You are on page 1of 17

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/5359972

Adolescent coping scales: A critical psychometric review

Article  in  Scandinavian Journal of Psychology · June 2008


DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-9450.2008.00669.x · Source: PubMed

CITATIONS READS

33 8,220

2 authors:

Sigrun Sveinbjornsdottir Einar B Thorsteinsson


University of Akureyri University of New England (Australia)
26 PUBLICATIONS   589 CITATIONS    155 PUBLICATIONS   4,159 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Mental health literacy View project

Depression and well-being View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Einar B Thorsteinsson on 07 October 2017.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 2008, 49, 533–548 DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-9450.2008.00669.x

Personality and Social Sciences


Blackwell Publishing Ltd

Adolescent coping scales: A critical psychometric review


SIGRUN SVEINBJORNSDOTTIR1 and EINAR BALDVIN THORSTEINSSON2
1
University of Akureyri, Iceland
2
University of New England, Australia

Sveinbjornsdottir, S. & Thorsteinsson, E. B. (2008). Adolescent coping scales: A critical psychometric review. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology,
49, 533–548.
Individual coping is identified as an important factor in relation to health and well-being. Although several coping scales have been developed, key
terms of coping such as nature and a number of primary and secondary factors (dimensions) are obscure. Coping scales, such as those that have been
developed through exploratory factor analysis (EFA), have been criticized for poor psychometric properties, yet the critique so far does not evaluate
development of the scales against best test-theoretical practice. The present study reviews six adolescent coping scales against ten detailed psychometric
criteria in relation to statistical choices throughout the process of scale development. All six scales measured poorly on several criteria. Best practice
had not been followed throughout their development and they suffered serious psychometric limitations. These findings indicate that there still is
empirical research to be pursued in search of latent constructs and possible dimensions of coping through the implementation of EFA.
Key words: Adolescence, review, coping scale, validity, reliability, best test-theoretical practice.
Dr Einar B. Thorsteinsson, Psychology School of Behavioural, Cognitive and Social Sciences, University of New England, NSW 2351, Australia.
Tel: + 61 2 6773 2587; fax: + 61 2 6773 3820; e-mail: ethorste@une.edu.au

Diverse psychosocial disorders (e.g., antisocial behavior, drug cluster into primary coping factors; coping strategies may be
use, depression, eating disorders) in the Western world occur of either a cognitive or behavioral nature; individuals either
frequently in adolescence (Rutter, 1995; Smith & Rutter, 1995). approach a problem or avoid it; and there is an interaction
The growing concern for adolescents who experience adjustment between coping strategies and situation characteristics. However,
problems emphasizes the need for identification of protective confusion still exists around other key issues within the field,
and risk factors, and development of evidence-based preventions. and consensus on how to define and hence measure coping
Different types of individual coping strategies are identified as has not as yet been reached. For instance, Steger (2006) noted
either protective or risk factors in relation to health and well-being a different factor structure for each of the numerous post hoc
(Lazarus, 1993; Masten, Best & Garmezy, 1990; Seiffge-Krenke, factor analytical studies conducted between 1987 and 2000 on
1998). For instance, in adolescence, individual coping strategies the Folkman and Lazarus scale of adult coping. Adolescent
are found to interact with well-being and adjustment (Jorgensen coping scales are also varied. For instance, while one researcher
& Dusek, 1990; Tolor & Fehon, 1987), success at school reports 80 strategies clustering into 18 primary factors
(Kristensson & Öhlund, 2005; Rijavec & Brdar, 1997; Whatley, (Frydenberg & Lewis, 1993), another suggests 20 strategies
Foreman & Richards, 1998), and physical and mental health clustering into three factors (Seiffge-Krenke, 1989). Further-
(Graue, Hanestad, Wentzel-Larsen, Sövik & Bru, 2004; Reid, more, conceptualization of second-order factors of coping is
Dubow & Carey, 1995; Reid, Dubow, Carey & Dura, 1994; inconsistent in the literature. Lazarus and Folkman (1984)
Seiffge-Krenke & Klessinger, 2000; Smith, Smoll & Ptacek, suggested that coping (adult) is either problem-focused or
1990; Steiner, Erickson, Hernandez & Pavelski, 2002; Zeidner emotion-focused, the former aiming at changing the situation or
& Hammer, 1990). managing the problem, and the latter at controlling the emotions
Lazarus’s cognitive transactional theory of coping (Lazarus related to the problem. Although Lazarus and Folkman did not
& Folkman, 1984) has been a major inspiration for coping present that dichotomy as statistically derived second-order
research during the last two decades. Based on that theory, a factors, some researchers on adult coping have done so, and
number of coping measures for adults (e.g., Billings & Moos, have suggested second-order factors relating either to the focus
1984; Carver, Scheier & Weintraub, 1989; Endler & Parker, (e.g., Billings & Moos, 1981; Ebata & Moos, 1991, 1994; Pearlin
1990; Folkman & Lazarus, 1985, 1988; McCrae, 1984; McCrae & Schooler, 1978) or to the outcome of coping (i.e., positive or
& Costa, 1986; Pearlin & Schooler, 1978; Stone & Neale, 1984) negative) (e.g., McCrae & Costa, 1986; Parkes, 1986). However,
and adolescents (e.g., Brodzinsky, Elias, Steiger, Simon & distinction between problem- and emotion-focused second-
Gill, 1992; Dise-Lewis, 1988; Ebata & Moos, 1991; Fanshawe order factors has not yet been statistically verified for adult or
& Burnett, 1991; Frydenberg & Lewis, 1993; Seiffge-Krenke, adolescent coping (Ficková, 1998; Jorgensen & Dusek, 1990;
1989) have been developed. Researchers generally agree on the Tobin, Holroyd, Reynolds & Wigal, 1989; Watson, Willson &
following issues related to coping: coping strategies (items) Sinha, 1998).

© 2008 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2008 The Scandinavian Psychological Associations. Published by Blackwell Publishing Ltd., 9600
Garsington Road, Oxford OX4 2DQ, UK and 350 Main Street, Malden, MA 02148, USA. ISSN 0036-5564.
534 S. Sveinbjornsdottir and E. B. Thorsteinsson Scand J Psychol 49 (2008)

Researchers who use exploratory factor analysis (EFA) in Size and representativeness of the construction sample is
psychological research are frequently found to make poor vital for construct validity, and gender, age, socioeconomic
statistical decisions throughout the process of EFA (Conway & background, and ethnicity must be known. The ratio of participants
Huffcutt, 2003; Fabrigar, Wegener, MacCallum & Strahn, 1999; per test item affects the standard error, yet there is no clear rule
Ford, MacCallum & Tait, 1986), leading to spurious outcomes. to follow. Ten participants to each item has been considered safe
Furthermore, psychological measures have been found to be (Child, 1990), and actually recommended (Nunnally, 1978). Yet
rationally derived rather than developed through systematic some consider a ratio of five to one sufficient (Gorsuch, 1983;
quantitative content validation (Haynes, Richard & Kubany, Tabachnick & Fidell, 1989) or even three to one for each gender
1995). Concerns about the methodological soundness of coping (Kline, 1993). A recent study on best practice using EFA
scales have been raised (Benson & Hagtvet, 1996; Compas, reported an error rate of 30% at a ratio of 20:1 on a sample size
Connor-Smith, Saltzman, Thomsen & Wadsworth, 1999), and of 260 (Costello & Osborne, 2005), indicating an even greater
coping scales of children, adolescents, and adults have been importance of having a high ratio of participants per item than
reviewed (e.g., Compas, Connor-Smith, Saltzman, Thomsen & previously believed. The criteria for the present review of scale
Wadsworth, 2001; Schwarzer & Schwarzer, 1996). Despite the construction are a minimum of 10 participants per item (five or
fact that most coping scales are developed according to classical more per gender) and a minimum of 500 participants.
test-theory and through EFA, review of the development of Choices of EFA procedures concern (a) model of analysis,
coping scales in relation to test-theoretical criteria and the (b) factor rotation, and (c) factor extraction method. Model of
implementation of EFA has, to our knowledge, not yet been analysis is most commonly a choice between common Factor
performed. In a recent exhaustive review of coping assessments Analysis (FA) and Principal Components Analysis (PCA), the
and the search for the structure of coping, it is suggested that default extraction method in SPSS. PCA has proven to obscure
confirmatory rather than exploratory factor analyses be used outcomes because of inflation of factor loadings, especially
for construction of primary coping factors, and confirmatory those of lower or moderate value, where FA has not (Fabrigar
hierarchical analyses be used for determination of higher order et al., 1999; Gorsuch, 1983; Widaman, 1993). Furthermore,
factors or dimensions of coping (Skinner, Edge, Altman & there is no prior knowledge of unique variance within the
Sherwood, 2003). However, we argue an investigation of the correlation matrix when a test is being developed, and to
foundations of the scales is needed, requiring an analysis of the uncover latent factors that are as uncontaminated by that kind
quality of the EFA procedures throughout scale development. of variability as possible, the choice of model of analysis should
Our hypothesis is that poor quality EFA procedures, as earlier be FA rather than PCA (Conway & Huffcutt, 2003). The criterion
detected in psychological research (Conway & Huffcutt, 2003; for the present review is FA.
Fabrigar, Wegener, MacCallum & Strahn, 1999; Ford, MacCallum Of a number of analytic rotation methods, the most essential
& Tait, 1986), might similarly be found in the development of distinction is between orthogonal and oblique rotation. Oblique
coping scales. The aim of the present review is to critically rotation allows correlations between factors, and since charac-
examine statistical choices throughout the development of teristics of human behavior are believed to be interrelated
adolescent coping scales as suggested by classical best test- (Child, 1990), the oblique factor rotation has been suggested as
theoretical practice. A brief presentation of psychometric issues a first choice. The combination of oblique rotations and FA
and criteria used for the present review is outlined below. See yields the simplest structure when compared with: oblique
the summary in Table 1. rotation and PCA; orthogonal rotation and PCA; and orthogonal
rotation and FA (Fabrigar et al., 1999). This lends further support
for the choice of FA and oblique rotation.
Best-practice as suggested by classical test-theory for Factor extraction is a challenge: extracting too many factors
development of a psychological test (overfactoring) or too few factors (underfactoring) leads to
Item selection is the base of content validity. If a literature error. Two commonly cited extraction methods, often used in
search provides limited knowledge, information from informed combination, are the latent root criterion (eigenvalue greater
individuals is needed (Haynes et al., 1995; Kline, 1986). Test than one or Kaiser’s criterion) and Cattell’s scree test (Child,
items must be clear and easily understood, and must be kept 1990; Tabachnick & Fidell, 1989). Kaiser’s criterion is generally
short, containing only one point or statement (DeVellis, 1991). not found to be a satisfactory method (Conway & Huffcutt,
For selection of the most salient items, up to twice as many 2003; Fabrigar et al., 1999; Velicer & Jackson, 1990). Parallel
items as will be included in the final version are needed in the Analysis (PA) is considered more accurate (Fabrigar et al., 1999;
initial stage as suggested by Kline (1986). To support replicability Hayton, Allen & Scarpello, 2004), especially when used in
of a test, different numbers of items per factor have been combination with a scree test. However, PA is not currently
suggested. MacCallum, Widaman, Zhang, and Hong (1999) available in commonly used statistical programs like SPSS,
have suggested a minimum of three to five items per factor, although it is possible to conduct it stepwise (Hayton et al.,
Fabrigar et al. (1999) have suggested four, while Gorsuch (1983) 2004). Therefore, a scree test has been suggested by some as
has suggested six items per factor. Four items per factor is the the best choice for the time being (Costello & Osborne, 2005),
criterion for the present review. and is the criterion for the present review. For future development

© 2008 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2008 The Scandinavian Psychological Associations.
Table 1. Assessment summary of adolescent coping scales

© 2008 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2008 The Scandinavian Psychological Associations.

Scand J Psychol 49 (2008)


Coping Across Coping Scale Adolescent
Adolescent Life Events and Situations for Children Coping Scale Coping Responses
Coping Orientation Coping Inventory Questionnaire and Youth (ACS) Long Inventory, Youth Form
Criteria (A-COPE) (LECI) (CASQ) (CSC-Y) Form (CRI-Y)a

Criterion 1 Items
1.1 Item selection for the scale representative 0 0 – – √ –
(derived from literature and appropriate “focus” groups)
1.2 Each item one statement √ √ √ √ √ √
1.3 Proportion of items in original pool/final test, √ – √ √ √ NA
if selected through EFA (less than 75%)
1.4 Ratio items to factor (4:1 or higher) 0 √ √ √ 0 √
Criterion 2 Sample
2.1 Size (500+) 0 √ √ √ √ 0
2.2 Representativeness (i.e., age, gender, 0 0 √ 0 √ 0
socio-economic background, ethnicity
2.3 Ratio participants to item (5+ per gender to item) 0 √b √ √ 0 0
Criterion 3 EFA procedure
3.1 Factor model (use FA unless small 0 0 0 0 0 NP
error variance then either FA or PCA)
3.2 Factor rotation (Oblique) 0 √ 0 0 √ NA
3.3 Factor extraction method (scree test, PA or both) 0 – √ √ – NA
Criterion 4 factors
4.1 Cut-off limit for item loadings (0.40 for PCA 0 0 √ √ – NA
and 0.30 for FA if N < 1,000; 0.25 to 0.30 if N > 1,000
and participants to item >10:1)
4.2 Purity of primary factors (Each item to – – √ √ – NA
load above criterion on one factor only)
4.3 Internal consistency (0.70+) 0 0 √ √ 0 0
4.4 Mean inter-item correlation (0.20 to 0.40) NP NP NPc NP NP NP
Criterion 5 Test-retest reliability

Review of adolescent coping scales


5.1 Sample size (100+) – 0 – √ √ √
5.2 Sample representativeness – 0 – 0 – –
5.3 Time interval (4 weeks+) NP √ NP 0 0 √
5.4 Correlation (0.70 if interval is ≤4 weeks, more than 4 NP – NP √ 0d –
weeks and less than 10 week 0.60, 10 weeks or more 0.50)
Criterion 6 Evidence of concurrent validity
6.1 Correlation with test that measures same concept 0 NP NP √ NP NP
(r > 0.70) poor tests (r = 0.30 to 0.50)
6.2 Sample size (100+) 0 – – √ – –
6.3 Sample representativeness – – – – – –
Criterion 7 Evidence of construct validity
7.1 Correlation with unrelated test (≤0.30) NP √ NP √ NP √
7.2 Sample size (100+) – √ – √ – √
7.3 Sample representativeness – – – 0 – –

535
© 2008 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2008 The Scandinavian Psychological Associations.

536 S. Sveinbjornsdottir and E. B. Thorsteinsson


Table 1. Continued

Coping Across Coping Scale Adolescent


Adolescent Life Events and Situations for Children Coping Scale Coping Responses
Coping Orientation Coping Inventory Questionnaire and Youth (ACS) Long Inventory, Youth Form
Criteria (A-COPE) (LECI) (CASQ) (CSC-Y) Form (CRI-Y)a

Criterion 8 Cross-validation
8.1 Sample size (500+) √ – √ – √ 0
8.2 Sample representativeness – – – – 0 –
8.3 Ratio participants to item (5+ for gender) √ – √ – √ 0
8.4 At least 90% of items are replicated 0 NP 0 NP 0 0
Criterion 9 Second-order factors in same/construction study
9.1 Factor model (FA used; PCA if error variance low) NP NP NP NP 0 NP
9.2 Factor rotation (Oblique) NP NP NP NP √ NP
9.3 Factor extraction method (scree plot, PA or both) NP NP NP NP √ NP
9.4 Cut-off limit for primary factor loading on secondary NP NP NP NP √ NP
order factors (0.40 for PCA and 0.30 for FA if N < 1,000;
0.25 to 0.30 if N > 1,000 and participants to item >10:1)
9.5 Purity of second-order factors (Primary factor should NP NP NP NP 0 NP
only load above criterion on one second-order factor)
Criterion 10 Test practicality
10.1 Test length (50 items or less) 0 √ √ √ 0 √
10.2 Reading level appropriate √ √ √ √ √ √

Note: ACS-Long Form covers both Specific and General Form. NP = not performed; √ = Criterion met; 0 = Criterion not met; – = Information not sufficient or information totally missing;
NA = Not applicable.
a
Factors constructed based on theory.
b
The exact number of males and females is not known; assumed to be equal.
c
Average item total correlations were reported, which were 0.57 for Factor 1, 0.59 for Factor 2, and 0.53 for Factor 3.
d
Correlation coefficients for the factors, General Form, were below 0.70 for 8 factors and above 0.70 for 10 factors. For the Specific Form, 10 factors had correlation coefficient below 0.70,
and 8 factors had correlation coefficients above 0.70.

Scand J Psychol 49 (2008)


Scand J Psychol 49 (2008) Review of adolescent coping scales 537

of tests, PA combined with a scree test seems likeliest to be METHOD


recommended as best practice.
Item loadings indicate factors, but test developers need to be Literature searches
cautious. First, different models of analysis may produce loading An electronic search was performed for self-rating adolescent coping
results that differ for the same sample and variables. PCA may scales for general situations in the databases PsycINFO, Eric, PubMed,
produce higher loadings than FA (Gorsuch, 1983), and the and Google Scholar. A large number of key articles on adolescent coping,
inflation is stronger for lower and moderate loadings, 0.40 or limited to years 1980 through 2007 were also cross-referenced. Key-
less, than for loadings of 0.80 or higher (Fabrigar et al., 1999; words for the search included various combinations of the following
concepts: coping scale, adolescence, reliability, validity, psychometric
Sveinbjornsdottir, 2001; Widaman, 1993). Second, factor loadings properties, test construction, scale construction. Six scales were found
are partly due to sample size, and correlations for larger samples that fitted the following inclusion criteria.
are usually lower than for smaller samples (Tabachnick &
Fidell, 1989). Therefore, a salient loading for one study may
be inappropriate for another. For the present review, if FA is Inclusion and exclusion criteria
employed and the number of participants is at least 500, the Included in the present review are coping scales, where coping strategies
loading criterion is 0.30 and as low as 0.25 if the number of are clustered in factors, and specifically developed for adolescent popu-
participants exceed 1,000. If PCA is employed, the criterion for lations between 12 and 18 years of age. Therefore, a coping checklist
minimum loading is 0.40 for 500 participants or more. such as Kidcope, a list of ten items (Spirito, Stark & Williams, 1988),
is excluded. Cognitive skills affect thinking and hence coping, and
An item is expected to load saliently on one factor only, and because of adolescents’ growing meta-cognitive skills, their coping
a minimum internal consistency for a factor is 0.70 (Kline, strategies may include cognitive coping strategies, whereas children’s
1993; Nunnally, 1978). However, since high internal consistency coping tends to be more concrete and related to observable behavior
may carry a risk of items being only slight variations of each (for review see Boekaerts, 1996). Therefore, coping scales developed
other (Child, 1990; Kline, 1993, 1994; Tabachnick & Fidell, 1989), for younger populations such as the Self-Reported Coping Measure
(Causey & Dubow, 1992), and then tested on adolescents without further
inter-item correlation for a factor is optimal when ≥ 0.20 and development or refinement (Ayers, Sandler & Twohey, 1998), were
≤0.40 (Briggs & Cheek, 1986). excluded. The same applied for coping scales developed for adults.
Minimum criteria for test-retest analysis are a correlation of Although adolescents and adults have similar cognitive capacities,
0.70 (Kline, 1986) or even 0.80 (Kline, 1993), 100 participants, adults have more experience and their social and economic circum-
and a three-month interval. However, over a three-month period stances are different to those of adolescents. Thus adults belong to a
different population. Scales developed for adults that have subsequently
a significant change in cognition may occur, affecting the been tested for use with adolescents with minimal modification were
adolescents’ coping behavior. Therefore, with a minimum excluded. Examples of such scales include COPE (Carver et al., 1989)
sample of 100 the correlation criterion for the present review is and the Ways of Coping Checklist (WCC; Folkman & Lazarus, 1980).
0.70 for a four-week interval, 0.60 for a four- to ten-week interval, The present review focuses on coping with general concerns or daily
and 0.50 for an interval longer than ten weeks. hassles. Therefore, measures of situation-specific coping, such as the
School Failure Coping Scale (SFCS; Rijavec & Brdar, 1997) and the
For further evidence of validity, administered simultaneously Responses to Stress Questionnaire (Connor-Smith, Compas, Wadsworth,
to the same minimum sample of 100, the correlation between Thomsen & Saltzman, 2000) were excluded. Scenario-based instru-
an examined scale and another one measuring the same variable ments such as the Cross-Cultural Coping Scale (Kuo, Roysircar &
(concurrent), is expected to be at least 0.70 unless one or both Newby-Clark, 2006), and population-specific coping scales, such as
scales are of poor quality then only a moderate correlation of the Coping Function Questionnaire for adolescent sport participants
(Kowalsky & Crocker, 2001), were likewise excluded.
0.50 is expected (Kline, 1986). Low correlation is expected Six adolescent coping scales were identified for inclusion, four
(<0.30) between an examined scale and another conceptually developed in the USA, one in Germany, and one in Australia: (1)
different (construct) scale. Replication, either through EFA Adolescent Coping Orientation, USA (A-COPE; Patterson & McCubbin,
cross-validation or confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) on a 1987), (2) Life Events and Coping Inventory, USA (LECI; Dise-Lewis,
representative independent sample of at least 500, and parti- 1988), (3) Coping Across Situations Questionnaire, Germany (CASQ;
Seiffge-Krenke, 1989), (4) Coping Scale for Children and Youth, USA
cipants five or more per gender, is proven if 90% of items and (CSC-Y; Brodzinsky et al., 1992), (5) Adolescent Coping Scale, Australia
the factor structure hold as suggested by Kline (1986). (ACS; Frydenberg & Lewis, 1993), and (6), Coping Responses Inventory,
Second-order (or any higher-order) factor analysis is safely con- Youth Form, USA (CRI-Y; Moos, 1993). An overview of the scales is
ducted if earlier factor analysis used oblique rotation. The criteria provided in Table 2.
for the present review are FA, oblique rotation, and scree test (or PA).
Scale practicality is of value for a scale-taker as well as the
administrator. Issues of importance are the scale taker’s reading RESULTS
level, and the level of complexity and length of the scale. No
general recommendations are found in the literature and criterion (1) Adolescent Coping Orientation for Problem Experiences
for the present review is arbitrary. For the possibility of extracting (A-COPE; Patterson & McCubbin, 1987)
factors, a minimum of 20 items is suggested, and so as not to The A-COPE is based on the Double ABCX model (McCubbin
strain the attention of scale-takers, a maximum of 50 items is & Patterson, 1981, 1982, 1983), an extension of Hill’s theory
suggested. on family response to stress (Burr, 1982; McCubbin & Patterson,

© 2008 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2008 The Scandinavian Psychological Associations.
© 2008 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2008 The Scandinavian Psychological Associations.

538 S. Sveinbjornsdottir and E. B. Thorsteinsson


Table 2. Overview of reviewed adolescent coping scales

Items Factor
Points
Age Sample Initial Final Items on a Loadings Internal
range Mean size males/ Model of number number per Likert allowed below consistency
Scale in years age femalesa EFA/rotation of items of items Factors factor scale .40 (loading) for factors Test-retest

Adolescent Coping Orientation, USAb 11 to 18b 15.6 185/241 PCA/orthogonal 95 54 12 2 to 8 5 4 (0.08 to 0.50 to 0.76 NP
(A COPE; Patterson & McCubbin, 1987) 0.37)
Life Events and Coping Inventory, USA 11 to 14b 12.6b 502 PCA/oblique 90 49 5 7 to 13 9 6 (0.30 to 0.62 to 0.86 NP
(LECI; Dise-Lewis, 1988) 0.38)
Coping Across Situations Questionnaire, 12 to 19 16 479/549 PCA/orthogonal 20 20 3 6 to 7 NAc 0 0.76 to 0.80 NP
Germany (CASQ; Seiffge-Krenke, 1989)
Coping Scale for Children and Youth USAb 10 to 15 13.0b 248/250 PCA/orthogonal 44 29 4 4 to 11 4 0 0.70 to 0.81 0.73 to 0.81
(CSC-Y; Brodzinsky et al., 1992)
Adolescent Coping Scale Long form, 12 to 18 13.9b 245/255 PCA/oblique 156 80 18 3 to 5 5 –d 0.62 to 0.83 0.49 to 0.82e,f
Specific and general concerns, Australia 0.54 to 0.85 0.44 to 0.84e,g
(ACS; Frydenberg & Lewis, 1993)
Coping Responses Inventory, Youth Form, – 15.3 147/168 NAh 72 48 8 6 4 NAh 0.55 to 0.79 0.29 to 0.34i
USA (CRI-Y; Moos, 1993)

Note: NP = not performed; – = information not reported; NA = not applicable.


a
Only total number given if no information about number of males and females.
b
Estimated from information given.
c
Yes or No format.
d
Impossible to estimate since items were factor-analyzed in three separate clusters.
e
General and specific form, for a 14-day test-retest interval eight of 18 factors rated below 0.70 (see Table 7 for further details).
f
For the specific form.
g
For the general form.
h
Factor construction based on theory.
i
Average for males (0.29) and females (0.34) for a 12 to 15 month interval.

Scand J Psychol 49 (2008)


Scand J Psychol 49 (2008) Review of adolescent coping scales 539

Table 3. A-COPE factor structure and internal consistency

Factors Items Example Internal consistency

Ventilating feelings 6 Get angry and yell at people 0.75


Seeking diversions 8 Sleep 0.75
Developing self-reliance and optimism 6 Try on your own to figure out how to deal 0.69
with your problems or tensions
Developing social support 6 Try to help other people solve their problems 0.75
Solving family problems 6 Talk to your father about what bothers you 0.75
Avoiding problems 5 Use drugs (not prescribed by a doctor) 0.71
Seeking spiritual support 3 Go to church 0.72
Investing in close friends 2 Be close to someone you care about 0.76
Seeking professional support salutary effect 2 Get professional counseling 0.50
(not a school teacher or a school counselor)
Engaging in demanding activity 4 Do a strenuous physical activity (jogging, biking etc.) 0.67
Being humorous 2 Joke and keep a sense of humor 0.72
Relaxing 4 Daydream about how you would like things to be 0.60

1983). The Double ABCX model refers to variables believed coping (Patterson & McCubbin, 1987). The number of partic-
to be of importance for the well-being of the family as a unit, ipants and demographic information is confusing (see pp. 171
and for each individual family member (McCubbin & Patterson, and 177), and 3 of the 12 A-COPE factors were excluded.
1983, 1986; Patterson & McCubbin, 1987). Therefore, the outcome for the full-length scale is blurred. In
Items were generated through structured interviews with 30 another study (Spirito et al., 1988), 49 students (demographic
students of both sexes from a suburban mid-western high school, information unclear) completed A-COPE and Kidcope on the
grades 10 through 12. A total of 95 coping items were identified same occasion. Correlations between A-COPE factors and
and factor analyzed, using PCA with orthogonal rotation, on Kidcope items were mixed and somewhat difficult to interpret;
467 junior and senior high school students, 185 males and 241 they ranged from 0.08 to 0.62, where most of the higher
females (mean age of 15.6 years; gender information missing correlations were between conceptually similar Kidcope items
for some participants). Participants were from suburban mid-western and A-COPE factors.
schools, mainly of middle to upper-middle socioeconomic status Four independent studies failed to replicate the A-COPE factor
(no further information). Responses on a five-point Likert scale structure. Three performed EFA (Fanshawe & Burnett, 1991;
with minimal variance were removed from the scale, leaving 68 Halvarsson, Lunner & Sjödin, 2001; Plancherel & Bolognini,
of 95 items for factor analysis. Twelve factors with eigenvalues 1995), and one CFA (ACSS; Burnett & Fanshawe, 1996).
of 1.0 or above were extracted, including 54 items, see Table 3. Through a post hoc approach of the 54 A-COPE items, the
Factorial purity is unclear; information was provided on how researchers created scales on their own data.
each item loaded on one factor only.
A second-order maximum likelihood (ML) analysis with Limitations of the A-COPE. First, information was missing
oblique rotation was conducted on an independent sample of on: (a) demographics for the construction sample, and (b) the
331 undergraduate volunteers (age 18; 192 females and 139 loadings of each item on all factors, informing on the degree
males, no further information) (Jorgensen & Dusek, 1990). Two of factorial purity. Secondly, questionable methods were
second-order factors with eigenvalues of 1.0 or greater emerged. employed, such as: (a) initial selection of coping items, (b)
Nine factors clustered together in a second-order factor named PCA for factor analysis, (c) orthogonal rotation of factors,
Salutary Effort (i.e., Engaging in Demanding Activity, Developing and (d) factor extraction based on eigenvalues. In addition,
Self-Reliance and Optimism, Developing Social Support, given the length of the scale, a second-order factor analysis
Seeking Diversions, Solving Family Problems, Seeking Spiritual should have been performed on the construction data. Thirdly,
Support, Investing in Close Friendships, Being Humorous, some reported statistical properties of the scale were unsatis-
and Seeking Professional Support effort). The remaining three factory, such as: (a) four factors were comprised of three
factors reflected angry reactions and avoidance, and the second- items or fewer, (b) three factors had internal reliability below
order factor was named Stress Palliation (Relaxing, Ventilating 0.70, (c) four items loaded below 0.40 on their factor, and (d)
Feelings, and Avoiding Problems). All factors loaded beyond one confirmatory factor analysis and three cross-validation
0.30 on their second-order factor less one (Seeking Professional studies on independent samples failed to replicate the factor
help, loading 0.22). structure of A-COPE. Finally, some important analyses were
not performed, such as: (a) inter-item correlations and (b)
Further information on validity. The A-COPE was examined test-retest reliability, and (c) correlation with an unrelated
through relationships between adolescent substance use and test.

© 2008 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2008 The Scandinavian Psychological Associations.
540 S. Sveinbjornsdottir and E. B. Thorsteinsson Scand J Psychol 49 (2008)

Table 4. LECI-Coping factor structure and internal consistency

Internal consistency

Construction Replication
Factors Items Example sample sample

Aggression 7 Hit someone or hurt someone physically 0.86 0.85


Stress-recognition 13 Write to someone else about it 0.79 0.80
Distraction 12 Do a hobby or something I enjoy 0.76 0.83
Self-destruction 8 Smoke cigarettes 0.86 0.85
Endurance 9 Just hold it in 0.62 0.68

(2) Life Events and Coping Inventory (LECI; Dise-Lewis, 1988) Limitations of the LECI. First, information was missing on (a)
The LECI assesses stressful life events and coping based on the demographics for the construction sample, (b) the ratio of
transactional model of stress and coping (Lazarus & Folkman, females and males for the total sample of 681 adolescents, (c)
1984) and Holmes and Rahe’s (1967) “social consensus method what statistical methods were chosen for factor extraction, and
of operationalizing stress” (Dise-Lewis, 1988, p. 485), including (d) loadings of each item on all factors, informing on the
minor and major incidents. The scale was developed stepwise: degree of factorial purity. Information on test-retest reliability
(a) initial item generation (N = 104), (b) item refinement and was unclear. Secondly, information provided for the construction
addition (N = 90), (c) data for scale construction (N = 502), (d) sample, including initial item generation, indicates homogeneity,
reliability data (N = 85), and (e) validity data (N = 198), with a total and the choice of PCA for factor analysis is questionable.
number of 681 participants for all the steps (11 to 14 years of age, Thirdly, there were unsatisfactory statistical properties such as:
M = 12.6, SD = 0.5). Participants were mainly white, of middle (a) the sample did not represent all socio-economic groups,
and upper-middle class background, from Denver, Colorado. (b) internal consistency reliability was below 0.70 on one factor,
Items were collected through individual and small group (c) six items loaded below 0.40 on their factor, (d) two of the
interviews of 104 participants, and were refined in three steps, highest loading items on the factor Stress-recognition were
using 90 participants. Wording was scrutinized, instructions conceptually unrelated to that factor. Finally, analyses concerning
finalized and two different types of rating systems compared. (a) inter-item correlations and (b) correlation with a related test,
There was no information on number of items in the original and (c) cross-validation of factor structure, were not performed.
pool. The final scale included 125 life events and 49 coping
items. Here, only the coping part of the LECI is reviewed. PCA
(3) Coping Across Situations Questionnaire (CASQ;
with oblique rotation was conducted with answers rated on a
Seiffge-Krenke, 1989)
five-point Likert scale, employing 502 participants. Five factors
were extracted; no information on factor extraction and factorial The CASQ was developed within the framework of cognitive-
purity is provided. Loading of items on factors is reported for transactional theory (Lazarus & Folkman, 1984; Seiffge-Krenke
one factor only. The factor Stress Recognition includes items & Shulman, 1993). Items were selected and developed in stages.
that mainly refer to seeking social support, yet the two highest First, adolescents’ main stressors and coping responses were
loading items (“Cry” and “Scream”) on that factor do not assessed from a sample of 107 adolescents, 12 to 18 years (no
have face validity. For factor structure and internal consistency, further information). The 110 identified coping responses
see Table 4. were categorized according to Westbrook’s coping scale (1979,
as cited in Seiffge-Krenke, 1989, p. 208), and 95% of responses
Further information on reliability and validity. A replication fell within three of Westbrook’s five factors (i.e., Active Coping,
sample of 85 participants completed the LECI twice with an Withdrawal, and Seeking Help from Others). Where the
11-week interval, and the correlation coefficient for individual meaning of the adolescents’ coping responses corresponded
items ranged from −0.13 to 0.61 with a mean of 0.32. The with the content of items from Westbrook’s scale, the items
correlation coefficient was reported in this way – “the stability from Westbrook’s scale were translated into German and the
of the set of coping strategy ratings was .98” (Dise-Lewis, 1988, wording adapted for adolescents. The final version of the CASQ
p. 493) – which is difficult to interpret with the item correlation included twenty items, each containing only one statement.
ranging from −0.13 to 0.61, and an interval of 11 weeks for PCA (this is assumed based on Kavsek & Seiffge-Krenke,
12-year old participants. A subset of the construction sample, 1996) and orthogonal rotation (Seiffge-Krenke, 1989) were
198 participants, was also administered the State-Trait Anxiety performed with yes or no ratings from 1,028 students aged 12
Inventory for Children and the Children’s Depression Inventory. through 19 years old (61 were 12, 70 were 13, 94 were 14, 119
Correlation between LECI coping factors and these measures were 15, 220 were 16, 210 were 17, 182 were 18, and 72 were
ranged from 0.00 to 0.30. 19) by Hessen, Nordrhein-Westfalen and Niedersachsen. Half

© 2008 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2008 The Scandinavian Psychological Associations.
Scand J Psychol 49 (2008) Review of adolescent coping scales 541

Table 5. CASQ factor structure, internal consistency, and average corrected item-total correlation

Internal Average corrected


Factors Items Example consistency item-total correlation

Active coping by means of social resources 7 I try to solve the problems with the help of 0.80 0.57
(Aktive Bewältigung unter Nutzung my friends (Ich versuche, mit Freunden meine
sozialer Ressourcen) Probleme gemeinsam zu lösen)
Internal coping (Internale 6 I think about the problem and try to find different 0.77 0.59
Bewältungsstrategien) solutions (Ich denke über das Problem nach und spiele
verschiedene Lösungsmöglichkeiten in Gedanke durch)
Withdrawal (Problemmeidendes Verhalten) 7 I let out my anger or desperation by shouting, crying, 0.76 0.53
banging doors, and so on (Ich mache meinem Ärger
und meiner Ratlosigkeit “Luft” durch Schreien, Heulen,
Türenknallen, etc.)

of the sample was from urban, and half from rural areas and furthermore assumed that whether they be approach or
parental occupation was representative of Western Germany in avoidant, coping responses would be either cognitive (affective) or
1984 when the data was collected. Sixty percent of mothers were behavioral. The authors expected final selection of items for
homemakers or working part-time (no further information). the CSC-Y to fall into these categories: (a) cognitive/affective
Fathers were white collar workers (54%), blue collar workers engagement; (b) behavioral engagement; (c) cognitive/affective
(25%), self-employed (14%), and professionals (7%). A scree avoidance; (d) behavioral avoidance; and (e) passive resignation.
test revealed three factors, with item loadings 0.40 or beyond, PCA with orthogonal rotation was performed on a sample of
and communality ranging from 0.26 to 0.62, see factor summary 498 Grade 6 and Grade 8 students (aged 10 through 15 years)
in Table 5. recruited from a suburban, mostly white, middle-class community
(no information on state or country). There were 224 Grade 6
Further information on validity. The CASQ factor structure students (114 females and 110 males, M = 11.6 years), and 274
was cross-examined in two independent studies and partly Grade 8 students (136 females and 138 males, M = 13.7 years).
replicated (Seiffge-Krenke, 1992; Seiffge-Krenke & Shulman, Responses were rated on a four-point Likert scale. A scree test
1990). Ten of the CASQ items (50%) loaded on corresponding suggested four factors and not five as originally assumed. A
factor for both the Israeli and the Finnish sample; the other ten second PCA analysis with orthogonal rotation analysis calling
items were either mixed or omitted. for four factors revealed 29 items of 44 (66%) with loading of
0.40 or beyond on one factor only. Test-retest results (one-week
Limitations of the CASQ. First, it was unclear how well the interval) were based on 145 participants randomly chosen from
sample represents the population. Secondly, questionable statistical the construction sample (i.e., 61 females and 84 males; 55 from
methods were chosen for both construction and validation studies grade 6 and 90 from grade 8), see Table 6.
such as PCA and orthogonal factor rotation. Second-order factor
analysis was not performed despite the high correlation between Further evidence of validity. First, the authors (Brodzinsky et al.,
two of the three factors (which indicates that there may be two 1992) reported low to moderate correlation between the four
distinct second-order coping factors rather than three). Thirdly, factors of the CSC-Y and seven out of ten of Kidcope’s coping
analyses concerning (a) test-retest, and (b) correlation with strategies (Spirito et al., 1988), with coefficients ranging from
scales, related as well as unrelated, were not performed. Finally, 0.30 to 0.62. Participants in that study were 60 boys and 60
two cross-validation studies with independent samples verified girls, with a mean age of 11.2 years, from a suburban com-
50% of the factor structure. munity. They were mostly white and middle class. Secondly, the
authors administered the four-factor CSC-Y and the six-factor
Self-Perception Profile for Children (SPPC) to 190 participants,
(4) Coping Scale for Children and Youth (CSC-Y; Brodzinsky 12- or 13-year-old Grade 7 students, mostly white and from
et al., 1992) a middle-class suburban community. Correlations were low to
The CSC-Y was developed with theoretical background in moderate (r ≤ 0.30) for 23 out of 24 relationships between the
pre-existing coping research on adults, which was mainly based two tests (the correlation between SPPC/Global Self-Worth and
on the transactional approach (Lazarus & Folkman, 1984). CSC-Y/Cognitive –Behavioral Problem Solving was r = 0.35;
Literature search on children’s and adults’ coping revealed 44 p < 0.001). Furthermore, the CSC-Y and the Kidcope were
items. The authors assumed that youth might: (a) approach a simultaneously administered in Slovenia to 166 high school
stressor to deal with or confront with it; (b) avoid a stressor students, 51 females and 36 males from a freshman class (mean
to reduce tension; or (c) become passive and resign. They age 14.9 years), and 54 females and 25 males from junior class

© 2008 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2008 The Scandinavian Psychological Associations.
542 S. Sveinbjornsdottir and E. B. Thorsteinsson Scand J Psychol 49 (2008)

Table 6. CSC-Y factor structure, internal consistency, test-retest, and inter factor correlations

Inter factor correlation


Internal
Factors Items Example consistency Test-retest 2 3 4

1. Assistance seeking (Assist) 4 I asked someone in my family 0.72 0.80 0.44* −0.32* –
for help with the problem
2. Cognitive-behavioral problem 8 I thought about the problem and tried 0.81 0.80 −0.24* –
solving (Probsol) to figure out what I could do about it
3. Cognitive avoidance (Cavoid) 11 I tried not thinking about the problem 0.80 0.81 0.37*
4. Behavioral avoidance (Bavoid) 6 I stayed away from things that 0.70 0.73
reminded me about the problem

Note: – = information not available.


* p < 0.001.

(mean age 16.9 years) (Ficková, 1998). Factor analysis was single-sex schools, one for each of the sexes, in metropolitan
performed on the CSC-Y’s factors in combination with the Melbourne, Australia (44.4% females, 55.6% males). Socio-
strategies of Kidcope using orthogonal rotation, and two factors economic background was distributed through four groups of
were extracted. However, much information is missing, such as fathers’ occupation: professional/administrative, white collar,
translation of the scales, the factor model, method of factor blue collar, and unemployed (43.0% professional, 40.9% blue
extraction, loadings of items on each of the factors, internal collar). The majority of the students were born in Australia
consistency, and the correlation between factors – therefore (72.8%). However, 31.3% of fathers were born in Australia and
interpretation of the results is difficult. 25% in Greece. The students responded to an open-ended question
on how they coped with a self-nominated major concern, and
Limitations of the CSC-Y. First, PCA and orthogonal rotation a total of 2,041 descriptions were generated. The number of
of factors is questionable. The construction sample was coping responses was reduced to 156 distinct items, which were
homogeneous, predominantly white, and middle class. Items tested on 30 students, aged 12 to 15 years old, for comprehension
were derived from the literature only, to fit a priori iden- of content and language, and found satisfactory. The response
tified categories of coping. There were no items included in format was a five-point Likert scale.
the scale that addressed acting out or risky behavior, such The 156 refined items were administered to 500 Year 7
as fighting, hitting, and substance use. These usually are through Year 11 students in Melbourne, Australia; there was no
included in adolescent coping questionnaires. The interval for further information on participants, factor model and rotation.
test-retest was one week only. Secondly, inter-item correlation, A scree test indicated 9 factors including 96 items. The remaining
cross-validation of factor structure, and confirmatory factor 60 items that failed to load on any factor were, nevertheless,
analysis on an independent representative sample were not factor analyzed separately, and five factors with eigenvalues
performed. above one were extracted, comprising 32 of those 60 items. The
authors then scrutinized and refined the two lots of items, in
total 128 items (96 plus 32), which resulted in a 50-item ques-
(5) Adolescent Coping Scale (ACS; Frydenberg & Lewis, 1993) tionnaire. That questionnaire was then administered to 712 Year
The ACS was developed in stages (Frydenberg, 1989, 1991, 7 to Year 12 students from 44 secondary schools, and factor
1997; Frydenberg & Lewis, 1991; Frydenberg & Lewis, 1993), analyzed, using orthogonal rotation; no further information on
based on the transactional model of stress and coping (Lazarus participants and factor model was provided. A scree plot revealed
& Folkman, 1984). The ACS consists of 79 coping items (plus 13 factors for those 50 items with internal consistency ranging
one open-ended item) in 18 factors, measuring coping in relation from 0.34 to 0.80 when identified for specific concerns, and
to either specific (Specific Form) or general concerns (General ranging from 0.35 to 0.78 when identified for general concerns.
Form), see Table 7. Three second-order factors were identified: For the Specific Form, 4 of the 13 factors had alpha values
Solving the Problem, Reference to Others, and Non-Productive below 0.70; 2 had alpha values below 0.50. For the General
Coping, see Table 8. A short form of the scale including 18 Form, 10 of 13 factors had alpha values below 0.70, and 4 factors
items was created out of the full scale (ACS Short Form). Items had alpha values below 0.50.
were derived from the literature, mainly from the Ways of The final version of the ACS consists of 18 factors including
Coping Checklist (WOCC), developed by Folkman and Lazarus all the 50 items as explained above and an additional 29 items
(Frydenberg, 1989), and also generated by 643 Year 11 (58.1%) derived from the original pool of 156 items. How these factors
and Year 12 (41.9%) students, 15 to 18 years old (Frydenberg were derived is not known. No information was provided for
& Lewis, 1993). Students surveyed for generation of coping factor extraction methods, factor model, and methods for
responses were derived from five co-educational, and two further refinement.

© 2008 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2008 The Scandinavian Psychological Associations.
Scand J Psychol 49 (2008) Review of adolescent coping scales 543

Table 7. ACS long form factor structure, internal consistency, and 14-day test-retest interval

Specific form General form

Internal Internal
Factor Items Example consistency Test-retest consistency Test-retest

Social support 5 Talk to other people to see what 0.79 0.77 0.80 0.81
they would do if they had the problem
Focus on solving the problem 5 Work at solving what’s causing the problem 0.77 0.64 0.72 0.71
Work hard and achieve 5 Keep up with work as required 0.77 0.64 0.68 0.66
Worry 5 Worry about my future 0.76 0.77 0.73 0.77
Invest in close friends 5 Meet with friends 0.78 0.82 0.77 0.84
Seek to belong 5 Make a good impression on others 0.71 0.76 0.67 0.75
who matter to me
Wishful thinking 5 Hope for the best 0.70 0.72 0.67 0.74
Not coping 5 I just give up 0.68 0.62 0.58 0.69
Tension reduction 5 Cry or scream 0.71 0.74 0.69 0.75
Social action 4 Organize an action or a petition 0.76 0.57 0.70 0.50
regarding my concern
Ignore the problem 4 Consciously “block out” the problem 0.71 0.66 0.68 0.57
Self-blame 4 Criticize myself 0.81 0.73 0.76 0.74
Keep to self 4 Keep my feelings to myself 0.72 0.53 0.70 0.49
Seek spiritual support 4 Let God take care of my worries 0.87 0.80 0.85 0.81
Focus on the positive 4 Remember those who are worse off so 0.70 0.55 0.68 0.58
my troubles don’t seem so bad
Seek professional help 4 Ask a professional person for help 0.83 0.72 0.84 0.72
Seek relaxing diversions 3 Find a way to relax; for example, listen to music, 0.62 0.49 0.54 0.44
read a book, play a musical instrument, watch television
Physical recreation 3 Play sport 0.64 0.58 0.64 0.60

Table 8. ACS specific form second-order factor structure

Number first
Second-order factors order factors First order factor

Solving the Problem 8 Seek Social Support, Focus on Solving the Problem, Physical
Recreation, Seek Relaxing Diversions, Invest in Close Friends,
Seek to Belong, Work Hard and Achieve, Focus on the Positive
Non-productive Coping 8 Worry, Seek to Belong, Wishful Thinking, Not Coping,
Ignore the Problem, Tension Reduction, Keep to Self, Self-Blame
Reference to Others 4 Seek Social Support, Seek Spiritual Support, Seek Professional Help,
Social Action

Note: SocSup and Belong, loaded beyond 0.40 on two-second-order factors. Thus SocSup loaded 0.48 and 0.45 on Solving the Problem and Reference
to Others respectively, and Belong loaded 0.41 and 0.55 on Non-Productive Coping and Solving the Problem respectively.

Final factor analysis. The 79 (and one open-ended) item and separately on each cluster of factors for all 500 participants,
18 factor ACS were consequently administered in five schools see Table 7.
to 673 students with varied socio-economic and cultural PCA and oblique rotation were performed on the 18 factors
background. The sample included students from Year 7 (23%), for the Specific Form. A scree test indicated three second-
Year 8 (16%), Year 9 (22%), Year 10 (25%), and Year 11 (13%) order factors with eigenvalues beyond one and loading 0.40 or
(1% missing assumed due to rounding). PCA and oblique beyond. No information was provided on internal consistency.
rotation were then performed on responses from those who had
complete data (500 of the 673 students). However, factor Further information on reliability and validity. Test-retest analyses
analysis was not performed on the full scale, as the ratio of (14 days interval) were computed for each item for both the
participants per item was found to be insufficient. The authors, General and the Specific Form of ACS. Participants were 101
therefore, randomly split the 18 factors into three clusters, Year 7 students (a subsample of the construction sample). Items
each including six of the factors. PCA was then performed with correlation coefficients of 0.32 or more, p < 0.001, were

© 2008 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2008 The Scandinavian Psychological Associations.
544 S. Sveinbjornsdottir and E. B. Thorsteinsson Scand J Psychol 49 (2008)

considered reliable. For the General Form, eleven items failed versus emotion-focus, or approach versus avoidance, and, (b)
to meet the criterion and the correlation ranged from 0.20 to 0.86 the method – that is behavioral or cognitive (Compas, 1987;
(M = 0.48). For the Specific Form, seven items failed, and Compas, Malcarne & Fondacaro, 1988; Lazarus & Folkman, 1984).
the correlation ranged from 0.21 to 0.79 (M = 0.49). Test-retest Domains were identified through literature on coping, and
coefficients, as calculated for each factor, ranged from 0.44 to items developed to fit both focus and method. Items were
0.84 for the General Form (M = 0.68), and 8 of the 18 factors derived in numerous ways: (a) reviewing descriptions of adoles-
had correlation coefficients below 0.70. For the Specific Form cents coping with life crisis and transitions; (b) looking into
the correlation ranged from 0.49 to 0.82 (M = 0.64), and 9 of how adolescents cope with serious illness and hospitalization;
the 18 factors had correlation coefficients below 0.70. (c) interviewing adolescents; (d) examining concepts of
A Spanish version of ACS Long Form, where 800 13–15 year adolescent coping in the literature; and (d) adapting items from
old secondary school students from middle-class families in the CRI-adult scale. Next, 40 pilot interviews were conducted
Bueno Aires, Argentina, participated, did not replicate 18 ACS with young people using about 100 coping items in ten potential
factors successfully (Richaud de Minzi, 2003). Instead, through coping factors. The group that was interviewed included both
several analyses, using FA and oblique rotation, 11 factors sexes and healthy youth and youth with problems (no further
emerged, including 47 of the 79 items from the ACS, dropping information). This pilot study resulted in a 72 refined items
almost half of the original coping items and fusing some of the questionnaire, adapted to the reading level of Grade 6 students.
original coping factors. Factor analysis to identify underlying constructs or factors
was not conducted. The 72 items were administered to a
Limitations of the ACS, Specific Form, and General Form. First, construction sample of 315 adolescents, 168 females (mean
information regarding the scale’s development is insufficient for age 15.3), and 147 males (mean age 15.2). Of these, 163 were
some of the studies reported, such as on (a) participants’ back- healthy (91 females and 72 males), 49 were depressed (30
ground, (b) choices of statistical methods (e.g., the rationale for females and 19 males), 58 had conduct disorder (21 females
18 primary factors), and (c) outcome (e.g., internal consistency of and 37 males), and 45 had rheumatic disease (26 females and
second-order factors). Furthermore, reports on procedures are 19 males). Part of the healthy group of the sample (n = 78) were
frequently unclear, such as on how the number of factors was siblings of those included in the sample that had depression,
derived and why information that could have led to further conduct disorder, or rheumatic disease. Participants were mostly
refinement of the scale sometimes was ignored, as for instance Caucasians (70%). Parents were reported as “moderately”
when clearly imperfect items and factors of low internal relia- educated, and of various socio-economic backgrounds. The
bility were included without further item refinement. Secondly, answering format was a four-point Likert scale. A revised form
some questionable methods were chosen, such as: (a) PCA and of the CRI-Y was administered in a 12 to 15 month follow-up,
(b) dividing items into three clusters and then factor analyzing including 254 of the original 315 participants. In addition, 51
each cluster separately. Thus, two-thirds of the items were youths with rheumatic disease and 34 of their siblings par-
excluded when factors were explored, and consequently there is ticipated in this follow-up; hence the second construction
no information on how the excluded items may have affected sample included 400 participants (221 females, 179 males).
the outcome for each cluster. Particularly, since coping factors Criteria for revision of the CRI-Y were: (a) good content and
are believed to be interrelated, this method is unsatisfactory. face validity for each item on the factor to which it belonged;
Thirdly, some reported properties of the scale are poor. For (b) varied response distribution for each item; (c) at least a
instance: (a) two factors had fewer than four items each; (b) moderate level of internal consistency for each factor; and (d)
internal consistency was below 0.70 for 9 of 18 factors, General minimized overlap between factors and combining of highly
Form, and the same applied for three of the factors for the inter-correlated factors. These criteria led to an eight factor
Specific Form; (c) test-retest reliability for a number of factors solution instead of the original ten factor solution, each factor
on both the Specific and the General Form was below 0.70; and including six items. Internal consistency ranged from 0.55–0.72
(d) the ACS factor structure failed to replicate. Finally, some for males and 0.59–0.72 for females, see Table 9.
important analyses were not performed: (a) factor analysis
including all items; (b) inter-item correlation, and (c) correlation Further information on reliability and validity. Test-retest correla-
with related and unrelated scales. Psychometric limitations of tions (12 to 15 months interval) were calculated for 254 adolescents,
the ACS are major and the scale is long (80 items). as the scale was implemented for the second time for the
construction sample. The average correlation between occasions
for the eight factors was r = 0.34 for females and r = 0.29 for
(6) Coping Responses Inventory, Youth Form (CRI-Y; males. Correlations between factors of r = ≥ 0.40 were numerous
Moos, 1993) for both sexes, the highest between “Logical Analysis” and
The CRI-Y (Ebata & Moos, 1991) is based on the theory of “Problem Solving”, r = 0.61.
transactional model of stress and coping (Lazarus & Folkman, Correlations between the CRI-Y and depression, anxiety,
1984), and two second-order factors of coping are included into alcohol and drug use, behavior problems, and self-confidence
the theoretical framework: (a) the focus such as problem-focused were calculated using the same sample. Information on measures

© 2008 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2008 The Scandinavian Psychological Associations.
Scand J Psychol 49 (2008) Review of adolescent coping scales 545

Table 9. CRI-Y factor structure and internal consistency

Internal consistency

Higher and first order factors Items Example Females (n = 221) Males (n = 179)

Approach coping responses


Logical Analysis 6 Did you think of different ways 0.68 0.69
to deal with the problem?
Positive Reappraisal 6 Did you tell yourself things 0.79 0.69
to make yourself feel better?
Seeking Guidance and Support 6 Did you talk with a parent or 0.69 0.72
other family member about the problem?
Problem Solving 6 Did you decide on one way to 0.71 0.68
deal with the problem and do it?

Avoidance responses
Cognitive Avoidance 6 Did you try to forget the whole thing? 0.72 0.71
Acceptance or Resignation 6 Did you feel that time would make a 0.59 0.55
difference – that the only thing to do was wait?
Seeking Alternative Rewards 6 Did you get involved in new activities? 0.69 0.71
Emotional Discharge 6 Did you take it out on other people 0.61 0.72
when you felt angry or sad?

is limited – some are based on existing scales, others are test-retest reliability (around 0.30), but not easily interpreted
self-rating questions prepared for the occasion. However, 45 since the interval between tests was far beyond upper limits
of a total of 48 correlations were low to moderate (r ≤ 0.30), (roughly one year); and (c) the eight coping factors of CRI-Y
indicating conceptual differences of measurements. The remaining failed to replicate across samples. Finally, analyses such as (a)
correlations which indicated conceptual relationships were between inter-item correlation, and (b) correlation with a related test
Depression and Cognitive Avoidance (r = 0.36), Depression and were not performed.
Emotional Discharge (r = −0.34), and Alcohol and Drug Use
and Emotional Discharge (r = 0.34).
Three independent cross-validation studies on the CRI-Y DISCUSSION
failed to replicate the factor structure. Forns, Amador, Kirchner, The aim of this study was to examine the design and construction
Gómez and Muro (2005) tested the 8 factors on 1,401 adoles- of adolescent coping scales and their psychometric properties
cents (12–16 years) in Barcelona (FA, oblique rotation). Internal in relation to best classical test-theoretical practice. Lack of
consistency for all factors was below 0.70 (ranging between report of important information as well as reporting of poor
0.40 and 0.63, six of eight factors below 0.60). When testing EFA choices, and therefore obscured outcomes, have frequently
for higher dimensions, the distinction between cognitive and been registered in psychological research (Conway & Huffcutt,
behavioral coping was not supported. A distinction between 2003; Fabrigar et al., 1999; Ford et al., 1986), and we presumed
approach and avoidance coping was partially supported. In a similar outcome in the present review of adolescent coping
the other two validation studies the researchers, through a post scales. That, we assumed, might to some extent explain semantic
hoc approach, created different scales on their own data based inconsistencies and confusion within the research field of
on the original 48 CRI-Y items (Eyles & Bates, 2005; Griffith, coping. We argued that meager psychometric quality of existing
Dubow & Ippolito, 2000). coping scales developed through classical test-theory and EFA
and imprecise statistical choices throughout the development of
Limitations of the CRI-Y. First, the criteria for selection of the the scales are intertwined.
construction sample is unclear and consequently, which popu- Six self-rating adolescent coping scales for general stressors
lation it is meant to represent. Information is missing on item and daily hassles where coping strategies were clustered in factors
selection and on participants used in the development of the were reviewed. The scales were examined in relation to ten
CRI-Y. Secondly, questionable methods were employed. The detailed test-theoretical criteria concerning reliability and evidence
factor structure was theoretically derived, hypothetical, and not of validity and the step-wise selection of statistical choices
tested statistically. Furthermore, high correlations between when implementing factor analysis during scale development.
several factors indicate a different factor structure and that some Similar to earlier reports on EFA practices in psychological
factors should possibly be combined. Thirdly, some reported research, we observed omission of information for all the
psychometric properties of the scale are not acceptable: (a) reviewed scales which limited our review. Furthermore, similar
internal consistency of one factor was below 0.60; (b) low to previous findings on EFA practices, the information provided

© 2008 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2008 The Scandinavian Psychological Associations.
546 S. Sveinbjornsdottir and E. B. Thorsteinsson Scand J Psychol 49 (2008)

by developers of the reviewed adolescent coping scales revealed Although an agreement on the importance of individual coping
poor psychometric properties for each scale on numerous criteria for well-being and health is reached, and research, including
throughout their development. The shortcomings concerned development of coping scales, constantly adds information to
several issues of psychometric importance and included: (a) the area, there is still empirical research to be pursued. The present
poor choices of statistical (EFA) procedures (e.g., initial item review of development of adolescent coping scales supports our
selection, model of factor analysis, and factor rotation); (b) hypothesis that semantic confusion within the research field
insufficient statistical properties or values as compared with of coping and poor psychometric properties of scales are inter-
accepted criteria (e.g., item loadings and internal reliability of twined. Given the importance of individual coping as either
factors, representativeness of samples); and (c) lack of important protective or risk factors in relation to health and well-being, we
reliability and or validity analyses (e.g., inter-item correlation, suggest that further research on adolescent coping with general
test-retest, cross-validation or confirmatory analysis). concerns, in search for both latent primary factors as well as
None of the six scales was developed through the preferable dimensions of coping, rely on precise methodology, including
FA. Rather, five were developed through PCA, and one scale high quality EFA procedures.
was hypothetical, based on a priori theory. Oblique rotation was
used for two of the five scales developed through EFA as per This project was supported by Rannís, http://www.rannis.is/
suggested best-practice allowing statistical analysis of second-
order factors or dimensions and orthogonal rotation for the
remaining three. None of the six scales was replicated across REFERENCES
samples. Instead, as with Steger’s (2006) notification of post Ayers, T. S., Sandler, I. N. & Twohey, J. L. (1998). Conceptualization
hoc studies and diverse outcomes of the Ways of Coping Scale and measurement of coping in children and adolescents. In T. H.
across samples, the outcomes for all replication studies of the Ollendick & R. J. Prinz (Eds.), Advances in clinical child psychology
(Vol. 20, pp. 243–301). New York: Plenum Press.
reviewed adolescent coping scales revealed different numbers
Benson, J. & Hagtvet, K. A. (1996). The interplay among design, data
of coping items (strategies), factor structure, and dimensions of analysis, and theory in the measurement of coping. In M. Zeidner
coping as compared with the original scales. These combined & N. S. Endler (Eds.), Handbook of coping: Theory, research,
shortcomings cast doubt on content and construct validity of the applications (pp. 83–106). New York: John Wiley and Sons.
reviewed scales and leave both researchers and clinicians unable Billings, A. G. & Moos, R. H. (1981). The role of coping responses and
social resources in attenuating the stress of life events. Journal of
to interpret and rely on scale-scores and to compare research
Behavioral Medicine, 4, 139–157.
outcomes. Billings, A. G. & Moos, R. H. (1984). Coping, stress, and social
Imprecise psychometric procedures that are employed during resources among adults with unipolar depression. Journal of Per-
scale development are likely to obscure the outcome. Therefore, sonality and Social Psychology, 46, 877–891.
awareness of adverse consequences for reliability and validity Boekaerts, M. (1996). Coping with stress in childhood and adolescence.
In M. Zeidner & N. S. Endler (Eds.), Handbook of coping: Theory,
of any construct, caused by poor test-theoretical practices, is
research, applications (pp. 452–484). New York: John Wiley and
needed, and only through detailed information and explanation Sons.
of the rationale for statistical choices is it possible (a) to examine Briggs, S. & Cheek, J. (1986). The role of factor analysis in the develop-
how closely a researcher follows best practice for test develop- ment and evaluation of personality scales. Journal of Personality,
ment, and (b) to repeat or replicate a study. The reason is not 54, 106–148.
Brodzinsky, D., Elias, M., Steiger, C., Simon, J. & Gill, M. (1992).
obvious why none of the reviewed adolescent coping scales was
Coping Scale for Children and Youth: Scale development and
developed according to best-practice. Ford et al. (1986), Fabrigar validation. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 13, 195–
et al. (1999), and Conway and Huffcutt (2003) have offered 214.
some possible explanations as to why researchers do not employ Burnett, P. C. & Fanshawe, J. P. (1996). Measuring adolescent coping
high quality EFA procedures. One is that education and training strategies: Revalidation of the adolescent coping orientation for
problem experiences. The Australian Educational and Developmental
in EFA procedures might be insufficient; another that tradition
Psychologist, 13, 40–53.
prefers PCA with orthogonal rotation; and finally that default Burr, W. (1982). Families under stress. In H. I. McCubbin, A. E. Cauble
options in statistical software lead researchers to use PCA and & J. M. Patterson (Eds.), Family stress, coping, and social support
orthogonal rotation without questioning their choice. These (pp. 5–25). Springfield, IL: Charles C. Thomas Publisher.
explanations, one or all, may well be true for the development Carver, C. S., Scheier, M. F. & Weintraub, J. K. (1989). Assessing
coping strategies: A theoretically based approach. Journal of
of the reviewed adolescent coping scales. The EFA procedure
Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 267–283.
is complex and requires that rationale for each decision must Causey, D. & Dubow, E. (1992). Development of a self-report coping
be understood and explained and that each step is carefully measure for elementary school children. Journal of Clinical Child
and deliberately chosen to secure an outcome as best possible. Psychology, 21, 47–59.
Omission of crucial information, as observed in the present Child, D. (1990). The essentials of factor analysis (2nd edn). London:
Cassell Educational Ltd.
review, supports the presumption that researchers do not seem
Compas, B. E. (1987). Coping with stress during childhood and adoles-
to find detailed information and careful choice of each statistical cence. Psychological Bulletin, 101, 393–403.
step in the procedure of scale development, including the EFA, Compas, B. E., Connor-Smith, J. K., Saltzman, H., Thomsen, A. H. &
all that important. Wadsworth, M. (1999). Getting specific about coping: Effortful and

© 2008 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2008 The Scandinavian Psychological Associations.
Scand J Psychol 49 (2008) Review of adolescent coping scales 547

involuntary responses to stress in development. In M. Lewis & D. Frydenberg, E. (1991). Adolescent coping styles and strategies: Is there
Ramsey (Eds.), Soothing and stress (pp. 229–256). Mahawah, NJ: functional and dysfunctional coping. Australian Guidance and
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Counselling Association, 1, 1–8.
Compas, B. E., Connor-Smith, J. K., Saltzman, H., Thomsen, A. H. & Frydenberg, E. (1997). Adolescent coping: Theoretical and research
Wadsworth, M. E. (2001). Coping with stress during childhood and perspectives. London: Routledge.
adolescence: Problems, progress, and potential in theory and Frydenberg, E. & Lewis, R. (1991). Adolescent coping in the Australian
research. Psychological Bulletin, 127, 87–127. context. Australian Educational Researcher, 18, 65–82.
Compas, B. E., Malcarne, V. & Fondacaro, K. (1988). Coping with Frydenberg, E. & Lewis, R. (1993). Adolescent Coping Scale: Admin-
stressful events in older children and young adolescents. Journal of istrator’s manual. Melbourne, Australia: ACER (The Australian
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 56, 405–411. Council for Educational Research Ltd).
Connor-Smith, J. K., Compas, B. E., Wadsworth, M. E., Thomsen, A. Gorsuch, R. (1983). Factor analysis (2nd edn). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence
H. & Saltzman, H. (2000). Responses to stress in adolescence: Erlbaum Associates.
Measurement of coping and involuntary stress responses. Journal of Graue, M., Hanestad, B. R., Wentzel-Larsen, T., Sövik, O. & Bru, E.
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 68, 976–992. (2004). The coping styles of adolescents with Type 1 diabetes are
Conway, J. M. & Huffcutt, A. I. (2003). A review and evaluation of associated with degree of metabolic control. Diabetes Care, 27,
exploratory factor analysis practices in organizational research. 1313–1317.
Organizational Research Methods, 6, 147–168. Griffith, A. G., Dubow, E. F. & Ippolito, M. F. (2000). Developmental
Costello, A. B. & Osborne, J. W. (2005). Best practices in exploratory and cross-situational differences in adolescents’ coping strategies
factor analysis: Four recommendations for getting the most Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 29, 183–204.
from your analysis. Practical Assessment, Research & Evaluation, Halvarsson, K., Lunner, K. & Sjödin, P. (2001). Development of a
10, 1–9. Swedish version of the Adolescent Coping Orientation for Problem
DeVellis, R. F. (1991). Scale development: Theory and applications. Experiences (A-Cope). Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 42,
Newbury Park, CA: Sage. 383–388.
Dise-Lewis, J. E. (1988). The life events and coping inventory: An Haynes, S. N., Richard, D. C. S. & Kubany, E. S. (1995). Content
assessment of stress in children. Psychosomatic Medicine, 50, validity in psychological assessment: A functional approach to
484–499. concepts and methods. Psychological Assessment, 7, 238–247.
Ebata, A. T. & Moos, R. H. (1991). Coping and adjustment in distressed Hayton, J. C., Allen, D. G. & Scarpello, V. (2004). Factor retention
and healthy adolescents. Journal of Applied Developmental Psy- decisions in exploratory factor analysis: A tutorial on parallel
chology, 12, 33–54. analysis. Organizational Research Methods, 7, 191–205.
Ebata, A. T. & Moos, R. H. (1994). Personal, situational, and contextual Holmes, D. & Rahe, R. (1967). The social readjustment rating scale.
correlates of coping in adolescence. Journal of Research on Adoles- Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 11, 213–218.
cence, 4, 99–125. Jorgensen, S. R. & Dusek, J. B. (1990). Adolescent adjustment and
Endler, N. S. & Parker, J. D. A. (1990). Multidimensional assessment coping strategies. Journal of Personality, 58, 503–513.
of coping: A critical evaluation. Journal of Personality and Social Kavsek, M. & Seiffge-Krenke, I. (1996). The differentiation of coping
Psychology, 58, 844–854. traits in adolescence. International Journal of Behavioral Development,
Eyles, D. J. & Bates, G. W. (2005). Development of a shortened form 19, 651–668.
of the Coping Responses Inventory-Youth with an Australian Kline, P. (1986). A handbook of test construction: Introduction to
sample. North American Journal of Psychology, 7, 161–170. psychometric design. London: Methuen.
Fabrigar, L., Wegener, D., MacCallum, R. & Strahn, E. (1999). Evalu- Kline, P. (1993). The handbook of psychological testing. London:
ating the use of exploratory factor analysis in psychological Routledge.
research. Psychological Methods, 4, 272–299. Kline, P. (1994). An easy guide to factor analysis. London: Routledge.
Fanshawe, J. P. & Burnett, P. C. (1991). Assessing school related Kowalsky, K. C. & Crocker, P. R. E. (2001). Development and validation
stressors and coping mechanisms in adolescents. British Journal of of the Coping Function Questionnaire for adolescents in sport.
Educational Psychology, 61, 92–98. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 23, 136–155.
Ficková, E. (1998). Coping scales for children and adolescents: Psycho- Kristensson, P. & Öhlund, L. S. (2005). Swedish upper secondary
metric relationships. Studia Psychologica, 40, 291–296. school pupils’ sense of coherence, coping resources and aggressive-
Folkman, S. & Lazarus, R. S. (1980). An analysis of coping in a ness in relation to educational track and performance. Scandinavian
middle-aged community sample. Journal of Health and Social Journal of Caring Sciences, 19, 77–84.
Behavior, 21, 219–239. Kuo, B. C. H., Roysircar, G. & Newby-Clark, I. R. (2006). Development
Folkman, S. & Lazarus, R. S. (1985). If it changes it must be a process: of the Cross-Cultural Coping Scale: Collective, avoidance, and
Study of emotion and coping during three stages of a college engagement coping. Measurement and Evaluation in Counseling
examination. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48, and Development, 39, 161–181.
150–170. Lazarus, R. S. (1993). Coping theory and research: Past, present, and
Folkman, S. & Lazarus, R. S. (1988). Manual for the ways of coping future. Psychosomatic Medicine, 55, 234–247.
questionnaire (Research edition). Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Lazarus, R. S. & Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, appraisal and coping. New
Psychologists Press. York: Springer.
Ford, J. K., MacCallum, R. C. & Tait, M. (1986). The application of MacCallum, R. C., Widaman, K. F., Zhang, S. & Hong, S. (1999).
exploratory factor analysis in applied psychology: A critical review Sample size in factor analysis. Psychological Methods, 4, 84–
and analysis. Personnel Psychology, 39, 291–314. 99.
Forns, M., Amador, J. A., Kirchner, T., Gómez, J. & Muro, P. (2005). Masten, A. S., Best, K. M. & Garmezy, N. (1990). Resilience and
Psychometric properties of the Spanish version of the Moos development: Contributions from the study of children who
Coping Response Inventory for Youth. Psychological Reports, 97, overcame adversity. Development and Psychopathology, 2, 425–
777–789. 444.
Frydenberg, E. (1989). The concerns and coping strategies of youth: A McCrae, R. R. (1984). Situational determinants of coping responses:
study of Australian adolescents. Unpublished Doctoral dissertation, Loss, threat, and challenge. Journal of Personality and Social
La Trobe University, Melbourne. Psychology, 46, 919–928.

© 2008 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2008 The Scandinavian Psychological Associations.
548 S. Sveinbjornsdottir and E. B. Thorsteinsson Scand J Psychol 49 (2008)

McCrae, R. R. & Costa, P. T. J. (1986). Personality, coping, and coping coping on adolescents’ depressive symptoms. Journal of Youth and
effectiveness in adult sample. Journal of Personality, 52, 385–405. Adolescence, 29, 617–630.
McCubbin, H. I. & Patterson, J. M. (1981). Family stress and adaptation Seiffge-Krenke, I. & Shulman, S. (1990). Coping style in adolescence:
to crisis. A double ABCX model of family behavior. In D. Olson & A cross-cultural study. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 21,
B. Miller (Eds.), Family studies review yearbook (pp. 87–106). Beverly 351–377.
Hills, CA: Sage Publications. Seiffge-Krenke, I. & Shulman, S. (1993). Stress, coping and relation-
McCubbin, H. I. & Patterson, J. M. (1982). Family adaptation to crisis. ships in adolescence. In S. Jackson & Rodriguez-Tomé (Eds.),
In H. I. McCubbin, A. E. Cauble & J. M. Patterson (Eds.), Family Adolescence and its social world (pp. 169–196). East Sussex:
stress, coping, and social support (pp. 26–47). Springfield, IL: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Charles Thomas Publisher. Skinner, E. A., Edge, K., Altman, J. & Sherwood, H. (2003). Searching
McCubbin, H. I. & Patterson, J. M. (1983). The family stress process: for the structure of coping: A review and critique of category
The Double ABCX Model of Adjustment and Adaptation. In H. I. systems for classifying ways of coping. Psychological Bulletin, 129,
McCubbin, M. Sussman & J. M. Patterson (Eds.), Social stress and 216–269.
the family: Advances and development in family (pp. 7–37). New Smith, D. J. & Rutter, M. (1995). Introduction. In M. Rutter & D. J.
York: Haworth Press. Smith (Eds.), Psychosocial disorders in young people: Time trends
McCubbin, H. I. & Patterson, J. M. (1986). Adolescent stress, coping and their causes (pp. 1–6). Chichester, UK: John Wiley & Sons.
and adaptation: A normative family perspective. In G. Leigh & G. Smith, R. E., Smoll, F. L. & Ptacek, J. T. (1990). Conjunctive moderator
Peterson (Eds.), Adolescents in a family context (pp. 256–276). variables in vulnerability and resiliency research: Life stress, social
Cincinnati, OH: South-Western Publishing Co. support and coping skills, and adolescent sport injuries. Journal of
Moos, R. M. (1993). Coping response inventory: Youth form: Profes- Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 360–370.
sional manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources. Spirito, A., Stark, L. J. & Williams, C. (1988). Development of a brief
Nunnally, J. (1978). Psychometric theory. New York: McGraw Hill. coping checklist for use with pediatric populations. Journal of
Parkes, K. R. (1986). Coping in stressful episodes: The role of individual Pediatric Psychology, 13, 555–574.
differences, environmental factors, and situational characteristics. Steger, M. F. (2006). An illustration of issues in factor extraction and
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 1277–1292. identification of dimensionality in psychological assessment data.
Patterson, J. M. & McCubbin, H. I. (1987). Adolescent coping style Journal of Personality Assessment, 86, 263–272.
and behaviors: Conceptualization and measurement. Journal of Steiner, H., Erickson, S. J., Hernandez, N. L. & Pavelski, R. (2002).
Adolescence, 10, 163–186. Coping styles as correlates of health in high school students.
Pearlin, L. I. & Schooler, C. (1978). The structure of coping. Journal Journal of Adolescent Health, 30, 326–335.
of Health and Social Behavior, 19, 2–21. Stone, A. A. & Neale, J. M. (1984). New measure of daily coping:
Plancherel, B. & Bolognini, M. (1995). Coping and mental health in Development and preliminary results. Journal of Personality and
early adolescence. Journal of Adolescence, 18, 459–474. Social Psychology, 46, 892–906.
Reid, G. J., Dubow, E. F. & Carey, T. (1995). Developmental and Sveinbjornsdottir, S. (2001). The assessment of adolescent coping:
situational differences in coping among children and adolescents Development of a new measure. Unpublished Ph.D., La Trobe
with diabetes. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 16, University, Melbourne.
529–554. Tabachnick, B. G. & Fidell, L. S. (1989). Using multivariate statistics
Reid, G. J., Dubow, E. F., Carey, T. C. & Dura, J. R. (1994). Contribution (2nd edn). New York: Harper Collins Publishers.
of coping to medical adjustment and treatment responsibility among Tobin, D. L., Holroyd, K. A., Reynolds, R. V. & Wigal, J. K. (1989).
children and adolescents with diabetes. Journal of Developmental The hierarchical factor structure of the coping strategies inventory.
and Behavioral Pediatrics, 15, 327–335. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 13, 343–361.
Richaud de Minzi, M. C. (2003). Coping assessment in adolescents. Tolor, A. & Fehon, D. (1987). Coping with stress: A study of male
Adolescence, 38, 321–330. adolescents’ coping strategies as related to adjustment. Journal of
Rijavec, M. & Brdar, I. (1997). Coping with school failure: Development Adolescent Research, 2, 33–42.
of the school failure coping scale. European Journal of Psychology Velicer, W. F. & Jackson, D. N. (1990). Component analysis versus
of Education, XII, 37–49. common factor analysis: Some issues in selecting an appropriate
Rutter, M. (Ed.) (1995). Psychosocial disturbances in young people: procedure. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 25, 1–28.
Challenges for prevention. New York: Cambridge University Press. Watson, D. C., Willson, L. R. & Sinha, B. K. (1998). Assessing the
Schwarzer, R. & Schwarzer, C. (1996). A critical survey of coping dimensional structure of coping: A cross-cultural comparison.
instruments. In M. Zeidner & N. S. Endler (Eds.), Handbook of International Journal of Stress Management, 5, 77–81.
coping: Theory, research, applications (pp. 107–132). New York: Whatley, S. L., Foreman, A. C. & Richards, S. (1998). The relationship
John Wiley and Sons. of coping style to dysphoria, anxiety, and anger. Psychological
Seiffge-Krenke, I. (1989). Bewältigung alltäglicher Problemsituationen: Reports, 83, 783–791.
Ein Coping Fragebogen für Jugendliche. Zeitschrift für Differentielle Widaman, K. F. (1993). Common factor analysis versus principal
und Diagnostische Psychologie, 4, 201–220. component analysis: Differential bias in representing model
Seiffge-Krenke, I. (1992). Coping behavior of Finnish adolescents: parameters? Multivariate Behavioural Research, 28, 263–311.
Remarks on a cross-cultural comparison. Scandinavian Journal of Zeidner, M. & Hammer, A. L. (1990). Life events and coping resources
Psychology, 33, 301–314. as predictors of stress symptoms in adolescents. Personality and
Seiffge-Krenke, I. (1998). Adolescents’ health: A developmental Individual Differences, 11, 693–703.
perspective. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Seiffge-Krenke, I. & Klessinger, N. (2000). Long-term effects of avoidant Received 3 October 2007, accepted 3 March 2008

© 2008 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2008 The Scandinavian Psychological Associations.

View publication stats

You might also like