationships
ence. My work builds on these findings to (England 2010). In previous eras, gender was
show how choice and constraint are two sides more widely acknowledged as a heuristic for
of the same coin: the “constraints” posed by a labor allocation (Bolzendahl and Myers 2004;
husband’s job, for instance, may lead a wife Scarborough et al. 2019). This ideology has
to “choose” to take on more household labor eroded in recent years, as younger generations
or decelerate her career (see also Gerson aspire to a new, more involved fatherhood and
1985). Personal agency and circumstantial a romantic partnership in which breadwinning
imperative are narratives that can be mixed and homemaking are shared by both partners
and matched to obscure the role of gender in (Cooper 2000; Gerson 2011; LaRossa 1988).
shaping paid and unpaid labor patterns. These Nevertheless, behaviors have remained rela-
respondents appealed to personal agency, for tively stagnant (Bianchi et al. 2000, 2012). An
instance, when they described their commit- optimistic assessment of this disjuncture is that
ment to self-expression: their preferences and behavioral change lags behind attitudinal
personality traits, they said, led them to take change; progress in the former augurs eventual
on certain chores. At the same time, they progress in the latter (Gershuny, Godwin, and
implied that because these characteristics Jones 1994). However, my findings remind us
were core to who they were, they were that attitudinal change alone is insufficient.
824 American Sociological Review 85(5)

When couples reduce cognitive dissonance by gender structure remain intact: men still
reframing rather than reforming their behavior, choose different college majors, enter more
they may inadvertently send gender “under- lucrative and less flexible occupations, and
ground,” where it can operate unchecked (Bea- face strong societal pressures to provide for
gan et al. 2008). Household labor imbalances their families (Cha 2013; Charles and Bradley
that are “about” efficiency or personality or an 2009; Riggs 1997). Women are still trained
“inevitable” result of circumstantial constraints from a young age to attend to others’ needs
are more difficult for egalitarians to contest and feelings; held more accountable for
than imbalances attributed to sexist ideas about domestic outcomes; and concentrated in fam-
men’s and women’s roles. ily-friendly—and less remunerative—occupa-
My analysis of the aggregate data shows tions (Charles and Grusky 2004; Erickson
the couples sampled here were not, as their 1993; Harman and Cappellini 2014; Thébaud,
narratives implied, operating independently of Kornrich, and Ruppanner 2019). Cultural
the gender structure (Risman 2004). They imperatives to “intensively” parent clash with
were neither “undoing” gender, in the sense of employers’ expectations of their “ideal work-
challenging or dismantling gender inequality ers,” making it difficult for one person to
(Deutsch 2007), nor enacting Lorber’s (2000) compete in both arenas (Becker and Moen
vision of a “feminist degendering movement.” 1999; Blair-Loy 2005; Hays 1998; Reid 2015).
Lorber envisioned what we might call a By de-gendering their labor allocation, cou-
“deep” de-gendering, in which gender differ- ples open a path through the contradictions of
ence is no longer the axis around which pat- our incomplete gender revolution, exerting
terns of caregiving and breadwinning turn. In control at the narrative level even as more
contrast, my respondents practice a “shallow” fundamental changes remain unrealized.
de-gendering, in which long-standing behav- De-gendering also appears to preserve mar-
iors are simply relabeled while the underlying ital harmony, perhaps serving as a twenty-first-
gendered patterns remain intact. century form of family myth-making
The fact that gender continued to shape (Hochschild and Machung 2012). The story
respondents’ practices despite their disavowal that keeps the peace is not one about overlook-
of its influence does not mean couples were ing inequality, as Hochschild and Machung
deluded or oblivious. Rather, gender’s effects found in the 1980s; instead, it is about acknowl-
were clearest at the aggregate level. Individ- edging inequality and denying its connection
ual couples cited differences in each partner’s to gendered forces. Stephanie’s and Carla’s
employment circumstances (hours worked, insistence on gendering household labor allo-
distance from home, level of flexibility) or cation processes may serve as a cautionary
personality traits to explain why one of them tale. Their husbands bristled at the implication
shouldered a heavier domestic load or took on they were less than egalitarian and appeared to
particular tasks. When those factors pointed a resent the occasions when they were asked to
couple toward a traditional labor allocation, it practice equality for its own sake. A gendered
was easy to write off as a coincidentally- understanding risked pitting husband against
gendered consequence of gender-neutral pro- wife, and a de-gendered understanding pitted
cesses. Only when those factors consistently the couple against their circumstances or
align with gender-traditionalism, as was the natures. Neither approach facilitated true
case across these respondents, does their equality, but the de-gendered version at least
gender-neutrality come into question. promoted peaceful cohabitation.
Couples’ eagerness to de-gender their labor In place of gender, I show that the majority
allocation process may be better understood as of couples understand their labor allocation
a form of adaptation than of self-deception. processes as a series of tradeoffs, including
Although ideals have changed considerably in between personal expression and spousal
recent years, many other elements of the equality. These findings connect to prior
Daminger 825

research on the relationship between gender dents’ narratives over time and through dis-
inequality and personal identity: because self- tinct life stages.
concepts are highly gendered, women and Another important limitation is the homo-
men often express themselves in distinct geneity of the sample. By design, the couples
ways, exacerbating gender segregation as interviewed were highly educated; uninten-
they self-select into different majors and tionally, the majority were also white and
occupational fields (Cech 2013). My research well-off. Thus, the results presented here
extends this theory into the domestic sphere should not be interpreted as representative of
by connecting household labor patterns—par- U.S. couples’ experiences. Class in particular
ticularly cognitive tasks—to individuals’ is associated with gender ideology; educa-
attempts to act in accordance with their invis- tional attainment is positively correlated with
ibly gendered identities. It also hints at the egalitarian beliefs (Bolzendahl and Myers
interaction between self-concept and context: 2004; Davis and Greenstein 2009; Scarbor-
particularly for men in the sample, paid and ough et al. 2019). Social class also shapes the
unpaid work appeared to activate different range of behavioral possibilities open to a
sets of skills or traits. In the feminized space couple. For instance, in describing the connec-
of domestic life, male respondents were com- tions between respondents’ prior choices and
monly depicted as laid-back, reactive, and current circumstances, I assume they enjoyed
disorganized. Yet these same men worked in considerable agency in their choices of occu-
occupations that reward assertiveness, proac- pation and employer, whether both partners
tivity, and organization. These findings sug- would work full-time, and whether they would
gest gender shapes both one’s sense of self allocate extra money toward paid service-
and the particular aspects of that self deemed providers (e.g., a nanny or house-cleaner) or
relevant in a given context. perform all housework themselves. Couples
with fewer resources are likely more con-
strained in their decision-making. It is notable,
Limitations and Suggestions
then, that although these respondents are argu-
for Future Research
ably among the least constrained in their paid
Despite these important results, several study work options and the most ideologically com-
limitations and unanswered questions suggest mitted to equality, they nevertheless struggle
avenues for future research. For instance, the to enact egalitarian ideals, and they explain
recruitment method and exclusion criteria that struggle in terms of constraint.
used here affect the generalizability of the Finally, future studies should take a closer
results. First, I required both members of a look at couples with a nontraditional labor
couple to participate in individual interviews. allocation. Consistent with data showing the
This requirement may have screened out persistence of gender gaps in household labor
people experiencing significant marital con- performance (Bianchi et al. 2012), the major-
flict, as such couples would presumably be ity of couples in this study reported that the
more reluctant to volunteer information about female partner, to varying degrees, takes on a
co-parenting and household affairs. Because greater share of physical and cognitive labor,
this article examines the processes that enable even though I oversampled for couples in
couples to avoid conflict, this limitation which the wife was employed full-time and
should not diminish the argument presented the husband was not. Throughout the article,
here. However, comparative research on cou- I highlighted distinctions between the tradi-
ples with a wider range of satisfaction levels tional and nontraditional groups, including
could establish how these respondents fit into the latter’s increased tendency to describe
the larger population of U.S. couples. Longi- their arrangements as contingent and con-
tudinal data tracking couples over a period of nected to deliberate choices. However, the
years could help disentangle cause and effect, narratives of traditional and nontraditional
in addition to assessing the stability of respon- couples had much in common, including a
826 American Sociological Review 85(5)

tendency to seek efficiency in household Notes


operations and to take personality into account   1. The cited figures represent average gender gaps and
as they divvied up labor. This resemblance undoubtedly mask variation in couples’ practices.
raises questions about the circumstances in For instance, recent research on dual-earner couples
with children indicates that approximately 9 percent
which similar desires and demographics lead of couples fall into a “parallel” group in which men
couples down very different pathways. The and women contribute approximately equally to
patterns identified here should be verified in a paid and unpaid labor (Hall and MacDermid 2009).
larger, more diverse sample of couples.   2. Gender ideology is a multifaceted construct (Davis
Continuing to close the gap between egali- and Greenstein 2009; Knight and Brinton 2017;
Scarborough et al. 2019). In this article, I define an
tarian ideals and non-egalitarian behaviors egalitarian as someone who supports gender equality
will likely require change at multiple levels. in both the public and private sphere and disavows
For couples, change may mean critically the idea that men and women are equal but funda-
examining assumptions and imagining new mentally distinct in their preferences and skills.
possibilities. In the short term, such a reckon-   3. My usage of “de-gendering” closely resembles Ber-
ns’s (2001). Berns (2001:265) describes a “patri-
ing might prompt a “re-gendering” of alloca- archal resistance discourse” surrounding domestic
tion processes and generate dissatisfaction violence that “degenders” the issue “by removing
and conflict of the sort the stably unequal gender from the framing of the problem.”
couples in this sample largely avoided. Over   4. One couple was unmarried but had been cohabiting
time, however, one would hope the benefits for four years.
  5. One participant completed 3.5 years of college but
of improved alignment between beliefs and did not receive a degree.
behaviors would outweigh the transition   6. I asked participants to express their ideal and actual
costs. Significant change is also needed at the allocations in percentage terms, but some offered
societal level: in how we raise boys and girls a qualitative rather than numeric assessment (e.g.,
and cultivate their sense of self; in continued things are “pretty even” or “she does most [of the
cognitive labor]”) or reported a domain-by-domain
efforts to reduce occupational segregation breakdown rather than an overall assessment.
and wage discrimination; in the supports pro- Despite variation in the form of their responses,
vided working parents; and in checks on the most participants made clear the direction and rela-
continued expansion of work hours. Only via tive size of their perceived labor imbalance.
concerted effort on multiple fronts will we  7. I did not directly ask respondents about their
relationship satisfaction, in part because I was
restart the stalled gender revolution. concerned I would be unlikely to get an accurate
response and in part because relationship satisfac-
tion was not central to the questions that originally
Acknowledgments
animated this research. Instead, I infer relationship
The author thanks Mary Brinton, Kathleen Gerson, satisfaction via indirect clues, including respon-
Hanna Katz, Eric Leventhal, the ASR editors and anony- dents’ answers to questions about recent conflict,
mous reviewers, and members of several Harvard Uni- previous attempts to change their division of labor,
versity seminars for their feedback on prior drafts of this and the factors (if any) they believe prevent them
research. She is especially grateful to Alexandra Kille- from achieving their ideal labor allocation.
wald and Jocelyn Viterna for their ongoing guidance
throughout the research and writing process.
References
Aud, Susan, William Hussar, Michael Planty, Thomas
Funding Snyder, Kevin Bianco, Mary Ann Fox, Lauren
This research was supported by a Small Research Grant Frohlich, Jana Kemp, and Lauren Drake. 2010. The
from the Weatherhead Initiative on Gender Inequality at Condition of Education 2010. (NCES 2010-028)
Harvard University and by the James M. and Cathleen D. National Center for Education Statistics, Institute of
Stone Ph.D. Scholars in Inequality and Wealth Concen- Education Sciences, U.S. Department of Education.
tration Fellowship. Bass, Brooke Conroy. 2015. “Preparing for Parenthood?
Gender, Aspirations, and the Reproduction of Labor
Market Inequality.” Gender and Society 29(3):362–
ORCID iD 85.
Allison Daminger https://orcid.org/0000-0002-9706 Beagan, Brenda, Gwen E. Chapman, Andrea D’Sylva,
-1672 and B. Raewyn Bassett. 2008. “‘It’s Just Easier for
Daminger 827

Me to Do It’: Rationalizing the Family Division of Damaske, Sarah. 2011. For the Family? How Class and
Foodwork.” Sociology 42(4):653–71. Gender Shape Women’s Work. New York: Oxford
Becker, Gary. 1993. A Treatise on the Family. Cam- University Press.
bridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Daminger, Allison. 2019. “The Cognitive Dimension of
Becker, Penny Edgell, and Phyllis Moen. 1999. “Scal- Household Labor.” American Sociological Review
ing Back: Dual-Earner Couples’ Work-Family 84(4):609–33.
Strategies.” Journal of Marriage and Family Daniels, Arlene Kaplan. 1987. “Invisible Work.” Social
61(4):995–1007. Problems 34(5):403–15.
Berns, Nancy. 2001. “Degendering the Problem and Gen- Davis, Shannon N., and Theodore N. Greenstein. 2009.
dering the Blame: Political Discourse on Women and “Gender Ideology: Components, Predictors, and Con-
Violence.” Gender & Society 15(2):262–81. sequences.” Annual Review of Sociology 35(1):87–
Bianchi, Suzanne M., Melissa A. Milkie, Liana C. Sayer, 105.
and John P. Robinson. 2000. “Is Anyone Doing the DeMaris, Alfred, and Monica A. Longmore. 1996. “Ide-
Housework? Trends in the Gender Division of House- ology, Power, and Equity: Testing Competing Expla-
hold Labor.” Social Forces 79(1):191–228. nations for the Perception of Fairness in Household
Bianchi, Suzanne M., Liana C. Sayer, Melissa A. Milkie, Labor.” Social Forces 74(3):1043–71.
and John P. Robinson. 2012. “Housework: Who Did, Deutsch, Francine M. 2000. Halving It All: How Equally
Does or Will Do It, and How Much Does It Matter?” Shared Parenting Works, revised ed. Cambridge,
Social Forces 91(1):55–63. MA: Harvard University Press.
Bittman, Michael, and Judy Wajcman. 2000. “The Rush Deutsch, Francine M. 2007. “Undoing Gender.” Gender
Hour: The Character of Leisure Time and Gender and Society 21(1):106–27.
Equity.” Social Forces 79(1):165–89. DeVault, Marjorie L. 1999. “Comfort and Struggle: Emo-
Blair-Loy, Mary. 2005. Competing Devotions: Career tion Work in Family Life.” ANNALS of the American
and Family among Women Executives. Cambridge, Academy of Political and Social Science 561(1):52–63.
MA: Harvard University Press. Emens, Elizabeth F. 2019. Life Admin: How I Learned
Blau, Francine D., and Lawrence M. Kahn. 2017. “The to Do Less, Do Better, and Live More. New York:
Gender Wage Gap: Extent, Trends, and Explana- Houghton Mifflin Harcourt.
tions.” Journal of Economic Literature 55(3):789– England, Paula. 2010. “The Gender Revolution.” Gender
865. and Society 24(2):149–66.
Bolzendahl, Catherine I., and Daniel J. Myers. 2004. Erickson, Rebecca J. 1993. “Reconceptualizing Family
“Feminist Attitudes and Support for Gender Equality: Work: The Effect of Emotion Work on Perceptions
Opinion Change in Women and Men, 1974–1998.” of Marital Quality.” Journal of Marriage and Family
Social Forces 83(2):759–90. 55(4):888–900.
Cech, Erin A. 2013. “The Self-Expressive Edge of Occu- Festinger, Leon. 1957. A Theory of Cognitive Disso-
pational Sex Segregation.” American Journal of Soci- nance. Palo Alto, CA: Stanford University Press.
ology 119(3):747–89. Fuwa, Makiko. 2004. “Macro-Level Gender Inequality
Cha, Youngjoo. 2013. “Overwork and the Persistence of and the Division of Household Labor in 22 Coun-
Gender Segregation in Occupations.” Gender and tries.” American Sociological Review 69(6):751–67.
Society 27(2):158–84. Gager, Constance T. 2008. “What’s Fair Is Fair? Role of
Charles, Maria, and Karen Bradley. 2009. “Indulging Our Justice in Family Labor Allocation Decisions.” Mar-
Gendered Selves? Sex Segregation by Field of Study riage and Family Review 44(4):511–45.
in 44 Countries.” American Journal of Sociology Gershuny, Jonathan, Michael Godwin, and Sally Jones.
114(4):924–76. 1994. “The Domestic Labour Revolution: A Process
Charles, Maria, and David B. Grusky. 2004. Occupa- of Lagged Adaptation?” Pp. 151–97 in The Social
tional Ghettos: The Worldwide Segregation of Women and Political Economy of the Household, edited by
and Men. Palo Alto, CA: Stanford University Press. M. Anderson, F. Bechhofer, and J. Gershuny. Cam-
Coleman, James S. 1990. Foundations of Social Theory. bridge, UK: Oxford University Press.
Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Gerson, Kathleen. 1985. Hard Choices: How Women
Coltrane, Scott. 1996. Family Man: Fatherhood, House- Decide about Work, Career and Motherhood. Berke-
work, and Gender Equality. Oxford, UK: Oxford ley: University of California Press.
University Press. Gerson, Kathleen. 2002. “Moral Dilemmas, Moral Strat-
Coltrane, Scott. 2000. “Research on Household Labor: egies, and the Transformation of Gender: Lessons
Modeling and Measuring the Social Embeddedness from Two Generations of Work and Family Change.”
of Routine Family Work.” Journal of Marriage and Gender and Society 16(1):8–28.
Family 62(4):1208–33. Gerson, Kathleen. 2011. The Unfinished Revolution:
Cooper, Marianne. 2000. “Being the ‘Go-To Guy’: Coming of Age in a New Era of Gender, Work, and
Fatherhood, Masculinity, and the Organization of Family. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
Work in Silicon Valley.” Qualitative Sociology Hall, Scott S., and Shelley M. MacDermid. 2009. “A
23(4):379–405. Typology of Dual Earner Marriages Based on Work
828 American Sociological Review 85(5)

and Family Arrangements.” Journal of Family and Mannino, Clelia Anna, and Francine M. Deutsch. 2007.
Economic Issues 30(3):215–25. “Changing the Division of Household Labor: A
Harman, Vicki, and Benedetta Cappellini. 2014. “Moth- Negotiated Process between Partners.” Sex Roles
ers on Display: Lunchboxes, Social Class and Moral 56(5–6):309–24.
Accountability.” Sociology 49(4):764–81. Martin, Patricia Yancey. 2004. “Gender as Social Institu-
Harmon-Jones, Eddie, Cindy Harmon-Jones, and Nicho- tion.” Social Forces 82(4):1249–73.
las Levy. 2015. “An Action-Based Model of Cogni- Milkie, Melissa A., Suzanne M. Bianchi, Marybeth J.
tive-Dissonance Processes.” Current Directions in Mattingly, and John P. Robinson. 2002. “Gendered
Psychological Science 24(3):184–89. Division of Childrearing: Ideals, Realities, and the
Hays, Sharon. 1998. The Cultural Contradictions of Relationship to Parental Well-Being.” Sex Roles
Motherhood, revised ed. New Haven, CT: Yale Uni- 47(1–2):21–38.
versity Press. Nyman, Charlott, Lasse Reinikainen, and Kristina Eriks-
Hochschild, Arlie, and Anne Machung. 2012. The Second son. 2018. “The Tension between Gender Equality
Shift: Working Families and the Revolution at Home, and Doing Gender: Swedish Couples’ Talk about the
revised ed. New York: Penguin Books. Division of Housework.” Women’s Studies Interna-
van Hooff, Jenny H. 2011. “Rationalising Inequality: tional Forum 68:36–46.
Heterosexual Couples’ Explanations and Justifica- Offer, Shira. 2014. “The Costs of Thinking about Work
tions for the Division of Housework along Tradition- and Family: Mental Labor, Work-Family Spillover,
ally Gendered Lines.” Journal of Gender Studies and Gender Inequality among Parents in Dual-Earner
20(1):19–30. Families.” Sociological Forum 29(4):916–36.
Horne, Rebecca M., Matthew D. Johnson, Nancy L. Pedulla, David S., and Sarah Thébaud. 2015. “Can We
Galambos, and Harvey J. Krahn. 2018. “Time, Money, Finish the Revolution? Gender, Work-Family Ideals,
or Gender? Predictors of the Division of Household and Institutional Constraint.” American Sociological
Labour across Life Stages.” Sex Roles 78:731–43. Review 80(1):116–39.
Juhn, Chinhui, and Simon Potter. 2006. “Changes in Reid, Erin. 2015. “Embracing, Passing, Revealing,
Labor Force Participation in the United States.” Jour- and the Ideal Worker Image: How People Navigate
nal of Economic Perspectives 20(3):27–46. Expected and Experienced Professional Identities.”
Katz-Wise, Sabra L., Heather A. Priess, and Janet S. Organization Science 26(4):997–1017.
Hyde. 2010. “Gender-Role Attitudes and Behavior Ridgeway, Cecilia L., and Shelley J. Correll. 2004.
across the Transition to Parenthood.” Developmental “Unpacking the Gender System: A Theoretical Per-
Psychology 46(1):18–28. spective on Gender Beliefs and Social Relations.”
Killewald, Alexandra, and Javier García-Manglano. Gender and Society 18(4):510–31.
2016. “Tethered Lives: A Couple-Based Perspective Riggs, Janet Morgan. 1997. “Mandates for Mothers and
on the Consequences of Parenthood for Time Use, Fathers: Perceptions of Breadwinners and Care Giv-
Occupation, and Wages.” Social Science Research ers.” Sex Roles 37(7/8):565–80.
60:266–82. Risman, Barbara J. 2004. “Gender as a Social Structure:
Knight, Carly R., and Mary C. Brinton. 2017. “One Egal- Theory Wrestling with Activism.” Gender & Society
itarianism or Several? Two Decades of Gender-Role 18(4):429–50.
Attitude Change in Europe.” American Journal of Risman, Barbara J., and Danette Johnson-Sumerford.
Sociology 122(5):1485–532. 1998. “Doing It Fairly: A Study of Postgender Mar-
Lachance-Grzela, Mylène, and Geneviève Bouchard. riages.” Journal of Marriage and Family 60(1):23–40.
2010. “Why Do Women Do the Lion’s Share of Sayer, Liana C. 2005. “Gender, Time and Inequality:
Housework? A Decade of Research.” Sex Roles Trends in Women’s and Men’s Paid Work, Unpaid
63(11–12):767–80. Work and Free Time.” Social Forces 84(1):285–303.
Lamont, Ellen. 2014. “Negotiating Courtship: Recon- Scarborough, William J., Ray Sin, and Barbara Risman.
ciling Egalitarian Ideals with Traditional Gender 2019. “Attitudes and the Stalled Gender Revolution:
Norms.” Gender and Society 28(2):189–211. Egalitarianism, Traditionalism, and Ambivalence
Lamont, Ellen. 2020. The Mating Game: How Gender from 1977 through 2016.” Gender and Society
Still Shapes How We Date. Berkeley: University of 33(2):173–200.
California Press. Smith, Herbert L., Constance T. Gager, and S. Philip
LaRossa, Ralph. 1988. “Fatherhood and Social Change.” Morgan. 1998. “Identifying Underlying Dimensions
Family Relations 37(4):451–57. in Spouses’ Evaluations of Fairness in the Divi-
Lee, Yun-Suk, and Linda J. Waite. 2005. “Husbands’ sion of Household Labor.” Social Science Research
and Wives’ Time Spent on Housework: A Compari- 27(3):305–27.
son of Measures.” Journal of Marriage and Family Smith, Tom W., Michael Davern, Jeremy Freese, and
67(2):328–36. Stephen Morgan. 2018. “General Social Surveys,
Lorber, Judith. 2000. “Using Gender to Undo Gender: A 1972–2018 [Machine-Readable Data File].” National
Feminist Degendering Movement.” Feminist Theory Science Foundation, NORC at the University of Chi-
1(1):79–95. cago [producer and distributor].
Daminger 829

Stone, Pamela. 2007. Opting Out? Why Women Really Tichenor, Veronica Jaris. 2005. Earning More and Get-
Quit Careers and Head Home. Berkeley: University ting Less: Why Successful Wives Can’t Buy Equality.
of California Press. New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University Press.
Thébaud, Sarah, Sabino Kornrich, and Leah Rup-
panner. 2019. “Good Housekeeping, Great Allison Daminger is a PhD candidate in Sociology and
Expectations: Gender and Housework Norms.” Social Policy at Harvard University. She uses qualitative
Sociological Methods & Research (https://doi.org/ methods to examine gender and class inequality in the
10.1177/0049124119852395). context of family life.

You might also like