You are on page 1of 13

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/241055232

The effect of shipping fee structure on consumers’ online evaluations and


choice

Article  in  Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science · November 2011


DOI: 10.1007/s11747-011-0281-2

CITATIONS READS

30 5,620

3 authors, including:

Nevena T. Koukova Joydeep Srivastava


Lehigh University University of Maryland, College Park
11 PUBLICATIONS   190 CITATIONS    32 PUBLICATIONS   1,763 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

All content following this page was uploaded by Nevena T. Koukova on 26 June 2016.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


J. of the Acad. Mark. Sci. (2012) 40:759–770
DOI 10.1007/s11747-011-0281-2

ORIGINAL EMPIRICAL RESEARCH

The effect of shipping fee structure on consumers’ online


evaluations and choice
Nevena T. Koukova & Joydeep Srivastava &
Martina Steul-Fischer

Received: 22 February 2011 / Accepted: 4 August 2011 / Published online: 21 August 2011
# Academy of Marketing Science 2011

Abstract This research examines how consumers respond delivery costs encourages consumers to view the shipping
to different shipping fee structures. Focusing on two of the fee as a cost of doing business rather than as a profit
most common shipping fee structures, flat rate shipping and generator, thus raising offer evaluations.
threshold-based free shipping, we first demonstrate that
offer evaluations are less (more) favorable with threshold- Keywords Shipping fee . Flat rate shipping . Threshold-
based free shipping when order value is below (above) the based free shipping . Free shipping . Progressive rate
free shipping threshold compared to flat rate shipping. shipping . Online shopping
However, when an alternative more important referent is
present, the effect of shipping fee structure is attenuated.
Second, we show that although perceptions of shipping fees
as a profit generator are higher (lower) under threshold- Direct-to-consumer business models, where the physical good
based free shipping relative to flat rate shipping for order is spatially separated from the consumer (Lewis 2006),
values below (above) the free shipping threshold, providing necessitate firms’ incurring order assembly and delivery costs
a justification for the shipping fee by explicitly linking it to (Rosen and Howard 2000). Firms typically recover these costs
by passing them on to consumers in the form of shipping fees
(Barsh et al. 2000). The growth in e-commerce (online
spending in the U.S. reached $142.5 billion in 2010, an
increase of about 10.5% over 2009) has accentuated the
Listed alphabetically, the three authors contributed equally to the
article. This research was partially funded by the Alison and Norman importance of shipping fees. Designing a shipping policy is
Axelrod’74 endowed summer research fellowship awarded to the first one of the biggest quandaries firms face as it requires
author by Lehigh University and by the summer research grant balancing the need to recover delivery costs and the need to
awarded to the second author by the Robert H. Smith School’s Center attract and retain consumers. Despite the topic’s importance,
for Excellence in Service and the Research Committee.
only a handful of studies have examined shipping fees (Lewis
N. T. Koukova (*) 2006; Lewis et al. 2006; Schindler et al. 2005). Further, the
College of Business and Economics, Lehigh University,
existing studies restrict their examination to comparing
Bethlehem, PA 18015, USA
e-mail: nkoukova@lehigh.edu promotional offers of free shipping to threshold-based free
shipping (Lewis 2006; Lewis et al. 2006).
J. Srivastava This paper attempts to contribute to the sparse literature
Robert H. Smith School of Business, University of Maryland,
by examining the effects of two common shipping fee
College Park, MD 20742, USA
e-mail: srivasta@rhsmith.umd.edu structures—flat rate and threshold-based free shipping.
With flat rate shipping, retailers charge a fixed fee
M. Steul-Fischer regardless of order value. With threshold-based free
Institute of Marketing, School of Business and Economics,
shipping, retailers waive the shipping fee for orders above
University of Erlangen-Nuremberg,
90403 Nuremberg, Germany a specific amount, but charge a fixed fee otherwise.
e-mail: martina.steul-fischer@wiso.uni-erlangen.de Estimates suggest that 49% of online retailers use flat rate
760 J. of the Acad. Mark. Sci. (2012) 40:759–770

and 43% use threshold-based free shipping (ComCult providing a justification for the shipping fee alters the effect
Research 2003). The experimentation occurring online and of shipping fee structure on inferences and evaluations. Our
the variety of fees indicate that retailers are grappling with results suggest that for order values below (above) the
the effect of shipping fees on consumer demand. For threshold for free shipping, consumers are more (less)
example, Amazon decreased its free shipping threshold likely to infer that shipping fees are used to increase profits
from $99 to $25, while Ann Taylor Loft changed its policy when the retailer employs threshold-based free shipping
from threshold-based free shipping to flat rate to progres- than flat rate shipping. However, retailers may be able
sive rate shipping. Thus, from a substantive as well as a to channel such inferences in a way to improve offer
practical perspective, there is a need to explore how evaluations by justifying the shipping fee structure employed.
consumers respond to different shipping fee structures. Specifically, we show that linking the shipping fee to the
The primary purpose of this research is to experimentally actual cost of delivery alters consumers’ inferred motive
examine how consumers evaluate and respond to flat rate regarding shipping fees and improves evaluations. The
and threshold-based free shipping. We focus on these two provision of justification for the shipping fee thus moderates
shipping fee structures because these are the most prevalent the interaction effect of shipping fee structure and order value
in the marketplace and most other structures are derived on consumer evaluations.
from them (e.g., flat rate shipping fee that increases This research contributes to the literature by systemati-
stepwise based on order value). Importantly, we examine cally examining the effect of two common shipping fee
factors that may moderate the effect of shipping fee structures on consumers’ inferences and evaluations, and
structure on consumer evaluations and thus provide a better testing boundary conditions for the effects. Specifically,
understanding of the conditions under which different adding to previous research (e.g., Lewis 2006; Lewis et al.
shipping fee structures affect consumer response. 2006), this paper investigates the effect of flat rate shipping
Since the shipping fee depends on order value for and compares it with threshold-based free shipping. We
threshold-based free shipping, we first examine whether demonstrate that a specific shipping charge may differ-
the effect of flat rate and threshold-based free shipping on entially affect offer evaluations depending on whether it
online offer evaluations depends on order value. The results is framed as a flat rate or a threshold-based shipping fee.
reveal that offer evaluations are less (more) favorable with We offer a theoretical explanation for the observed effect
threshold-based free shipping when order value is below and provide recommendations to retailers on how to
(above) the threshold compared to flat rate shipping. The frame the shipping fee in order to increase consumers’
theoretical argument is that, for threshold-based free evaluations. Further, given that data constraints prevented
shipping, the possibility of free shipping is the natural an examination of moderators in previous research (e.g.,
referent against which any shipping fee is evaluated. While Lewis 2006), the use of experimental methodology allows
for order values below the threshold the shipping fee is us to identify boundary conditions thus adding to our
likely to be coded as a loss relative to the referent and lead understanding of the effects of shipping fee structure. In
to lower evaluations, for order values above the threshold particular, we demonstrate that the presence of an
the shipping fee is likely to be coded as a gain and result in alternative important referent and the provision of justifi-
higher evaluations. However, if another referent that is cation for the shipping fee can systematically alter
more important than the shipping fee is present, such as (moderate) the interaction effect of shipping fee structure
product price discount, the effect of the shipping fee is and order value on offer evaluations.
attenuated regardless of order value. Given that the price of The next section provides the conceptual background
the focal product (and related discount) is more important and develops specific predictions for the effects of shipping
than shipping fees (Hamilton and Srivastava 2008), such an fees and potential moderators. We then report three experi-
alternative referent is likely to overshadow the effect of ments that test the hypotheses and explore boundary
shipping or draw attention away from the shipping fee, conditions. The paper concludes by discussing the theoret-
particularly from the natural referent of free shipping ical and managerial implications of the findings including
associated with threshold-based free shipping. The presence directions for future research.
of an alternative important referent thus moderates the
interaction effect of shipping fee structure and order value
on consumer evaluations of online offers. Conceptual background
Second, we propose that the shipping fee structure itself
may prompt consumers to think of retailer profit motives The explicit separation of shipping fee from the price of a
for employing this structure (Schindler et al. 2005). We product is an example of partitioned pricing where the total
examine consumers’ inferences about the extent to which price is divided into two or more mandatory components
shipping fees are used to generate profits and whether such as a base price and a surcharge (e.g., Chakravarti et al.
J. of the Acad. Mark. Sci. (2012) 40:759–770 761

2002; Cheema 2008; Hamilton and Srivastava 2008; value because “it does not cost any more,” threshold-based
Hamilton et al. 2010; Morwitz et al. 1998; Xia and Monroe free shipping encourages people to increase order value and
2004). However, partitioning the total price into product reach the free shipping threshold to avoid the shipping fee
price and shipping fee has only been used as a context in (Lewis et al. 2006). Order value thus assumes particular
which to study the effect of price partitioning rather than significance for threshold-based free shipping. We argue
the differential effects of various shipping fee structures on that a critical difference in evaluating offers with flat rate
consumer response. Specifically, while previous studies and threshold-based free shipping lies in the referent used
have compared partitioned (i.e., shipping charge listed to evaluate the shipping fee.
separately) and non-partitioned (shipping included in the The notion that evaluations are reference dependent and
total price) offers, our focus is on the effect of different based on a comparison with a standard or a referent is well
shipping fee structures such as flat rate and threshold-based acknowledged (Kahneman and Tversky 1979). Evaluations
free shipping. depend on both the absolute and relative value compared to
More relevant to the current investigation, in an analysis some referent. For example, when a potential buyer
of online book purchase data, Smith and Brynjolfsson assesses the value of a house, she is likely to anchor on
(2001) find that consumers are almost twice as sensitive to some reference (e.g., comparable houses in the area, last
changes in the shipping fee than to changes in the price of house sold, average house) and make a judgment by
the book. Hamilton and Srivastava (2008) also argue that comparing the current house with the referent (Hsee and
consumers are more sensitive to the price of partitioned Leclerc 1998). However, if the buyer is shown two houses,
components that are less important and provide relatively she is less likely to think about the referent used in the
low consumption benefits (e.g., shipping) than to the price earlier evaluation and is more likely to make a judgment by
of components that are more important and provide comparing the two houses. The reason for the shift in
relatively high consumption benefits (e.g., the product). reference from the single house to the two houses is that
Lewis et al.’s (2006) analysis reveals that promotional free people rely more on vivid, easily available information than
shipping increases order incidence but reduces order values, on pallid background information that requires more
whereas threshold-based free shipping encourages larger effortful processing to access (Hsee and Leclerc 1998). In
orders but has little impact on order incidence. Although general, the preference reversals between separate and joint
free shipping raises order incidence, the increased revenues evaluations can be explained by the “evaluability” hypoth-
are not enough to offset the cost of shipping. esis—attributes that are hard to evaluate independently
In sum, a shipping fee is an important component that is loom larger in joint evaluations, while attributes that are
likely to influence consumers’ online purchasing behavior easy to evaluate independently loom larger in separate
and decision making. While previous research has explored evaluations (Hsee et al. 1999).
the effects of promotional free shipping and threshold- We conceptualize a similar “shift in reference” mecha-
based free shipping (Lewis et al. 2006), this paper is the nism underlying the effects of shipping fee structures on
first to examine the effect of flat rate shipping and compare consumer evaluations. First, consider an online offer with
it to the effect of threshold-based free shipping on flat rate shipping. In evaluating the fee, consumers are
consumer evaluations and inferences. Further, we study likely to use referents such as the shipping fee last paid, the
the role of order value on the effect of shipping fee structure fee charged by an online retailer used frequently, or some
on consumers’ inferences and evaluations, as well as the abstract distributional information such as “average fee”
moderating role of an alternative important referent and based on prior experience or knowledge. Now consider an
shipping fee justification on the interaction effect of online offer with threshold-based free shipping. For such
shipping fee structure and order value on consumer offers consumers are likely to use the potential free
evaluations. shipping as the natural referent. Flat rate and threshold-
based free shipping are thus likely to differ in the frame of
Order value as a moderator of the effect of shipping fee reference that consumers use in their evaluations. Specifi-
structure on offer evaluations cally, while flat rate shipping is likely to invoke a memory-
based referent based on prior knowledge or experience, in
Proponents of both flat rate and threshold-based free the case of threshold-based free shipping the referent shifts
shipping structures speculate on identical reasons for their to stimulus-based free shipping. In other words, flat rate
success. Both are believed to be “clean” and “simple” for shipping is typically less evaluable as it is relatively
consumers and can be easily communicated such that it is difficult to assess given the lack of a precise referent. In
clear to shoppers how much they are paying for shipping. contrast, threshold-based free shipping is more evaluable as
Both shipping fee structures are believed to increase order it is relatively easy to assess, given the more vivid and
value. While flat rate shipping presumably increases order precise referent of free shipping (Hsee et al. 1999). Unlike
762 J. of the Acad. Mark. Sci. (2012) 40:759–770

Hsee et al. (1999), the evaluability of shipping fees is be expected based on the economic value of the bargain
inherent to the shipping fee structure and does not depend (Darke and Dahl 2003). As such, an online offer that
on joint versus separate evaluation (which is not considered emphasizes a discount off the regular product price is likely
in the current research). Our focus is on how consumers to draw consumers’ attention away from the shipping fee,
evaluate a product offer with a specific shipping fee thereby attenuating the effect of shipping fee structure on
structure (separate evaluation), and we do not consider consumer evaluations. Specifically, consumers are likely to
situations in which two offers with different shipping fee shift their focus to the more important alternative referent
structures are evaluated simultaneously (joint evaluation). when evaluating the online offer (Kahneman and Miller
The implication is that because threshold-based free 1986). We thus predict that regardless of order value,
shipping provides a precise referent against which to consumer evaluations of online offers will not differ across
evaluate the shipping fee, order value assumes differential flat rate and threshold-based free shipping when an
significance. When the order value is below the threshold, alternative important referent such as a price discount is
the shipping fee is likely to be coded as a loss or penalty provided. Study 1a examines the effect of shipping fee
relative to the possibility of free shipping. For equivalent structure, order value, and the presence of another impor-
total prices, consumer evaluations are likely to be less tant referent on offer evaluations.
favorable relative to an offer with flat rate shipping.
H1a: Compared to an online offer with flat rate shipping,
Conversely, when the order value is above the threshold,
a threshold-based free shipping offer will be evalu-
the lack of shipping fee is likely to be coded as a gain or a
ated less (more) positively when the order value is
bonus and offer evaluations are likely to be more favorable
below (above) the threshold for free shipping in the
relative to an offer with flat rate shipping. The differential
absence of an alternative important referent.
effect of the two shipping fee structures on offer evaluations
H1b: When an alternative important referent is present,
for order values below or above the threshold is likely to be
consumer evaluations will not differ across flat rate
driven, in part, by perceptions of shipping policy fairness.
and threshold-based free shipping structures, regard-
Previous research highlights the importance of perceived
less of order value.
price fairness in influencing consumers’ reactions to prices
(e.g., Campbell 1999; Kahneman et al. 1986). Moreover, In examining how threshold-based free shipping may
fairness perceptions mediate the effect of price discounts or affect offer evaluations, it is important to consider the level
price premiums relative to other consumers on purchase of the free shipping threshold itself. It is possible that the
satisfaction (e.g., Darke and Dahl 2003). Thus, when the referent of free shipping may loom larger for threshold-
order value is below the threshold for threshold-based free based free shipping when the order value is just below
shipping, the loss associated with incurring the shipping fee (versus well below) the free shipping threshold. Just
(compared to the referent of free shipping) will be missing out on reaching the threshold and incurring a
perceived as less fair than a flat rate that every consumer “loss” may have a more pronounced effect on consumer
must pay, leading to less positive offer evaluations. On the evaluations when the threshold is easy to reach rather than
other hand, for offers above the threshold, the gain difficult to reach (e.g., Kahneman and Miller 1986). For
associated with free shipping will be perceived as fairer example, Lassar et al. (1997) found that just missing out on
than flat rate shipping, leading to more positive offer a warranty created a less favorable reaction relative to
evaluations. having the problem within the warranty period or long after
its expiration. Study 1b examines the robustness of the
Alternative important referent as a moderator of the effect results across different free shipping thresholds for an order
of shipping fee structure and order value on offer value below the threshold and rules out the level of
evaluations closeness of the threshold as an alternative explanation for
the lower evaluations when threshold-based free shipping is
Our conceptualization of the effects of shipping fee employed (relative to flat rate shipping).
structure is based on different referents being used in the
evaluations. It follows that providing an alternative referent Inferences of profit motive and the moderating role
that is more important than the shipping charge may draw of shipping fee justification on the effect of shipping fee
consumer attention away from the referents used in structure and order value on offer evaluations
evaluating the shipping fee. The price of the focal product
has been shown to be more important than the shipping fee Given that consumers are skeptical of shipping charges and
(e.g., Hamilton and Srivastava 2008). Further, in general, may view them as a way for firms to make additional
consumers are bargain driven such that the subjective profits (Schindler et al. 2005), we examine whether
importance of a bargain is often greater than what would consumer perceptions of the extent to which a shipping
J. of the Acad. Mark. Sci. (2012) 40:759–770 763

fee is used to generate profits rather than as a means to regard the shipping fee as a way for retailers to generate
recover delivery costs is affected by whether the shipping profits. Thus, consumer perceptions of shipping fees as a
fee structure is flat rate or threshold-based free shipping. profit generator are likely to be higher (lower) when the
According to attribution theory, consumers will seek to order value is below (above) the threshold for threshold-
meaningfully explain firms’ shipping fee structure in terms based free shipping relative to flat rate shipping.
of underlying reasons such as profits and/or cost (Fiske and Further, we examine whether providing a (cost) justifi-
Taylor 1991). Attribution research indicates that the cation for the shipping fee moderates the effect of shipping
inclination to search for causal explanations for others’ fee structure and order value on perceptions of shipping
behavior is particularly high following an unexpected and/ fees as a profit generator and offer evaluations. While
or negative outcome (Folkes 1984; Kelley 1967). For people believe that retailers are entitled to a certain level of
example, the underlying motives of some sellers for a price profit on the price of the product (Kahneman et al. 1986),
increase may be perceived as positive or benevolent, the same entitlement does not hold for shipping fees.
whereas other sellers’ motives may be perceived to be However, people recognize that retailers incur delivery
negative, driven by the extent to which the seller is taking costs and are entitled to recover the “cost of doing
advantage of or exploiting consumers (Campbell 1999). business” (Schindler et al. 2005). The implication is that
Campbell (1999) demonstrates that a seller’s inferred consumer inferences of shipping fees as a profit generator
motive is an important causal construct that underlies and evaluations of online offers may be more favorable
perceptions of price (un)fairness. As such, we argue that when retailers link the shipping fee structure to their actual
the extent to which consumers infer a profit generating shipping cost. When a justification for a negative outcome
motive for a retailer’s shipping fee is likely to affect how is provided, the outcome is perceived as fairer than when
the retailer’s online offer is evaluated. the same outcome arises with no justification offered (Bies
Attribution theory highlights locus (whether the cause and Shapiro 1988).
resides within or outside of the actor) and controllability We suggest that when a credible statement with a
(whether the cause is subject to volitional alteration or rationale for the shipping fee structure (that links the
not) as two central characteristics of causality (Weiner shipping fee to the actual cost of shipping) is provided,
2000). They both trigger judgments about responsibility the effect of shipping fee structure on consumer perceptions
for the obtained outcome, affecting consumers’ evalua- of shipping fees as a profit generator and offer evaluations
tions and satisfaction (e.g., Tsiros et al. 2004). Given the is likely to be attenuated. For example, a retailer may
salience of free shipping for a threshold-based free include a statement on its website claiming that the
shipping structure, consumers paying a shipping fee for shipping fees correspond to what the shipping company is
order values below the threshold are likely to seek an actually charging. Thus, for order values below the
explanation for why the retailer charges a fee for orders threshold, consumers provided with such a justification
below the threshold. They may believe that since profits will no longer view the shipping fee as a penalty but as a
are relatively low on small order values, retailers com- cost of doing business. As such, regardless of order value,
pensate by charging a shipping fee. Alternatively, they consumer perceptions of shipping fees as a profit generator
may believe that retailers attempt to make a profit on and evaluations of the online offers are not likely to differ
shipping fees from consumers with low order values in across the flat rate and threshold-based free shipping
order to subsidize the high order value consumers. structures when the rationale for shipping fees is clearly
Consumers facing a reasonable flat rate fee will be less communicated. Study 2 examines the moderating role of
likely to search for a causal explanation for the fee justification for shipping fee structure on consumer percep-
because all consumers pay the same shipping fee, tions of shipping fees as a profit generator and evaluations
regardless of order value. Supporting this notion, Haws of online offers.
and Bearden (2006) demonstrate that differences between
consumers result in the greatest perception of unfairness H2a: Compared to an online offer with flat rate shipping,
and the lowest overall satisfaction (relative to seller, time, a threshold-based free shipping offer will lead to
and price setter differences). Thus, for order values below higher (lower) perceptions of shipping fees as a
the free shipping threshold, consumers facing threshold- profit generator when the order value is below
based free shipping will be more likely to infer a profit (above) the threshold for free shipping.
motive for the shipping charge (e.g., retailer is trying to H2b: When a justification for the shipping fee is present,
make money on shipping) relative to consumers presented perceptions of shipping fees as a profit generator
with flat rate shipping that every consumer is paying. In and offer evaluations will not differ across flat rate
contrast, consumers with order values above the threshold and threshold-based free shipping structures, regard-
are less likely to seek an explanation and less likely to less of order value.
764 J. of the Acad. Mark. Sci. (2012) 40:759–770

Study 1a: order value and an alternative important tive/Desirable/Reasonable). Perceptions of quality (α=.83;
referent “The flash drive is:” 1 = Low quality/Worse than others/Not
appealing; 7 = High quality/Better than others/Appealing)
Study 1a tests H1, which predicts that compared with flat and price perceptions (r=.63; “The price that the online
rate shipping, an online offer with threshold-based free store is asking for the flash drive is” 1 = Low/Unaccept-
shipping will be evaluated less (more) favorably when the able; 7 = High/Acceptable) were also measured.
order value is below (above) the free shipping threshold.
However, the effect will be attenuated in the presence of an Results and discussion
important alternative referent. The rationale is that consum-
ers will focus on the important referent, drawing attention Offer evaluations A 2x2x2 ANOVA (the ninth condition
away from the shipping fee and specifically the natural was excluded) revealed a significant two-way interaction
referent of free shipping associated with threshold-based between shipping structure and order value (F(1, 152)=
free shipping, regardless of order value. 7.45, p<.01) and the predicted three-way interaction (F(1,
152)=6.74, p<.01). The results in Table 1 show that the
Method evaluations differed across the two alternative referent
conditions.
One hundred and seventy-nine undergraduate students In the absence of the alternative referent, the results
(47% female, age ranging from 18 to 26 years, mean age support H1a. For order values below the free shipping
=20.1 years) from a U.S. university participated in the threshold, the online offer was evaluated less favorably in
study for extra credit. Participants were assigned at random the threshold-based free shipping than in the flat rate
to one of eight conditions of a 2 (shipping fee structure: flat condition (M’s=4.02 and 4.95; F(1, 170)=4.87, p<.05).
rate and threshold-based free shipping) x 2 (order value: However, for order values above the threshold, the online
$34.99 and $44.99) x 2 (alternative referent: none and $2 offer was evaluated more favorably in the threshold-based
price discount) between-subjects design. Participants imag- free shipping than in the flat rate condition (M’s=4.72 and
ined that they were buying a new flash drive. They were 3.37; F(1, 170)=9.35, p<.01). The total price was also
told that they came across an online store that had suitable lower in the threshold condition.
flash drives and the model they liked was described in Conversely, in the presence of an important referent in
detail. The online store either charged a flat rate shipping the form of a $2 price discount, there were no significant
fee of $6 for all order values, or shipped free for order differences across the threshold-based free shipping and the
values above $40 and $6 otherwise. The flash drive was flat rate conditions when the order value was below the free
priced either below or above the threshold of $40 for free shipping threshold (M’s=4.75 and 4.46; F(1, 170)=0.44, p
shipping at $34.99 and $44.99, respectively. When the >.51) or when the order value was above the free shipping
alternative referent was present, it was stated that $34.99 threshold (M’s=4.73 and 4.39; F(1, 170)=0.62, p>.43).
($44.99) was a discounted price, and the regular price was These results support H1b. Further, holding total price
$36.99 ($46.99). Another condition was added where the constant at $50.99, there was no significant difference
flash drive was priced at $50.99 with a referent of $52.99 as between the ninth condition where the flash drive was
the regular price and threshold-based free shipping. The priced at $50.99 with threshold-based free shipping and the
total price was the same as the high order value with flat high order value with flat rate shipping condition (M’s=
rate shipping condition (44.99+6.00=$50.99). 4.74 and 4.39; F(1, 170)=.62, p>.43). These results cannot
Offer evaluations were measured by averaging responses be attributed to perceived quality and price as these
to three seven-point items (α=.93; “The online store’s offer measures did not vary significantly with any of the factors
is” 1 = Unattractive/Undesirable/Unreasonable; 7 = Attrac- (all p’s>.13).

Table 1 Study 1a: means (standard deviations) of dependent measures

Shipping fee structure Threshold-based free shipping Flat rate shipping

Order value (Total price) $34.99 ($40.99) $44.99 ($44.99) $50.99 ($50.99) $34.99 ($40.99) $44.99 ($50.99)

No alternative referent
Offer evaluations 4.02 (1.00) 4.72 (1.61) 4.95 (1.35) 3.37 (1.37)
Alternative referent
Offer evaluations 4.75 (1.24) 4.73 (1.28) 4.74 (1.54) 4.46 (1.34) 4.39 (1.58)
J. of the Acad. Mark. Sci. (2012) 40:759–770 765

Discussion Study 1a supports H1 by demonstrating that To demonstrate the robustness and generalizability of
order value moderates the effect of shipping fee structure on findings in the presence of an important referent, we
consumer evaluations. Online offers were evaluated less conducted another study with 171 participants similar to
(more) favorably with threshold-based free shipping com- Study 1a except that the alternative referent was in the form
pared to flat rate shipping when the order value was below of a 20% price discount. This study also measured affect as
(above) the threshold for free shipping. The findings are participants were asked to indicate their feelings such as
consistent with the argument that the referent used to happiness and delight about the price of a coffeemaker. The
evaluate the fee under threshold-based free shipping results rule out affect as an alternative explanation for the
structure is free shipping. A follow-up study with 50 findings and corroborate the findings of Study 1a.
participants from an online paid panel provides further Despite the replications and robustness checks, a
support for this premise. Using a scenario similar to that potential limitation of Study 1a is that the referent of free
used in the main study, except for an offer, participants were shipping may have loomed large in threshold-based free
randomly assigned to either flat rate or threshold-based free shipping, particularly in the low order value condition,
shipping conditions. Participants were asked to respond to because the order value was just below the threshold for
the following questions: “In evaluating the retailer’s free shipping ($34.99 compared with the threshold of $40).
shipping fee policy, you compared the current shipping Study 1b tests the robustness of the results across different
fee to the shipping fees you have typically paid in the past” free shipping thresholds for an order value below the
and “In evaluating the retailer’s shipping fee policy, you threshold.
compared the current shipping fee to free shipping” (1 =
Strongly disagree; 7 = Strongly agree). The results show
that participants in the threshold-based free shipping
condition disagreed more with the first statement (M’s= Study 1b: robustness to different thresholds
4.12 and 5.34; F(1, 48)=3.45, p<.05) but agreed more with
the second statement (M’s=5.69 and 3.45; F(1, 48)=8.65, p Study 1b assesses the robustness of the findings in Study 1a
<.01). The main study findings, along with this follow-up to different thresholds of free shipping. Although our
data, suggest that the referent of free shipping is naturally conceptualization based on reference dependence does not
adopted for threshold-based free shipping while the referent predict variations in evaluations due to the near miss effect,
for flat rate shipping is relatively imprecise based on prior it is plausible that an offer may be evaluated less favorably
experience or knowledge. Accordingly, for online offers when the order value is just below versus well below the
with threshold-based free shipping and order values below free shipping threshold.
the threshold, the shipping fee is coded as a loss relative to
the free shipping referent and evaluations are lower Method
compared to offers with flat rate shipping. In contrast, for
order values above the threshold, the avoidance of shipping One hundred and thirty-seven participants (60% female,
fee is coded as a gain and evaluations are more favorable age ranging from 18 to 70 years, mean age=31.30 years)
compared to equivalent offers with flat rate shipping. from a paid online consumer panel in the U.S. participated
Study 1a also provides strong support for H1b. The in the study. Participants were randomly assigned to one of
results show that the effect of shipping fee structure and three conditions: flat rate, $25 threshold for free shipping,
order value on offer evaluations is attenuated when an and $75 threshold for free shipping. Participants imagined
alternative important referent is present. This is consistent that they were devoted coffee drinkers and were looking to
with the rationale that the presence of an important referent buy a new coffeemaker as theirs had broken down.
overshadows the effect of shipping fee (or draws attention Depending on the condition, the online store charged either
away from shipping fee), thus attenuating the effects. a flat rate shipping fee of $5.95 regardless of order value,
To confirm the mediating role of shipping policy fairness free shipping for orders above $25 and $5.95 otherwise, or
on the effect of shipping fee structure and order value on free shipping for orders above $75 and $5.95 otherwise.
offer evaluations, we conducted a study in a different The order value (coffeemaker price) was held constant at
product category (coffeemaker) with 157 participants $19.99. A pretest ensured that the difference between order
similar to the no alternative referent conditions of Study value and threshold was regarded as much larger in the $75
1a. We replicated the results of study 1a regarding offer versus the $25 threshold-based free shipping conditions.
evaluations, and confirmed that the effect was mediated by Similar to Study 1a, evaluations were measured by
shipping policy fairness perceptions (α=.92; 1 = Unaccept- averaging responses to three seven-point items (α=.91;
able/Not fair/Not justified; 7 = Acceptable/Fair/Justified; “The online offer is” 1 = Unattractive/Undesirable/Unrea-
Sobel z=2.21, p<.05). sonable; 7 = Attractive/Desirable/Reasonable). Participants
766 J. of the Acad. Mark. Sci. (2012) 40:759–770

were also asked if they believed that the retailer was trying Study 2: inferred motive and justification for shipping
to get consumers to spend more on a specific visit (1 = fees
Disagree; 7 = Agree).
Study 2 tests H2, which predicts that consumers are more
Results and discussion (less) likely to infer that retailers use shipping fees to
generate profit under threshold-based free shipping relative
An ANOVA with offer evaluations was significant (F(1, to flat rate shipping when the order value is below (above)
135)=5.04, p<.01). Specifically, planned contrasts revealed the free shipping threshold. However, these differences will
that the online offer was evaluated less favorably in the $25 be attenuated when a justification for shipping fee structure
threshold free shipping than in the flat rate condition (M’s= is provided.
4.42 and 5.38; F(1, 135)=7.87, p<.01). Further, the online
offer was evaluated less favorably in the $75 threshold free Method
shipping than in the flat rate condition (M’s=4.41 and 5.38;
F(1, 135)=7.21, p<.01). There was no difference across the One hundred and forty-one undergraduate students (60%
two threshold conditions (p>.97). These findings support female, age ranging from 18 to 23 years, mean age=
H1a. 19.9 years) from a U.S. university participated in the study
Another ANOVA with perceptions of the extent to for extra credit. Participants were randomly assigned to one
which the retailer was trying to get consumers to spend of eight conditions of a 2 (shipping fee structure: flat rate
more revealed a significant effect (F(1, 133)=11.14, p and threshold-based free shipping) x 2 (order value: $34.99
<.001). Planned contrasts showed that participants in the and $44.99 or $50.94) x 2 (justification: absent and present)
$25 threshold free shipping condition agreed more that between-subjects design. Participants were asked to imag-
the retailer was trying to get people to spend more per ine that they were devoted coffee drinkers and had to buy a
visit than those in the flat rate condition (M’s= 5.58 and new coffeemaker.
4.46; F(1, 133)= 10.89, p< .01). Participants in the $75 In the flat rate shipping condition the retailer charged a
threshold free shipping condition were also more likely to flat rate shipping fee of $5.95 for all order values while in
agree that the retailer was trying to get people to spend the threshold-based free shipping structure the retailer
more per visit than those in the flat rate condition (M’s= offered free shipping for order values above $40 and
6.10 and 4.46; F(1, 133) =20.82, p< .001). There was no $5.95 otherwise. In the low order value condition the
difference across the $25 and $75 threshold free shipping coffeemaker was priced at $34.99 while in the high order
conditions (p> .14). value condition it was priced at either $44.99 or $50.94. To
Next, we tested whether the perceptions of the extent ensure that the two offers were financially equivalent in the
to which the retailer was trying to get consumers to high order value condition, $44.99 was used in the flat rate
spend more mediated the effect of shipping fee structure condition for a total of $50.94 (including flat rate shipping
on offer evaluations. The first two steps of the mediation fee of $5.95) and $50.94 was used in the threshold-based
analyses (Baron and Kenny 1986) were reported above. free shipping condition. When a justification for shipping
In addition, we ran a model that included perceptions of fees was present, participants read the following: “Our
the extent to which the retailer was trying to get shipping charge is based on what the shipping company
consumers to spend as another (continuous) factor. The charges us to deliver our products to your doorstep. We do
effect of perceptions was significant (F(1, 132)=3.87, not make a profit on shipping charges. In fact, we absorb
p<.05), and the interaction between shipping structure and the cost of shipping for bigger orders, allowing us to
order value became insignificant (F(2, 132)=2.48, p=.09). [charge a flat fee no matter what your order size is]/[offer
A Sobel test confirmed that perceptions marginally free shipping].” This statement was not provided when
mediated the effect of shipping structure on offer evalu- justification was absent.
ation (Sobel z=1.91, p<.06). Perceptions of shipping fees as profit generator were
Study 1b shows that our conceptualization of free measured by averaging responses to two seven-point items
shipping being used as a referent for threshold-based free (r=.54). Specifically, participants were asked “How likely
shipping is robust across different thresholds. It also rules is that the online store is making a profit on the shipping
out the level of closeness to the threshold as an alternative fees?” and “How likely is that the shipping fee corresponds
explanation for the lower evaluations when threshold-based to the true shipping cost of the online retailer?” (1 = Very
free shipping is employed (relative to flat rate shipping). unlikely; 7 = Very likely). Evaluations of the online offer
Finally, Study 1b attests to the generalizability of the were measured as in Study 1a. Participants were also asked
findings since the participants were part of an online “How acceptable is the price of the coffeemaker?” (1 =
consumer panel. Unacceptable; 7 = Acceptable).
J. of the Acad. Mark. Sci. (2012) 40:759–770 767

Results and discussion threshold-based free shipping and the flat rate conditions
when the order value was below the threshold for free
Perceptions of shipping fees as profit generator A 2x2x2 shipping (M’s=5.15 and 4.91; F(1, 133)=0.53, p>.46) or
ANOVA on perceptions of shipping fees as a profit above the threshold for free shipping (M’s=4.89 and 5.09;
generator revealed a significant effect of justification (F(1, F(1, 133)=0.36, p>.54). These findings are consistent with
133)=6.51, p<.05) and a significant two-way interaction H2b.
between shipping fee structure and order value (F(1, 133)=
5.55, p<.05), which were qualified by the predicted three- Discussion Study 2 demonstrates that perceptions of the
way interaction (F(1, 133)=7.02, p<.05). extent to which retailers make profit off shipping fees
In the justification absent condition, for order values systematically vary in the threshold-based free shipping
below the free shipping threshold, perceptions that the structure depending on whether order value is above or
retailer was making a profit on shipping fees were higher in below the free shipping threshold. Compared to flat rate
the threshold-based free shipping condition than in the flat shipping, the belief that the retailer is making profit on
rate condition (M’s=4.92 and 3.65; F(1, 133)=7.55, p shipping fees is higher under threshold-based free shipping
<.01). The pattern reversed for order values above the free structure when the order value is below the free shipping
shipping threshold (M’s=3.43 and 4.47; F(1, 133)=4.60, p threshold. The result is in the opposite direction for order
<.05). These findings support H2a. values above the free shipping threshold. However, provid-
In the justification present condition, there was no ing a justification for the shipping structure such that there
significant difference across the threshold-based free ship- is a clear link between the shipping fee and the actual cost
ping and the flat rate conditions when the order value was of delivery attenuates the effect. Consistent with our
below the threshold for free shipping (M’s=3.22 and 3.81; argument, justification about the shipping fees encourages
F(1, 133)=1.64, p>.20) as well as when the order value consumers to view shipping fees as the cost of doing
was above the threshold for free shipping (M’s=3.32 and business rather than as a way for the firm to make
3.76; F(1, 133)=1.02, p>.31). These findings support H2b. additional profits. Importantly, justification for shipping
fees results in more favorable offer evaluations under
Offer evaluations A 2×2×2 ANOVA on offer evaluations threshold-based free shipping when order value is below
revealed a significant three-way interaction (F(1,133)= the free shipping threshold relative to when no justification
6.73, p<.05). No other effects were significant (all p’s is provided.
>.19). Table 2 shows that the pattern of results clearly
differs across the two justification conditions.
In the justification absent condition, the results replicate
those of Study 1a. For order values below the free shipping General discussion
threshold, the online offer was evaluated less favorably in
the threshold-based free shipping condition than in the flat Motivated by the experimentation occurring online with
rate shipping condition (M’s=4.57 and 5.14; F(1, 133)= regards to shipping fees, coupled with the relatively limited
2.82, p<.10). The reverse was true for financially equiva- research on the effects of shipping fee structure, the main
lent offers with order values above the free shipping purpose of this paper is to examine the effect of shipping
threshold (M’s=5.10 and 4.35; F(1, 133)=4.42, p<.05). fee structure on consumer response. This paper focuses on
However, in the presence of a justification for shipping two common shipping fee structures—flat rate and
structure, there was no significant difference across the threshold-based free shipping. In contrast to the prior

Table 2 Study 2: means (standard deviations) of dependent measures

Shipping fee structure Threshold-based free shipping Flat rate shipping

Order value (total price) $34.99 ($40.94) $50.94 ($50.94) $34.99 ($40.94) $44.99 ($50.94)

Justification - Absent
Perceptions of shipping fees as generating profits 4.92 (1.53) 3.43 (1.46) 3.65 (1.22) 4.47 (1.66)
Offer evaluations 4.57 (0.97) 5.10 (1.17) 5.14 (0.99) 4.35 (0.86)
Justification - Present
Perceptions of shipping fees as generating profits 3.22 (1.46) 3.32 (1.43) 3.81 (1.09) 3.76 (1.07)
Offer evaluations 5.15 (0.92) 4.89 (1.12) 4.91 (0.86) 5.09 (1.02)
768 J. of the Acad. Mark. Sci. (2012) 40:759–770

empirical research (Lewis 2006; Lewis et al. 2006), the consumers to view the shipping fee as the cost of doing
experimental methodology allows us to compare the two business rather than to assume that the retailer is using the
focal shipping fee structures. Importantly, we also examine shipping fee to make additional profits.
factors such as order value, an alternative important The limitations of this research bear comment. Despite
referent, and justification for the shipping fee structure that attempts to make the task as realistic as possible, and the
moderate the effect of shipping fee structure on consumer use of different populations including paid online panels,
response, thus providing a better understanding of the the studies reported here are laboratory experiments. While
conditions in which shipping fee structures affect consumer lab experiments allow us to examine different shipping fee
evaluations. structures and test moderator variables in a controlled
Based on the premise that evaluations are reference setting, thereby clarifying the theoretical underpinnings of
dependent (Kahneman and Tversky 1979), we argue that our findings, some aspects of online shopping such as
consumers use different referents to evaluate flat rate and shopping cart abandonment are not captured. Across our
threshold-based free shipping. Specifically, while the referent studies, we do examine offer evaluations, and it is arguable
used to evaluate flat rate shipping is likely to be based on that low offer evaluations may lead to cart abandonment.
prior experience and/or knowledge and thus somewhat Further, it is conceivable that most carts are abandoned by
imprecise, the referent used to evaluate threshold-based free “explorers” rather than “shoppers.” The findings reported
shipping is the possibility of free shipping, which is more here are specific to the shipping fee and threshold values
precise. While a memory-based referent requires more effort used in the studies. Although the values were selected
to access in the case of flat rate shipping, the stimulus-based based on actual online retailers and were varied across
referent of free shipping in the case of threshold-based free studies, it is important for future research to replicate the
shipping is vivid and easily accessible. Accordingly, for findings with other values, products, and settings.
threshold-based free shipping, order values below the Notwithstanding the limitations, the findings have
threshold are coded as a loss, whereas order values above important theoretical and managerial implications. First,
the threshold are coded as a bonus relative to the natural the results indicate that consumers use different referents to
referent of free shipping. evaluate shipping fee structures. While the referent for flat
Consistent with our conceptualization, Study 1a shows rate shipping is somewhat imprecise, the free shipping
that relative to flat rate shipping, evaluations are less (more) referent for threshold-based free shipping is stimulus-based
favorable when an online offer has threshold-based free and vivid. It is possible that threshold-based free shipping
shipping for order values below (above) the threshold. may lead to higher order values as consumers strive to
However, when an important alternative referent is provid- reach the free shipping threshold (Lewis et al. 2006).
ed, there are no differences in consumer evaluations across However, there may be unintended consequences. If
the two shipping fee structures. The important referent is consumers are shopping for a specific product (as examined
likely to shift attention away from the shipping fee and the in this paper) and do not wish to increase basket value just
associated referents, thus attenuating the effect of shipping to exceed the free shipping threshold, they are likely to
fee structure on consumer evaluations. Study 1b confirms perceive the shipping policy as unfair and may not
that the results are robust regardless of whether the order complete the purchase, leading to low evaluations and cart
value is just below versus well below the threshold for free abandonment. Further, the salience of the free shipping
shipping. referent may get consumers to fixate on order values that
Study 2 demonstrates that perceptions of shipping fees just exceed the free shipping threshold, hindering order
as a way for retailers to generate additional profits are values that are much higher than the free shipping
higher (lower) in the threshold-based free shipping condi- threshold. The implication is that while the free shipping
tion than in the flat rate condition for order values below referent in threshold-based free shipping may have a
(above) the threshold for free shipping. Given that positive effect in terms of orders that exceed the threshold,
consumer perceptions of shipping fees as a profit generator there may be a negative effect in that consumers may be
differ across the two shipping fee structures based on order content just to exceed the threshold. The idea that the free
value, providing a credible justification for the shipping fee shipping referent may fixate consumers to just exceed the
that links the fee to the actual cost of delivery should threshold and thus distract from higher order values is
attenuate the effects. Consistent with our conceptualization, offered as an area for future research.
Study 2 demonstrates that perceptions of shipping fees as a Second, our findings suggest that providing important
profit generator as well as consumer evaluations do not alternative referents such as a price promotion or reduction
differ across the flat rate and threshold-based free shipping draws attention away from the referents used in evaluating
structures for all order values when a justification is shipping fee structures. This strategy is likely to be more
provided. The justification for the shipping fee encourages effective for online retailers with unknown or low reputa-
J. of the Acad. Mark. Sci. (2012) 40:759–770 769

tion because consumers pay more attention to surcharges structure, retailers have to decide on specific fees and
from such retailers (Cheema 2008). Drawing attention away values based on their average delivery costs and average
from shipping fees may encourage consumers to scale up order value.
their order values and complete the purchase process (and
thus less cart abandonment).
Third, retailers should carefully consider the specifics of References
using a threshold-based free shipping structure. Relative to
flat rate shipping, consumer attributions are less favorable Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator-mediator
with threshold-based free shipping when the order value is variable distinction in social psychological research: conceptual,
below the threshold and shipping is not free. Thus, although strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 51, 1173–82.
consumers may be motivated to increase their order value
Barsh, J., Blair, C., & Grosso, C. (2000). How E-tailing can rise from
to pass the threshold for free shipping, it may result in the ashes. The Mckinsey Quarterly, 3, 98–109.
negative attributions and affect future purchase decisions Bies, R. J., & Shapiro, D. L. (1988). Voice and justification: their
negatively. It may be more beneficial for retailers to charge influence on procedural fairness and judgments. Academy of
Management Journal, 31, 676–685.
flat rate shipping fees as it leads to more positive inferences
Campbell, M. C. (1999). Perceptions of price unfairness: antecedents
and offer evaluations. This recommendation is based on our and consequences. Journal of Marketing Research, 36, 187–199.
studies and is subject to change as more evidence regarding Chakravarti, D., Krish, R., Paul, P., & Srivastava, J. (2002). Partitioned
order value and incidence rate amasses in the literature. presentation of multicomponent bundle prices: evaluation,
choice, and underlying processing effects. Journal of Consumer
Importantly, retailers can alleviate consumer perceptions of Psychology, 12(3), 215–29.
shipping fees as an additional way for retailers to make Cheema, A. (2008). Surcharges and seller reputation. Journal of
profits by providing a justification for the shipping structure Consumer Research, 35(2), 167–77.
that they use. A credible justification is likely to have a ComCult Research. (2003). ComCult Study eCommerce. http://www.
comcult.com.
positive impact on image, sales, and profitability.
Darke, P. R., & Dahl, D. W. (2003). Fairness and discounts: the
Although these findings are consistent with reference subjective value of a bargain. Journal of Consumer Psychology,
dependent evaluations, they suggest that consumers may 13(3), 328–38.
believe that since retailers make relatively little profit on Fiske, S. T., & Taylor, S. E. (1991). Social cognition. New York:
McGraw-Hill.
small orders, they compensate by charging a shipping fee.
Folkes, V. S. (1984). Consumer reactions to product failure: An
Alternatively, consumers may believe that retailers attempt attributional approach. Journal of Consumer Research, 10
to make money on the shipping fee from consumers with (March), 398–409.
small orders. Future research may further explore the Hamilton, R. W., & Srivastava, J. (2008). When 2 + 2 is not the same
as 1 + 3:variations in price sensitivity across components of
specific inferences and attributions consumers make as a
partitioned prices. Journal of Marketing Research, 45(4), 450–
result of the shipping fee structure employed, and outline 61.
other boundary conditions (moderators) for the inferred Hamilton, R. W., Srivastava, J., & Abraham, A. T. (2010). When
motives. For example, the shipping policy of competitors should you nickel and dime your customers? A manager’s guide
to benefits-based price partitioning. MIT Sloan Management
may be used as a reference and guide the attributions the
Review, 52(1), 59–67.
consumers make about the focal retailer. In addition, it Haws, K. L., & Bearden, W. O. (2006). Dynamic pricing and
will be interesting to empirically examine the effect of consumer fairness perceptions. Journal of Consumer Research,
various shipping fee structures on order incidence, 33(December), 304–311.
Hsee, C. K., & Leclerc, F. (1998). Will products look more attractive
shopping basket (order) value, and profitability. Finally,
when presented separately or together? Journal of Consumer
future research may also investigate how consumers Research, 25(September), 175–86.
respond to very high shipping fees, sometimes as high Hsee, C. K., Loewenstein, G. F., Blount, S., & Bazerman, M. H.
as 50% of the price of the product for some TV offers. (1999). Preference reversals between joint and separate evalua-
tions of options: a review and theoretical analysis. Psychological
Arguably these retailers are offering their products at
Bulletin, 125(5), 576–90.
lower prices than competitors and are compensating the Kahneman, D., & Miller, D. T. (1986). Norm theory: comparing
lower product margin with higher shipping and handling reality to its alternatives. Psychological Review, 93(2), 136–53.
charges. Although they may offer the lowest product Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1979). Prospect theory: an analysis of
decision under risk. Econometrica, 47(March), 163–91.
price and entice consumers, the perceived unfairness of
Kahneman, D., Knetsch, J. L., & Thaler, R. (1986). Fairness as a
the shipping policy may have a negative effect on retailer constraint on profit seeking: entitlements in the market. The
satisfaction and product purchase likelihood. American Economic Review, 76(4), 728–741.
In sum, it is imperative that online retailers employ a Kelley, H. H. (1967). Attributional theory in social psychology. In D.
Levine (Ed.) Nebraska symposium of motivation (pp. 192-238).
shipping fee structure that strikes a balance between the
Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.
need to attract and retain customers and the need to Lassar, W. M., Folks, V. S., Grewal, D., & Costley, C. (1997).
recoup delivery costs. In implementing any shipping fee Consumer affective reactions to product problems when the
770 J. of the Acad. Mark. Sci. (2012) 40:759–770

timing of warranty expiration varies. Journal of Business Schindler, R. M., Morrin, M., & Bechwati, N. N. (2005). Shipping charges
Research, 42, 265–70. and shipping charge skepticism: Implications for direct marketers’
Lewis, M. (2006). The effect of shipping fees on customer acquisition, pricing formats. Journal of Interactive Marketing, 9, 41–53.
customer retention, and purchase quantities. Journal of Retailing, Smith, M. D., & Brynjolfsson, E. (2001). Consumer decision making
82, 13–23. at an internet shopbot: brand still matters. The Journal of
Lewis, M., Singh, V., & Fay, S. (2006). An empirical study of the impact Industrial Economics, 49(4), 541–58.
of nonlinear shipping and handling fees on purchase incidence and Tsiros, M., Mittal, V., & Ross, W. T. (2004). The role of attributions in
expenditure decisions. Marketing Science, 25, 51–64. customer satisfaction. Journal of Consumer Research, 31, 476–
Morwitz, V. G., Greenleaf, E. A., & Johnson, E. J. (1998). Divide and 483.
prosper: consumers’ reactions to partitioned prices. Journal of Weiner, B. (2000). Attributional thoughts about consumer behavior.
Marketing Research, 35(November), 453–63. Journal of Consumer Research, 27(December), 382–387.
Rosen, K. T., & Howard, A. L. (2000). E-retail: gold rush or fool’s Xia, L., & Monroe, K. B. (2004). Price partitioning on the internet.
gold. California Management Review, 42(3), 72–100. Journal of Interactive Marketing, 18(4), 63–73.

View publication stats

You might also like