You are on page 1of 23

Ps)Chological Bulletin Copyright I986b> the American Ps>thological Association. Inc.

1986, Vol. 100. No. 1.78-100 0033-2909/86/S00.75

Gaze and Eye Contact: A Research Review

Chris L. Kleinke
University of Alaska, Anchorage

Research on gaze and eye contact was organized within the framework of Patterson's (1982) sequen-
tial functional model of nonverbal exchange. Studies were reviewed showing how gaze functions to
(a) provide information, (b) regulate interaction, (c) express intimacy, (d) exercise social control,
and (e) facilitate service and task goals. Research was also summarized that describes personal,
experiential, relational, and situational antecedents of gaze and reactions to gaze. Directions were
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

given for a functional analysis of the relation between gaze and physiological responses. Attribution
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

theories were integrated into the sequential model for making predictions about people's perceptions
of their own gazing behavior and the gazing behavior of others. Data on people's accuracy in report-
ing their own and others' gaze were presented and integrated with related findings in attribution
research. The sequential model was used to analyze research studies measuring the interaction be-
tween gaze and personal and contextual variables. Methodological and measurement issues were
discussed and directions were outlined for future research.

These lovely lamps, these windows of the soul. (Du Bartas) 1 The area of "gaze research" has been plagued by the accumulation
of miscellaneous empirical bits and facts, replete with ad hoc (and
The eyes have one language everywhere. (George Herbert)
often post hoc) interpretations of specific outcomes. There have
There are often voice and words in a silent look. (Ovid) been complaints that the field cannot really go anywhere unless it
is attached to the broader domain of non-verbal communication or
For eyes can speak and eyes can understand. (Chapman)
linked up with relevant theories of interaction and communication.

The significance of the eyes in human relationships has fasci-


Because of the need for systematizing gaze and eye contact re-
nated writers and philosophers for centuries. Tomkins (1963)
search into a theoretical context (see Kleinke, 1972), this will be
provided an historical review of writings on this topic from the
a major goal of this article. Patterson's (1982, 1983) sequential
times of ancient civilizations. Other writers who are acknowl-
model of nonverbal exchange offers the most promising frame-
edged for their insightful descriptions of human visual interac-
work for this task and is used to provide the structure for this
tions include Goffman (1964), Ortega y Gasset (1957), Sartre
review. Patterson's (1976) intimacy-arousal model is also dis-
(1942/1956), and Simmel (1921). As a topic in psychological
cussed and evaluated. An attributional approach is presented
research, human visual behavior is relatively new. Eye contact
for studying gaze and eye contact, within the context of the se-
did not appear as a reference in Psychological Abstracts until
quential model and in relation to self-awareness of one's own
1966, and there were only 13 references to eye contact between
gazing behavior and awareness of gaze by others. Because the
1966 and 1972. During these years, the publication of several
sequential model is new, the organization of gaze and eye con-
classic articles set the stage for a burgeoning interest in eye con-
tact studies within this model is a matter of judgment, and some
tact research (Argyle & Dean, 1965; Ellsworth & Carlsmith,
flexibility is needed to fit the myriad gaze and eye contact stud-
1968; Exline, 1963, 1971; Kendon. 1967). In 1973, there were
ies into this article. In preparation for the following review of
31 references to eye contact in Psychological Abstracts and in
gaze and eye contact research, it will be useful to consider some
1978 the number reached 67. Research on visual behavior con-
definitions and measurement issues.
tinues to appear in the psychological literature at a steady rate
and research reviews on this topic during the past 10 years have
contained an ever-increasing number of citations (Argyle & Some Definitions
Cook, 1976; Ellsworth & Ludwig, 1972; Harper, Wiens, & Ma- Harper etal. (1978, p. 173) outlined a number of definitions
tarazzo, 1978; Kleinke. 1975; Knapp, 1978). of visual behavior that were originally suggested by von Cra-
This review is presented to bring the research literature on nach (1971). Face-gaze is the direction of one person's gaze at
human visual behavior up-to-date and to analyze theoretical another's face, and eye-gaze is the direction of one's gaze at an-
issues and research directions that have become important dur- other's eyes. Mutual gaze refers to two people gazing at each
ing the last several years. The current state of research on hu- other's faces, and eye contact defines two people gazing at each
man visual behavior was aptly described by an anonymous re- other's eyes. Looking and gazing refer more generally to a gaze
viewer of this article: in the direction of another's face. Gaze avoidance is defined as
intentional avoidance of eye contact and gaze omission is de-
fined as a failure to look without the intention of avoiding eye
contact. Staring is defined as a gaze or look that persists regard-
I am grateful to Betty J. House for her helpful editorial comments.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Chris
L. Kleinke, Psychology Department, University of Alaska, Anchorage.
1
Alaska 99508. This and the following quotations are from Stevenson (1967).

78
GAZE AND EYE CONTACT 79

less of the behavior of the other person (Ellsworth, Carlsmith, measurements of gaze. However, it appears that outside of very
& Henson, 1972). As I show later, these definitions require more special viewing conditions, the discrimination between eye-
accuracy than is present in most research studies because it is gaze and face-gaze is not easily made (Argyle & Cook, 1976. p.
relatively difficult for people to discriminate between face-gaze 41; Harper etal., 1978. p. 177).
and eye contact. In this review, therefore, gaze, looking, glance.
and staring imply the direction of one's gaze toward another's Validity
face and (possibly) eyes.2 Mutual ga:e and eye contact imply
that two people are simultaneously looking at each other's face Studies comparing recordings of gaze by observers with re-
and (possibly) eyes. cordings made by recipients have found agreement proportions
ranging from .68 to .99 (Argyle & Cook, 1976, p. 45; Harper et
al.. 1978, p. 178). Validity, like reliability, varies with length of
Measurement Issues measurement interval, distance between interactants, and head
Discussions about gaze measurement by Argyle and Cook angle of the gazing person. Rime and McCusker (1976) found
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

(1976), Exline and Fehr (1982), and Harper el al. (1978) allow that measurements of gaze through a one-way mirror were less
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

us to arrive at the following recommendations and conclusions. accurate than measurements of gaze from television. F. C. Har-
ris and Ciminero (1978) cautioned that observers in gaze rein-
forcement studies should be trained to attend only to the gazing
Parameters of Gaze behavior under study and to disregard cues (e.g., positive rein-
The most common procedure for measuring gaze in an ex- forcement) that may bias them to record gaze when it did not
perimental setting is to have two or more observers behind a occur. Thayer (1977) reported that youngchildren are less accu-
one-way mirror press buttons connected to clocks and counters rate in discriminating on-face and off-face gaze than adults. Ar-
or to recording pens when one person directs his or her gaze gyle and Cook (1976, p. 47) and Harper et al. (1978, p. 178)
toward the face of another. Accuracy of recording can be en- concluded that under ordinary conditions, the agreement about
hanced by having interactants facing each other directly, by gaze by observers and recipients is good. Recipients, like ob-
placing observers directly behind receivers, by keeping the dis- servers, find it difficult to discriminate between eye-gaze and
tance between interactants as small as possible, by testing the lace-gaze (Argyle & Cook. 1976, p. 47: Harper et al., 1978,
visual acuity of observers, by training observers with feedback p. 177).
trials, by using eye-level lighting to reduce eye shadows, by ob-
taining reliability estimates from all observers, and by schedul- Correlations With Other Behaviors
ing fatigue breaks (Exline & Fehr. 1982). Recordings of gaze
People's gazing behaviors do not correlate highly with their
can also be made from films and videotapes. One camera and a
verbal ratings. Breed and Porter (1972) and Goldberg, Kiesler,
mirror or two cameras with a split screen can be used to simul-
and Collins (1969) reported small positive correlations between
taneously photograph two people, and accuracy can be en-
gaze and positive affective ratings. A tabulation of correlations
hanced by recording from slow motion or from individual
between rating form responses and measures of gaze frequency
frames. Ga:e duration refers to the length (or percentage) of
and duration in the following studies yielded a proportion of
time one person gazes at another and is the measure reported
statistically significant correlations equal to chance (KJeinke et
in most studies. Gaze frequency refers to the number of glances
al.. 1977; KJeinke, Staneski, & Berger, 1975; KJeinke, Staneski,
made by one person toward another. A measure of average
& Pipp. 1975; KJeinke & Walton. 1982; Walsh, Meister, &
length of glances (glance duration) can be derived by dividing
KJeinke, 1977). Correlations between gaze and verbal ratings
gaze duration by gaze frequency (KJeinke, Desautels, & Knapp.
may be low because people are generally not aware of their gaz-
1977; Russo, 1975). Although glance duration is not indepen-
ing behaviors in an interaction. It is recommended later that
dent of gaze duration and gaze frequency, it is a measure worthy
researchers assess self-awareness of gaze as well as perceived
of further study. For example. Russo (1975) concluded that
gaze from others.
friendship and affiliation were more accurately reflected by mu-
Factor analysis research also indicates that gaze is relatively
tual glance duration than by mutual gaze duration.
independent of other behaviors. Wellens and Faletti (1978) re-
ported one factor for verbal liking and a second factor for non-
Reliability verbal behaviors (smiling, talking, gazing, distancing; see also
Latta, 1976). Staneski and KJeinke (1978) factor-analyzed data
The preferred measure of interobserver reliability is the pro-
from four experiments that included verbal ratings, gaze fre-
portion of agreement between observers on the presence or ab-
quency and duration, seating distance, amount of talking, and
sence of gaze (Argyle & Cook. 1976; Exline, 1963). Researchers
heart rate. In general, each of these measures was identified as
have reported proportions of agreement on gaze recordings
an independent factor. Staneski and KJeinke (1978) also com-
ranging from .66 to .94 (Argyle & Cook, 1976, p. 40; Duncan
puted multiple regressions using nonverbal behaviors to predict
&Fiske, 1977, chap. 4). Lower reliabilities occur when interob-
participants' ratings of liking for a person with whom they were
server agreement is measured in time intervals less than I s,
interacting. Men tended to like the other person when their own
when interactants are relatively far apart (roughly more than 3
talking level was high and when their gazing was low. Women.
m), and when the head of the gazing person is at an angle
(roughly greater than 30°) from the face of the recipient. Argyle
and Cook (1976, p. 41) concluded that trained observers work- • See KJrkland and Lewis (1976) for a discussion of words denoting
ing under good viewing conditions can make highly reliable eye fixation.
80 CHRIS L. KLEINKE

in contrast, expressed more liking when their own talking level The association between gaze and liking appears to be
was low and their gazing was high. Because the multiple corre- learned. Children do not use eye contact to judge affiliation and
lations were low and limited in generality by small sample sizes friendship until about age 6 (Abramovitch & Daly, 1978; Post
and confounding by various experimental treatments, further & Hetherington, 1974). Girls make this discrimination at a
research on this issue is needed. A worthwhile study would be younger age than boys.
one using people's levels of gaze to predict their ratings of liking Gaze also influences people's liking for each other, with mod-
for others in positive, neutral, and negative contexts (see Bayes, erate amounts of gaze generally preferred over constant or no
!972;Lochman&Allen, 1981: Shrout & Fiske, 1981). gaze (Argyle, Lefebvre, & Cook, 1974; Exline, 1971). Gary
(1978b) and Kendon and Ferber (1973) observed that people
use gaze as a cue for deciding whether to pursue conversations
Functional Classification
with one another.
Patterson's (1982, 1983) sequential model is based on the Several studies found that gaze enhances attraction and liking
premise that patterns of nonverbal interaction are moderated when people are introduced in a social interaction. Participants
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

by the meaning or function of the nonverbal behaviors being in a study by Stass and Willis (1967) chose to work with a con-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

exchanged. In laying the groundwork for the sequential model, federate who gazed at them during their introduction. Research
Patterson developed a functional classification for nonverbal be- participants evaluated each other more favorably when the ex-
haviors based on classifications advanced by Argyle (1972), Ek- perimenter seated them facing each other to facilitate mutual
man and Friesen (1969), and Kendon (1967). The functional gaze (Scherer, 1974). British college students rated a same-sex
categories proposed by Patterson are those of (a) providing in- peer they met in an experiment as more pleasant and less ner-
formation, (b) regulating interaction, (c) expressing intimacy, vous when the person gazed at them continuously rather than
(d) exercising social control, and (e) facilitating service or task not at all (Cook & Smith. 1975). Gazing confederates in a study
goals. These functional categories are used here to organize a by Exline and Eldridge (1967) received more favorable evalua-
large number of independently conducted gaze and eye contact tions than nongazing confederates when they communicated a
studies. Because gazing behaviors can concurrently serve more positive message.
than one function (Patterson, 1982, p. 234), the following func- Attentiveness. Interviewers are evaluated by interviewees as
tional classification is necessarily subjective. For practical rea- more attentive when their gaze is relatively high rather than low
sons, studies are not further subdefined according to whether (Breed, 1972; Kelly, 1978; KJeinke, Staneski, & Berger, 1975).
they used an encoding or decoding methodology (Harper et al., Interviewees give briefer responses when an interviewer does
1978.chap. I). not gaze at them (Aiello, 1977a; KJeinke, Staneski, & Berger,
1975). Gazing people are also viewed as more attentive during
social encounters (KJeinke, Bustos, Meeker, & Staneski, 1973;
Providing Information
KJeinke. Staneski, & Pipp, 1975). Women rated male dating
The majority of studies summarized in this section focused partners as silent, passive, and inattentive when they gave low
on judgments made (information gained) about people based levels of gaze during a role-playing interaction (Lochman & Al-
on their gazing behaviors. Gaze influences evaluations of liking len, 1981). Men's ratings were not influenced by their date's
and attraction, attentiveness, competence, social skills and gazing behavior during the role-playing interaction. Norton and
mental health, credibility, and dominance. Patterson (1982) de- Pettegrew (1979) reported a positive correlation between self-
fines informative nonverbal behaviors as being either communi- ratings of attentiveness and willingness to gaze toward other
cative or indicative. Communicative behaviors are motivated by people.
a goal or purpose. Indicative behaviors are not. Because the Competence. Researchers have determined that interviewers
identification of a goal or purpose for another person's gaze is (Sodikoff, Firestone, & Kaplan. 1974), experimenters (Le-
often subjective. I do not distinguish between studies of com- Compte & Rosenfeld, 1971), and speakers (Beebe, 1974) are
municative gaze and studies of indicative gaze in this review. more favorably evaluated when they engage in high rather than
However, the reader may wish to keep this distinction in mind low levels of gaze toward their audience. Participants in a study
because the perception and interpretation of intentions and mo- by Neely, Cherulnik, and Russ (1977) felt less comfortable
tives for gaze are relevant to theories of attribution that are dis- when an interviewer gazed 50% rather than 100% or 0°o of the
cussed later. time. This was possibly because the 50% gaze was cued ran-
Liking and allraclion. A number of studies have indicated domly and might have appeared unnatural.
that people use gaze as a sign of liking and attraction when ob- Wheeler, Baron, Michell, and Ginsburg (1979) reported a
serving social interactions. People in photographs (Lim, 1972; positive correlation between an interviewee's eye contact with
Mehrabian, I968a; Scherer & Schiff, 1973) and videotapes an interviewer and estimates made by observers of the inter-
(KJeinke, Meeker, & La Fong, 1974; Naiman & Breed, 1974; viewee's intelligence. High school students in a study by KJeck
Thayer & Schiff. 1974) appear to like each other more when and Nuessle (1968) evaluated an interviewee in a film more fa-
they share a relatively high rather than low amount of gaze. Men vorably (but also as more tense) when the interviewee gazed at
and women were viewed as sharing more sexual interest when the interviewer 80% rather than 15% of the time. Job applicants
their mutual gaze was high rather than low (Thayer & Schiff, were evaluated more favorably when they gazed at the inter-
1977). The positive feelings associated with gaze generalize to viewer during an employment interview (Forbes & Jackson,
observers, who favor people when they gaze at moderate rather 1980;Imada&Hakel, !977;Tessler&Sushelsky, 1978). People
than low levels while approaching others (Gary. 1978a) or in in photographs were judged as more deserving of a job or a
social interactions (Abele, 1981; Shrout & Fiske, 1981). higher salary when they gazed straight ahead rather than when
GAZE AND EYE CONTACT 81

they gazed downward (Amalfitano & Kalt, 1977; Tankard, competence, social skills and mental health, credibility, and
1970). dominance. Gaze also serves to express the intensity of feelings.
Several studies have been conducted in which research par- The conditions under which gaze-intensified feelings satisfy in-
ticipants evaluated counselors in videotapes when they gazed at timacy versus social control functions should be outlined in fu-
their clients a low, moderate, or large amount of the time (Fretz, ture research.
Com, Tuemmler. & Bellet, 1979; Haase & Tepper, 1972; Kelly
& True. 1980; LaCrosse, 1975; Tipton & Rymer, 1978). Most Regulating Interaction
favorable ratings were given to counselors when they directed
the highest amounts of gaze toward their clients and least favor- Research reviews on nonverbal regulation of human interac-
able ratings were given when counselors engaged in the lowest tion indicate that linguistic, paralinguistic, and kinesic vari-
amounts of gaze toward their clients.3 ables play a role in conversational sequencing (Feldstein & Wel-
Social skills and menial health. Gazing behavior has been kowitz, 1978;Rosenfeld. 1978). The operation of visual behav-
defined as a component of assertiveness (Romano & Bellack, ior in regulating interactions was discussed by Argyle and Cook
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

(1976) and Harper et al. (1978). Two relevant research areas


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

1980) and social skills (Bellack, Hersen, & Lamparski, 1979;


Bellack, Hersen, & Turner, 1978, 1979; Conger &Farrell, 1981; identified by these authors are synchronization and regulation.
Glasgow & Arkowitz. 1975: Greenwald. 1977). Low levels of Synchronization. Kendon (1967) reported a perfect rank-or-
gaze in therapeutic interviews communicate noninvolvement der correlation between rate of gaze shifts in two-person con-
and are implicated in judgments of depression (Waxer, 1974) versations and a .81 correlation between interactants' length of
and mental illness (Mancuso. Litchford, Wilson. Harrigan, & gaze. Condon (1980) and his colleagues observed synchroniza-
Lehrer, 1983). tion between verbal and kinesic behaviors (including eye move-
Credibility. The common belief thai it is difficult to lie when ments) of normal adults. Behavior synchronization was not
looking into someone's eyes is reflected by credibility judg- present to the same degree in schizophrenic adults and autistic
ments. A witness in a videotaped courtroom trial was judged as children.
more credible when he did not avert his gaze from the attorney Regulation. Kendon's (1967) phrase-by-phrase analysis of
who was questioning him (Hemsley & Doob. 1978). Airline two-person interactions indicated that speakers ended an utter-
travelers were more likely to be perceived as suspicious and suit- ance with a prolonged gaze to communicate that it was the lis-
able for search if they avoided eye contact while being ques- tener's turn to speak. Levine and Sutton-Smith (1973) found
tioned by an inspector in a mock customs inspection (Kraut & that adults (but not children) ended an utterance with a gaze
Poe. 1980). and also began an utterance with a gaze. They concluded that
Dominance. Exline (1971) and Ellsworth (1975) described gaze aversion before an utterance is useful for collecting one's
how gaze is used to communicate threat and dominance. In ac- thoughts (encoding speech) and that gaze after an utterance is a
cordance with these observations, British college students asso- means for receiving feedback. Duncan (1972) identified a head
ciated potency and power most strongly with continuous gaze turn in the direction of the listener as a "turn-yielding" cue and
and least strongly with no gaze (Argyle et al., 1974). J. A. Gra- Duncan and Niederehe (1974) described a head turn by a lis-
ham and Argyle (1975) found that gaze had greatest influence tener away from a speaker as a sign of the listener's intention to
on ratings of "potency," and that facial expressions had greatest speak. Knapp, Hart, Fredrich, and Shulman (1973) observed
influence on ratings of "evaluation." Gaze and facial expres- that breaking eye contact was a reliable occurrence at the com-
sions both contributed to ratings of activity. Hillabrant (1974) pletion of an interview.
manipulated gaze between two men in a videotaped interaction It has been widely reported that people gaze more while lis-
and Thayer (1969) manipulated gaze between two men in a live tening than while speaking (Argyle & Cook, 1976, chap. 5;
encounter. Men in both situations were evaluated as being more Duncan & Fiske. 1977, chap. 5; KJeinke. Staneski, & Berger,
dominant when their gaze was high rather than low. Dovidio 1975; KJeinke, Staneski, & Pipp, 1975). However, there are
and Ellyson (1982) reported that high gazing-while-speaking ra- some qualifications. Equivalent amounts of gazing while speak-
tios were directly related to ratings of power in an interaction. ing and listening were found with research participants who
Communicating feelings. Gaze appears to communicate the were given high status or who were discussing issues on which
intensity but not the valence of feelings. Kimble and Olszewski they had expertise (Ellyson, Dovidio, & Corson, 1981; Ellyson,
(1980) and KJmble, Forte, and Yoshikawa (1981) asked re- Dovidio, Corson. & Vinicur. 1980). LaFrance & Mayo (1976)
search participants to communicate positive and negative feel- observed that black Americans looked more when speaking and
ings so they would be interpreted as weak or strong. Participants less when listening.
Summary. There is evidence of synchronization between
gazed more when communicating strong feelings than when
gaze and other behaviors and of the regulatory function of gaze
communicating weak feelings. This was true whether or not the
as a turn-taking cue. Recent studies, however, suggest that gaze
feeling was positive or negative.
synchronization and the operation of gaze in turn taking are
Summary. All of the studies in this section except those on
less reliable than previously believed because they depend on
communicating feelings focused on evaluations given to people
the context and motives of the interactants (Beattie, 1978, 1979;
as a function of their gazing behaviors. These studies did not
manipulate contextual and motivational factors that moderate
judgments of gazing behaviors. Their conclusions are, there- J
These results can be contrasted with data for talking levels suggest-
fore, relevant primarily for situations in which we are making ing that people prefer others who talk 50% of the time in social interac-
more or less objective observations of others. Gaze is used to tions, whereas they favor counselors who talk 33% of the lime in a coun-
gain information about liking and attraction, attentiveness, selingsession(KJeinke,Kahn.&Tully, 1979; KJeinke &Tully, 1979).
82 CHRIS L. KLEINKE

Kendon, 1978; Lazzerini, Stephenson, & Neave. 1978: Rutter, ship or attempting to ingratiate themselves with others. College
Stcphenson, Ayling, & White. 1978; Rutter. Stephenson. Laz- women gazed more at a female confederate when they were try-
zerini, Ayling, & White, 1977). Studies of race and status have ing to make friends (Pellegrini, Hicks, & Gordon, 1970), and
also qualified earlier findings that people gaze more while listen- college men gazed more at a woman when they wanted to inter-
ing than while speaking. Because gaze operates w i t h i n a com- est her in a social conversation (Lefebvre, 1975).
munication system, its function as a regulator cannot be studied Threat and dominance. Researchers have demonstrated that
in isolation (Duncan & Fiske, 1977, chap. 11; Kendon. 1978). gaze functions to communicate threat and dominance during
The function of gaze in regulating interactions will be better conversations, during defense of personal space, in situations
understood when the influence of context and personal factors with aggression and anger, and when role-playing assertiveness.
(e.g.. race, status, and motives) are delineated. This is one of the In conversations, Exline, EUyson, and Long (1975) determined
purposes of the sequential model. that people exert dominance by gazing while speaking. Gazing
while listening appears to serve an information gathering func-
tion. Hughes and Goldman (1978) reported that men and
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Expressing Intimacy
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

women were less likely to violate the personal space of a man


Patterson (1982) views the expression of intimacy as a spon- who was standing in front of the buttons in an elevator when
taneous affective reaction that can be contrasted with the self- the man was staring at them. Men, but not women, were also
conscious, managed social control and service-task functions of less likely to violate the personal space of a staring woman.
nonverbal behaviors. When given the choice, men were equally likely to violate the
One example of intimacy expression is found in studies space of gazing men and gazing women (Buchanan, Goldman,
showing that gazing behavior increases as a function of positive & Juhnke, 1977). Women preferred to violate the space of a
attraction. Mehrabian (I968a, 1968b) reported that research gazing woman. Ellsworth and Carlsmith (1973) found that col-
participants gazed more when they approached an imaginary lege men were less likely to deliver electric shocks to a male
person they liked rather than disliked. Participants in a study confederate who had angered them when the confederate gazed
by Griffitt, May, and Veitch (1974) gazed more at opposite-sex at them constantly. College men delivered more shocks to a gaz-
peers when they had previously been exposed to sexually arous- ing confederate when his gaze was not constant and when it
ing slides. Another example of intimacy expression comes from appeared that the shocks would make him gaze less. Male psy-
studies in which people gazed more after receiving positive eval- chiatric patients gazed at each other more when role-playing
uations (Coutts, Schneider, & Montgomery, 1980; Exline & assertive responses to negative situations (Eisler, Hersen, Miller,
Winters, 1965; Walsh etal., 1977) or warm nonverbal responses &Blanchard, 1975).
(Ho & Mitchell, 1982). College men were not any more or less competitive when
Summary. People gaze more when they share feelings of playing a two-person game with a male confederate who gazed
warmth and liking. However, high levels of gaze do not always at them constantly rather than not at all (Klcinkc & Pohlen,
indicate intimacy and liking (Patterson. 1982, p. 236). The chal- 1971). The confederate's gazing behavior did, however, influ-
lenge for researchers is to outline conditions under which gaze ence the men's affective evaluations. Men rated a very competi-
expresses (or is viewed as expressing) intimacy. The stages of tive confederate as most friendly and cooperative when he
intimacy described by the social penetration model may shed gazed. It is possible that the competitive confederate appeared
light on this question (Altman & Taylor, 1973). Patterson (1982, more "upfront" and candid when he gazed. The constantly gaz-
p. 245) suggests that gaze functions most commonly to express ing and competitive confederate might have also intimidated
intimacy in unstructured and nonevaluative interactions. It will participants into giving ratings that were socially desirable.
be useful to determine how aware people are of when they give Gaze aversion has been shown to communicate submissive-
or receive gaze that is motivated by feelings of intimacy. ness and appeasement. Participants in a study by M. G. Efran
and Cheyne (1974) averted their gaze when following an experi-
menter's instructions to intrude on the shared space of two con-
Social Control
federates. Reduced gaze between dating partners who role-
The social control function of gaze is viewed by Patterson played conflicts was correlated with behaviors of disapproval
(1982, 1983) as primarily communicative because it is directed and expressions of lower interpersonal power (Lochman & Al-
toward a goal or purpose. Researchers have investigated the len, 1981). Gaze aversion can also be used to assert oneself with-
functions of gaze in persuasion and deception, ingratiation, out the risk of expressing threat and dominance. For example,
threat and dominance, escape and avoidance, and compliance. J. Z. Rubin, Steinberg, and Gerrein (1974) determined that
Persuasion and deception. People tend to increase their gaze gaze aversion is used at a contested traffic intersection as a tactic
when attempting to be persuasive and deceptive. Mehrabian for gaining the right of way.
and Williams (1969) and Timney and London (1973) found Escape and avoidance. Prolonged and unexplained gaze can
that research participants gave relatively high amounts of gaze function as a stimulus for eliciting escape and avoidance. Ells-
when they were instructed to be persuasive. College students worth et al., (1972) and Greenbaum and Rosenfeld (1978) had
gazed more at counselors (Sitton & Griffin, 1981) and inter- experimenters stand on street corners and gaze constantly or
viewers (Burns &Kintz, 1976; Riggio& Friedman, I983)when not at all at pedestrians and motorists who were waiting for a
intentionally giving false answers to their questions. Research red light. When the light changed to green, pedestrians and driv-
participants gazed more while lying to a person who could de- ers crossed the intersection significantly faster when they had
liver an electric shock when detecting deceit (Mehrabian, 1971). received constant gaze from the experimenter. Other examples
Ingratiation. People gaze more when they are seeking friend- of escape and avoidance are found in the following studies. Stu-
GAZE AND EYE CONTACT 83

dents in a college library remained in their seats for shorter peri- at them while they were working on a puzzle (Reis & Werner,
ods of time when they were confronted by an experimenter who 1974).
sat at their table and stared at them (B. J. Smith, Sanford, & Ellsworth and Langer (1976) compared the effects of staring
Goldman, 1977). Worshippers in church pews were more likely in an ambiguous and unambiguous context. In the unambigu-
to make space for someone who stared at them (Campbell & ous context, women shoppers were approached by a female ex-
Lancioni, 1979). perimenter who pointed to a female confederate and said that
The escape response to staring appears at early developmen- she had lost a contact lens and needed assistance. In the ambigu-
tal levels. Retarded adults responded to a threatening stare with ous context, the experimenter said that the female confederate
avoidance behaviors of averting gaze and turning away (Bailey. looked ill and needed help. When it was clear to shoppers what
Tipton, & Taylor, 1977). The authors of this study suggested sort of help the confederate needed, they were more likely to
that profoundly retarded adults had shorter avoidance latencies assist her when she gazed at them constantly rather than not at
than mildly retarded adults because of high sensitivity to phylo- all. When the confederate's need was ambiguous, shoppers were
more likely to offer assistance when she did not gaze at them.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

genetically conditioned releasing stimuli. Field (1981) con-


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

cluded that infants avoid prolonged gaze as a reaction to height- KJeinke (1980) compared the effects of gaze on compliance
ened arousal. with requests that were either legitimate or illegitimate. People
Ellsworth et al. (1972) conceptualized the stare as a demand in an airport were approached by a gazing or nongazing female
for a response. People are motivated to move away from a experimenter who asked them for a dime. In the legitimate con-
stranger who is staring when the meaning of the stare is ambigu- dition, the experimenter explained that she needed the dime to
ous and no other appropriate response is available. If a staring call someone to pick her up. In the illegitimate condition, the
stranger is appealing or has other favorable attributes, a cogni- experimenter explained that she wanted to buy gum or a candy
tive assessment of this staring behavior can result in favorable bar. When the request was legitimate, more people gave dimes
responses. For example, pedestrians did not cross the street as to gazing experimenters. When the request was illegitimate,
fast to escape a staring experimenter when the experimenter more people gave di mes to nongazing experimenters. It was sug-
was dressed and made up to be physically attractive (Kmiecik, gested that gaze aversion during the legitimate request might
Mausar, & Banziger, 1979). People were also less likely to avoid have helped the experimenter come across as humble and
a staring experimenter when the experimenter smiled (Elman, tactful.
Schulte. & Bukoff. 1977). In a similar vein, time appeared to Summary. People use gaze to exert social control when at-
tempting to be persuasive and deceptive, ingratiating, and when
pass more quickly for research participants who engaged in con-
directly exerting threat and dominance. Whether someone re-
tinuous eye contact with a confederate who was smiling rather
sponds to gaze by escapi ng or complying depends on the context
than scowling (Thayer & SchifT. 1975).
and on his or her perceptions of the gazing person's motives.
Compliance. Research on gaze and compliance extends the
Patterson (1982, 1983) defines gaze in a social control function
above conclusions about escape and avoidance by delineating
as managed, intentional, and situational. It would be useful to
the importance of how gaze is perceived in terms of ambiguity
determine how aware or mindful people are of when they use
and appropriateness. People comply more to requests from gaz-
gaze for exerting social control. Patterson also suggests that gaze
ing rather than nongazing experimenters in situations in which
in a social control function is maintained until it achieves its
gaze is interpreted positively and serves to bring the experi-
intended purpose. Managed gaze may, therefore, often exceed
menter psychologically closer. For example, KJeinke (1977) and
the limits of comfort or appropriateness in an interaction and
Brockner. Pressman, Cabitt. and Moran (1982) reported that
result in an unstable exchange.
people were more willing to return "misplaced" dimes and
more willing to lend dimes to gazing rather than nongazing ex-
perimenters. People were also more willing to accept leaflets Service Task
(KJeinke & Singer. 1979) and to give charitable contributions
Patterson (1982, 1983) distinguishes service-task functions
(Bull & Gibson-Robinson, 1981) or a nickel for five pennies from intimacy and social control functions by pointing out that
(Ernest & Cooper, 1974) when an experimenter was gazing. service-task functions apply more to the goals and outcomes of
Drivers were more likely to stop for gazing hitchhikers (M. Sny- an interaction than to its intimacy or affective quality. To the
der, Grether, & Keller. 1974), pedestrians were more likely to degree that this is true, gaze in a service-task function is less
help a gazing experimenter pick up dropped coins (Valentine, likely to be subject to cognitive-affective assessment of its causes
1980) and dropped questionnaires (Goldman & Fordyce, and motives. Researchers have investigated two general service-
1983), and bystanders were more likely to help an injured gaz- task functions of gaze: information seeking and facilitation of
ing jogger (Shetland & Johnson, 1978). Research participants communication in interpersonal relations, learning situations,
were more willing to change their attitude expression when they and bargaining. In accordance with Patterson's distinction,
received social reinforcement from a gazing rather than nongaz- studies of gaze in a service-task function measured task and
ing interviewer (Goldman. 1980). outcome rather than attributions and affect. However, in all hu-
In situations in which an experimenter's gaze is interpreted man interactions people will potentially make judgments about
negatively or viewed as inappropriate, one would expect less one another. Gaze in a service-task function that violates norms
compliance under conditions of gaze rather than no gaze. Pas- or expectations is as likely as any gaze to result in an unstable
sengers on a subway train were less likely to help an experi- exchange and cognitive-affective assessment.
menter who stared at them between stops and research partici- Information seeking. Gaze is used for seeking information
pants were less willing to help a confederate who had stared about personal evaluations. For example, Walsh et al. (1977)
84 CHRIS L. KLEINKE

explained their finding that women gazed more at men who Learning. A number of studies have shown that visual acces-
gave negative rather than neutral evaluations on the basis of the sibility contributes to the teaching-learning process. College
women's motive to search for the cause of the negative evalua- students performed better on a digit coding task when they re-
tion. This explanation might also be relevant for Coutts et al.'s ceived instructions from a gazing rather than nongazing experi-
(1980) research participants who increased their gaze after re- menter (Fry & Smith, 1975). Primary school children had
ceiving negative reactions and nonverbal immediacy from a greater recall for stories that were read by a gazing rather than
confederate. In a similar vein, Exline and Messick (1967) and nongazing teacher (Otteson & Otteson, 1980). Bloom (1975)
Burton, McGregor, and Berry (1979) viewed the increased gaze reported that adult gaze was important for stimulating vocal-
given by dependent people toward a nonreinforcing experi- ization in young children, and Garcia and Trujillo (1977) dem-
menter as a reflection of their need for information and feed- onstrated the importance of experimenter gaze as part of a
back. training package for imitative responding in retarded children.
Information seeking was also demonstrated by increased Verbal reprimands for disruptive behavior were more effective
gaze toward physically attractive people. College men in studies when accompanied with gaze (Van Houten, Nau, MacKenzie-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

by Kleck and Rubenstein (1975) and Fugita, Agle, Newman, Keating, Sameoto. & Colavecchia, 1982).
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

and Walfish (1977) gazed more at college women who were Gaze also facilitates student participation and satisfaction in
dressed and made up to be attractive. College men in studies by learning situations. For example, college students participated
KJeinke, Staneski, and Berger (1975) and KJeinke, Staneski, more in a seminar when they could make eye contact with the
and Pipp (1975) did not gaze more at college women who had instructor (Caproni, Levine, O'Neal, McDonald. & Garwood,
been categorized by judges as high or low in physical attractive- 1977). In a similar vein, Pedersen (1977) reported positive cor-
ness. The differences in attractiveness in the first studies were relations between eye contact availability with the instructor
probably more extreme than in the second studies. Coutts and and students' ratings of their performance and enjoyment in
Schneider (1975) reported positive correlations between gaze a college class. Breed and Colaiuta (1974) found that college
directed by research participants toward opposite-sex peers and instructors were better liked and produced superior student per-
experimenter ratings of the peers' physical attractiveness. formance when they gazed at students a relatively high rather
Two additional examples of information seeking are found in than low amount of time during their lectures.
the following studies. First, people gazed more when they were Cooperation and bargaining. Gaze can foster cooperation by
given unconfirmed expectancies about an impending interac- facilitating the communication of positive intent. Participants
tion (Ickes, Patterson, Rajecki. & Tanford, 1982). Second, be- in one study preferred a seating arrangement permitting mutual
cause strangers looked at each other more (while listening) dur- view of another person with whom they were cooperating (Jelli-
ing a task-oriented interaction than did friends, it was con- son & Ickes, 1974). Participants in another study engaged in
cluded that gaze during this situation serves more as a means longer glances when playing a cooperative versus competitive
of collecting information than as a method for expressing affect game(Foddy, 1978). A third group of studies found that people
(Rutter&Stephenson, 1979). negotiating from a position of strength were more likely to bar-
Langer. Fiske, Taylor, and Chanowitz (1976) formulated a gain and compromise when they communicated face-to-face
novel-stimulus hypothesis to account for the conflict people ex- rather than over a telephone (Morley & Stephenson, 1969,
perience when they want to look at others who are different but 1970a, 1970b). Other researchers reported that visual access fa-
feel uncomfortable because it is not socially acceptable to do cilitated cooperation in the Prisoner's Dilemma game (Gardin,
so. The hypothesis was supported in an experiment showing Kaplan. Firestone, & Cowan, 1973; Wichman, 1970).
that research participants gazed more at novel (crippled or One exception to gaze-facilitated cooperation occurs when
pregnant) versus normal people when their gazing behavior was people use gaze to threaten one another. In this case, coopera-
unobserved. Participants gazed less at novel versus normal peo- tion is enhanced when a barrier is present to prevent visual ac-
ple when their gazing behavior was observed. KJeck (1968) re- cess (Carnevale. Pruitt. & Seilheimer, 1981; Lewis* Fry, 1977).
ported similar results in a study showing that participants gazed In a competitive situation, research participants preferred a
more at a confederate when the confederate had been made up seating arrangement where they could see the other person but
to look like an amputee. Research participants gazed more at a the other person could not see them (Jellison & Ickes, 1974).
person in a wheelchair when interaction with the person was Summary. People generally get along better and communi-
not anticipated and gazed less when interaction was anticipated cate more effectively when they look at each other. One excep-
(Thompson, 1982). Participants in a study by KJeck et al. tion is in bargaining interactions where cooperation can be un-
(1968) did not gaze more at a confederate who was described dermined when gaze is used for expressing dominance and
as having epilepsy but who did not have a novel appearance. threat.
Interpersonal interactions. Gaze enhances people's comfort Although research investigating gaze in a service-task func-
during interpersonal interactions when it facilitates communi- tion has emphasized measures of task and outcome, the opera-
cation. Several unpublished studies cited by Argyle and Cook tion of cognitive-affective assessment should not be neglected.
(1976, p. 152) indicated that office workers preferred face-to- For example, whereas people may solve tasks more effectively
face communication over video or telephone communication, with someone who gazes, they are also likely to form judgments
especially for developing interpersonal relations. Research par- about that person's personality and motives. Similarly, although
ticipants who attempted to communicate with a person under novel people may recognize someone's gaze for its information-
five conditions of visibility (normal, dark glasses, mask, one- seeking function, they still hold expectations about the amount
way mirror, no visibility) expressed more satisfaction as visibil- of gaze in a given context that is acceptable and appropriate.
ity increased (Argyle. Lalljee, & Cook, 1968). The sequential model is therefore relevant to gaze in a service-
GAZE AND EYE CONTACT 85

task function because it describes what happens when gaze vio- that children may not learn to avoid gaze from others until after
lates a service-task script and results in an unstable interaction. they are 4- to 5-vears old because gaze from very young children
is accepted as an endearing behavior.
Se.\ differences in gazing. A review of research literature in-
Antecedent Factors in the Sequential Model
dicates that females generally gaze more in social dyadic inter-
As Patlerson (1982. 1983) pointed out, the preceding func- actions than males (Argyle & Cook. 1976, p. 147: Duncan &
tional classification provides a structure for analyzing gazing Fiske, 1977, chap. 4: Harper et al., 1978, p. 216). These differ-
behavior but it does not by itself account for the dynamics of ences are found in children as well as adults. The fact that fe-
gaze as a function of individual and environmental variables. males often gaze more than males has been explained in terms
That is why Patterson derived his sequential model, which out- of sex roles, which influence females to be more affiliative, at-
lines three stages of nonverbal interaction: antecedents, prein- tentive, and sensitive (Ellsworth & Ludwig, 1972: Frieze &
teraction mediators, and interaction phase. Antecedent factors Ramsey, 1976).
include personal, experiential, and relational-situational deter- It would be interesting to identify situations in which women
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

minants of nonverbal involvement. A number of research stud- gaze less than men. Henley (1977) pointed out that women avert
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

ies have provided information about antecedents moderating their eyes more than men in encounters that are uncomfortable
gaze and eye contact. A summary of these studies provides a and threatening. This observation was supported by Cary
background for analyzing the dynamics of gaze and eye contact (I978a), who found that male pedestrians responded to a star-
(as a function of preinteraction mediators) during the interac- ing experimenter by gazing, whereas female pedestrians re-
tion phase of an encounter. sponded by averting their gaze. Schneider, Coutts, and Garrett
(1977) did not find sex differences in gazing behavior in triads.
Sex differences in reactions to gaze. Several studies have indi-
Personal Factors
cated that women have more tolerance and more favorable reac-
The research studies covered in this section are difficult to tions than men when receiving gaze from others. For example,
organize because they did not follow a theoretical structure. women gave more help in picking up dropped coins to a female
Their results are the "miscellaneous empirical bits and facts" experimenter who gazed at them (Valentine & Ehrlichman,
alluded to at the beginning of this article. A summary of the 1979). Men gave more help to a male experimenter who did not
essential findings follows. gaze at them. Kleinke et al. (1973) introduced college men and
Age. Scaife and Bruner (1975) reported a steadily increasing women in pairs and left them in a room to get acquainted. After
responsiveness to adult gaze by infants as they matured in age their conversation, an experimenter told participants that one
from 2 to 14 months. Ashear and Snortum ( 1 9 7 1 ) found that person (whose gaze was supposedly recorded through a one-way
gaze toward a constantly gazing woman was highest for kinder- mirror) had gazed at the other person an unusually high, an
garten and second-grade children, lower for preschool children, average, or an unusually low amount of the time. Women were
and lowest for fifth- and eighth-grade children. The decreased most favorable toward men whose gaze had ostensibly been
gaze of older children was attributed to heightened self-con- high. Women were also more favorable toward men when they
sciousness while talking with adults. L. Harris (1968) measured believed their own gaze had been high. Men's reactions were
the gaze of younger (3-4.5 years) and older (4.5-5 years) pre- exactly opposite. Men were most favorable toward women when
school children toward nongazing male and female adults and they were told the woman's gaze or their own gaze had been low.
found that the gaze of younger children toward the woman was Ellsworth and Ross (1975) introduced college students to each
higher than the gaze of all other children. This difference was other in same-sex pairs. One of the participants was assigned to
explained on the basis of the younger children's conditioned be a "speaker" and to give a self-description to the other partici-
dependency on the nurturance of women. Levine and Sutton- pant, who was given the role of "listener." Listeners were se-
Smith (1973) recorded gaze in natural interactions between cretly instructed to give high levels of gaze or no gaze to the
people in four age groups: 4-5 years, 6-9 years, 10-12 years, speaker. Female speakers disclosed more personal information
and adult. Gazing increased from the first to the second age about themselves to listeners who gazed. Female speakers also
group, decreased for the third age group, and increased to a liked gazing listeners more than nongazing listeners. Male
maximum for adults. They concluded that "many factors in- speakers, in contrast, disclosed more and felt greater liking
fluence gazing at all ages, but the prepotency of these factors when the listener did not gaze.
varies with age" (p. 404). Adams and Kirkevold (1978) ob- Aiello (1972, I977a, I977b) elaborated the relationship be-
served no differences in gazing behavior at a fast food restaurant tween physical closeness and gazing by showing that men gazed
by people in four age groups (under 12 years. 12-17 years, 18- least at close interaction distances (2-5 ft) and increased their
22 years, over 22 years). Muirhead and Goldman (1979) ob- gaze as interaction distances were lengthened to 6. 10. and 15
served different amounts (but no meaningful pattern) of mutual ft. Women gazed most at intermediate distances (around 6 ft)
gaze in various sex combinations of young, middle-aged, and and gazed less at closer as well as farther distances. Aiello con-
senior adults. cluded that people seek levels of intimacy that will result in
Scheman and Lockard (1979) conducted a developmental greatest comfort. Women gazed most at intermediate seating
study of gaze avoidance. Adult experimenters stared at children distances because that is where they apparently felt most com-
in a shopping center and recorded whether children averted gaze fortable. Men gazed most at far seating distances because that
or engaged in eye contact. Compared with 5- to 9-year-old chil- is where they apparently felt most comfortable.
dren, children whose estimated ages were between 18 months These sex differences in reactions to gaze might be inter-
and 4 years were less likely to avert their gaze. It was suggested preted in terms of sex role socialization (Mischel, 1970). The
CHRIS L. K1EINKE

tendency for women to prefer higher levels of gaze from others jecki. Ickes, & Tanford. 1981). Fromme and Beam (1974) re-
than men appears at an early age. KJeinke et al. (1977) had pre- ported that high-dominant men expressed their dominance to-
school boys and girls play a word game with a woman who gazed ward a gazing confederate with physical approach whereas
either 20% or 80%. of the time. Girls tended to like the 80% high-dominant women expressed their dominance by increas-
gazing woman more than the 20% gazing woman. Boys showed ing their gaze. Rago (1977) observed that retarded male adults
a significant preference for the 20% gazing woman. It was sug- who were behaviorally submissive gazed more at a male experi-
gested lhat boys interpreted high levels of gaze from the woman menter than retarded adults who were behaviorally dominant.
as a sign that they were doing something wrong because boys LaFrance and Carmen (1980) reported a positive relation be-
experience more negative sanctions from teachers in school. tween amount of gaze by college students and feminine-typed
Personality Before people can be categorized according to responses on the Bern Sex Role Inventory. Ickes and Barnes
whether they gaze very much or very little, it is necessary to (1978) recorded more gaze in opposite-sex dyads when at least
demonstrate that gazing is a relatively consistent and stable be- one member had an androgynous sex-role orientation. Slane,
havior (Mischel, 1968). Three studies have reported relatively Dragan, Crandall. and Payne (1980) reported different patterns
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

high correlations (ranging from r = .57 to .98) between gazing of gazing behavior by repressers and sensitizers in response to
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

behaviors of individual adults toward different experimenters stress.


over different periods of time (Daniell & Lewis, 1972; Libby, A final group of studies failed to correlate gazing behavior
1970; Patterson. 1973). Coates (1978) reported consistency in with personality measures. Kendon and Cook (1969) found no
gazing behaviors of infants. significant correlations between FIRO-B scores and gazing be-
Studies relating gazing behavior to personality measures can havior. J. S. Efran (1968) failed to replicate a previous study
be roughly divided between those that do and do not take into indicating a positive correlation between gaze and scores on the
account contextual variables. The latter approach is recom- Marlow-Crowne Social Desirability Scale (J. S. Efran &
mended and is more compatible with the sequential model. Re- Broughton, 1966). R. A. Snyder and Sutker( 1977) reported in-
searchers who wish to study gaze as a function of personality consistent correlations between gaze and various dominance
measures are encouraged to avoid post hoc interpretations and scales. Hobson, Strongman, Bull, and Craig (1973) found no
to make predictions derived from the three stages of the sequen- relation between gaze aversion and Taylor Manifest Anxiety
tial model. Scale scores. No differences in gaze were evident for people with
Exline's (1963) study offers a good example of the recom- high and low scores on Snyder's self-monitoring scale (Dabbs,
mended approach. He reported that women with high scores Evans, Hopper, & Purvis, 1980; Kleinke& Walton, 1982). Dun-
on a measure of need for affiliation gazed more at other women can and Fiske (1977, chap. 6) reported no consistent corre-
in cooperative rather than competitive interactions. Women lations between people's self-ratings on adjective checklists and
with low need for affiliation gazed more at other women in a their gazing behaviors.
competitive interaction. Need for affiliation scores were not re- Culture. The "always repeated rarely heeded" advice that
lated to gazing behaviors of men. Another study taking context research findings should be replicated in other cultures reminds
into account found that high-control (Fundamental Interper- us that most of the studies reported in this review were con-
sonal Relations Orientation-Behavior Questionnaire [FIRO- ducted in the United States, Canada, or Great Britain (Ells-
B]) research participants gazed as much when speaking as when worth & Ludwig. 1972). A few studies of gazing behavior in
listening (Ellyson et al., 1980). Low-control participants gazed other cultures have been reported in the research literature.
more when listening. A third study of gaze, personality, and Some studies found cultural similarities. For example, Bond
context determined that gaze in a normal interview was posi- and Komai (1976) reported that Japanese college students re-
tively correlated with extraversion and with measures of posi- sponded to direct gaze from a male experimenter with in-
tive self-concept (Wiens. Harper, & Matarazzo, 1980). In an in- creased self-manipulations, decreased torso movements, and
terview with awkward silences, interviewees with low self-con- shorter response latencies. These results were consistent with
cepts and high scores on neuroticism and trait anxiety reacted research findings in western cultures. Bond and Goodman
with increased gaze toward the interviewer. (1980) extended Argyle et al.'s (1974) findings by demonstrating
Studies that did not include contextual variables found some that Chinese college students associated short, frequent glances
relations between people's gazing behaviors and their scores on (vs. longer but fewer glances) with activity and potency. Yoshida
personality tests. High levels of gazing behavior have been re- and lida ( 1 9 8 1 ) replicated Argyle et al.'s (1968) results by show-
lated to high nurturance scores on the Edwards Personality Pro- ing that Japanese college students preferred an interviewer who
file Schedule (Libby & Yaklevich. 1973). to high scores on the was not wearing dark glasses.
FIRO-B inclusion and affection scales (Exline, Gray, & Other studies found cultural differences. Watson (1970) com-
Schuette. 1965). and to high scores on the FIRO-B dependency pared gazing behaviors of college students from "contact" cul-
scale ( E x l i n e & Messick, 1967). High levels of gazing have also tures (Arabs. Latin Americans, Southern Europeans) and from
been related to measures of assertiveness (Galassi et al., 1976). "non-contact" cultures (Asians, Indians-Pakistanis, Northern
Machiavellianism (Exline, Thibaut. Brannon. & Gumpert, Europeans) and found greater levels of gaze by students from
1961). social skill (Cherulnik, Neely, Flanagan, & Zachau, contact cultures. Argyle and Cook (1976, pp. 29-33) described
1978). self-concept (Fugita et al., 1977), and internal control a number of differences between cultures in rules that are used
(Lefcourt. Martin, Pick. & Saleh. 1985; Lefcourt & Wine, for defining appropriate and inappropriate gazing behaviors.
1969). For example, Navaho Indians are taught that it is impolite to
Argyle and Cook (1976. p. 141) cited several studies showing gaze directly at another person during a conversation. The Jap-
a positive relation between gaze and extraversion (see also Ra- anese look more at the neck than at the eyes. Among the Luo
GAZE AND EYE CONTACT 87

of Kenya, a man and his mother-in-law turn their backs on one lace, 1977; Krop. Messinger. & Reiner, 1973; Morris & O'Neill.
another while speaking. Too little gaze in some cultures is inter- 1976).
preted as a sign of dishonesty and insincerity. In other cultures, Gazing person's physical attractiveness. Several studies
turning the eyes downward is viewed as a gesture of respect. found that college men gave unfavorable ratings to unattractive
People in some cultures wear veils or take other precautions to women but not to attractive women who engaged in low levels of
protect themselves from the "Evil Eye." A fascinating historical gaze during an interaction (Kleinke, Staneski, & Berger, 1975;
account of this phenomenon is provided by Tomkins (1963; see KJeinke, Staneski, & Pipp, 1975; Scherwitz & Helmreich,
also Machovec, 1976). 1973). KJeinke, Staneski, and Pipp (1975) suggested that we
Clinical populations. Attempts to differentiate gazing of peo- learn to make different interpretations about (gazing) behaviors
ple with various clinical diagnoses have met with only limited of attractive and unattractive people such that attractive people
success. This limited success is due to problems including the are tolerated more when they are nonresponsive.
need for adequate descriptions of patient populations and pa- Anxiety. White interviewers in a study by Word, Zanna, and
tient clinical state, control of interviewer behavior and inter- Cooper (1974) gazed less at black interviewees than at white
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

view content, appropriate sampling of interview segments for interviewees, presumably as a result of heightened anxiety from
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

analysis, and consistency in choice of measures of gaze (Rutter, interacting with a person of different race. These results were
1973). Because of these problems, it is difficult to reach mean- elaborated by Ickes (1984), who found that whites who desire
ingful conclusions about the results of the following studies. interactions with blacks gazed more at a black versus a white
Massie (1978) reported that mothers of prepsychotic infants interviewer. Whites who avoid interactions with blacks gazed
gave less gaze and touch to their infants during the first six less at a black versus a white interviewer. Hobson et al. (1973)
months of life than mothers whose infants were normal. S. E. aroused anxiety in research participants by having an experi-
Harris (1978) found that adult schizophrenics directed compar- menter tell them they were not doing well in an interview. These
atively more gaze than normal adults toward their mother. Ar- anxious participants did not demonstrate gaze aversion from an
gyle and Cook (1976, chap. 6) and Harper et al. (1978, pp. 222- interviewer who was different from the experimenter.
226) summarized a number of studies showing that autistic Moods. Several investigators studied gaze as a function of
children and schizophrenic, depressed, and neurotic adults en- moods that were induced in a laboratory setting. Natale (1977)
gage in comparatively low levels of gazing behavior. Rutter used Velten's (1968) technique to induce elation, neutral feel-
(1976, 1977, 1978) concluded that schizophrenics do not differ ings, or depression. Elated participants engaged in most eye
from nonschizophrenics in their gazing behavior and that contact and depressed participants engaged in least eye contact
schizophrenics avert their gaze in the same manner as non- during a subsequent interaction. Participants in a study by Mc-
schizophrenics when they are embarrassed. Clintock and Hunt (1975) were interviewed on topics designed
Summary. Researchers have reported relations between gaze to arouse passivity, pleasantness, or unpleasantness. Partici-
and age, sex, personality, culture, and clinical diagnosis. Al- pants who experienced passivity engaged in less eye contact
though gaze is a relatively consistent and stable behavior, results than other participants. Fifth-grade boys in a study by Zimmer-
from studies of personal factors do not always fit a meaningful man and Brody (1975) gazed at each other more after viewing
pattern. This occurs largely because motivation and context a film in which boys engaged in warm versus cold interactions.
were not taken into account. Age differences, for example, are In a study that did not involve induced moods, Waxer (1974)
moderated by situational influences on the gazer's self-con- reported that depressed psychiatric patients gazed less at an in-
sciousness, anxiety, and dependence. Sex differences are moder- terviewer than nondepressed psychiatric patients.
ated by motives for affiliation, dependence, threat, and domi- Embarrassment. Research participants in three experiments
nance. Ickes (1981) integrated personality and sex variables gazed less at interviewers who asked personal and embarrassing
into a model that defines masculine sex-typed gazing as instru- questions (Edelmann & Hampson. 1981; Exline et al., 1965;
mental and feminine sex-typed gazing as expressive. The se- Schulz & Barefoot, 1974). Modigliani (1971) induced embar-
quential model demonstrates that it is not fruitful to study the rassment by giving research participants a task on which they
effects of personal factors on gazing in isolation. Researchers failed. These embarrassed participants gazed less at others dur-
should make a priori predictions about the relation between ing the study than participants in a second group who had expe-
personal factors and gazing behavior in particular contexts and rienced success. College women in a study by Ellsworth, Fried-
specific types of interactions. man, Perlick, and Hoyt (1978) preferred a female confederate
who gazed at them when they were uncertain about the experi-
ment and apparently felt the need to compare their feelings with
Experiential Factors others. Embarrassed women preferred a confederate who did
not gaze at them. British college students significantly de-
Patterson (1982, 1983) views experiential factors as relating creased their gaze toward an experimenter who revealed embar-
to the influence on nonverbal behaviors of recent or similar ex- rassing information (Edelmann & Hampson, 1979).
periences. Experiential factors include one's operant as well as Self-disclosure. Several researchers reported that research
respondent learning history. participants engaged in less gazing behavior when they dis-
Behavioral consequences. Kleinke, Staneski. and Berger closed intimate and personal information (Carr & Dabbs, 1974;
(1975) used operant reinforcement to increase the gaze of re- Goldberg &Wellens. !979;Sundstrom, 1978). Anderson (1976)
search participants tosvard an interviewer. Other researchers reported a curvilinear relation showing most gaze during dis-
have used operant reinforcement to enhance the gazing behav- closure on topics of medium intimacy and least gaze during
ior of clients in therapy programs (Clancy, 1976; Finch & Wal- disclosure on low- and high-intimacy topics.
CHRIS L. KLEINKE

Summary. Gaze is influenced by one's learning history and and nurturance, Cohen and Campos (1974) observed that in-
moods. People tend to gaze less when they are anxious, de- fants gave more eye contact to an adult stranger when they were
pressed, and embarrassed and to gaze more when they are near their mother than when they were near their father. Carr,
elated. People gaze less when disclosing personal information, Dabbs. and Carr (1975) reported that 2-year-olds consistently
and appear to be more tolerant to low levels of gaze from attrac- tried to remain within their mother's visual field.
tive versus unattractive others. Approval and stains. J. S. Efran and Broughton (1966) and
Mehrabian and Friar (1969) found that research participants
gazed more at high-status rather than low-status people, pre-
Relational Factors
sumably because high-status people are in a better position to
Patterson (1982, 1983) grouped relational and situational give social approval. J. S. Efran (1968) and Fugita (1974) con-
factors in a common category because they often interact with ducted experiments in which participants interacted with con-
one another to influence nonverbal involvement. However, be- federates who were introduced as having high or low status and
cause the research studies falling into these categories are dis- who either gave or withheld social approval. When the confeder-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

tinct, and because all antecedent factors ultimately interact ate was introduced as having high status, participants gazed
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

with one another, [hey are separated here. more when the confederate was approving. Approval behavior
Love and friendship. Z. Rubin (1970) and Goldstein, Kilroy, did not influence gaze of participants toward a confederate of
and Van de Voort (1976) found that opposite-sex couples who low status.
expressed strong love for each other on a love scale engaged in Competence. Burroughs. Schultz, and Autrey (1973) re-
more mutual gaze than couples with low scores on the love scale. ported that research participants gave more gaze and leadership
Husbands and wives engaged in more eye contact when they votes to a confederate who made high- rather than low-quality
shared small rather than large levels of marital discord (Beier & arguments in a group discussion. Preschool children in a study
Sternberg, 1977). Russo( 1975) reported greater amounts of eye by Vaughan and Waters (1981) gazed more at peers who were
contact between elementary school children who were friends high rather than low in social competence.
rather than nonfriends. Coutts and Schneider (1976) recorded Summary. People gaze more at others who are in a position
more gaze between female friends than female strangers. Noller to provide (or receive) nurturance and approval. Further re-
(1980) reported that married couples with low scores on a mari- search should help delineate the kinds of relationships that are
tal adjustment scale engaged in relatively high amounts of vi- conducive to communicating nurturance and approval.
sual dominance. Spouses' looking patterns were significantly
correlated in couples with high and moderate marital adjust-
Situational Factors
ment, but not in couples with low marital adjustment.
Dependency. Research participants directed more gaze to- Researchers have investigated the effects of situational factors
ward an experimenter when they were made to feel highly de- on gazing behavior from a variety of approaches and their re-
pendent for assistance with a problem-solving task (Nevill, sults are difficult to organize in a meaningful fashion. Patterson
1974). L. Harris (1968) observed that younger preschool girls (1982, 1983) suggested that situational influences on nonverbal
gazed more at an adult woman than did older preschool girls interactions can be studied using the methodology of ecological
while being tested for toy preferences. These results were inter- psychology (Barker. 1968; Wicker, 1979).
preted with the explanation that younger preschool girls are Interactive setting. The nature of an interactive setting influ-
more dependent on women for attention and approval. Studies ences gazing behavior, as evidenced by the greater willingness
showing that preschool boys gaze less than preschool girls at of people in small towns versus large cities to engage in mutual
women suggest that boys are less dependent than girls on gaze with a stranger (Newman & McCauley, 1977; McCauley,
women for attention and approval (Ashear & Snortum. 1971; Coleman, & De Fusco, 1978). Two other examples are that peo-
L. Harris, 1968; Kleinke et al.. 1977). The fact that lastborn ple share more mutual gaze when seated opposite rather than
men gaze more than firstborn men was interpreted as sign of beside each other (Muirhead & Goldman, 1979), and that re-
conditioned dependency (Ickes & Turner, 1983). search participants gazed more at a confederate behind a one-
Mothers and infants. One group of studies in this section way mirror when they thought the confederate could see them
focused on identifying predictible patterns of gaze between (Goldberg & Mettee, 1969).
mothers and infants. Robson, Pedersen, and Moss (1969) re- Seating distance. A number of researchers reported that peo-
ported positive correlations between maternal affection and ple gaze less when seated close together rather than at a distance
mother-infant gazing, and Arco, Self, and Gutrecht (1979) (Argyle & Dean, 1965; Argyle & Ingham, 1972; Coutts & Led-
demonstrated that infants reciprocated experimentally deter- den, 1977; Coutts & Schneider, 1975; Fukuhara, 1977; Gold-
mined levels of gaze from their mothers. Peery and Stern (1976) berg etal., 1969; Patterson. !977;Russo. 1975; Schulz& Bare-
found reliable gazing frequency distributions for mothers and foot. 1974; Stephenson, Rutter, & Dore. 1973). The results of
infants across different activities. these studies have been used to support Argyle and Dean's
Other researchers explored gazing behavior as a function of (1965) equilibrium model (see Argyle & Cook, 1976, chap. 3).
infant age. Friedman et al. (1976) measured greater amounts Other researchers did not find a significant relation between
of eye contact between mothers and 4-month-old infants than seating distance and gazing behavior (Carr & Dabbs, 1974;
between mothers and 6- and 8-month-old infants. Farrah, Chapman. 1975; Kleinke, Staneski, & Pipp, 1975).
Hirschbiel, and Jay (1980) reported that mothers gazed more There is a methodological problem associated with using
at 6-momh-old than at 20- or 30-month-old infants. gaze-seating distance studies to test the equilibrium model. It
In a third type of study, which may be related to dependency has been pointed out that recordings of gaze are inflated when
GAZE AND EYE CONTACT 89

interactants are seated far apart (Knight, Langmeyer, & Lund- ond person will reciprocate with increased intimacy. When they
gren, 1973; Martin & Rovira, 1981; Stephenson & Rutter, are labeled negatively, the second person will compensate with
1970). Patterson (1975) concluded that this bias in gaze record- decreased intimacy. Because the intimacy-arousal model is rel-
ing is limited to experiments in which differences between gaz- evant only when people are motivated to seek labels for intima-
ing and nongazing are very subtle, and that it is not an issue cy-induced arousal (see Ellsworth. 1977, 1978;Knowles. 1977),
when recording gaze in natural interactions. There is also a con- it was subsumed by Patterson (1982, 1983) into his sequential
ceptual problem associated with gaze-seating distance studies model as a preinteraction mediator. The intimacy-arousal
as tests of the equilibrium model (see Cappella, 1981). A mech- model has raised a number of important research questions
anism is needed to account for reciprocation versus compensa- having to do with the relation between nonverbal intimacy and
tion. I suggest later how reciprocation and compensation can be physiological arousal and with people's predispositions to seek
analyzed by incorporating an attributional approach within the labels for arousal.
sequential model. Defining arousal. Patterson (1976, 1978)basedtheintimacy-
Distraction. Research participants gazed less at an experi- arousal model on self-labeling of changes in physiological
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

menter when they were recalling material involving competing arousal, but other researchers have used the term arousal more
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

rather than noncompeting associations (Stanley & Martin, generally. For example, Ellsworth and Langer (1976) defined the
1968). Argyle and Graham (1976) reported steadily decreasing stare as a stimulus "that elicits attention, arousal, and a sense
amounts of mutual gaze between people when they were ex- of interpersonal involvement" (p. 122). Ellsworth et al. (1972)
posed to visual stimuli that competed more and more for their denned the stare as a "salient stimulus which forcibly involves
attention. the subject in an interpersonal encounter and demands a re-
Speaking order. Lesko and Schneider (1978) demonstrated sponse" (p. 311). It is important for researchers to be clear
that gaze is influenced by speaking order by showing that the whether they are using "arousal" to mean physiological arousal
second speaker in a three-person group gazed most at the person or psychological arousal associated with heightened attention
who had just spoken. and interpersonal involvement. When measuring physiological
Summary. People gaze more at each other in settings condu- responses, researchers should be aware that different physiologi-
cive to communication. Gaze is enhanced in situations provid- cal measures are not necessarily correlated with one another,
ing face-to-face interaction, personalism, and minimal distrac- and that the notion of a generalized state of arousal has been
tion. Studies investigating the effects of situational factors on seriously challenged (Hassett, 1978).
gazing behavior are sometimes difficult to interpret because Ga:e and physiological responses. Research shows that un-
personal, experiential, and relational antecedents were not der some conditions, gaze can result in heightened physiological
taken into consideration. responses. Nichols and Champness (1971) and Strom and Buck
(1979) recorded increased galvanic skin responses when re-
Preinteraction Mediators search participants received direct gaze from an experimenter.
College men had significantly higher heart rates when they
In Patterson's (1982, 1983) sequential model, the effects of played a competitive game with a male confederate who stared
antecedent factors on nonverbal interactions are assumed to be at them (KJeinke & Pohlen, 1971). College women had slightly
mediated by behavioral predispositions, potential changes in higher heart rates when they received large amounts of gaze
arousal, and cognitive-affective assessment. The elaboration of from a female confederate in a social comparison experiment
these preinteraction mediators, as they operate in the interac- (Ellsworth et al., 1978). Heart rates of research participants
tion phase of the sequential model, is an important goal for gaze were significantly increased when a confederate became more
and eye contact research. intimate with gazing, smiling, and direct body orientation
(Coutts et al., 1980). Several investigators concluded that there
was evidence of physiological arousal in electroencephalogram
Behavioral Predispositions
(EEC) recordings of research participants who were exposed to
Behavioral predispositions are relatively stable characteris- high levels of gaze (Gale, Kingsley. Brookes, & Smith, 1978;
tics of individuals that mediate their responses in nonverbal in- Gale, Lucas, Nissim, & Harpham. 1972; Gale, Spratt, Chap-
teractions. Behavioral predispositions are primarily a product man, & Smallbone. 1975).
of the personal factors discussed earlier. Patterson (1982, 1983) Other researchers reported no effects of gaze on physiological
believes that behavioral predispositions are more or less auto- responses. Experimenter gaze did not significantly influence
matic and do not involve cognitive processes to the same degree skin conductance levels and skin conductance responses of par-
as changes in arousal and cognitive-affective assessment. ticipants in a study by Patterson, Jordan, Hogan, and Frerker
(1981). Leavitt and Donovan (1979) measured skin conduc-
tance and cardiac responses of mothers who viewed their gazing
Potential Changes in Arousal
and nongazing infants on a television monitor. No effects were
Patterson (1976. 1978) formulated an intimacy-arousal found for skin conductance. The cardiac data were complex
model based on the cognitive-arousal theories of Schachter and difficult to interpret because there was no neutral stimulus
(1964) and Mandler (1975). The intimacy-arousal model pro- control group. Martin and Gardner (1979) reported that gaze
poses that sufficiently large increases (or decreases) in intimate did not influence EEC responses in a noninteractive situation.
behaviors by one person toward another will result in changes To reconcile these inconsistent findings of the effects of gaze
in the second person's physiological arousal. When these on physiological responses, it is necessary to explain how the
changes in physiological arousal are labeled positively, the sec- influence of gaze on physiological responses works. It cannot be
90 CHRIS L. KJ.EINKE

accepted as "reasonable" (Bern. 1972, p. 46) that physiological theories of attribution and ingratiation. For example, gaze that
responses can be elicited by another person's gaze. It is neces- is attributed to situational causes may be interpreted differently
sary to understand the mechanics of this effect. KJeinke and from gaze that is attributed to the gazing person's dispositions
Pohlen ( 1 9 7 1 ) invoked F. K. Graham and Clifton's (1966) asso- (Jones & Davis, 1965; Jones & McGillis, 1976; Kelley, 1967).
ciation of heart rate deceleration with orienting reactions and Reactions to gaze are also likely to depend on perceptions of a
heart rate acceleration with defensive reactions to explain why gazing person's competitiveness, dependency, status, and men-
men had higher heart rates when playing a competitive game tal health. Ingratiation research suggests that perceived motives
with a gazing confederate. These results can also be related to for someone's gaze may vary according to whether he or she is
Obrist's (1976) research with the assumption that participants dependent or not dependent on the recipient's acceptance and
were tensing their muscles in response to the gazing confeder- approval (Jones & Wortman, 1973; Kleinke, Staneski, &
ate's provocation. In threatening situations, gaze from another Weaver, 1972; Staneski, Kleinke, & Meeker, 1977). Gaze would
person might evoke an active coping response with increased be a useful behavior to incorporate into studies of self-presenta-
heart rate and only minimal changes in overt behavior (Obrist, tion (Schlenker, 1980). Gazing behavior might also be a useful
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

1976). It is not known whether participants in published gaze


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

manipulation (or measure) in studies of self-focused attention


experiments were sufficiently stressed for an active coping re- (KJeinke, 1978; WickJund, 1975).
sponse. Se/f-OHweness ofga:e. Two questions related to self-aware-
Another study shedding light on the mechanics of gaze and ness of gaze concern people's accuracy in reporting their gaze
physiological responses was conducted by KJeinke (1981). Col- and the effects of self-perceived gazing behavior on one's atti-
lege women attempted to alter their bodily responses while re- tudes.
ceiving false biofeedbaek and either 85% or no gaze from a fe- People's accuracy in reporting their gazing behavior has only
male confederate. The unexpected finding that participants' recently come under investigation. KJeinke's (1981) research
heart rates were significantly higher in the no-gaze condition participants estimated their gaze as 17%, whereas it was actually
was explained in the following way.4 In the no-gaze condition, only 1 %. Participants in KJeinke and Walton's (1982) study esti-
participants typically followed the confederate's example of mated their gaze more accurately during an interview (esti-
concentrating and closing her eyes. Concentration is associated mated = 58%, actual = 45%), but it is not known whether these
with environmental rejection and the possible tensing of the estimates were biased by assumptions from base rates (Mischel,
muscles that result in increased heart rate (Hassett, 1978; 1968).6 It would be interesting to determine whether self-esti-
Obrist, 1976). In the 85% gaze condition, participants typically mates of gazing behavior are more accurate when people are
focused their attention on the confederate and objects in the experiencing mind fulness (Langer, 1978), personal involve-
room. Environmental intake and attention to external events ment (Taylor & Fiske, 1978), or self-awareness (Wicklund,
are associated with decreased heart rate (Hassett, 1978). 1975). Individual differences in self-awareness of gaze are also
worthy of further study. KJeinke and Walton (1982) found no
relation between accuracy in self-estimates of gazing, smiling,
Cognitive-Affective Assessment
and talking and scores on M. Snyder's (1974) self-monitoring
Cognitive-affective assessment reflects the influence on reac- scale.
tions to another person's gaze of attributions made about the With regard to the effects of self-perceived gazing on atti-
gazing person's intentions and motives. Other issues bearing on tudes. Bern's (1972) self-perception theory predicts that peo-
cognitive-affective assessment include people's relative aware- ple's attitudes are most likely influenced by their gazing behav-
ness of their own gazing behaviors and delineation of conditions iors when they are aware of their gaze and attribute it to their
under which people are most likely to be aware of and seek attri- own choice. KJeinke, Staneski, and Berger (1975) attempted to
butions for gaze they receive from others. demonstrate Bern's (1972) theory by reinforcing college men to
Attribution? about causes and motives. Kelley and Michela increase their gaze toward a female interviewer. Results indi-
(1980) denned attribution research as the study of perceptions cated that increased gaze had no effect on evaluations of the
about antecedents or causes of another person's behavior and interviewer, but it was not known whether participants were
uliribiitional research as the study of reactions to another's be- aware that their gazing behavior had increased. Participants'
havior. They combined these research approaches into a model verbal ratings were influenced when they were made aware of
describing how reactions to another person's behavior are in- their (ostensible) gaze toward another person with false feed-
fluenced by one's motives and expectations and one's percep- back (KJeinke etal.. 1973).
tions about the behavior's intentions and causes. Kelley and Mi- Auvireness ol ga:efrom others. The same questions related
chela's (1980) model provides a useful structure for analyzing to self-awareness of gaze are applicable to the awareness of gaze
cognitive-affective assessment in the sequential model. It can be from others. Ellsworth (1975) described gaze as a relatively sa-
predicted that gaze from another person will result in compen- lient stimulus in human interactions, but people's accuracy in
sation when the recipient wishes to avoid closeness with the gaz- estimating another person's gaze is often low. Argyle and Wil-
ing person and when the gaze is viewed as inappropriate, un-
modifiable. and the product of undesirable motives.5 Gaze is 4
Mean heart rates were 77.3 beats/min in the 85% gaze condition
likely to result in reciprocation when the recipient is motivated and 82.4 beats/min in the no-gaze condition. F( 1. 36) = 4.17. p < .05.
to seek closeness with the gazing person and when the gaze is 5
A parallel discussion relating theories of attribution to compensa-
viewed as appropriate, modifiable, and resulting from desirable tion and reciprocation in self-disclosure can be found in Kleinke(l979).
6
intentions and motives. Many hypotheses about what consti- Participants estimated that they smiled 549t- of the time and talked
tutes appropriate and inappropriate gaze can be derived from 611 of the time. The actual values were 19"i and 54°o, respectively.
GAZE AND EYE CONTACT 91

Mams (1969) found that research participants did not distin- If the compensatory response does not result in a stable ex-
guish between the gazing behaviors of a confederate who inten- change, the interaction will be terminated.
tionally gazed 80% or 20% of the time. Participants in KJeinke's Kleinke. Staneski. and Pipp (1975) pointed out the progres-
(1981) study estimated that an 85% gazing confederate had sion in gaze and eye contact research from simple comparisions
gazed 27% of the time and that a nongazing confederate had between levels of gaze to investigations of gaze in relation to
gazed 7% of the time. One factor affecting accuracy in assessing specific contexts and situations. It is useful now to examine how
gaze received from another person is one's attention and mind- the interaction phase of the sequential model can account for
fulness toward the interaction. It can be predicted that people the results of these studies.
will have greatest accuracy in reporting another person's gaze
when they are in novel situations (Langer. 1978), when they are Avoidance and Escape
experiencing emotional arousal or personal involvement (Tay-
lor & Fiske, 1978). when they are threatened with unexpected Studies of avoidance and escape are a good example of unsta-
consequences (Pittman & Pittman, 1980), and when they have ble exchanges that were terminated because of excessively high
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

empathy toward the gazing person (Harvey, Yarkin, Lightner. gaze from another person. It is noteworthy that staring did not
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

& Town, 1980; see Kleinke. 1986. chap. 8). Personal motives result in as much escape when the staring person smiled or was
and expectations also affect the assessment of another's gaze. physically attractive. Future research should determine the de-
Research participants interacted with a peer, falsely believing gree to which escape from staring is mediated by self-labeling
the peer would view them with a facial scar or a history of epi- of physiological responses versus cognitive-affective assessment.
lepsy (KJeck & Strenta, 1980). As a result of this self-perceived
stigma, participants assumed they were given reduced amounts Gaze and Compliance
of eye contact.
The operation of the interaction phase was demonstrated in
In addition to learning more about accuracy in assessing an-
studies comparing conditions under which gaze results in in-
other's gaze, researchers are encouraged to explore people's
creased versus decreased compliance. People were more likely
awareness of how another's gaze influences their own feelings
to terminate interactions with gazing experimenters in contexts
and behaviors. Research suggests that people can be influenced
that were ambiguous or illegitimate. Experimenter gaze in these
by another's gaze without being aware of it (Zajonc, 1980) and
interactions was apparently assessed as inappropriate and the
that their awareness of this influence should be greatest when
interaction remained unstable. In contrast to these findings,
the effects of the gaze are salient and plausible (Nisbett & Wil-
gaze resulted in increased compliance when it came from ex-
son, 1977; Taylor & Fiske, 1978). Studies that did not take these
perimenters whose requests were unambiguous or legitimate.
research conclusions into account have found mixed results.
Gaze from experimenters in these contexts presumably resulted
Tankard (1970) found no difference in evaluations given by re-
in a stable interaction because it was expected and assessed as
search participants who did or did not notice the gazing pat-
appropriate. As with escape and avoidance, it is not known how
terns of a person they were rating in a photograph. Experi-
much compliance under conditions of gaze versus no gaze is
menter gaze led to increased compliance in Brockner et al.'s
influenced by cognitive-affective assessment versus self-labeled
(1982) experiment, but the subjects were not able to report the
physiological responses.
effects of this gaze on their behavior. Cook and Smith (1975)
reported that about half of their research participants noticed
the gazing behaviors of a confederate who gazed at them con- Gazing Person's Physical Attractiveness
stantly, intermittently, or not at all during a conversation. Par- Research suggesting that attractive women receive the "bene-
ticipants who noticed the confederate's gazing behavior were fit of the doubt" when withholding gaze was related to cultural
more influenced in their ratings of the experimenter than par- biases influencing expectations about (gazing) behaviors viewed
ticipants who did not. as acceptable or unacceptable from others with different
amounts of physical attractiveness. People apparently make
Interaction Phase different cognitive-affective assessments when receiving high
and low amounts of gaze from attractive versus unattractive
Patterson's (1982. 1983) sequential model describes how re- others.
sponses in interpersonal interactions are moderated by prein-
teraction mediators. People enter interactions with expectations
Positive and Negative Evaluations
about the amount of intimacy or involvement that is appropri-
ate. When the expected and actual involvement are discrepant, Previous models of nonverbal interaction predicted recipro-
the interaction is described as unstable and it can be predicted cation to gaze in positive interactions and compensation to gaze
that the discrepantly high or low involvement will result in com- in negative interactions. However, the interaction phase of the
pensation. The compensatory response in unstable interactions sequential model explains how these outcomes can be reversed.
is determined by a second stage of cognitive-affective assess- Ellsworth and Carlsmith (1968) and Reis and Halek (1976)
ment. When the valence of this cognitive reassessment is posi- reported that research participants responded positively to ex-
tive (the other person's intentions are judged favorably, the dis- perimenters who gazed during an interview or news report with
crepant intimacy is accepted, potential physiological responses favorable content. The experimenter's gazing behavior in these
are labeled positively), the compensatory response will be one situations was apparently expected and assessed as appropriate.
of approach. When the valence of the cognitive reassessment is In contrast, Kleinke and Pohlen (1971 (concluded that the com-
negative, the compensatory response will be one of avoidance. bination of constant gaze and cooperation by a confederate dur-
92 CHRIS L. KLEINKE

ing a two-person game was "inappropriate and vexing." Scher- functions and motives for gaze. Some examples of differential
witz and Helmreich (1973) similarly concluded that too much perceptions of gaze have been demonstrated in studies compar-
gaze from an experimenter who delivered a favorable evaluation ing men and women. The influence of experiential and situa-
was viewed by recipients as a "breach of a norm" because it was tional factors on gaze might often be automatic or unconscious,
too intimate for the nature of the interaction. but situations can be identified in which certain types of people
Expectations and assessment also moderate reactions to gaze engage in cognitive-affective assessment of gaze. More research
in negative interactions. Participants in a study by Schaefler and is needed to outline people's expectations about the meaning
Patterson (1980) reacted unfavorably to experimenter gaze in a and appropriateness of gazing behavior in different contexts and
crowded room primarily when they expected an incompatible relationships. It would also be useful to learn more about how
group discussion. Ellsworth and Carlsmith (1968) concluded people interpret gazing behaviors when they are undergoing
that research participants preferred an interviewer with losv various kinds of experiences or emotions.
gaze during a negative interview because of their assessment of
the interviewer's tactfulness in averting gaze during an awkward
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

interaction. Coutts et al. (1980) concluded that their research Potential Physiological Arousal
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

participants reciprocated with high amounts of gaze to a nega-


tive confederate in order to encourage a level of intimacy appro- It would be useful to demonstrate conditions under which
priate for a stable exchange. Ickeset al. (1982) found that people gaze results in increases, decreases, or no change in physiologi-
increased their gaze when expecting a negative interaction in cal responding. One way to do this would be to assess (or manip-
order to encourage a friendly outcome. Participants in Ells- ulate) a recipient's expectations, with the hypothesis that unex-
worth and Carlsmith's (1973) experiment delivered electric pected gazing (or nongazing) is more apt to cause physiological
shocks to a gazing confederate when it appeared that the shocks changes than expected gazing (or nongazing; see Cappella &
would make the confederate gaze less. Greene, 1982; Ickes et al., 1982). Judgments about whether a
gazing person has positive or negative intentions might also

Summary have differential effects on physiological responses.


In studying the effects of gaze on physiological responses, it
Although the above studies were conducted prior to the devel- would be advisable to make the distinction between physiologi-
opment of the sequential model, the model provides a useful cal responses that are internally detected, externally detected,
framework for integrating and explaining their results. At pres- and undetected (Cialdini, Petty, & Cacioppo, 1981, pp. 373-
ent, the compatibility between gaze and eye contact research 375). Having accomplished such a demonstration of gaze-in-
and the sequential model is inferential and retrospective. It will duced physiological change, an attempt could be made to in-
be necessary in future research to predict and measure the de- fluence the labels attached to this experience (see Coutts et al.,
gree to which reactions to gaze are mediated by (a) expectations 1980; KJeinke, 1981). It would be important to distinguish be-
about appropriate amounts of gaze in a given interaction, (b) tween behavioral measures and self-ratings of affect and to con-
attributions about the gazing person's motives, and (c) self-la- ceptualize self-labeling of physiological responses as a cause-
beling of gaze-induced physiological responses (see KJeinke, seeking as well as a social comparison process (Maslach,
1980). The inclusion of dispositional. physiological, and cogni- 1979a). Researchers must also bear in mind that self-labeling of
tive measures should help accomplish this goal. arousal occurs primarily when people experience unexplained
physiological changes, and that this is a rare occurrence in ev-
eryday life (Marshall & Zimbardo, 1979; Maslach, 1979a,
Some Research Directions
1979b). Finally, within-subjects designs are probably more rele-
A number of research questions were suggested during the vant than between-subjects designs for studying physiological
course of this review. Two general directions for gaze and eye responses related to changes in intimacy (Patterson, 1976,
contact research can be outlined. First, researchers can add to 1978).
our knowledge about the functions of gaze and the operation of
its antecedents. Second, we should learn more about the effects
of preinteraction mediators on gazing behavior and on reac- Cognitive-Affective Assessment
tions to gaze from others. I suggest some questions to be an-
swered about preinteraction mediators in the next section. Suggestions were given earlier about how theories of attribu-
tion can be used to make predictions about explanations and
interpretations that are given to one's gaze and the gazing be-
Behavioral Dispositions
haviors of others. Investigators have measured attributions
There are a number of ways behavioral dispositions can be about gazing and nongazing experimenters in field settings by
included in the paradigm shift (Bern, 1972) in social psychology asking recipients to evaluate the experimenters (Brockner et al.,
toward cognitive processes. Recent interest in cognitive pro- 1982: see also R. J. Smith & Knowles, 1978, 1979). Patterson
cesses has led researchers to measure predispositions to attend (1982) suggested Cacioppo and Petty's (1981) thought-listing
to causes and explanations for behaviors, and it is likely that procedure as an index of cognitive-affective assessment. Patter-
these measures can be related to an awareness of one's own and son's (1983, p. 168) video replay method might also prove use-
another's gaze (Cacioppo & Petty, 1981; Snyder, 1974). Person- ful for this purpose. It would be valuable to learn more about
ality studies on gaze and reactions to gaze might be expanded situations or personality traits that are predictive of high versus
to explore the relation between personality and perceptions of low accuracy in reporting gazing behavior by oneselfand others.
GAZE AND EYE CONTACT 93

Concluding Comment studying the environment of human behavior. Stanford, CA: Stanford
University Press.
The sequential model has provided a useful framework for Bayes, M. A. (1972). Behavioral cues of interpersonal warmth. Journal
organizing gaze and eye contact research. The sequential model of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 39, 333-339.
Beattie. G. W. (1978). Floor apportionment and gaze in conversational
views interpersonal gaze as a dynamic rather than static pro-
dyads. British Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology. 17. 7-15.
cess. It suggestsa multivariate approach in which gaze is denned
Beattie, G. W. (1979). Contextual constraints on the floor-apportion-
as part of a system of involvement behaviors. Researchers are
ment function of speaker-gaze in dyadic conversation. British Journal
encouraged to take into account the interaction process denned of Social and Clinical Psychology, IS, 391-392.
by the sequential model in their studies of gazing behavior. Beebe. S. A. (1974). Eye contact: A nonverbal determinant of speaker
credibility. The Speech Teacher, 23.21-25.
Beier. E. G., & Steinberg, D. P. (1977). Marital communication: Subtle
References cues between newly-weds. Journal of Communication, 27, 92-103.
Bellack, A. S., Heisen, M., & Lamparski, D. (1979). Role-play tests for
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Abele, A. (1981). Acquaintance and visual behavior between two inter- assessing social skills: Are they valid? Are they useful? Journal of Con-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

actants: Their communicative function for the impression formation sulting and Clinical Psychology, 47, 335-342.
of an observer. European Journal of Social Psychology. I I , 409-425. Bellack. A. S., Hersen, M., & Turner, S. M. (1978). Role-play tests for
Abramovitch, R., & Daly, E. M. (1978). Children's use ofhead orienta- assessing social skills: Are they valid? Behavior Therapy, 9, 448-461.
tion and eye contact in making attributions of affiliation. Child Devel- Bellack. A. S., Hersen, M.. & Turner, S. M. (1979). Relationship of role
opment. 49, 5 \9-522. playing and knowledge of appropriate behavior to assertion in the
Adams, R. M., & Kirkevold, B. (1978). Looking, smiling, laughing, and natural environment. Journal oj Consulting and Clinical Psychology,
moving in restaurants: Sex and age differences. Emironmemal Psy- 47, 670-678.
chology and Nonverbal Behavior. 3, 117-121. Bern. D. J. (1972). Self-perception theory. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.). Ad-
Aiello. J. R. (1972). A test of equilibrium theory: Visual interaction in vances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 6 pp. 1-62). New
relation to orientation, distance, and sex of interactants. Psychonomic York: Academic Press.
Science. 27,335-336. Bloom. K. (1975). Social elicitation of infant vocal behavior. Journal of
Aiello, J. R. (1977a). A further look at equilibrium theory: Visual inter- Experimental Child Psychology. 20, 51-58.
action as a function of interpersonal distance. Emironmemal Psy- Bond. M. H., & Goodman, G. N. (1980). Gaze patterns and interaction
chology and Nonverbal Behavior, I , 112-140. contexts: Effects on personality impressions and attributions. Psy-
Aiello. J . R . ( l 9 7 7 b ) . Visual interaction at extended distances. Personal- chology: An International Journal of Psychology in the Orient. 23.
ity and Social Psychology Bulletin, 3, 83-86. 70-77.
Altman, I., & Taylor, D. (1973). Social penetration: The development of Bond. M. H.. & Komai. H. (1976). Targets of gazing and eye contact
interpersonal relationships. New York: Holt. Rinehart & Winston. during interviews: Effects on Japanese nonverbal behavior. Journal of
Amalfitano, J. G., & Kalt, N. C. (1977). Effects of eye contact on the Personality and Social Psychology'. 34. 1276-1284.
evaluation of job applicants. Journal of Employment Counseling, 14, Breed. G. (1972). The effect of intimacy: Reciprocity or retreat? British
46-48. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, II, 135-142.
Anderson. D. R. (1976). Eye contact, topic intimacy, and equilibrium Breed, G., & Colaiuta, V. (1974). Looking, blinking, and sitting: Non-
theory. Journal of Social Psychology, 100, 313-314. verbal dynamics in the classroom. Journal of Communication. 24.
Arco, C. M., Self, P. A., & Gutrecht, N. (1979). The effect of increased 74-81.
maternal visual regard on neonatal visual behavior. Journal of Clini- Breed, G.. & Porter, M. (1972). Eye contact, attitudes, and attitude
cal Child Psychology. 8, 117-120. change among males. Journal of Social Psychology, 120. 211-217.
Argyle, M. (1972). Non-verbal communication in human social interac- Brockner, J.. Pressman, B.. Cabitt, J., & Moran, P. (1982). Nonverbal
tion. In R. A. Hinde (Ed.), Non-verbal communication. Cambridge, intimacy, sex. and compliance: A field study. Journal of Nonverbal
England: Cambridge University Press. Behavior, 6, 253-258.
Argyle, M., & Cook, M. (1976). Ga:e and mutual gaze. Cambridge, Buchanan, D. R., Goldman, M., Juhnke, R. (1977). Eye contact, sex,
England: Cambridge University Press. and the violation of personal space. Journal of Social Psychology, 103,
Argyle, M., & Dean, J. (1965). Eye-contact, distance, and affiliation. 19-25.
Sociometry, 28. 289-304. Bull, R., & Gibson-Robinson, E. (1981). The influence ofeye-gaze, style
Argyle, M., & Graham, J. A. (1976). The Central European of dress, and locality on the amounts of money donated to charity.
experiment—looking at persons and looking at objects. Environmen- Human Relations. 34, 895-905.
tal Psychology and Nonverbal Behavior, 1.6-16. Burns, J. A.. & Kintz. B. L. (1976). Eye contact while lying during an
Argyle, M., & Ingham, R. (1972). Gaze, mutual gaze and proximity. interview. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 7, 87-89.
Semiolica, 6, 32-49. Burroughs, W, Schultz, W., & Autrey, S. (1973). Quality of argument,
Argyle, M., Lalljee, M., & Cook, M. (1968). The effects of visability on leadership votes, and eye contact in three-person leaderless groups.
interaction in a dyad. Human Relations, 21. 3-17. Journal of Social Psychology. 90. 89-93.
Argyle, M., Lefebvre, L. M.. & Cook. M. (1974). The meaning of five Burton. A., McGregor. H.. & Berry, P. (1979). The effects of social rein-
patterns of gaze. European Journal of Social Psychology, 4, 125-136. forcement on dominant and dependent mildly intellectually handi-
Argyle, M., & Williams, M. (1969). Observer or observed? A reversible capped school-leavers. British Journal of Social and Clinical Psychol-
perspective in person perception. Soclomeliy, 32,396-412. ogy. IS. 129-133.
Ashear, V., & Snortum. J. R. (1971). Eye contact in children as a func- Cacioppo, J. T. & Petty, R. E. (1981). Social psychological procedures
tion of age, sex, social and intellective variables. Developmental Psy- for cognitive response assessment: The thought listing technique. In
chology: 4. 479. T. V. Merluzzi, C. R. Glass, & M. Genest (Eds.), Cognitive assess-
Bailey, K. G., Tipton. R. M., & Taylor. P. F. (1977). The threatening ment. New York: Guilford Press.
stare: Differential response latencies in mild and profoundly retarded Campbell, D. E.. & Lancioni. G. E. (1979). The effects of staring and
adults. American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 81, 599-602. pew invasion in church settings. Journal of Social Psychology, 108,
Barker, R. G. (1968). Ecological psychology: Concepts and methods for 19-24.
94 CHRIS L. KJ-EINKE

Cappella, J. N. (1981). Mutual influence in expressive behavior: Adult- Daniell, R. J., & Lewis, P. (1972). Stability of eye contact and physical
adult and infant-adult dyadic interaction. Ps\-chological Bulletin. 89. distance across a series of structured interviews. Journal of Consult-
101-132. ing and Clinical Psychology. 39, 172.
Cappella. J. N.. & Greene. J. O. (1982). A discrepancy-arousal explana- Dovidio, J. F.. & Ellyson. S. L. (1982). Decoding visual dominance:
tion of mutual influence in expressive behavior for adult and infant- Attributions of power based on relative percentages of looking while
adult interaction. Communication Monographs. 49. 89-114. speaking and looking while listening. Social Psychology Quarterly,
Caproni, V.. Levine, D., O'Neal. E., McDonald. R.. & Garwood, G. 45, 106-113.
(1977). Seating position, instructor's eye contact availability, and stu- Duncan. S. D. (1972). Some signals and rules for taking speaking turns
dent participation in a small seminar. Journal of Social Psychology, in conversations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology: 23,
103. 315-316. 283-292.
Carnevale. P. J. D.. Pruitt, D. G., & Seilheimer. S. D. (1981). Looking Duncan, S. D.. & Fiske, D. W. (1977). Face-to-face interaction: Re-
and competing: Accountability and visual access in integrative bar- search, methods, andtheory. Hillsdale. NJ: Erlbaum.
gaining. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 40, II1-120. Duncan. S. D.. & Niederehe, G. (1974). On signalling that it's your turn
Carr, S. J., & Dabbs, J. M. (1974). The effects of lighting, distance and to speak. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 10, 234-247.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

intimacy of topic on verbal and visual behavior. Sociometry. J7, 592- Edelmann. R. J., & Hampson, S. E. (1979). Changes in non-verbal be-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

600. havior during embarrassment. British Journal of Social and Clinical


Carr, S. J.. Dabbs. J. M., & Carr, T. S. (1975). Mother-infant attach- Psychology, IS. 385-390.
ment: The importance of the mother's visual field. Child Develop- Edelmann, R. J.. & Hampson, S. E. (1981). Embarrassment in dyadic
meni, 46. 331-338. interaction. Social Behavior and Personality, 9. 171-177.
Cary, M. S. (I978a). Does civil inattention exist in pedestrian passing? Efran, J. S. (1968). Looking for approval: Effects on visual behavior of
Journal of Personality and Social Psycho/og): 36, 1185-1193. approbation from persons differing in importance. Journal ofPerson-
Cary, M. S. (I978b). The role of gaze in the initiation of conversation. ality and Social Psychology, 10. 21-25.
Social Psychology: 41. 269-271. Efran. J. S., & Broughton, A. (1966). Effect of expectancies for social
Chapman, A. J. (1975). Eye contact, physical proximity and laughter: approval on visual behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
A re-examination of the equilibrium model of social intimacy. Social chology, 4. 103-107.
Behavior and Personality. 3, 143-155. Efran. M. G., & Cheyne, J. A. (1974). Affective concomitants of the
Cherulnik, P. D.. Neelv, W. T., Flanagan, M.. & Zachau. M. (1978). invasion of shared space: Behavioral, physiological, and verbal indi-
Social skill and visual interaction. Journal of Social Psychology. 104, cators. Journal of Personality and Social Pswhology, 29, 219-226.
263-270. Eisler. R. M., Hersen. M., Miller. P. M.. & Blanchard, E. B. (1975).
Cialdini. R. B.. Petty. R. E.. & Cacioppo. J. T. (1981). Altitude and Situational determinants of assertive behaviors. Journal of Consult-
attitude change. In M. R. Rosenweig & L. W. Porter (Eds.). Annual ing and Clinical Psychology, 43. 330-340.
review of psychology (Vo\. 32, pp. 357-404). Palo Alto, CA: Annual Ekman, P.. & Friesen, W. V. (1969). The repertoire of nonverbal behav-
Reviews. ior: Categories, origins, usage and codings. Semiotica, 1.49-97.
Clancy. H. G. (1976). Integrating Ihe environment in therapy. Man- Ellsworth. P. C. (1975). Direct gaze as a social stimulus: The example
Emironmem Systems. 6. 305-315. of aggression. In P. Pliner, L. Krames, & T. Alloway (Eds.), Nonverbal
Coates, B. (1978). Consistency of attachment behavior in the human communication of aggression (pp. 53-76). New \ork: Plenum Press.
infant: A multivariate approach. Child Study Journal, 8, 131-148. Ellsworth, P. C. (1977. August). Some questions about the role of arousal
Cohen. L. J., & Campos, J. J. (1974). Father, mother, and stranger as in the interpretation of direct gaze. Paper presented at the meeting of
elicitors of attachment behaviors in infancy. Developmental Psychol- the American Psychological Association, San Francisco.
ogy. 10, 146-154. Ellsworth, P. C. (1978). The meaningful look. Semio/ica. 24, 341-351.
Condon, W. S. (1980). The relation of interactional synchrony to cogni- Ellsworth, P. C.. & Carlsmith. J. M. (1968). Effects of eye contact and
tive and emotional processes. In M. R. Key (Ed.). The relationship of verbal content on affective response to a dyadic interaction. Journal
verbal and nonverbal communication (pp. 49-65). The Hague, The of Personality and Social Psychology. 10. 1 5-20.
Netherlands: Mouton. Ellsworth, P. C., & Carlsmith, J. M. (1973). Eye contact and gaze aver-
Conger, J. C... & Farrell. A. D. (1981). Behavioral components ofhetero- sion in an aggressive encounter. Journal of Personality and Social Ps.\'-
social skills, Behavior Therapy. 12, 41-55. chology, 28. 280-292.
Cook, M., & Smith. M. C. (1975). The role of gaze in impression forma- Ellsworth, P. C., Carlsmith, J. M.. & Henson, A. (1972). The stare as a
tion. British Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 14. 19-25. stimulus to flight in human subjects: A series of field experiments.
Coutts, L. M., & Ledden. M. (1977). Nonverbal compensatory reac- Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 21. 302-311.
tions to changes in interpersonal proximity. Journal of Social Ps \~chol- Ellsworth, P. C., Friedman, H. S., Perlick, D., & Hoyt, M. E. (1978).
ogy. 102. 283-290. Some effects of gaze on subjects motivated to seek or to avoid social
Coutts, L. M.. & Schneider, F. W. (1975). Visual behavior in an unfo- comparison. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 14,69-87.
cused interaction as a function of sex and distance. Journal of Experi- Ellsworth, P. C.. & Langer, E. J. (1976). Staring and approach. An inter-
mental Social Psychology, II, 64-77. pretation of the stare as a nonspecific activator. Journal of Personality
Coutts, L. M., & Schneider, F. W. (1976). Affiliative conflict theory: An and Social Psychology, 33. 117-122.
investigation of the intimacy equilibrium and compensation hypoth- Ellsworth. P. C.. & Ludwig, L. M. (1972). Visual behavior in social in-
esis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 34, 1135-1142. teraction. Journal of Communication. 22, 375-403.
Coutls, L. M., Schneider. F. W., & Montgomery, S. (1980). An investiga- Ellsworth, P. C., & Ross, L. D. (1975). Intimacy in response to direct
tion of Ihe arousal model of interpersonal intimacy. Journal of Exper- gaze. Journal of Experimental S(tcial Psychology, 11, 592-613.
imental Social Psychology, 16. 545-561. Ellyson, S. L., Dovidio, J. F., & Corson. R. L. (1981). Visual differences
Cranach. M. von (1971). The role of orienting behavior in human inter- in females as a function of self-perceived expertise. Journal of Nonver-
action. In A. H. Esser (Ed.), Behavior and environment: The use of bal Behavior. 5, 164-171.
space by animals and men (pp. 217-237). New York: Plenum Press. Ellyson, S. L., Dovidio, J. F., Corson. R. L.. & Vinicur. D. L. (1980).
Dabbs, J. M.. Evans. M. S.. Hopper, C. H., & Purvis, J. A. (1980). Self- Visual dominance behavior in female dyads: Situational and person-
monitors in conversation: What do they monitor? Journal of Person- ality factors. Social Psychology Quarterly, 43. 328-336.
ality and Social Psychology, 39. 278-284. Elman. D.. Schulte. D. C.. & Bukoff. A. (1977). Effects of facial expres-
GAZE AND EYE CONTACT 95

sion and stare duration on walking speed: Two field experiments. En- Fry, R., & Smith. G. F. (1975). The effects of feedback and eye contact
vironmental Psychology and Non-verbal Behavior, 2, 93-99. on performance of a digit-coding task. Journal of Social Psychology,
Ernest, R. C., & Cooper, R. E. (1974). "Hey mister, do you have any 96, 145-146.
change?": Two real world studies of proxemic effects on compliance Fugita. S. S. (1974). Effects of anxiety and approval on visual interac-
with a mundane request. Personality and Social Psychology' Bulletin. tion. Journal ol Personality and Social Psychology, 29, 586-592.
1. 158-159. Fugita, S. S.. Agle. T. A.. Newman, [., & Walfish, N. (1977). Atlraclive-
Exline, R. V. (1963). Explorations in the process of person perception: ness, self-concept, and a methodological note about gaze behavior.
Visual interaction in relation to competition, sex, and need for affil- Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 3. 240-243.
iation. Journal of Personality, 31, 1-20. Fukuhara. S. (1977). An empirical study of visual behavior in social
Exline, R. V. (1971). Visual interaction: The glances of power and pref- interaction: E>e contact in a dyad as affected by interaction distance
erence. In J. K. Cole (Ed.). Nebraska Symposium an Motivation (Vet. and the affiliation need. Japanese Journal of Experimental Social
19, pp. 162-205). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press. Psychology, I T. 30-38.
Exline, R. V., & Eldridge, C. (1967, April). Effects of i\m patterns of a Galassi, J. P., Hollandsworth, J. G.. Radecki, J. C., Gay, M. L., Howe,
speaker's visual behavior upon the perception of the authenticity of M. R., & Evans, C. L. (1976). Behavioral performance in the valida-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

his verbal message. Paper presented at the meeting of the Eastern tion of an assertiveness scale. Behavior Therapy. 7, 447-452.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Psychological Association, Boston. Gale, A., Kingsley, E., Brookes. S.. & Smith. D. (1978). Cortical arousal
Exline, R. V., Ellyson, S. L., & Long. B. (1975). Visual behavior as an and social intimacy in the human female under different conditions
aspect of power role relationships. In P. Pliner, L. Krames, & T. Allo- of eye contact. Behavioral Processes, 3. 271-275.
way (Eds.). Nonverbal communication of aggression (pp. 21-52). New Gale, A.. Lucas. B., Nissim, R., & Harpham. D. (1972). Some EEC
\brk: Plenum Press. correlates of face-to-face contact. British Journal of Social and Clini-
Exline, R. V., & Fehr, B. J. (1982). The assessment of gaze and mutual cal Psychology. II. 326-332.
gaze. In K.. R. Scherer & P. Ekman (Eds.). Ham/book of methods in Gale, A., Spratt, G., Chapman. A. J., & Smallbone, A. (1975). EEG
nonverbal behavior research. Cambridge, England: Cambridge Uni- correlates of eye contact and interpersonal distance. Biological Psy-
versity Press. chology. 3. 237-245.
Exline, R. V., Gray, D., & Schuette. D. (1965). Visual behavior in a Garcia, E. E., & Trujillo. A. (1977). The effects of experimenter facial
dyad as affected by interview content and sex of respondent. Journal
orientation during imitation maintenance. Journal of Applied Behav-
of Personality and Social Psychology; 3. 201-209.
ior Analysis, 10. 95.
Exline. R. V, & Messick, D. (1967). The effects of dependency and so-
Gardin, H., Kaplan, K. J.. Firestone, I. J., & Cowan, G. A. (1973). Prox-
cial reinforcement upon visual behavior during an interview. British
emic effects on cooperation, attitude, and approach-avoidance in a
Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology. 6, 256-266.
Prisoner's Dilemma game. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
Exline, R. V., Thibaut, J., Brannon, C., & Gumpert, P. (1961). Visual
ogy, 21. 13-18.
interaction in relation to Machiavellianism and an unethical act.
Glasgow. R. E.. & Arkowitz, H. (1975). The behavioral assessment of
American Psjvliologisi, 16, 396.
male and female social competence in dyadic heterosexual interac-
Exline, R. V., & Winters, L. C. (1965). Affective relations and mutual
tions. Behavior Therapy, 6.488-498.
glances in dyads. In S. S. Tomkins & C. E. bard (Eds.), Affect, cogni-
Goffman. E. (1964). Behavior in public places. New York: Free Press.
tion, and personality (pp. 319-350). New York: Springer.
Goldberg, G. N.. KJesler, C. A., & Collins, B. E. (1969). Visual behavior
Farrah, D. C.. Hirschbiel, P.. & Jay. S. (1980). Toward interactive syn-
and face-to-face distance during interaction. Sociometry, 32. 43-53.
chrony: The gaze patterns of mothers and children in three age
Goldberg, G. N.. & Mettee, D. R. (1969). Likingand perceived commu-
groups. International Journal of Behavioral Development, S. 215-
nication potential as determinants of looking at another. Psycho-
224.
nomic Science. 16, 277-278.
Feldstein, S., & Welkowitz, J. A. (1978). A chronography of conversa-
Goldberg, M. L., & Wellens. A. R. (1979). A comparison of nonverbal
tion: In defense of an objective approach. In A. W. Siegman & S.
compensatory behaviors within direct fact-to-face and television-me-
Feldstein (Eds.), Nonverbal behavior and communication. Hillsdale.
diated interviews. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 9, 250-260.
NJ: Erlbaum.
Goldman. M. (1980). Effect of eye contact and distance on the verbal
Field, T. (1981). Infant arousal attention and affect during early interac-
reinforcement of attitude. Journal of Social Psychology, 111, 73-78.
tions. Advances in Infancy Research, 1, 57-100.
Finch, B. E.. & Wallace, C. J. (1977). Successful interpersonal skills Goldman, M.. & Fordyce, J. (1983). Prosocial behavior as affected by
training with schizophrenic inpatients. Journal of Consulting and eye contact, touch, and voice expression. Journal of Social Psychol-
Clinical Psychology, 45, 885-890. ogy, 121. 125-129.
Foddy, M. (1978). Patterns of gaze in cooperative and competitive nego- Goldstein, M. A., Kilroy. M. C., & Van de Voort, D. (1976). Gaze as a
tiation. Human Relations, 31.925-938. function of conversation and degree of love. Journal of Psychology.
Forbes, R. J., & Jackson, P. R. (1980). Nonverbal behavior and the out- 92. 227-234.
come of selection interviews. Journalof Occupational Psychology, 53, Graham, F. K., & Clifton, R. K. (1966). Heart-rate change as a compo-
65-72. nent of the orienting response. Psychological Bulletin, 65, 305-320.
Frelz, B. R., Cora. R.. Tuemmler, J. M.. & Bellet. W. (1979). Counselor Graham, J. A.. & Argyle. M. (1975). The effects of different patterns of
nonverbal behaviors and client evaluations. Journal of Counseling gaze combined with different facial expressions on impression forma-
Psychology, 26, 304-311. tion. Journal of Human Movement Studies. I, 178-182.
Friedman, S., Thompson, M. A., Crawley, S., Criticos, A., Drake, D., Greenbaum. P.. & Rosenfeld, H. M. (1978). Patterns of avoidance in
lacobbo, M., Rogers, P. P.. & Richardson, L. (1976). Mutual visual response to interpersonal staring and proximity: Effects of bystanders
regard during mother-infant play. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 42. on drivers at a traffic intersection. Journal of Personality and Social
427-431. Psychology. 36, 575-587.
Frieze. 1. H., & Ramsey, S. J. (1976). Nonverbal maintenance of tradi- Greenwald, D. P. (1977). The behavioral assessment of differences in
tional sex roles. Journal of Social Issues. 32, 133-141. social skill and social anxiety in female college students. Behavior
Fromme, D. K.. & Beam, D. C. (1974). Dominance and sex differences Therapy. 8. 925-937.
in nonverbal responses to differential eye contact. Journal of Re- Griffitt. W., May, J., & Veitch, R. (1974). Sexual stimulation and inter-
search in Personality, 8, 76-87. personal behavior: Heterosexual evaluative responses, visual behav-
96 CHRIS L. KLEINKE

ior, and physical proximity. Journal of Personality and Social Psy- Jones, E. E., & Wortman, C. D. (1973). Ingratiation: An atlributional
chology. 30. 367-377. approach. Morristown. NJ: General Learning Press.
Haase, R. F. & Tepper, D. T. (1972). Nonverbal components ofem- Kelley, H. H. (1967). Attribution theory. In D. Levine (Ed.). Nebraska
pathie communication. Journal of Counseling Ps\tiiologv. 19, 417- Symposium on Motivation (Vol. 15, pp. 192-238). Lincoln: Univer-
424. sity of Nebraska Press.
Harper. R. G., Wiens, A. N., & Matarazzo, J. D. (1978). Nonverbal Kelley, H. H., & Michela. J. L. (1980). Attribution theory and research.
communication: The state of/heart. New York: Wiley. In M. R. Rosenzweig & L. W. Porter (Eds.), Annual review of psychol-
Harris, F. C, & Ciminero, A. R. (1978). The effect of witnessing conse- ogy (Vol. 31, pp. 457-501). Palo Alto, CA: Annual Reviews.
quences on the behavioral recordingof experimental observers. Jour- Kelly, E. W. (1978). Effects of counselor's eye contact on student-clients'
nal of Applied Behavior Analysis, II. 513-521. perceptions. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 46, 627-632.
Harris, L. (1968). Looks by preschoolers at the experimenter in a Kelly, E. W., & True. J. H. (1980). Eye contact and communication of
choice-of-toys game: Effects of experimenter and age of child. Journal facilitation conditions. Perceptual and Malar Skills. 51. 815-820.
of Experimental Child Psychology, 6. 493-500. Kendon, A. (1967). Some functions of gaze-direction in social interac-
Harris. S. E. (1978). Schizophrenics' mutual glance patterns. Psychia- tion. Acta Psychologica, 26. 22-63.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

try, 41, 83-91. Kendon, A. (1978). Looking in conversation and the regulation of turns
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Harvey. J. H., Yarkin, K. L., Lighlner. J. M., & Town, J. P. (1980). at talk: A comment on the papers of G. Beattie and D. R. R utter et
Unsolicited interpretation and recall of interpersonal events. Journal al. British Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 17, 23-24.
of Personality and Social Psychology, 38. 551-568. Kendon. A., & Cook, M. (1969). The consistency of gaze patterns in
Hasselt, J. (1978). .-1 primer of psychopltysiology. San Francisco: Free- social interaction. British Journal of Psychology', 60, 481-494.
man.
Kendon. A.. & Ferber, A. (1973). A description of some human greet-
Hemsley. G. D.. & Doob, A. N. (1978). The effect of looking behavior
ings. In P. M. Michael & J. H. Cook (Eds.), Comparative ecology and
on perceptions of a communicator's credibility. Journal of Applied
behavior of primates. London: Academic Press.
Social Psychology. 8. 136-144.
KJmble. C. E., Forte, R. A., & Yoshikawa, J. C. (1981). Nonverbal con-
Henley, N. M. (1977). Body politics: Power, it?.v. and nonverbal commu-
comitants of enacted emotional intensity and positivity: Visual and
nication. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
vocal behavior. Journal of Personality. 49, 271-283.
Hillabrant, W. (1974). The influence of locomotion and gaze direction
KJmble, C. E., & Olszewski, D. A. (1980). Gaze and emotional expres-
on perceptions of interacting persons. Personality and Social Psychol-
sion: The effects of message positivity-negativiiy and emotional in-
ogy Bulletin. I. 237-239.
tensity. Journal of Research in Personality, 14. 60-69.
Ho, R.. & Mitchell, S. (1982). Students' nonverbal reactions to tutors'
Kirkland. J., & Lewis, C. (1976). Glance, look, gaze, and stare: A vocab-
warm/cold behavior. Journal of Social Psychology. 118. 121-130.
ulary for eye-fixation research. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 43, 1278.
Hobson, G. N., Strongman, K. T. Bull, D., & Craig. G. (1973). Anxiety
Kleck, R. E. (1968). Physical stigma and nonverbal cues emitted inface-
and gaze aversion in dyadic encounters. British Journal of Social and
lo-face interaction. Human Relations, 21, 19-28.
Clinical Psychology, 12. 122-129.
Kleck, R. E.. Buck, P. L., Goller, W. L., London, R. S.. Pfeiffer, J. R., &
Hughes, J., & Goldman, M. (1978). Eye contact, facial expression, sex,
Vukcevic, D. P. (1968). The effect of stigmatizing conditions on the
and the violation of personal space. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 46.
use of personal space. Psychological Reports, 23. 1 1 1 - 1 1 8 .
579-584.
KJeck, R. E., & Nuessle, W. (1968). Congruence between the indicative
Ickes, W. (1981). Sex role influences in dyadic interaction: A theoretical
and communicative functions of eye contact in interpersonal re-
model. In C. Mayo & N. M. Henley (Eds.). Gender and nonverbal
lations. British Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 7. 241-246.
behavior. New York: Springer-Verlag.
Kleck, R. E.. & Rubenstein. C. (1975). Physical attractiveness, per-
Ickes, W. (1984). Compositions in black and white: Determinants of
ceived attitude similarity, and interpersonal attraction m an opposite-
interaction in interracial dyads. Journal of Personality and Social
sex encou nter. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 31, 107-
Psychology. 47. 330-341.
114.
Ickes, W.. & Barnes. R. D. (1978). Boys and girls together—and alien-
ated: On enacting stereotyped sex roles in mixed-sex dyads. Journal KJeck, R. E., & Strenta. A. (1980). Perceptions of the impact of nega-
of Personality and Social Psychology, 36, 669-683. tively valued physical characteristics on social interaction. Journal of
Ickes, W., Patterson, M. L.. Rajecki, D. W.. & Tanford, S. (1982). Behav- Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 861-873.
ioral and cognitive consequences of reciprocal versus compensatory KJeinke, C. L. (1972). Interpersonal attraction as it relates to gaze and
responses to preinteraction expectancies. Social Cognition, I. 160- . distance between people. Representative Research in Social Psychol-
190. ogy. 3. 105-120.
Ickes, W., & Turner, M. (1983). On the social advantages of having an Kleinke, C. L. (1975). l-'irst impressions: The psychology of encounter-
older, opposite-sex sibling: Birth order influences in mixed-sex dyads. ing others. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45. 210-222. Kleinke, C. L. (1977). Compliance to requests made by gazing and
Imada, A. S., & Hakel. M. D. (1977). Influence of nonverbal communi- touching experimenters in field settings. Journal of Experimental So-
cation and rater proximity on impressions and decisions in simulated cial Psychology. 13. 218-223.
employment interviews. Journal ofApplied Psychology, 62.295-300. KJeinke. C. L. (1978). Self-Perception: The psychology of personal
Jellison, J. M., & Ickes, W. J. (1974). The power of the glance: Desire to awareness. San Francisco: Freeman.
see and be seen in cooperative and competitive situations. Journal of KJeinke, C. L. (1979). Effects of personal evaluations. In G. J. Chelune
Experimental Social Psychology. 10, 444-450. and Associates, Self-disclosure: Origins, patterns, and implications of
Jones, E. E.. & Davis. K. E. (1965). From acts to dispositions: The attri- openess in interpersonal relations (pp. 57-59). San Francisco: Jossey-
bution process in person perception. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances Bass.
in experimental social psychology (Vol. 2. pp. 219-266). New York: Kleinke. C. L. (1980). Interaction between gaze and legitimacy of re-
Academic Press. quest on compliance in a field setting. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior.
Jones, E. E., & McGillis, D. (1976). Correspondent inferences and the 5,3-12.
attribution cube: A comparative reappraisal. In J. H. Harvey, W. J. KJeinke, C. L. (1981, April). A note on the relationship between gaze
Ickes, & R. F. Kidd (Eds.), Neirdirections in attribution research (Vol. and arousal. Paper presented at the meeting of the Western Psycho-
1, pp. 389-420). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. logical Association. Los Angeles.
GAZE AND EYE CONTACT 97

KJeinke, C. L. (1986). Meeting and undemanding people. New York: Langer. E. J., Fiske, S. T., Taylor, S. E.. & Chanowitz, B. (1976). Stigma,
Freeman. staring, and discomfort: A novel-stimulus hypothesis. Journal of Ex-
Kleinke, C. L., Busies, A. A., Meeker. F. B., & Sianeski, R. A. (1973). perimental Social Psychology, 12,451-463.
Effects of self-attributed and oiher-aitributed gaze on interpersonal Latta, R. M. (1976). There's a method to our madness: Interpersonal
evaluations between males and females. Journal of Experimental So- attraction as a multidimensional construct. Journal of Research in
cial Psychology, 9, 154-163. Personality, 10, 76-82.
KJeinke, C. L.. Desautels, M. S., & Knapp, B. E. (1977). Adult gaze Lazzerini, A. J., Stephenson, G. M., & Neave, H. (1978). Eye-contact
and affective and visual responses of preschool children. Journal of in dyads: A test of the independence hypothesis. British Journal of
Genetic Psychology, 131. 321-322. Social and Clinical Psychology, 17. 227-229.
KJeinke, C. L., Kahn, M. L., & Tully, T. B. (1979). First impressions of Leavitt, L. A., & Donovan, W. L. (1979). Perceived infant temperament,
talking rales in opposite sex and same sex interactions. Social Behav- locus of control, and maternal physiological response to infant gaze.
ior and Personality, 7, 81 -91. Journal of Research in Personality, 13, 267-278.
KJeinke, C. L., Meeker, F. B., & La Fong, C. (1974). Effects of gaze, LeCompte. W. F., & Rosenfeld, H. M. (1971). Effects of minimal eye
.touch, and use of name on evaluation of "engaged" couples. Journal contact in the instruction period on impressions of the experimenter.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

of Research in Personality. 7. 368-373. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 7, 211-220.


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

KJeinke, C. L., & Pohlen, P. D. (1971). Affective and emotional re- Lefcourt. H. M., Martin, R. A., Pick, C. M.. & Saleh, W. E. (1985).
sponses as a function of other person's gaze and cooperativeness in a Locus of control for affiliation and behavior in social interactions.
two-person game. Journal of Persona/in- and Social Psychology, 17, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 48. 755-759.
308-313. Lefcourt, H. M., & Wine, J. (1969). Internal versus external control
KJeinke, C. L., & Singer. D. A. (1979). Influence of gaze on compliance of reinforcement and the development of attention in experimental
with demanding and conciliatory requests in a field setting. Personal- situations. Canadian Journal of Behavioral Science. 1, 167-181.
ity and Social Psychology Bulletin. 5. 387-390. Lefebvre, L. M. (1975). Encoding and decoding of ingraliation in
KJeinke, C. L.. Sianeski, R. A.. & Berger, D. E. (1975). Evaluation of modes of smiling and gaze. British Journal of Social and Clinical
an interviewer as a function of interviewer gaze, reinforcement of Psychology. 14, 33-42.
subject gaze, and interviewer attractiveness. Journal of Personality Lesko, W. A.. & Schneider, F. W. (1978). Effects of speaking order and
and Social Psychology. 6. 7-12. speaker gaze level on interpersonal gaze in a triad. Journal of Social
KJeinke, C. L., Staneski, R. A.. & Pipp, S. L. (1975). Effects of gaze, Psychology, 105. 185-195.
distance, and attractiveness on males' first impressions of females.
Levine, M. H., & Sutton-Smith, B. (1973). Effects of age, sex, and task
Representative Research in Social Psychology, 6, 7-12.
on visual behavior during dyadic interaction. Developmental Psychol-
KJeinke, C. L., Staneskj, R. A., & Weaver. P. (1972). Evaluation of a
ogy. 9, 400-405.
person who uses another's name in ingratiating and noningratiating
Lewis, W. L.. & Fry. W. R. (1977). Effects of visual access and orienta-
situations. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 8, 457-466.
tion on the discovery of integrative bargaining alternatives. Organiza-
KJeinke, C. L., & Tully, T. B. (1979). Influence of talking level on percep-
tional Behavior and Human Performance. 20, 75-92.
tions of counselors. Journal of Counseling Psychology. 26, 23-29.
Libby, W. L. (1970). Eye contact and direction of looking as stable indi-
KJeinke, C. L., & Walton, J. H. (1982). Influence of reinforced smiling
vidual differences. Journal of Experimental Research in Personality,
on affective responses in an interview. Journal of Personality and So-
4.303-312.
cial Psychology. 42. 557-565.
Libby, W. L., & Yaklevich, D. (1973). Personality determinants of eye
Kmiecik, C., Mausar, P., & Banziger, G. (1979). Attractiveness and in-
contact and direction of gaze aversion. Journal of Personality and So-
terpersonal space. Journal of Social Psychology: 108, 277-278.
cial Psychology. 27, 197-206.
Knapp, M. L. (1978). Nonverbal communication in human interaction.
Lim, B. (1972). Interpretations from pictures: The effects on judgments
New York: Holt, Rineharl & Winston.
of intimacy of varying social context and eye contact. Journalism
Knapp. M. L.. Hart, R. P., Fredrich, G. W., & Shulman, G. M. (1973).
Abstracts, 10. 152-153.
The rhetoric of good-bye: Verbal and nonverbal correlates of human
Lochman, J. E., & Allen, G. (1981). Nonverbal communicatoin of cou-
leave taking. Speech Monographs. 40, 182-198.
ples in conflict. Journal of Research in Personality, 15, 253-269.
Knight, D. J., Langmeyer, D., & Lundgren, D. C. (1973). Eye-contact,
Machovec, F. J. (1976). The evil eye: Superstition or hypnotic phenome-
distance, and affiliation: The role of observer bias. Sociomerry. 36,
non? American Journal of Clinical Hypnosis. 19, 74-79.
390-401.
Mancuso, J. C. Litchford, G. B., Wilson, S. D., Harrigan, J. A., & Le-
Knowles, E. S. 11977. August). Affective and cognitive mediators of spa-
hrer, R. (1983). Inferences of mental illness from noninvolvement.
tial behavior. Paper presented at the meeting of the American Psycho-
Journal of Personality, 51, 49-61.
logical Association, San Francisco.
Mandler, G. (1975). Mind and emotion. New York: Wiley.
Kraut, R. E., & Poe, D. (1980). Behavioral roots of person perception:
The deception judgments of customs inspectors and laymen. Journal Marshall, G. D., & Zimbardo, P. G. (1979). Affective consequences of
inadequately explained physiological arousal. Journal of Personality
of Personality and Social Psychology: 39. 784-798.
Krop, H., Messinger, J.. & Reiner, C. (1973). Increasing eye contact by and Social Psychology, 37, 970-988.
covert reinforcement. Interpersonal Development, 4, 51-57. Martin, W. W., & Gardner, S. N. (1979). The relative effects of eye-gaze
LaCrosse, M. B. (1975). Nonverbal behavior and perceived counselor and smiling on arousal in asocial situations. Journal of Psychology,
attractiveness and persuasiveness. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 102, 253-259.
22, 563-566. Martin, W. W.. & Rovira. M. L. (1981). An experimental analysis of
LaFrance, M., & Carmen, B. (1980). The nonverbal display of androg- discriminability and bias in eye-gaze judgment. Journal of Nonverbal
yny. Journal of Personality and Social Psycholog); J8. 36-49. Behavior, 5, 155-163.
LaFrance, M., & Mayo, C. (1976). Racial differences in gaze behavior Maslach, C. (1979a). The emotional consequences of arousal without
during conversation: Two systematic observational studies. Journal reason. In C. E. Izard (Ed.). Emotions in personality and psychopa-
of Personality and Social Psychology, 33, 547-552. thologyipp. 565-590). New York: Plenum Press.
Langer, E. J. (1978). Rethinking the role of thought in social interaction. Maslach, C. (1979b). Negative emotional biasing of unexplained
In J. H. Harvey, W. J. Ickes, & R. F. Kidd (Eds.), New directions in arousal. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 37. 953-969.
attribution research (Vol. 2, pp. 35-58). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Massie. H. N. (1978). Blind ratings of mother-infant interaction in
98 CHRIS L. KLEINKE

home movies of prepsycholic and normal infants. American Journal Norton, R. W., & Pettegrew, L. S. (1979). Attentiveness as a style of
of Psychiatry. 13, 1371-1374. communication: A structural analysis. Communication Monographs,
McCauIey. O, Coleman, O., & De Fusco, P. (1978). Commuter's eye 46, 13-26.
contact with strangers in city and suburban train stations: Evidence Obrist, P. A. {1976). The cardiovascular-behavioral interaction—as it
of short-term adaptation to interpersonal overload in the city. Envi- appears today. Psychophysiology, 13.95-107.
ronmental Psycholog)'and Nonverbal Behavior, 2, 215-225. Ortega y Gasset, J. (1957). Man and people. New York: Norton.
McClintock, C. G., & Hunt. R. G. (1975). Nonverbal indicators of Otteson, J. P., & Otteson. C. R. (1980). Effect of teacher's gaze on chil-
affect and deception in an interview setting. Journal of Applied Social dren's story recall. Perceptual and Motor Skills. 50. 35-42.
Psychology, 5, 54-67. Patterson, M. L. (1973). Stability of nonverbal immediacy behaviors.
Mehrabian, A. (1968a). Inference of attitudes from the posture, orienta- Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 9, 97-109.
tion, and distance of a communicator. Journal of Consulting and Patterson, M. L. (1975). Eye-contact and distance: A re-examination of
Clinical Psychology, 32, 296-308. measurement problems. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
Mehrabian. A. (1968b). Relationship of attitude to seated posture, ori- 1, 600-603.
entation, and distance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Patterson, M. L. (1976). An arousal model of interpersonal intimacy.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

10. 26-30. Psychological Revien; 83. 235-245.


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Mehrabian. A. (1971). Nonverbal betrayal of feeling. Journal of Experi- Patterson, M. L. (1977). Interpersonal distance, affect, and equilibrium
mental Research in Personality, 5, 64-73. theory. Journalof Social Psychology, 707.205-214.
Mehrabian, A., & Friar, J. T. (1969). Encoding of attitude by a seated Patterson, M. L. (1978). Arousal change and cognitive labeling: Pursu-
communicator via posture and position cues. Journal of Consulting ing the mediators of intimacy exchange. Environmental Psychology
and Clinical Psychology, 31. 330-336. and Nonverbal Behavior, J, 17-22.
Mehrabian, A., & Williams, M. (1969). Nonverbal concomitants of per- Patterson, M. L. (1982). A sequential functional model of nonverbal
ceived and intended persuasiveness. Journal of Personality and Social exchange. Psychological Re\'ien; 89, 231-249.
Psychology, 13, 37-58. Patterson, M. L. (1983). Nonverbal behavior: A functional perspective.
Mischel, W. (1968). Personality and assessment. New York: Wiley. New York: Springer.
Mischel, W. (1970). Sex-typing and socialization. In P. H. Mussen (Ed.). Patterson, M. L.. Jordan. A., Hogan. M. B., & Frerker. D. (1981). Effects
Carmichaet's manual of child psychology (pp. 3-72). New \brk: Wi- of nonverbal intimacy on arousal and behavior adjustment. Journal
ley. of Nonverbal Behavior. 5, 184-198.
Modigliani. A. (1971). Embarrassment, facework. and eye contact: Test- Pedersen, D. M. (1977). Relationship of ratings of classroom perfor-
ing a theory of embarrassment. Journal of Personality and Social Psy- mance and enjoyment with seat selection. Perceptual and Motor
chology, 17. 15-24. Skills, 45. 601-602.
Morlev, I. E.. & Stephenson. G. M. (1969). Interpersonal and inlerparty Peery, J. C., & Stern, D. N. (1976). Gaze duration frequency distribu-
exchange: A laboratory simulation of an industrial negotiation at the tions during mother-infant interaction. Journal of Genetic Psychol-
plant level. British Journal of Psychology, 60, 543-545. ogy. 129. 45-55.
Morley. I. E., & Stephenson. G. M. (1970a). Formality in experimental Pellegrini, R. J., Hicks, R. A., & Gordon, L. (1970). The effects of an
negotiations: A validity study. British Journal of Psychology. 61. 383- approval-seeking induction on eye-contact in dyads. British Journal
384. of Social a/id Clinical Psychology, 9, 373-374.
Morley, I. E.. & Stephenson. G. M. (1970b). Strength of case, communi- Pittman, T. S., & Pittman. N. L. (1980). Deprivation of control and the
cation systems and outcomes of simulated negotiations: Some social attribution process. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39.
psychological aspects of bargaining. Industrial Relations Journal, I, 377-389.
19-29. Post. B., & Hetherington, E. M. (1974). Sex differences in the use of
Morris. R. J.. & O'Neill, J. H. (1976). Teaching instruction following to proximity and eye contact in judgments of affiliation in preschool
severely retarded youth: The establishment of eye contact. Scandina- children. Developmental Psychology, 10, 881-889.
vian Journal of Behavior Therapy. 5. 167-178. Rago, W. V. (1977). Eye gaze and dominance heirarchy in profoundly
Muirhead, R. D., & Goldman, M. (1979). Mutual eye contact as mentally retarded males. American Journal of Menial Deficiency, 82,
affected by seating position, sex and age. Journal of Social Psychology, 145-148.
109, 201-206. Rajecki, D. W., Ickes, W.. & Tanford, S. (1981). Locus of control and
Naiman, T. H.. & Breed, G. (1974). Gaze duration as a cue for judging reactions to strangers. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 7,
conversational lone. Representative Research in Social Psn'hology, 5, 282-289.
115-122. Reis, H. T, & Halek. D. (1976). On the role of arousal response to a
Natale, M. (1977). Induction of mood states and their effect on gaze staring individual. Unpublished manuscript, University of Rochester.
behaviors. Journal ofConsulting and Clinical Psychology, 45, 960. Reis, H. T, & Werner, A. (1974, April). Some inter- and intrapersonal
Neely, W. T., Cherulnik, P. D., & Russ, R. G. (1977). Interviewees' re- consequences of eye contact. Paper presented at the meeting of the
ported level of comfort as a function of an interviewer's visual atten- Eastern Psychological Association. Philadelphia.
tion. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 44. 1073-1074. Riggio, R. E., & Friedman. H. S. (1983). Individual differences and cues
Nevill, D. (1974). Experimental manipulation of dependency motiva- to deception. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology; 45, 899-
tion and its effects on eye contact and measure of field dependency. 915.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 29, 72-79. Rime. S., & McCusker, L. (1976). Visual behavior in social interaction:
Newman, J., & McCauIey. C. (1977). Eye contact with strangers in city, The validity of eye-contact measures under different conditions of
suburb, and small town. Environment and Behavior, 9, 547-558. observation. British Journal of Psychology. 67, 507-514.
Nichols, K. A., & Champness, B. G. (1971). Eye gaze and the GSR. Robson. K. S., Pedersen, F. A., & Moss, H. A. (1969). Developmental
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 7. 623-626. observations of diadic gazing in relation to the fear of strangers and
Nisbetl, R. E., & Wilson, T. D. (1977). Telling more than we can know: social approach behavior. Child Development. 40, 619-627.
Verbal reports on mental processes. Psychological Review. 84. 231- Romano, J. M., & Bellack, A. S. (1980). Social validation of a compo-
259. nent model of assertive behavior. Journal ofConsulting and Clinical
Noller. P. (1980). Gaze in married couples. Journal of Nonverbal Behav- Psychology, 48.478-490.
ior, 5, 115-129. Rosenfeld. H. M. (1978). Conversational control functions ofnon verbal
GAZE AND EYE CONTACT 99

behavior. In A. W. Siegman & S. Feldsiein (Eds.), Nonverbal behavior Sitton. S. C, & Griffin, S. T. (1981). Detection of deception from clients'
and communication. Hillsdalc, NJ: Erlbaum. eye contact patterns. Journal ofCounseling Psychology, 28, 269-271.
Rubin, 3. Z., Steinberg. B. D., & Gerrein, J. R. (1974). How to obtain Slane, S.. Dragan, W., Crandall, C. J., & Payne, P. (1980). Stress effects
the right of way: An experimental analysis of behavior at intersec- on the nonverbal behavior of repressers and sensitizers. Journal of
tions. Perceptual and.Motor Skills, 19, 1263-1274. Psychology, 106, 101-109.
Rubin, Z. (1970). Measurement of romantic love. Journal of Personality Smith, B. J., Sanford, F, & Goldman, M. (1977). Norm violations, sex,
and Social Psychology, 16. 265-273. and the "blank stare." Journalof Social Psychology, 103. 49-55.
Russo, N. F. (1975). Eye contact, interpersonal distance, and the equi- Smith, R. J., & Knowles, E. S. (1978). Attributional consequences of
librium theory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 31, personal space invasions. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
497-502. 4, 429-433.
Rutter, D. R. (1973). Visual interaction in psychiatric patients: A re- Smith, R. J., & Knowles, E. S. (1979). Affective and cognitive mediators
view. British Journal of Psychiatry, 123, 193-202. of reactions to spatial invasions. Journalof Experimental Social Psy-
Rutter, D. R. (1976). Visual interaction in recently admitted and chology. 15. 437-452.
chronic long-stay schizophrenic patients. British Journal of Social Sn\der, M. (1974). The self-monitoring of expressive behavior. Journal
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

and Clinical Psychology. 15. 295-303. of Personality and Social Psychology. JO, 526-537.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Rulier, D. R. (1977). Visual interaction and speech patterning in remit- Snyder, M., Grether. J.. & Keller, K. (1974). Staring and compliance: A
ted and acute schizophrenic patients. British Journal of Social and field experiment on hitchhiking. Journal of Applied Social Psychol-
Clinical Psychology; 16, 357-361. ogy. 4. 165-170.
Rutter, D. R. (1978). Visual interaction in schizophrenic patients: The Snyder, R. A., & Sutker, L. W. (1977). The measurement of the con-
timing of looks. Briiish Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 17. struct of dominance and its relationship to nonverbal behavior. Jour-
281-282.
nal of Psychology, 97, 227-230.
Rutter. D. R., & Stephenson. G. M. (1979). The functions of looking:
Sodikoff, C. L.. Firestone, I. J.. & Kaplan. K. J. (1974). Distance match-
Effects of friendship on gaze. British Journal of Social and Clinical
ing and distance equilibrium in the interview dyad. Personality and
Psychology. 18. 203-205.
Social Psychology Bulletin, 1, 243-245.
Rutler, D. R., Stephenson, G. M., Ayling, K.. & White, P. A. (1978).
Staneski. R. A.. & KJeinke.C. L. (1978. April). Nondependencetanven
The timing of looks in dyadic conversation. British Journal of Social
verbal and nonverbal measures of attraction. Paper presented at the
and Clinical Psychology. 16. 191-192.
meeting of the Western Psychological Association, San Francisco.
Rutler. D. R., Stephenson, G. M., Lazzerini, A. J., Ayling, K., & White.
Staneski, R. A., Kleinke, C. L., & Meeker, F. B. (1977). Effects of ingrati-
P. A. (1977). Eye-contact: A chance product of individual looking?
ation, touch, and use of name on evaluation of job applicants and
Briiish Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology. 16, 191 -192.
interviewers. Social Behavior and Personality, 5, 13-19.
Sartre. J. P. (1956). Being ami nothingness. New York: Philosophical
Stanley, G., & Martin, D.S.(I968). Eye-contact and the recall of mate-
Library. (Original work published 1942)
rial involving competitive and noncompetitive associations. Psycho-
Scaife, M..& Bruner. J. S. (1975). The capacity for joint visual attention
mimic Science. 11. 337-338.
in the infant. Naiure, 25J, 265-266.
Stass, J. W., & Willis. F. N. (1967). Eye-contact, pupil dilation, and per-
Schachter, S. (1964). The interaction of cognitive and physiological
sonal preference. Psychonotnic Science. 7, 375-376.
components of emotional state. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advance-; in
Stephenson. G. M., & Rutter, D. R. (1970). Eye-contact, distance, and
experimental social psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 49-86). New York: Aca-
affiliation: A re-evaluation. Briiish Joiirna/ofSocialandClinicalPsy-
demic Press.
chology. 61, 385-393.
Schaeffer, G. H., & Patterson, M. L. (1980). Intimacy, arousal, and small
Stephenson, G. M.. Rutter, D. R.. & Dore, S. R. (1973). Visual interac-
group crowding. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 38,
tion and distance. Briiish Journal of Psychology. 64. 251-257.
283-290.
Stevenson, B. (1967). The home book of quotations. New York: Dodd
Scheman, J. D., & Lockard, J. S. (1979). Development of gaze aversion
Mead.
in children. Child Development, 50. 594-596.
Strom. J. C.& Buck, R. W. (1979). Staringand participants'sex: Physi-
Scherer, S. E. (1974). Influence of proximity and eye contact on impres-
ological and subjective reactive responses. Personality and Social Psy-
sion formation. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 38, 538.
chology Bulletin. 5. 114-117.
Scherer, S. E., & Schiff, M. R. (1973). Perceived intimacy, physical dis-
Sundstrom. E. (1978). A test of equilibrium theory: Effects of topic inti-
tance, and eye contact. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 36, 835-841.
macy and proximity on verbal and nonverbal behavior in pairs of
Scherwitz, L., & Helmreich. R. (1973). Interactive effects of eye contact
friends and strangers. Environmental Psychology and Nonverbal Be-
and verbal content on interpersonal attraction in dyads. Journal of
havior. 3, 3-16.
Personality and Social Psychology; 25. 6-14.
Tankard, J. W. (1970). Effects of eye position on person perception. Per-
Schlenker, B. R. (1980). Impression management: The self-concept, so-
cial identity, and interpersonal relations. Monterey, CA: Brooks/Cole. ceptual and Motor Skills. 31. 883-893.
Schneider, F. W., Coutts, L. M., & Garrett, W. A. (1977). Interpersonal Taylor. S. E., & Fiske, S. T. (1978). Salience, attention, and attribution:
gaze in a triad as a function of sex. Perceptual and Motor Skills. 44, Topof the head phenomena. In L. Berkowiu (Ed.), Advances in exper-
imental social psychology (Vol. I I , pp. 249-288). New York: Aca-
184.
Schulz, R.. & Barefoot. J. (1974). Nonverbal responses and affiliative demic Press.
conflict theory. Briiish Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 13, Tessler, R.. & Sushelsky, L. (1978). Effects of eye contact and social sta-
1-7. tus on the perception of a job applicant in an employment interview-
Shetland, R. L., & Johnson. M. P. (1978). Bystander behavior and kine- ing situation. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 13, 338-347.
sics: The interaction between helper and victim. Environmental Psy- Thayer. S. (1969). The effect of interpersonal looking duration on domi-
chology and Nonverbal Behavior, 2, 181 -190. nancejudgments. Journalof Social Psychology, 79. 285-286.
Shroul. P. E., & Fiske. D. W. (1981). Nonverbal behaviors and social Thayer, S. (1977). Children's detection of on-face and off-face gazes.
evaluation. Journal of Personality, 49, 115-128. Developmental Psychology. 13. 673-674.
Simmel. G. (1921). The sociology of the senses: Visual interaction. In Thayer. S.. & Schiff, W. (1974). Observer judgment of social interaction:
R. E. Park & E. W. Burgess (Eds.). Introduction to the science of soci- Eye contact and relationship inferences. Journal of Personality and
ology. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Social Psychology. 30. 1 10-1 14.
100 CHRIS L. KLEINKE

Thayer. S.. & Scruff, W. (1975). Eye-contact, facial expression, and the Watson. O. M. (1970). Proxemic behavior: A cross-cultural study. The
experience of time. Journal of Social Psychology, 95. I17-124. Hague. The Netherlands: Mouton.
Thayer, S., & Schiff, W. (1977). Gazing patterns and attribution of sex- Waxer, P. (1974). Nonverbal cues for depression. Journal of Abnormal
ual involvement. Journal of Social Psychology, 101, 235-246. Psychology. 83, 319-322.
Thompson. T. L. (1982). Gaze toward and avoidance of the handi- Wellens. A. R.. & Faletti, M. V. (1978). Interrelationships of six mea-
capped: A field experiment. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior. 6. 188- sures of interpersonal attraction. Psychological Reports, 42, 1022.
195. Wheeler, R. W., Baron, J. C., Michell, S., & Ginsburg, H. J. (1979). Eye
Timney, B..& London, H . ( I 9 7 3 ) . Body language concomitants of per- contact and the perception of intelligence. Bulletin of the Psycho-
suasiveness and persuasibility in dyadic interaction. International nomic Society. 13, 101-102.
Journal of Group Tensions, 3. 48-67. Wichman, H. (1970). Effects of isolation and communication on coop-
Tipton, R. M., & Rymer. R. A. (1978). A laboratory study of the effects eration i n a two-person game. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
of varying levels of counselor eye contact on client-focused and prob- chology: 16. 114-120.
lem-focused counseling styles. Journal ofCounseling Psychology. 25, Wicker, A. W. (1979). An introduction to ecological psychology. Monte-
200-204. rey. CA: Brooks/Cole.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Tomkins, S. S. (1963). Affect, imagery, consciousness (Vol. 2). New \brk: Wicklund, R. A. (1975). Objective self-awareness. In L. Berkowitz
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Springer. (Ed.). Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 8, pp. 262-


Valentine, M. E. (1980). The attenuating influence of gaze upon the 269). New York: Academic Press.
bystander intervention effect. Journal of Social Psychology, 11, 197- Wiens, A. N.. Harper, R. G., & Matarazzo, J. D. (1980). Personality
203. correlates of nonverbal interview behavior. Journal of Clinical Psy-
Valentine, M. E..& Ehrlichman, H.( 1979). Interpersonal gaze and help-
chology, 36, 205-215.
ing behavior. Journal of Social Psychology; in?, 193-198.
Word. C. Q, Zanna, M. P.. & Cooper, J. (1974). The nonverbal media-
Van Houten, R., Nau, P. A., MacKenzie-Keating, S. E.. Sameoto. D.,
tion of self-fulfilling prophecies in interracial interaction. Journal of
& Colavecchia, B. (1982). An analysis of some variables influencing
Experimental Social Psychology, 10. 109-120.
the effectiveness of reprimands. Journal at Applied Behavior Analy-
Vbshida, F, & lida, T. (1981). An analysis of visual behaviors in conver-
sis, 15. 65-83.
sational dyads. Japanese Journal of Experimental Social Psychology.
Vaughan, B. E., & Waters, E. (1981). Attention structure, sociometric
20, 109-118.
status, and dominance: Interrelations, behavioral correlates, and rela-
Zajonc. R. B. (1980). Feeling and thinking: Preferences need no infer-
tionships to social competence. Developmental Psychology, 17. 275-
ences. American Psychologist. 35. 151-175.
288.
Zimmerman, B. J., & Brody. G. H. (1975). Race and modeling influ-
Velten, E. (1968). A laboratory task for induction of mood states. Be-
ences on the interpersonal play patterns of boys. Journal of Educa-
havior Research and Therapy, 6, 473-482.
tional Psychology. 67. 591-598.
Walsh. N. A.. Meister, L. A., & Kleinke, C. L. (1977). Interpersonal
attraction and visual behavior as a function of perceived arousal and
evaluation by an opposite sex person. Journal of Social Psychology, Received June 3, 1985
103. 65-74. Revision received December 9, 1985 •

You might also like