You are on page 1of 11

Clinical Focus

Two Approaches to the Facilitation of


Grammar in Children With Language
Impairments: Rationale and Description

Patricia L. Cleave
Dalhousie University, Halifax, Nova Scotia

Marc E. Fey
University of Kansas Medical Center, Kansas City

This article describes in detail the design specially designed stories employed in both
and rationale of two effective approaches for programs are outlined, as are the unique
the facilitation of grammar in children with features of each program. In addition, we
language impairments. One program was provide our clinical impressions and sugges-
clinician directed and the other was adminis- tions for modifying the programs to increase
tered by parents. The focused stimulation their effectiveness and efficiency.
techniques, cyclical goal attack strategy, and

F
ey and his colleagues (Fey, Cleave, Long, & al., 1993). An additional 4-1/2 months of treatment led to
Hughes, 1993; Fey et al., 1994; Fey, Cleave, & additional gains, although, in general, improvements were
Long, 1997) have reported on the effects of two not as great in this second phase as they were in the first
intervention packages designed to foster grammatical (Fey et al., 1997). Gains in each phase were most consis-
development in preschoolers with impaired language. tent for those children who received the clinician-directed
One program was administered by a speech-language intervention. This consistency did not come without cost,
pathologist and the other by parents. In previous reports of however. Over two intervention phases (a total treatment
this intervention project, the approaches were outlined plus evaluation time of approximately 10 months), the
briefly, as is typical in research papers. This paper clinician program required approximately 3.5 times more
describes in detail the two approaches and their rationales. clinician time than did the parent program.
Each component of these comprehensive grammar Phonological development, which was not directly
facilitation approaches is described and exemplified. This targeted, was not affected by either intervention approach.
level of detail is necessary for clinicians interested in Small improvements noted in the speech of children who
replicating our approaches and in performing clinical received immediate treatment were no greater than the
manipulations designed to match and improve the pro- gains made by children who got intervention only after a
grams’ effectiveness. 4-1/2 month waiting period (Fey et al., 1994).
Thirty-one children between the ages of 3;8 and 5;10 at The two language intervention programs were based on
the outset of the investigation were involved in the identical principles, but because they involved different
treatment efficacy study. Although they varied somewhat intervention agents, they also differed in many important
in terms of their receptive language and phonological respects. The common features of the two programs are
skills, the primary area of difficulty for all the children was discussed first, and then the unique aspects of the clinician-
expressive language, particularly morphosyntactic devel- directed and parent-directed programs are described. Finally,
opment (see Fey et al., 1993, for a complete subject we include some of our clinical impressions and sugges-
description). Participation in either program for 4-1/2 tions for modifications and extensions of the programs.
months resulted in clinically significant effects on the
children’s grammatical development as measured by
Developmental Sentence Scoring (DSS; Lee, 1974) (Fey et
Common Features of the Two Programs
The clinician-directed and parent-directed programs
Ruth Watkins served as guest editor for this article. shared many features. These included the method for

22 American Journal
American of Speech-Language
Journal of Speech-Language Pathology •• Vol.
Pathology Vol. 66 •• 1058-0360/97/0601-0022
No. 1 February 1997
© American Speech-Language-Hearing Association
setting goals, the goal attack strategy adopted, and the use To attempt to achieve these basic intervention goals,
of focused stimulation procedures. individualized intermediate and specific goals were chosen
for each child. Goals were selected based on a language
sample collected while the child was interacting with a
Goal Setting Method parent or other primary caregiver. For an existing structure
When developing a language intervention plan, it is to be considered as a direct target for treatment (i.e., a
possible to set a number of goals representing various specific goal), it had to have been omitted in more than
levels of specificity (Fey, 1986; Fey & Cleave, 1990). half of the contexts in which it was judged to be obligatory
Basic goals, which are the most general goal statements of or rarely or never used in contexts in which it could have
an intervention plan, are based on a child’s social- been used to positive effect. For a new structure to be
interactional skills. They identify those areas of weakness considered, the child minimally had to provide evidence of
that present the greatest obstacles to the child’s social- a cognitive/semantic readiness and a communicative need
communicative success and focus the intervention for the structure. This typically came in the form of
program on those areas. Based on interactions with adults, sentences in which a required form was consistently
all the children in our programs were judged to be omitted or in failure to use developmentally appropriate
normally responsive and able to play an assertive role in forms judged to be important for communicative success,
conversation. Thus, they were judged to be active conver- such as phrasal and clausal postmodifiers (e.g., the dog in
sationalists (Fey, 1986). Because all of the children had the window; the dog that barks).
impairments in expressive grammar as their main diffi- The list of potential specific goals generated in this
culty, one set of basic goals was selected. The basic manner was always much too large to allow for direct
intervention goals were: focus on each. Therefore, the list had to be trimmed in
some meaningful way. One way in which the list of
1. to increase the frequency and consistency of the child’s potential targets was shortened was by selecting intermedi-
use of grammatical forms and operations that typically ate goals. Intermediate goals reflected general areas of
are used infrequently and inconsistently;
weakness in which several potential specific goals played a
2. to foster the child’s acquisition of new content-form subordinate role. For example, common intermediate
interactions to perform available conversational acts; and objectives included auxiliary verbs, nominative case
3. to set the child’s existing language-learning mecha- pronouns (e.g., he, she) and sentence coordination (e.g.,
nisms in motion to promote the acquisition of general and, but, or) and/or subordination (e.g., John hit the ball
linguistic principles, and to foster broad, systemic before he ran). Each of these categories or operations
changes in the child’s grammar. involves numerous specific forms. For example, auxiliaries
included modals (e.g., can, should), forms of do and have
The first goal stemmed from the fact that all the (e.g., Did you eat? Have you finished?), and all of the
children had as part of their language system some manifestations of be (e.g., is, are). The identification of
grammatical structures that were used inconsistently or in a intermediate goals reflects the fact that language is a
limited number of contexts. For example, the copula (e.g., system that comprises related structures. It was our
John is sad; the girls are angry) may have been present in intention to devise intervention plans that would lead the
some statements and omitted altogether in questions. For children to an awareness of broad linguistic categories and
these structures, treatment focused on encouraging the principles.
child to use the language forms consistently in all obliga- Our approach to goal selection was based on the
tory contexts. assumption that by presenting related specific goals in
The second basic goal focused on the acquisition of ways that demonstrate their similar grammatical properties
language structures that the child never used even though as well as their different meanings, the child might
their use was required to correctly convey meanings the recognize the broader category. If such insights can be
child was expressing. For example, the child may have triggered through intervention, the child may be in a better
talked about past events and occasionally used irregular position to attend to and learn the other members of the
past tense forms but never used the regular past tense category. Therefore, by working on a few targets, it may be
marker. possible to facilitate the acquisition of related forms with
The third goal was somewhat different from the other little or no further treatment. Although this rationale is not
two in that it did not focus on the development of specific common in language intervention, it has a well-known
language structures. Rather, it represented a more general parallel in phonological intervention. There, the intermedi-
goal of intervention to have broad, system-wide effects on ate goals are phonological processes or rules (e.g., stopping
the children’s language. Both intervention programs sought or final consonant deletion) and the specific goals are the
to encourage the children to observe grammatical forms specific speech sounds targeted, which are selected from
and operations, seek the boundaries for their use, and the larger set of sounds affected by the target process or
recognize that their use under certain conditions is obliga- rule (Hodson & Paden, 1991; Tyler & Sandoval, 1994;
tory. Stated differently, this basic goal reflects our interest Tyler & Watterson, 1991).
in stimulating children’s existing language-learning Following the identification of the intermediate goals,
processes so that the children can learn language outside of the target set of specific goals was chosen. Because of our
the clinical setting more efficiently. clinical hypotheses cited above and our need to standardize

Cleave • Fey 23
our procedures across children, four specific goals were time also has some practical advantages. During group
targeted for each child. In all of our cases, two or more of sessions, the clinician never has to target more than one
the specific goals exemplified the more general grammati- goal per child in any one session. It also may be easier for
cal principle, category, or operation designated by the parents learning new techniques to focus on one goal at a
intermediate goal. For example, when auxiliary usage was time rather than learning to employ the appropriate
an intermediate goal, at least two auxiliary forms (e.g., is procedures for several different goals.
and will or is and are) were chosen as specific goals. If In our programs, the cycles were 4 weeks long with
phrasal postmodification was targeted, prepositional each goal being targeted for one week. Again, the 4-week
phrases headed by in and under might have been selected cycle was selected to standardize the procedures across
(e.g., the juice in the pitcher, the doll under the bed). subjects rather than from any belief on our part that it was
To summarize the goal selection procedure for our ideal. In some cases, more or less time on each specific
programs, the basic goals were the same for all the goal may be optimal. During week 1 of a cycle, goal 1 was
children, because they all demonstrated primary difficulty targeted and then, regardless of whether any progress was
with expressive grammatical skills. For each child, demonstrated, the second goal was focused on during
individualized intermediate goals were selected to target week 2. Goals 3 and 4 followed in weeks 3 and 4, respec-
areas of general weakness. Finally, four specific grammati- tively. During week 5, a new cycle was begun with goal 1
cal structures that exemplified the broader intermediate as the target. After a cycle, related goals were sometimes
level categories and principles were identified as interven- combined. For example, if a child had the auxiliaries is
tion goals. and will as two separate goals, after one or two cycles, we
combined the two auxiliaries into one goal and targeted
them together during the same week. When there was
Cyclical Goal Attack Strategy evidence of productive spontaneous use of a target by the
To deal with the four specific intervention goals child, it was dropped as a goal or combined with other
concurrently, we used a cyclical goal attack strategy in specific targets directed at the same intermediate goal. In
both programs. To our knowledge, reports of this goal order to maintain four active specific goals, we added a
attack strategy have involved phonological intervention new goal whenever a space was left open.
exclusively (Hodson & Paden, 1991; Tyler, Edwards, &
Saxman, 1987; Tyler & Sandoval, 1994), although the
potential for this strategy in other forms of intervention has Focused Stimulation Procedures
been noted by Fey (1986, 1988). Under this approach, one Focused stimulation, as described by Fey (1986), was
goal is worked on for a specified period of time, for the basic approach used in both the clinician-directed and
example, 1 or 2 weeks. Then, regardless of whether any parent-directed treatment programs. With focused stimula-
progress is demonstrated, the second goal is targeted in the tion, the interventionist provides a high density of a target
second period. Goal 3 is the focus of the third period, goal language form or operation in a variety of semantically and
4 the fourth, and so on, until all goals have been targeted. pragmatically appropriate contexts (Fey, 1986; Johnston,
After completion of the first cycle, a second cycle is begun 1985). The child is not required to respond at any point in
with goal 1. When a goal is attained, it is dropped or time. However, efforts are made to arrange the linguistic
combined with an existing target and a new goal may be and nonlinguistic contexts to encourage the child to
added to the cycle. attempt the target form.
There were three rationales for the choice of a cyclical A number of procedures can be used in a focused
goal attack strategy. First, this strategy is based on the stimulation approach. The parents in the parent treatment
assumption that progress on any language goal will be program were trained to apply the same procedures that were
gradual (Hodson & Paden, 1991). Children can be taught employed by the clinician in the clinician treatment program.
to produce a new form in a relatively short period of time. In both programs, natural conversation between the child and
However, the net result of such teaching frequently is not the intervention agent was the context for intervention. Care
the language rule the clinician intended to teach (Connell, was taken to ensure that the various procedures were used in
1987; Fey, 1988; Johnston, 1988; Leonard, 1981). Second, pragmatically appropriate contexts. In addition, target
the cyclical goal attack strategy allows the child to be structures were contrasted with related structures to highlight
actively engaged in learning more than one target at a time. the semantic and syntactic features of the target. For ex-
Not only is this how normal language acquisition occurs, it ample, when targeting nominative case pronouns, accusative
is also critical for the assumptions underlying the choice of case forms would be modeled in close proximity (e.g., Give it
intermediate goals. It seems more likely that a child will to him. He needs it). The goal of intervention was for the
note the grammatical similarities and the semantic differ- children to learn the underlying language rule(s) that govern
ences between two related specific goals if they are the use of their target structures, rather than isolated language
targeted together or in close temporal proximity rather than behaviors. To achieve this, it was necessary to demonstrate
working on one goal to mastery before focusing on the the critical circumstances under which a target form is and is
related goal. The third rationale is that the cyclical goal not used (Connell, 1982; 1987). By presenting both target
attack strategy allows the clinician to present fairly and contrasting forms together, these circumstances were
concentrated “doses” of the target during the period when highlighted, thus (potentially) simplifying the rule induction
it is the focus. The fact that only one goal is focused on at a for the child.

24 American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology • Vol. 6 • No. 1 February 1997


Modeling the Targets. There were many ways in which into its individual constituents. In the following inter-
the correct use of a target was demonstrated to the child. change, the adult produces breakdowns of the child’s
Simple modeling involved the use of a target by the adult utterance and then builds up her own elliptical utterances.
that was not contingent on a child’s attempt at the target. It Note that the adult is also expanding the child’s utterances
was used to demonstrate the range of appropriate uses of by adding missing grammatical markers. By breaking the
target structures. The linguistic context was manipulated so child’s utterance into its constituent phrases, the grammati-
that the target was modeled in contexts in which it might cal markers within the phrases can be placed into contexts
be most readily perceived and analyzed, for example, that are potentially more salient and informative, thus
sentence initial position or elliptical contexts. The follow- increasing the chance that the child will incorporate them
ing exchange demonstrates modeling of the target will into the language system.
while mixing up some juice. Child: I need book, not boot.
Child: We need water. Adult: Oh, the book.
Adult: Will you get some? Not the boot.
Child: Yeah. OK, I’ll get the book.
Adult: You will? Child: No, I get it.
Good. Then I will get the glasses. Adult: No, I will.
I will get it.
When the child attempted the target, there were three
different types of sentence recasts (Nelson, 1989) used by Eliciting Attempts at the Targets. The linguistic and
the clinician and parents to respond. The simplest recasts nonlinguistic environments were manipulated in ways that
were simple expansions in which errors in the child’s increased the likelihood that the child would attempt the
utterance were corrected. The following is an example. target form. This gave the clinician opportunities to operate
Child: Him too big. on the child’s own sentences even when the child’s
productions contained errors in the use of target forms.
Adult: Yeah, he is too big.
False assertions were used to encourage the child to
A second type of recast kept the child’s utterance produce sentences that required use of the target. These
largely intact but changed the basic modality of the were carefully planned so that the child’s correction of the
utterance. For example, the child’s use of a declarative adult obligated the use of the target structure. In the
sentence could be changed into a yes/no question. This example below, the child’s language goal is the use of
resulted in the auxiliary form being placed in the sentence can’t and the child and adult are putting a puzzle together.
initial position, which may be more salient than a sentence
Adult: That can go there (knowing that the piece does
internal position. Also, an affirmative statement could be not fit).
recast as a negative. For example, the child’s utterance Child: No it not.
“This too hard” could be recast as a question (e.g., Is it too
Adult: Yes, it can.
hard?) or as a negative (e.g., It isn’t too hard) while adding Child: No.
missing grammatical markers, if needed. Recasts of this Adult: Yeah, you’re right. That piece can’t go there.
type can be very informative because they demonstrate the
variety of ways in which a structure, such as the copula or Feigned misunderstandings in which the adult intention-
auxiliary, can be used (e.g., to affirm, request, correct) ally misinterprets an utterance by the child also were used.
(Nelson, 1989; Watkins & Pemberton, 1987). As with false assertions, care was taken so that the child’s
Buildups and breakdowns were the third type of recast. correction of the adult would require the target form. In the
A buildup involves taking one or more child utterances or following example, the target is I.
phrases and building it up to a single, complete sentence.
Child: Me want it.
Buildups demonstrate a way of using a given structure in a Adult: (pointing to another boy in the group) He wants
more complex utterance. The following exchanges it?
demonstrate buildups.
Child: No, me want it.
Child: I drawed a picture. It pretty. Adult: No, I do (pointing to herself).
Adult: Yeah, you drew a pretty picture.
Thus, even when the child failed to use the target form,
Child: me get another cookie. me hungry.
Adult: Oh, you’re gonna get another cookie because the adult recast the child’s sentence to provide a salient
you’re hungry. model of the structure in context.
Forced-choice questions (Crystal, Fletcher, & Garman,
These exchanges often result in sentences that differ 1976) that provided a model of the correct use of the target
from the child’s utterance in more than one structure. were also employed. Again, these prompts were designed
When more than a single element in the child’s sentence so that the child’s response required the targeted structure.
undergoes change, the recast is viewed as complex as For example, if the use of articles was the goal, an appro-
compared to a simple recast when only a single aspect of priate forced-choice question would be, “Do you want an
the child’s sentence is modified (Nelson, 1989; Nelson, apple or an orange?” These may be especially effective
Welsh, Camarata, Butkovsky, & Camarata, 1995). when they are responses to child utterances in which the
A breakdown involves breaking the child’s utterance clinician feigns confusion, as in the following example.

Cleave • Fey 25
Child: I get it. procedures can stop conversation rather than facilitate it,
Adult: You already got it or you will get it now? all the procedures were used in a coordinated fashion rather
than relying on one or two.
Other contingent queries, especially requests for Use of “Syntax Stories.” The focused stimulation
elaboration, were used to encourage the child to code techniques outlined above also were used to create stories
linguistically omitted semantic information. This technique that were used in both programs. These stories were
is particularly useful for language goals that involve written with a focus on a particular grammatical goal, and
sentence coordination or subordination. By carefully they were designed to make liberal use of the same focused
employing this technique, the clinician often can get the stimulation procedures described earlier in the narrative
child to create a compound or complex sentence using a context. The stories were developed and stored using a
vertical structure (Fey, 1986; Schwartz, Chapman, Terrell, word processor so that the same stories could be used with
Prelock, & Rowan, 1985). This is demonstrated in the next different children. The search and replace function of the
example in which a child is showing the clinician a favorite computer software made it simple to customize the same
book. story for several children by substituting the names of the
Child: I like that. new child and that child’s friends and family for the names
Adult: Why do you like swimming? already existing in the story. Each week, the parents were
Child: Cool me off. given a story that highlighted their child’s goal. Although
Adult: Oh, you like swimming because the water cools these stories might have been used in many different ways,
you off. the parents were asked only to read their story to their child
once a day.
An important part of this procedure is the building up of An example of a story is contained in the Appendix.
the child’s original utterance by the adult using the Examples of the focused stimulation procedures are
additional information supplied by the child in response to labelled in the Appendix, although these labels were not
the adult’s query. So, this technique is like a buildup in present on samples given to the children and their parents.
which the adult first queries to get the child to produce
additional material, then recasts both child utterances into a
single, more complex form. The Clinician Program
The nonlinguistic environment also was manipulated to Although the two intervention programs shared many
increase the likelihood that the child would attempt the features, there were some significant differences between
target form or structure (Constable, 1983; Fey, 1986; the two approaches. In this section, the unique aspects of
Lucas, 1980). Routine events were violated by omitting a the clinician program are described. The children involved
step or performing it incorrectly. For example, when in the clinician program were seen three times a week,
mixing up juice, the water would be forgotten, prompting once in an individual session and twice in a small group
the child to ask for it. This might set up an exchange like involving four to six children. All sessions lasted for one
the following: hour. In the individual session, the child’s goal for the
Child: Us need water. week was introduced. This goal then served as the focus
Adult: We do. for the child during the two group sessions of the same
We do need some water. week.

Withholding objects and turns also served to encourage


the child to make a statement or request. For example, Individual Sessions
skipping a child’s turn may elicit the response It my turn. In the individual session, focused stimulation was the
Then, the clinician could respond Oh, it is your turn or Is it main treatment approach, but a structured, imitation
your turn? Similarly, by “forgetting” the glue during an art protocol was used at the beginning of each session. A brief
activity, the children were encouraged to ask for it. Hiding story was acted out with the child imitating the clinician’s
objects was another technique used to elicit and provide sentences. These stories were neither as carefully planned
models for questions by setting up a guessing game (e.g., nor as creative as the stories described in the last section
Where are the cups? Are they in the drawer?). and exemplified in the Appendix. They were created by the
Finally, violation of object function was used. An clinician using props that had been selected for their ability
example of this strategy is for the adult to try to stir the to stimulate many sentences containing target forms and
juice with a spoon that is obviously too small. This operations. They differed from traditional imitation stimuli,
procedure often results in sentences that require a negative such as sets of unrelated pictures, however, in that the
form (e.g., That won’t work. Don’t use that spoon.) same characters were involved in related events throughout
Both the linguistic and nonlinguistic manipulations the imitation exercise. In this way, even though they lacked
were designed to encourage the children to use sentences many properties of good children’s stories, the imitation
that required the target structure. Obviously, the children stories exemplified and required imitation of the targets in
frequently made errors in their attempts. This then set the a bona fide text (e.g., going shopping, preparing for a
stage for the adult to respond using one of the types of party).
recasts. Because the various procedures were useful for Each imitation story involved imitation of the target
different things and because the overuse of some of these form and a contrast form. The contrast target was chosen

26 American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology • Vol. 6 • No. 1 February 1997


such that it highlighted the structural and semantic proper- towards a particular child ranged from 25 to 100 in the
ties of the target. For instance, if the target was the group sessions.
auxiliary will, possible contrast targets were the auxiliary By having both individual and group sessions, the
can or is or the regular past tense marker -ed. clinician program had the advantages of each. During the
The protocol began with 10 rote imitations of the target individual sessions, the clinician could focus specifically
form (e.g., the nominative case pronouns he and she) using on the child’s target and, therefore, provide a higher
no props. Then the clinician began to act out the story’s density of the target structure than in the group sessions. In
events using the props, requiring the child to imitate 10 the group sessions, the children were exposed to the goals
productions of the contrast target (e.g., accusative case of the other children. The children also served as models
pronouns him and her) and then 10 imitations of the target. for each other. The children did not have difficulties with
Finally, there were 10 imitations of the target alternated exactly the same language structures, although consider-
with 10 imitations of the contrast target (e.g., Now, he able overlap was common. Therefore, within the group,
needs the sponge. Give it to him). there was often a child who had mastered a particular
The imitation protocol took approximately 10 minutes structure. By setting up the situation, the clinician could
at the beginning of the individual session. We did not encourage that child to use another child’s target appropri-
assume that the imitative procedures would teach the child ately as a model for the second child. The group also
a new form. Rather, this exercise was included as a way to provided an opportunity to encourage peer interaction.
focus the child’s attention on the week’s target and how its
use differed from a related form. We hypothesized that
contrasting the target with another form would stimulate The Parent Program
and focus the child’s hypothesis-testing mechanism so that The parent program included both group sessions,
greater benefit could be derived from the more naturalistic which involved five or six sets of the parents, and indi-
models of the target presented in the rest of the treatment vidual sessions, which involved the parents and their child.
sessions and from exposure to the target outside the clinic For the children who lived with both parents, both parents
(Connell, 1982, 1986, 1987). were encouraged to be involved whenever possible. In our
Following the imitation protocol, the child and clinician program, both parents participated in approximately one-
played briefly with the story props if the child had shown third of the cases.
an interest in them. Then the clinician read the child’s new
story for the week. This story was sent home for the
parents to read daily for that week. The remainder of the Group Sessions
session was spent in play using whatever materials were The parents were taught to use the same focused
required for the activity and employing focused stimulation stimulation procedures that the clinician employed in the
procedures. Play with Play-Doh, a doctor kit, and an clinic-based program. For the first 12 weeks of the pro-
airport are examples of the type of activity that took place gram, the parents met as a group with the clinician weekly
in treatment. The individual session ended with 10 imita- for 2 hours without their children. It was during these
tions of sentences containing the target using no props. sessions that the focused stimulation techniques were
taught. Handouts that defined the techniques and contained
examples of each were given to the parents. The group
Group Sessions reviewed videotapes of the procedures being applied. For
The group sessions were structured into three sections. the first few weeks, these were tapes of the clinician using
For 20 to 40 minutes, the children participated coopera- the techniques, but in later weeks, videotapes of the parents
tively in a group activity such as baking muffins, popping themselves were used. Role plays were used to allow the
popcorn, carving a pumpkin, making Play-Doh, and parents to practice and receive corrective feedback. In
making crafts such as masks. A snack time lasted for 15 to addition, written transcripts emphasizing the new tech-
20 minutes. The children learned roles of passing out niques were studied, analyzed, and discussed.
napkins, juice, and snacks and talked as they ate their Because the parents were only gradually adding
snack. Finally, if time permitted, a free play period took procedures to their repertoire, goals were not cycled during
place. The children played with their choice of toys such the first 4 weeks. Goal 1 remained the target so that the
as blocks, dress-up clothes, houses, and cars. During this parents did not have to adjust to a new goal each week.
time, the clinician spent a few minutes with each child or Weekly cycling of goals began in week 5. See Table 1 for
each naturally formed small group of children. Through- the topics and goal schedule for each week.
out the session, the clinician used focused stimulation As noted earlier, we asked the parents in both programs
procedures to highlight the particular week’s goal for each to read specially constructed stories to their children. In the
child. parent program, stories were also used in teaching the
The number of models of each target provided in the parents the focused stimulation techniques. At the first
individual and group sessions varied greatly, depending session, the parents were given three stories and asked to
primarily on the target form itself. For example, opportuni- study the characteristics of each for a discussion at the next
ties for presenting relative clauses were considerably more meeting. One of the stories was written along the guide-
difficult to generate than were opportunities for the past lines of the stories used in the intervention program (see
tense -ed or the copula is. The number of models directed Appendix). This meant that it told an interesting story and

Cleave • Fey 27
TABLE 1. Weekly parent group sessions. the parents constructed a story similar to those they had
been reading to their children. The use of the procedures to
Specific highlight the target was stressed. For the next 4 weeks, the
Week Topic Goal parents constructed the stories that they read to their
1 Introduction to parent programming and
children. To ensure that the parents’ stories provided
focused stimulation—principles and rationale. sufficient models, they were edited by the clinician before
being used as the child’s weekly story. Writing these
2 Demonstrating use of the target using 1 stories for 4 weeks gave the parents practice in using the
focused modeling and contrast forms techniques with their child’s particular goals under
Story for goal 1 is provided
conditions that did not require split-second decisions about
3 Responding using recasts 1 how to respond. It also required the parents to identify
Story for goal 1 is provided situations in which the language target could be used
frequently. After the parents had been writing stories for 4
4 Responding using recasts 1
Story for goal 1 is provided
weeks, we provided stories from our own collection for the
rest of the program.
5 Constructing a story using focused stimulation 2 After the completion of the 12 weekly sessions, the
techniques parents continued to meet on a monthly basis. During these
Story for goal 2 is provided sessions, the procedures were reviewed to increase the
6 Eliciting the target using questions—closed- 3 probability that the parents would continue to use all the
ended versus open-ended procedures. Weekly stories for the next month were
Parents bring a story they wrote focusing on provided. These meetings were also important for the
goal 4. Story for goal 3 is provided support they provided to the parents, both from the
7 Eliciting the target using forced choice and 4
clinician and the other parents. In these monthly meetings
contingent questions where there was less of a focus on learning procedures,
Parents bring a story they wrote focusing on other issues, such as difficulties at school, discipline, and
goal 1. An edited version of their goal 4 story concerns about the future, were frequently brought up by
is returned to be used as the week’s story the parents and discussed by the group.
8 Eliciting the target using false assertions and 1 Throughout the program, the parents were seen indi-
feigned misunderstandings and by manipulating vidually with their child on a monthly basis. For the first 3
nonverbal environment months, these visits took place in the children’s homes. For
Parents bring a story they wrote focusing on the remainder of the program, the visits were held in the
goal 2. An edited version of their goal 1 story
is returned to be used as the week’s story
clinic. These appointments had many purposes. The
parents were videotaped using the focused stimulation
9 Integrating techniques and incorporating them 2 procedures while interacting with their child. The tapes
into daily activities were then reviewed so that the parents were able to
Parents bring a story they wrote focusing on evaluate their own performance. This also gave the
goal 3. An edited version of their goal 2 story
is returned to be used as the week’s story clinician an opportunity to provide feedback in a situation
that was less threatening than the group meetings. These
10 Integrating techniques and incorporating them 3 videotapes were used in the group meetings. By thoroughly
into daily activities reviewing the tape with the parents before showing it to the
An edited version of the parent’s goal 3 story
is returned to be used as the week’s story
group, any anxiety that the parents may have felt appeared
to be lessened. In addition to videotaping the parent and
11 Integrating techniques and incorporating them 4 child, the clinician demonstrated the techniques for the
into daily activities parents when necessary and dealt with any problems or
Weekly story is provided by the clinician for the concerns that arose. The individual appointments also
rest of the program
provided an opportunity to monitor the children’s progress
12 Integrating techniques and incorporating them 1 so that goals could be modified when appropriate.
into daily activities

Clinical Impressions and Suggestions for


was written so that a particular language structure was Modifications
frequently modeled and contrasted with other language The intervention programs were conducted as part of a
forms (i.e., it was constructed using focused stimulation research project involving group comparisons. As such, we
procedures). A second story also had an interesting story made a number of decisions to meet research requirements
line but did not contain a language focus. The third story and to standardize procedures across children. In clinical
had an obvious language focus, but there was really no application of the programs, some modifications should be
coherent story involved. The parents’ impressions of the made to meet individual children’s needs. In addition,
stories were used in the second session to begin a discus- although objective data were not always collected, we can
sion of focused stimulation. In the fifth session, after make a number of observations and suggestions for
modeling and various forms of recasts had been covered, modification or extension of the programs.

28 American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology • Vol. 6 • No. 1 February 1997


One such modification would be to combine the treatment In conducting the programs, we made a number of
packages and thus involve the parents in a parent program clinical observations that may be important in planning
while simultaneously providing clinic-based services (see similar programs in the future. Goals that targeted later
Fey et al., 1997). In order to examine the effects of each of developing, less frequent forms, such as relative clauses or
the programs individually, the parents in the clinician- embedded clauses headed by that, were more difficult,
directed program were not taught the focused stimulation particularly for the parents in the parent-directed program.
techniques. However, they were aware of their children’s This difficulty was due, in part, to the relative infrequency
specific goals and most observed both the individual and of such forms, especially in conversations. In addition,
group treatment sessions. For a few parents, these unguided appropriate contexts for later-developing structures often
observations were enough for them to learn the techniques were difficult for the parents to identify because the forms
and apply them, at least in the parent-child tapings done used serve multiple functions. For example, that can be
during the assessment periods. In general, however, it ap- used as a relative pronoun (e.g., I want the dog that
peared that learning to use focused stimulation techniques, barked) or as a much more common demonstrative
such as sentence recasts, required formal instruction as was pronoun (e.g., I want that) or determiner (e.g., I want that
offered in the parent program (Fey et al., 1997). dog). If such targets are addressed in parent programs such
As part of the experimental control, the children were as ours, additional parent training or alternative interven-
randomly assigned to the clinician-directed or parent- tion contexts, such as a greater reliance on stories and other
directed programs. In a clinical situation, how services are non-narrative textual forms, seems necessary.
delivered should be determined based on an individual The developing complexity of the children’s sentences
family’s needs, with the parents as active participants in had another negative impact on parental performance. In our
the decision-making. Both child and parent factors should study, only parents in the parent program used sentence
be considered in making the decision. Furthermore, in recasts frequently in their final spontaneous language sample
cases where a formal parent program is not appropriate, it with their child (i.e., greater than 38 recasts in a 30-minute
would be possible to teach parents the focused stimulation sample) (Fey et al., 1997). All of the parents in the parent
techniques individually, ideally in conjunction with their program whose children had DSSs of 5.0 or below (i.e.,
child’s intervention sessions. In this situation, the parents characteristic of many children 2-1/2 to 3-1/2 years old) were
would not benefit from the support offered by a parent in the group of frequent recasters. In contrast, some parents in
group, but the children would have the benefit of more the parent program whose children had higher DSSs used no
frequent models of their targets in non-intervention more recasts than the parents of children who received no
contexts, provided by individuals to whom they were parent training (i.e., less than 20 recasts in a 30-minute
highly emotionally attached. sample). Clearly, additional instruction and practice is
Another decision we made for experimental reasons was necessary to keep parents focused on using their facilitative
to continue the programs unmodified for a second 5-month techniques even when their children are producing lengthy
phase of treatment following the first 5-month period. and simple, but well-formed, sentences.
Based on what we know now, some modifications would For all goals, the parents found it easier to model and
have been appropriate and desirable for a number of the recast target forms than to elicit them. Based on their
children. For example, for children with significant reports and our observations, arranging verbal and nonver-
phonological problems, improved intelligibility should bal contexts in ways that made target forms necessary was
have been adopted as a basic goal and appropriate interme- particularly difficult for many parents. One way to improve
diate and specific goals should have been selected to this situation would be to rely more on commercially
facilitate greater intelligibility (Fey et al., 1994). Our available children’s literature. Parents could be instructed
experimental regimen also required us to maintain four in the use of focused stimulation techniques as they shared
specific goals throughout the study. Clearly, some children storybooks with their children. This type of activity would
(and parents) would have functioned better with more ensure opportunities for the use of intervention procedures,
goals and others would have benefitted from having fewer especially for forms that are infrequent in conversation
than four specific goals at a time. (Whitehurst et al., 1988; Scott, 1995). As noted above, this
A final example of modifications that could and probably also might have a positive impact on children’s early
should be made in clinical implementations involves the literacy development (Fey, Catts, & Larrivee, 1995).
syntax stories created for our studies. These stories were used In both programs, we used the syntax stories to highlight a
without pictures or other props to model intervention targets child’s specific target forms. One of the activities we used to
in a narrative context. In addition, parents were required teach the parents the intervention techniques was to have
merely to read the stories to their children. The same stories them write stories for their children for 4 weeks. A number of
could be used in many other ways to provide greater focus on parents reported that writing the stories for the first 4 weeks
preliteracy skills as well as grammatical targets. For example, was difficult but valuable in helping them learn to apply
after reading the stories, parents could have the children focused stimulation techniques. Obviously, this activity
illustrate the stories, using their own drawings. With greater would not be appropriate for some parents. Still, this
familiarity of the story’s content, a child could tell the story procedure might assist parents in better understanding their
in a picture-by-picture fashion or even retell the stories, children’s intervention programs and might also help them to
giving the parents more opportunities to use focused stimula- learn and apply the intervention procedures more reliably in
tion procedures. both conversational and narrative contexts.

Cleave • Fey 29
The focus of the research project and the prime diffi- Services, Ontario. This funding was administered by the Research
culty of all the children was the development of grammati- and Program Evaluation Unit in cooperation with the Ontario
cal skills. Thus, less attention was given to other language Mental Health Foundation and was funded from the MCSS
areas, although they were not ignored entirely. For ex- Research Grants Program. We also gratefully acknowledge
equipment contributions from the Ontario District Association of
ample, as already discussed, the stories exposed the
the Society for the Preservation of Barbershop Quartet Singing in
children to narrative structure and required the parents to America (the Barbershoppers).
read to their children. In addition, the group sessions in the
clinician-directed program provided occasions for peer References
interaction, which the clinician facilitated as necessary.
Thus, there were many opportunities to focus on social Connell, P. J. (1982). On training language rules. Language,
interactive skills. Still, numerous possibilities exist for Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 13, 231–248.
Connell, P. J. (1986). Teaching subjecthood to language-
making the programs more comprehensive and more
disordered children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research,
appropriate for more children. 29, 481–492.
Finally, for any intervention program, the cultural Connell, P. J. (1987). Teaching language rules as solutions to
background of the family must be considered and appropri- language problems: A baseball analogy. Language, Speech,
ate techniques developed. This is true for clinician- and and Hearing Services in Schools, 18, 194–205.
parent-directed programming (see van Kleek, 1994, for a Constable, C. M. (1983). Creating communicative context. In
discussion dealing with cultural bias and parent programs). H. Winitz (Ed.), Treating language disorders: For clinicians
The discourse and story styles we used would not be by clinicians (pp. 97–120). Baltimore, MD: University Park
appropriate for children from all cultures and thus other, Press.
culturally sensitive forms of discourse should be developed Crystal, D., Fletcher, P., & Garman, M. (1976). The grammati-
cal analysis of language disability. London: Edward Arnold.
when necessary.
Fey, M. E. (1986). Language intervention with young children.
San Diego, CA: College-Hill Press.
Conclusions Fey, M. E. (1988). Generalization issues facing language
interventionists: An introduction. Language, Speech, and
If we are to improve the services provided to individuals Hearing Services in Schools, 19, 272–281.
with language impairments, it is important that investiga- Fey, M. E., Catts, H. W., & Larrivee, L. S. (1995). Preparing
tions of intervention effectiveness be performed. Once preschoolers for the academic and social challenges of school.
such studies have been completed, clinicians have a vested In M. E. Fey, J. Windsor, & S. F. Warren (Eds.), Language
interest not only in the results but also in the details of the intervention: Preschool through the elementary years (pp. 3–
intervention programs evaluated. In this article, the two 38). Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes.
intervention programs, whose effectiveness was docu- Fey, M. E., & Cleave, P. L. (1990). Efficacy of intervention in
mented by Fey and his colleagues, and the rationale behind speech-language pathology: Early language intervention. In
them were described. J. Wheeland (Ed.), Seminars in Speech and Language, 11,
165–182.
It is important to remember that the intervention
Fey, M. E., Cleave, P. L., & Long, S. H. (1997). Two models of
programs described here have been evaluated as total grammar facilitation in children with language impairments:
packages. Both programs involved a number of compo- Phase 2. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research,
nents. There is a growing literature supporting the impor- 40, 5–19.
tance of sentence recasts that maintain the essential Fey, M. E., Cleave, P. L., Long, S. H., & Hughes, D. L. (1993).
meaning of a child’s utterance but correct or modify its Two approaches to the facilitation of grammar in language-
form in one or more ways (e.g., Fey et al., 1997; Nelson, impaired children: An experimental evaluation. Journal of
1989; Nelson et al., 1995; Watkins & Pemberton, 1987). Speech and Hearing Research, 36, 141–157.
Similarly, the contrastive imitation used in the clinician- Fey, M. E., Cleave, P. L., Ravida, A. I., Dejmal, A. R., Easton,
based approach has been successful in earlier applications D., & Long, S. H. (1994). Effects of grammar facilitation on
the phonological performance of children with speech and
(Connell, 1986, 1987). The importance of any other
language impairments. Journal of Speech and Hearing
components and the effects of interactions among compo- Research, 37, 594–607.
nents on the success of intervention are less clear. The Hodson, B. W., & Paden, E. P. (1991). Targeting intelligible
contribution of these components (e.g., the imitation speech: A phonological approach to remediation (2nd ed.).
protocol, the number or breadth of goals, the cyclical goal Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.
attack strategy) should be the focus of future research. It is Johnston, J. R. (1985). Fit, focus and functionality: An essay on
important that speech-language pathologists continue to early language intervention. Child Language Teaching and
develop more effective ways of working with children Therapy, 1, 125–134.
with language impairments. To do this, well designed and Johnston, J. R. (1988). Generalization: The nature of change.
carefully implemented treatment efficacy research is Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 19, 314–
329.
needed. It is hoped that this report helps in that endeavor.
Lee, L. (1974). Developmental sentence analysis. Evanston, IL:
Northwestern University Press.
Author Notes Leonard, L. B. (1981). Facilitating linguistic skills in children
This research was conducted in London, Ontario, Canada, with specific language impairment. Applied Psycholinguistics,
while the authors were at the University of Western Ontario. It 2, 89–118.
was sponsored by the Ministry of Community and Social Lucas, E. (1980). Semantic and pragmatic language disorders:

30 American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology • Vol. 6 • No. 1 February 1997


Assessment and remediation. Rockville, MD: Aspen Systems phonological and language impairment. Child Language
Corporation. Teaching and Therapy, 7, 141–160.
Nelson, K. E. (1989). Strategies for first language teaching. In M. van Kleek, A. (1994). Potential cultural bias in training parents
L. Rice & R. L. Schiefelbusch (Eds.), The teachability of as conversational partners with their children who have delays
language (pp. 263–310). Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes. in language development. American Journal of Speech-
Nelson, K. E., Welsh, J., Camarata, S. M., Butkovsky, L., & Language Pathology, 3(1), 67–78.
Camarata, M. (1995). Available input for language-impaired Watkins, R. V., & Pemberton, E. F. (1987). Clinical applica-
children and younger children of matched language levels. tions of recasting: Review and theory. Child Language
First Language, 15, 1–17. Teaching and Therapy, 3, 311–325.
Schwartz, R., Chapman, K., Terrell, B., Prelock, P., & Whitehurst, G. J., Falco, F. L., Lonigan, C. J., Fischel, J. E.,
Rowan, L. (1985). Facilitating word combination in language- DeBarsyshe, B. D., Valdez-Menchaca, M. C., & Caulfield,
impaired children through discourse structure. Journal of M. (1988). Accelerating language development through book
Speech and Hearing Disorders, 50, 31–39. reading. Developmental Psychology, 24, 552–559.
Scott, C. M. (1995). Syntax for school-age children: A discourse
perspective. In M. E. Fey, J. Windsor, & S. F. Warren (Eds.), Received April 11, 1996
Language intervention: Preschool through the elementary Accepted September 25, 1996
years (pp. 3–38). Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes.
Tyler, A. A., Edwards, M. L., & Saxman, J. H. (1987). Clinical Contact author: Patricia L. Cleave, PhD, School of Human
application of two phonologically based treatment procedures. Communication Disorders, Dalhousie University, 5599
Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 52, 393–409. Fenwick Street, Halifax, NS, Canada, B3H 1R2
Tyler, A. A., & Sandoval, K. T. (1994). Preschoolers with Email: pcleave@is.dal.ca
phonological and language disorders: Treating different
linguistic domains. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services
in Schools, 25, 215–134.
Tyler, A. A., & Watterson, K. H. (1991). Effects of phonologi- Key words: intervention, grammar, preschool, language
cal versus language intervention in preschoolers with both impairment

Appendix
Dad’s Bad Joke

Target: are Well, just a little, said Warren.


We are very brave. model
Neil and Warren liked to play in the attic. We really are. breakdown
It was fun up there but it was a little scary too.
They always turned on the light so they Well, what are we going to do? asked
could see. Neil. model

One day Neil and Warren started to go I am going downstairs, said Warren. contrast auxiliary
upstairs. Are you coming with me? model/recast
Are you? breakdown
Where are you going? asked Dad. model
Are you going to the attic? model Why are you going downstairs?
asked Neil. model
Yes, we are, shouted Warren. recast/breakdown Are you going to hide? model
Neil and I are going up now. buildup Are you going for help? model
We are going to play up there. model
I am going to look for a flashlight,
Oh you are, are you, thought Dad. breakdown/recast said Warren. contrast auxiliary

Dad had a big grin on his face. Are you really? Neil asked. model
Warren and Neil started to play.
They were having lots of fun. Yes I am, answered Warren. contrast auxiliary
Then, something happened. Are you coming with me? model
The light went out. Or are you going to wait here. model

Oh no, said Neil. Oh no! said Neil.


We’re in the dark. model We are going together. model
You are going downstairs. model
We sure are, said Warren. breakdown So I am going downstairs. contrast auxiliary
Are you afraid? recast
Are you afraid of the dark? model So Neil and Warren started to go downstairs.
It was very dark.
No I’m not, Neil answered. contrast auxiliary At the bottom of the stairs, Warren bumped
I am not afraid. contrast auxiliary into someone.
Are you? model/recast (continued)

Cleave • Fey 31
Ouch! yelled a voice. I am going to turn on a light, answered
the voice. contrast auxiliary
Are you all right, Warren asked. model
Are you okay? model You are? asked both Neil and Warren. model

Watch where you’re going, said the Yes, I am, said the voice. contrast auxiliary
voice angrily. model
What are you doing? model And suddenly, the lights were on.

We are looking for a flashlight, Neil Dad!! shouted Neil and Warren.
responded. model What are you doing? model

You are looking for a flashlight?! said Well, said Dad, I thought I would play
the voice. model a trick on you.
Are you both looking for a flashlight? recast When you weren’t looking, I turned off
the light in the attic.
Yes we are, said Neil. breakdown It was just a joke.
We are in the dark so we’re staying
together. buildup Warren and Neil were really mad.

But who are you? asked Warren. model We are very angry Dad, said Warren. model
And what are you doing here. model Yes, we are, agreed Neil. breakdown
Don’t you ever play such a dirty trick
The voice walked away. again.

Where are you going? Neil screamed. model And you know what?
He never did.

32 American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology • Vol. 6 • No. 1 February 1997

You might also like