You are on page 1of 15

| |

Received: 29 October 2018    Revised: 17 August 2019    Accepted: 22 August 2019

DOI: 10.1111/eth.12949

RESE ARCH PAPER

Non‐verbal mate retention behaviour in women and its relation


to couple's relationship adjustment and satisfaction

Jitka Lindová1,2  | Kateřina Klapilová1,2  | Devin Johnson1 | Adéla Vobořilová2 |


Barbora Chlápková2 | Jan Havlíček1,3

1
National Institute of Mental Health,
Klecany, Czech Republic Abstract
2
Department of Anthropology, Faculty of The function of mate retention strategies is to preserve the bond between romantic
Humanities, Charles University, Prague,
partners and to prevent desertion, which is also why such behaviours are often elic‐
Czech Republic
3
Department of Zoology, Faculty of
ited by the presence of a potential rival. Most existing studies on mate retention are
Science, Charles University, Prague, Czech based on self‐reports, which are prone to various biases. In this study, we observed
Republic
non‐verbal behaviour of 47 long‐term romantic heterosexual couples during their in‐
Correspondence terview with a female experimenter, whom the women in the couples may have per‐
Jitka Lindová, National Institute of Mental
Health, Topolová 748, 250 67, Klecany,
ceived as a potential rival. We measured non‐verbal displays of mate retention tactics
Czech Republic and Department of (direct guarding, intimacy inducement, appearance enhancement, and love and care
Anthropology, Faculty of Humanities,
Charles University, José Martího 31, 162 52,
display), as well as relationship maintenance strategies of openness and positivity. We
Prague, Czech Republic. expected that relationship quality as reported by both partners in the couple would
Email: jitka.lindova@seznam.cz
be positively associated with ‘benefit‐provisioning’ mate retention behaviours and
Funding information
negatively associated with a ‘cost‐inflicting’ mate retention tactic (direct guarding).
The study was supported by the Czech
Science Foundation, grant number Relationship quality was assessed by Spanier's Dyadic Adjustment Scale using both
P407/17‐16622S. This publication was
the total score of dyadic adjustment and a Dyadic Satisfaction subscale. We found
further supported by project Nr. LO1611
with financial support from the Ministry a significant association between both partners' dyadic adjustment and satisfaction
of Education of the Czech Republic under
and woman's appearance enhancement behaviours (such as self‐touch, hair flip and
the NPU I programme; by the Ministry of
Education, Youth and Sports—Institutional primp) in the presence of her partner and a potential rival. We also found a nega‐
Support for Longterm Development of
tive association between partner's dyadic adjustment and woman's direct guarding
Research Organizations—Charles University,
Faculty of Humanities (Charles Univ, Fac of partner using short looks, and a positive association between woman's intimacy‐
Human 2017), and by Charles University
inducing behaviour using long gaze and her own dyadic satisfaction and her partner's
Research Centre programme, grant number
204056. dyadic adjustment. Our results highlight the specific role of female attractiveness in
later stages of romantic relationships within the context of mate retention and draw
Editor: C. Rutz
attention to the significance of the use of observational techniques in addition to
self‐reports.

KEYWORDS
appearance enhancement, dyadic satisfaction, glance, non‐verbal behaviour, relationship
maintenance, self‐touch

Ethology. 2019;00:1–15. © 2019 Blackwell Verlag GmbH |  1


wileyonlinelibrary.com/journal/eth  
|
2       LINDOVÁ et al.

1 |  I NTRO D U C TI O N Mortel, 2008). More recently, experimental research on perception


had inspired the notion of adaptive cognitive relationship maintenance
In all cultures, humans tend to form long‐term romantic relationships mechanisms. The ultimate function of such mechanisms can be, for in‐
(Benshoof & Thornhill, 1979). Within the context of evolutionary stance, to derogate the attractiveness of alternatives to current part‐
psychology, the term ‘mate retention tactics’ refers to a variety of ners (Karremans, Dotsch, & Corneille, 2011; Koranyi & Rothermund,
behavioural strategies whose function is to maintain the bond be‐ 2012; Lundström & Jones‐Gotman, 2009; Maner, Gailliot, & Miller,
tween romantic partners and to prevent partners from switching to 2009; Maner et al., 2008; Miller, 1997). So far, however, little attention
alternative mates (Buss, 1988a; Buss & Shackelford, 1997). has been paid to the expression or detection of mate retention tactics
Since the goal of mate retention tactics is to prevent partner's on the microlevel of non‐verbal behaviour of the partners. As far as we
infidelity or desertion, such behaviours are more frequent when an were able to find, the only pattern of non‐verbal behaviour that has
individual perceives an increased risk of partner's infidelity (Buss been studied in the context of mate retention is the display of love. In
& Shackelford, 1997). Mate retention tactics can take the form of evolutionary terms, love is viewed as an emotion that aids mate reten‐
‘benefit‐provisioning’ mate retention, which strengthens a relation‐ tion by displaying fidelity, sexual access and commitment (Buss, 2006).
ship, or ‘cost‐inflicting’ mate retention, which can be either intersex‐ Gonzaga, Keltner, Londahl, and Smith (2001) stress the specific func‐
ual, that is directed towards the romantic partner, or intra‐sexual, tion of love display in times of uncertainty, when it is supposed to reas‐
that is directed towards potential competitors (Buss, 1988a; Buss & sure the partner about one's own commitment. These authors further
Shackelford, 1997; Miner, Starratt, & Shackelford, 2009). The main bring some evidence that head nods, Duchenne smiles, gesturing and
intersexual mate retention tactics are as follows: (a) direct guarding; leaning towards a partner may constitute a display of love. They show
(b) negative inducements, such as threats of infidelity, mate's pun‐ that the experience of love and non‐verbal displays of love during a
ishment for the perceived threat of infidelity, emotional and com‐ positive interaction with a partner are reliable predictors of not only
mitment manipulation, or derogation of competitors; and (c) positive long‐term commitment and pro‐relationship behaviour (playful and
inducements in the form of presents/dinners, sexual inducements, constructive communication, enhancement of trust), but also of part‐
emphasis on love and caring, enhancements of physical appear‐ ners' behaviour in contexts where the relationship is threatened (con‐
ance, submission and debasement. Unlike the other tactics, positive structive conflict resolution, teasing).
inducements are ‘benefit‐provisioning’ mate retention strategies. In our study, we tested whether non‐verbal displays of a number
Intrasexual mate retention manipulations include (a) public signals of mate retention tactics and relationship maintenance strategies
of possession; (b) derogation of mate to rivals; (c) threats to rivals, can serve as indicators of relationship quality. Since mate retention
and even violence (Buss, 1988a). There is evidence that benefit‐pro‐ tactics are thought to be stimulated by a threat to the relationship
visioning mate retention tactics are positively associated with rela‐ in the form of an alternative mate (Buss & Shackelford, 1997; Maner
tionship satisfaction, while cost‐inflicting mate retention tactics are et al., 2008), couples participating in our study were exposed to an
negatively associated with it (Salkicevic, Stanic, & Grabovac, 2014). attractive female experimenter. This experimenter first individually
A similar concept, which originated in social psychology and was interviewed the female and the male of the couple, and then con‐
later developed also by evolutionary scientists, is the notion of rela‐ ducted with both partners an interview during which the couple's
tionship maintenance mechanisms (Maner, Rouby, & Gonzaga, 2008; behaviour was recorded. We opted for a set‐up with a female ex‐
Stafford & Canary, 1991), that is behaviours or cognitive mechanisms perimenter, whom we expected to be perceived as a potential threat
whose function is to preserve the well‐being of a long‐term couple to the relationship by the woman, rather than a male experimenter,
(Rusbult, Olsen, Davis, & Hannon, 2001). An influential typology of rela‐ because it has been found that during the initial phases of a rela‐
tionship maintenance strategies was developed through factor analysis tionship, women's non‐verbal signals play a more important role
by Stafford and Canary (1991), who identified the following relation‐ than men's non‐verbal signals. In particular, it has been shown that
ship maintenance strategies: positivity, openness, assurances, networks women use subtle non‐verbal displays to show their interest in a
and responsibilities. These behaviours can be applied on an everyday mate, whereas men tend to be more direct and verbal (Moore, 1995;
basis to maintain well‐being in relationships (Dindia & Baxter, 1987), but McCormick & Jones, 1983; Grammer, Honda, Juette, & Schmitt,
they are also supposed to play a role when a relationship is threatened 1999). Moreover, women are considered to be generally more non‐
(Maner et al., 2008) or a couple faces relationship problems (Dindia & verbally expressive than men (Hall, 1984; Fischer & LaFrance, 2015).
Baxter, 1987). The use of relationship maintenance behaviours cor‐ Accordingly, we hypothesised that women's mate retention strat‐
relates with relationship satisfaction and stability (Canary, Stafford, & egies might be based on less overt non‐verbal signals than men's
Semic, 2002; Rusbult et al., 2001; Weigel & Ballard‐Reisch, 1999, 2008). mate retention strategies. We limited our analyses to microscopic
Mate retention and relationship maintenance strategies have been behaviours that could be observed in this kind of laboratory settings.
studied mainly on the level of long‐term behavioural patterns and In particular, we measured the following displays of the mate reten‐
attitudes and identified based on verbal self‐reporting (Buss, 1988a; tion tactics: (a) appearance enhancement, (b) love and care displays,
Buss & Shackelford, 1997; Stafford, 2011; Stafford & Canary, 1991). (c) intimacy inducements, and (d) direct guarding. Additionally, we
It is, however, known that self‐reporting methods are susceptible to also observed the display of two relationship maintenance strate‐
various biases including social desirability and self‐awareness (Van de gies, namely (e) positivity and (f) openness. Our starting hypothesis
LINDOVÁ et al. |
      3

was that ‘benefit‐provisioning’ mate retention tactics (appearance from several phone calls leading up to the study. The experimenter
enhancement, love and care displays, and intimacy inducements), as was an attractive female (rated at the 75th percentile in a previous
well as positivity and openness displayed by women will be associ‐ study by Lindová et al., 2016). She was close in age to the average
ated with higher levels of adjustment and satisfaction in a couple. of female participants and wore no make‐up because it could influ‐
We further expected that a higher incidence of the ‘cost‐inflicting’ ence perception of her attractiveness (Batres et al., 2018). She in‐
mate retention tactic (direct guarding) by women will correlate with terviewed participants on subjects connected to relationships and
a lower level of dyadic adjustment and satisfaction. And finally, we sexuality. We analysed the behaviour of both partners during the
hypothesised that men belonging to better adjusted couples will en‐ couples' interview with the experimenter, which was held after par‐
gage in more displays of love directed at their partners as a strategy ticipant's individual interviews with the experimenter at the end of
to reassure a partner about their commitment. the testing session. The couple's interview was conducted under
standardised conditions, always in the same experimental room, with
the couple seated next to each other on a sofa and the experimenter
2 | M E TH O DS
in a chair opposite them and somewhat to the side (Figure 1). The
first two questions, chosen for relative emotional neutrality, were
2.1 | Participants
‘What made you take part in the study?’ and ‘Could you describe
The study included 47 childless heterosexual couples who had been your usual day?’ This interview was recorded with a hidden camera.
together for at least 3  years (M  =  5.3  years, SD  =  2  years) and co‐ After the interview, both partners were informed about the fact that
habited for a minimum of 6 months (M = 2.9 years, SD = 1.7 years). the couple's interview had been video recorded. We showed them
The age of individual partners ranged between 21 and 39  years the camera and the recording, and they were debriefed. Unless they
(x̄  = 26.4 years for women, SD = 3.4; x̄ = 27.3 years for men, SD = 3.8). expressed disagreement, they were asked to sign an informed con‐
Couples were recruited through fliers at 25 gynaecologists' offices sent form related to the use of this recording taken with a hidden
in Prague to participate in a larger study, the Intimate Behavior in camera for the purpose of the study.
Cohabiting Couples Project (for details, see Havlicek, Husarova, In case participants expressed disagreement with being re‐
Rezacova, & Klapilova, 2011). Participation was rewarded by 2000 corded, the recording was deleted in their presence.
CZK (88 USD). Participants signed an informed consent form, in The study procedure included a partial and temporary con‐
which they agreed to provide data on their relationship and sexual cealment of the focus of the study to participants, who were video
life, including their behaviour, attitudes and appearance, for scien‐ recorded by a hidden camera in order to observe their non‐verbal
tific use. A total of 48 couples were video recorded for the purpose response to a potential rival of the female partner. The design was
of this study, whereby one couple's data were later excluded after chosen to maintain spontaneity of participants' behaviour, and we
the couple decided to withdraw from the study. Ethical aspects believe that such a minimum level of concealment of the aims of the
of the study were approved by the Institutional Review Board of study was needed to obtain ecologically valid results.
Charles University, Faculty of Science (No. 2009/7). Sample size
used in this study was similar to earlier studies that used systematic
2.3 | Measures
behavioural observation. Given our sample size of 47 couples, we
performed a series of power analyses with different levels of ex‐
2.3.1 | Questionnaire
pected effect size values to estimate power of respective effects
according to Ackerman and Kenny (2016). Power estimates for the To measure partners' current relationship quality and satisfac‐
actor and partner effects of β = 0.3, 0.35 and 0.4 were 0.56, 0.74 and tion, we used Spanier's Dyadic Adjustment Scale (DAS; Spanier,
0.90, respectively. Therefore, we set the smallest effect size of our
interest to lie between β = 0.35 and 0.4.

2.2 | Procedure
Data for this study were collected as part of a three‐ to four‐hour‐
long testing session, which was part of the above‐mentioned larger
study. During the whole testing procedure, each partner sepa‐
rately completed several questionnaires, including inventories on
sexual behaviour and extrapair behaviour (Hurlbert Index of Sexual
Compatibility: Hurlbert, White, Powell, & Apt, 1993; Extramarital
Behavioral Intentions Scale: Buunk, 1982; Sociosexual Orientation
Inventory: Gangestad & Simpson, 1990; and Extra‐Dyadic Behavior
Experience Inventory: Havlicek et al., 2011) and had an individual F I G U R E 1   Scheme of participants' seating during the interview.
interview with the experimenter, who was known to the participants E, experimenter; P, participant
|
4       LINDOVÁ et al.

1976). This questionnaire consists of 32 items, and its total score The following two categories were formed so as to match the
(whereby higher scores indicate higher adjustment) is deter‐ content of Buss's (1988a) Sexual Inducement tactic. Since overt sex‐
mined by four factors: Dyadic Consensus (13 items), Affectional ual behaviour could not be expected in a laboratory, we chose two
Expression (4 items), Dyadic Satisfaction (10 items) and Dyadic categories of behaviour which literature describes as being used to
Cohesion (5 items). The DAS questionnaire was completed by both increase intimacy in a couple. McCormick and Jones (1983) mention
partners in each couple, and their individual scores were used in gazing at a partner as a light intimacy escalation display. The stron‐
subsequent analyses. In addition to using the DAS total score as gest escalating effect is, in their view, achieved by various forms
a reliable measure of relationship's overall quality, we also per‐ of partner touching. We have therefore classified a long glance at
formed models with the Dyadic Satisfaction subscale. We focused a partner as a subtle (I) and touching of a partner as an overt (II)
specifically on dyadic satisfaction so as to be better able to com‐ Intimacy Inducement.
pare our results with previous studies which used various meas‐ Direct Guarding was assessed by the frequency of short glances
ures of relationship satisfaction (e.g. Hooley & Hahlweg, 1989; at a partner, because it has been found that visual monitoring of a
Salkicevic et al., 2014; Weigel & Ballard‐Reisch, 2008). The DAS partner is one of the most frequent patterns of mate guarding (Buss,
total score measuring dyadic adjustment is not suitable for such 1988a).
comparison because it also includes assessment of agreement be‐ Openness was identified with an open upper body posture,
tween the partners, expressed intimacy and behaviour performed where arms are spread, close to the upper body, resting on a lap,
together. draped over the arm of a sofa, or resting on knees, including one arm
crossing the body (see Appendix 1). A previous study found that in
attractive opposite‐sex pairs, open body posture conveys receptiv‐
2.3.2 | Behavioural coding
ity/trust (Burgoon, 1991).
We coded three minutes of each video recording, starting with the Some of the measured behaviours may have other functions,
moment the couple was asked the first question. A fixed 3‐min time so that for instance a smile can be a display of love, of positivity,
interval was used to standardise the length of observation while or it can function as an intimacy inducement. To distinguish among
using a maximum of time when all couples were still answering the the various tactics such behaviours could belong to, particular be‐
above‐mentioned questions. A catalogue of 20 states (postures) and havioural displays were associated with only one tactic, namely that
acts was developed to code the participants' non‐verbal behaviour with the least equivocal function. Therefore, to measure Positivity (as
(see Appendix 1). These acts and postures represent microscopic opposed to negativity), we included only laughter (positively loading
displays of mate retention or relationship maintenance tactics la‐ on this variable), looking around (negatively loading) and a negative
belled Appearance Enhancement, Love and Care, Intimacy Inducements headshake (likewise negatively loading) (see Appendix 1 for an over‐
I and II, Direct Guarding, Positivity and Openness. This range of acts view of all behavioural measures).
and postures was chosen based on reviewed literature sources. A We paid special attention to objectiveness and reliability of cod‐
pilot inspection of 15 randomly chosen videos was performed to ing. In the next step, we have therefore carefully defined all codes
explore the variability of behaviours our subjects produced during (see Appendix 1 for all definitions). The codes were assigned to seg‐
the interview. This resulted in an elimination of behaviours which ments of the analysed video recordings with a temporal resolution
our subjects did not display and those that would be difficult to rec‐ of 33.33ms (one video frame). Coding was performed using Interact
ognise, and a further specification of definitions of the final set of 9 software. We measured the frequency of all behavioural acts as
behaviours selected for coding. well as the duration and frequency of body positions and looks at a
To capture non‐verbal displays of Appearance Enhancement, we partner. Each recording was coded separately for the male and the
measured self‐grooming, including self‐touch on thighs and upper female partner. The coding of one couple by one coder took approx‐
body except for face and hands, as well as primping and hair flip‐ imately 2.5 hr.
ping. McCormick & Jones (1983); see also Lockard & Adams ( 1980) All video recordings were coded in their entirety by the same
describe grooming escalation in connection with appearance en‐ female coder (JL) who was blind to all questionnaire scores of
hancement and identify it with behaviours such as hair smoothing the couples including relationship satisfaction scores, and had no
and self‐touching. Moore (1985) observed primping and hair‐flipping other experience with the behaviour or appearance of the couple
performed by women who are trying to attract the attention of po‐ aside from the 3‐min videos. She is experienced in coding and in
tential mates in a social setting. Self‐touches such as face scratch‐ the use of observational software. About one third of recordings
ing or hands rubbing, which are more typically used as adaptors and was also coded by a second female coder trained to 80% agree‐
perceived as cues to nervousness (Ekman & Friesen, 1972), were not ment. Data provided by the second coder were used only to cal‐
included. culate interrater agreement. The coding procedure was similar to
Observation of Love and Care displays was based on Gonzaga et other studies that used behavioural analysis (e.g. Bagner & Eyberg,
al. (2001), with focus on head nods, smiles and gesticulation. Leaning 2007; McMahon, Barnett, Kowalenko, & Tennant, 2006). The sec‐
towards a partner was not included because partners sat on a sofa ond coder's average Cohen's Kappa with the principal coder was
next to one another. 0.76 (range 0.67–0.84). The main points of disagreement between
LINDOVÁ et al. |
      5

the two coders were when either one coder coded a behaviour men and women within a dyad and estimated both the actor effects
which the other coder did not detect, or when one coder coded of independent predictors (effects of men's predictors on men's de‐
some behaviour as a single act while the other coded it as two pendent variable, and women's predictors on women's dependent
separate acts. variable) and the partner effects of independent predictors (effects
of men's predictors on women's dependent variable, and women's
predictors on men's dependent variable). Independence of predic‐
2.4 | Data analysis
tors was checked by a correlational analysis (see Appendix 2).
We calculated individual composite scores of Appearance Gender differences between the DAS total scores were com‐
Enhancement, Love and Care, Intimacy Inducement I and II, Direct puted using a paired t test for two dependent samples. Spearman's
Guarding, and Positivity by summing up the frequencies of all behav‐ correlation was used to assess the association of DAS total scores
iours belonging to the category in question (see Appendix 1 for a with age, education and between‐partner correlation in the DAS
Behavioural Catalogue). A composite score of Openness was calcu‐ total score.
lated by adding up the duration of all open upper body positions.
In the next step, we applied Actor‐Partner Interdependence
Models (APIMs) for dyadic data (Kenny, Kashy, & Cook, 2006) 3 | R E S U LT S
to identify the effect of actor and partner variables Appearance
Enhancement, Love and Care, Intimacy Inducement I and II, Direct DAS total scores were significantly higher in women than in men
Guarding, Positivity and Openness, and the covariate length of relation‐ (x̄ for men  =  118.3 and women  =  121.7; t [46]  =  −2.18, p  =  .035,
ship on both the DAS total score (APIM 1) and the Dyadic Satisfaction Cohen's d = 0.34). We found no significant correlation between the
(APIM 2). The APIM provides separate but simultaneous estimates DAS total score and age or education level in either sex (men/age: ρ
of actor and partner effects for members of a dyad (Ackerman, [47] = 0.13, p = .40; men/education: ρ [45] = 0.14, p = .36; women/
Donnellan, & Kashy, 2011). Our model thus distinguished between age: ρ [47] = −0.07, p = .65; women/education: ρ [45] = 0.19, p = .21).

TA B L E 1   Descriptive statistics of variables entered in APIM 1 and 2

Men Women
% behaviours of re‐
Variable x̄ SD x̄ SD spective variable

DAS total score 118.35 10.349 121.72 9.54  


Dyadic satisfaction 43.51 3.544 43.56 4.40  
Love and carea 19.55 10.16 21.28 9.61  
Smilea 7.53 3.8 8.38 3.93 39
Head noddinga 4.66 3.23 6.34 4.3 27
a
Illustrator 7.36 7.17 6.55 6.28 34
Appearance enhancement a 1.19 1.55 3.13    
a
Self‐touch 0.85 1.3 1.23 1.4 48
Hair flipa 0.04 0.29 0.66 0.94 16
a
Primp 0.30 0.81 1.23 1.86 36
Intimacy inducement Ia 3.60 3.228 4.979 3.48  
a
Intimacy inducement II 1.21 3.413 0.979 2.07  
Direct guardinga 10.30 6.178 8.596 4.34  
Positivitya −0.06 3.15 4.79    
Laughtera 2.72 1.94 7.13 4.17 66
Look arounda 1.55 1.91 1.45 1.82 20
Head shakea 1.23 1.75 0.89 1.24 14
b
Openness: Upper body 139.88 70.69 123.65 70.42  

  x̄ SD couple      
c
Length of relationship 63.0 24.2      
a
Number of behavioural acts.
b
Time in seconds.
c
Time in months.
|
6       LINDOVÁ et al.

DAS scores of partners within couples were significantly correlated within a dyad because the association between both their and their
(ρ [47] = 0.431, p = .002). partner's behaviour and their DAS total score is statistically different.
The means and standard deviations for all variables entered into Appearance Enhancement displayed by women had a positive
the APIM 1 and 2, as well as means and standard deviations for indi‐ effect on their own as well as their partner's Dyadic Adjustment
vidual behaviours included in these composite behavioural variables (p  =  .012 and .004, respectively; Figure 2a,b). We also found that
are listed in Table 1. Intimacy Inducement I as displayed by women had a positive effect
The R2 of the full APIM 1 was .188 for men, with men's DAS total on their own Dyadic Adjustment (p = .019; Figure 3a). The effect of
score as a dependent variable, men's behaviours as the actor vari‐ women's Intimacy Inducement I on their partner's Dyadic Adjustment
ables and women's behaviours as the partner variables. For women, did not reach the level of statistical significance (p = .057). The ef‐
2
the R of the full APIM 1 was .082, with women's DAS total score as fect of women's Openness on their own and their partner's Dyadic
a dependent variable, women's behaviours as the actor variables and Adjustment was not statistically significant (p  =  .098 and .072, re‐
men's behaviours as the partner variables. spectively). Similarly, the predicted negative effect of women's
Interaction between gender and the actor and partner effects, Direct Guarding on couple's Dyadic Adjustment did not reach the
2
respectively, was statistically significant in APIM 1 (χ [7]  =  15.8, level of statistical significance (actor effect: p = .095, partner effect:
p = .027; χ 2 [7] = 17.6, p = .014; a combined test of interactions be‐ p = .090). Women's displays of Love and Care, Intimacy Inducement II
tween gender and both the actor and partner effect: χ 2 [14] = 32.8, and Positivity did not have a significant effect on either their part‐
p < .003). This indicates that men and women should be distinguished ner's or their own Dyadic Adjustment, and no behavioural displays

F I G U R E 2   Relationship between women's Appearance Enhancement behaviours and their own and their partner's Dyadic Adjustment
score and Dyadic Satisfaction score
LINDOVÁ et al. |
      7

F I G U R E 3   Relationship between women's Sexual Inducement I and their Dyadic Adjustment score and their partner's Dyadic Satisfaction
score and relationship between women's Direct Guarding behaviours and their partner's Dyadic Satisfaction score

observed in men had a statistically significant effect on either their women had a significant effect on their partner's Dyadic Satisfaction
own or their partner's Dyadic Adjustment (Table 2). (p = .006; Figure 3b). Finally, we found a significant negative effect
The R2 of the full APIM 2 was .235 for men (with men's Dyadic of women's Direct Guarding on their partner's Dyadic Satisfaction
Satisfaction as a dependent variable and men's behaviour as an actor (p  =  .043; Figure 3c). Women's displays of Love and Care, Intimacy
and women's behaviour as a partner variable) and .129 for women Inducement II, Positivity and Openness did not have a significant ef‐
(with women's Dyadic Satisfaction as a dependent variable and fect on their partner's or their own Dyadic Satisfaction. Similarly as
women's behaviour as an actor and men's behaviour as a partner in APIM 1, behavioural displays observed in men had no effect on
variable). their partner's or their own Dyadic Satisfaction (Table 3).
In APIM 2, interaction between gender and actor effects was An overview of standardised effects of actor and partner vari‐
not statistically significant (χ 2 [7]  =  11.1, p  =  .133), but interaction ables and the covariate on Dyadic Satisfaction in men and women is
between gender and partner effects did reach the level of statistical given in Figures 4 and 5, respectively.
2
significance (χ [7] = 17.6, p = .014), and the same holds of the in‐
teraction between gender and both the actor and partner effect (χ 2
[14] = 26.6, p = .022). 4 | D I S CU S S I O N
This analysis has also shown a statistically significant effect
of Appearance Enhancement displayed by women on their own The main aim of the current study was to investigate possible as‐
and their partner's Dyadic Satisfaction (p  =  .045 and .007, respec‐ sociations between non‐verbal displays of mate retention per‐
tively; Figure 2c,d). Likewise, Intimacy Inducement I as displayed by formed by women when a couple was interviewed by an attractive
|
8       LINDOVÁ et al.

TA B L E 2   Results of Actor–Partner Interdependence Model 1 establishing the effect of observational actor and partner variables and
covariates on a Dyadic Adjustment Scale

Full model
DAS total
prediction Men Women  
2
R 0.188 0.082 Combined sample    

d (actor d (partner
Partner ef‐ Partner effect effects effects
Actor effect fect (W–M) Actor effect (M to W) Actor effect Partner effect M–W) M–W)

  Estimate Estimate Estimate Estimate Estimate Estimate Z Z


a ** *
Appear. Enh 0.373 1.885   1.597   −0.082 0.985 0.901 1.096 −1.799† 
Love & Carea 0.217 −0.083 0.063 0.180 0.140 0.048d −0.692 1.191
Intimacy Ind. Ia −0.862 0.922†  1.123*  0.043 0.131 0.482 2.678**  −1.213
a
Intimacy Ind. II −0.033 −0.562 −0.069 −0.087 −0.051 −0.324 −0.038 0.497
Direct Guard. a 0.062 −0.715†  −0.686†  0.309 −0.312 −0.203 −1.405 1.897† 
Positivitya −0.317 −0.189 −0.129 0.203 −0.223 0.007 0.290 0.610
Opennessb −0.025 0.048†  −0.043†  −0.021 −0.034 0.014 −0.536 −1.909† 
Length of Rel.c −0.003 −0.121†  −0.062 −1.702† 

Note: Entries to individual predictors are estimates obtained by generalised least squares analysis with correlated errors and restricted maximum
likelihood estimation.
Abbreviations: M, men; W, women.
a
Number of behavioural acts.
b
Time in seconds.
c
Time in months.

< 0.1.
*< 0.05.
**< 0.01.

opposite‐sex experimenter and the couple's adjustment and satis‐ corresponds to predictions made on the basis of our study's design,
faction. In accordance with our predictions, we found an association which aimed at creating a potential relationship‐threatening situa‐
between non‐verbal displays of appearance enhancement in women tion for the female partner.
(including self‐touch, hair flip and primping) and both their own and We confirmed a higher frequency of appearance enhancement
their male partners' satisfaction and adjustment in their romantic in the presence of a potential female rival in those women who be‐
relationship. We also found an association between women's inti‐ longed to better adjusted and more satisfied couples. Several au‐
macy inducements (in the form of a long glance at a partner) and thors found that appearance enhancement is relatively frequently
their dyadic satisfaction, as well as their partners' dyadic adjust‐ and effectively used by women, both in the context of mate selec‐
ment. Furthermore, we confirmed a negative association between tion and intrasexual competition (Buss, 1988b; Fisher & Cox, 2010),
women's direct guarding by visual monitoring and their partners' and in connection with mate retention (Buss, 1988a). Such results
dyadic satisfaction. And finally, we detected a predicted but statisti‐ are in accordance with findings to the effect that physical attrac‐
cally non‐significant association between women's postural open‐ tiveness is among the main characteristics that men value in women
ness and their partners' relationship satisfaction. (Buss, 1987; Buunk, Dijkstra, Fetchenhauer, & Kenrick, 2002). For
Contrary to our prediction, we found no significant relation be‐ high‐quality couples, direct confrontation with a potential female
tween the frequency of displays of love and care (loaded by head rival may specifically trigger in women the use of appearance‐en‐
nods, smiles and gesticulation) by either partner and the couple's hancing behaviours in the sense of a mate retention tactic. Based on
dyadic adjustment or satisfaction. Similarly, neither non‐verbal be‐ our results, however, we cannot exclude an alternative explanation,
haviours of positivity or negativity (such as laughter, looking around namely that appearance enhancement is used by women non‐spe‐
or negative head shaking), nor advanced intimacy inducements cifically to benefit the relationship across various contexts. Although
(partner touching) correlated with the dyadic adjustment or satis‐ Miner et al. (2009) include appearance enhancement among the
faction of the partners. Overall, women's behaviour was markedly benefit‐provisioning tactics which reduce a risk of partner infi‐
more strongly associated with both their own and their partners' delity by increasing partner's dyadic satisfaction, but they do not
dyadic adjustment and satisfaction than men's behaviour, which bring any empirical evidence for associating this tactic with dyadic
LINDOVÁ et al. |
      9

TA B L E 3   Results of the Actor–Partner Interdependence Model 2 establishing the effect of observational actor and partner variables and
covariates on a Dyadic Satisfaction subscale
Full Model
Dyad. Satisf.
prediction Men Women  
2
R 0.126 0.293 Combined sample  

d (actor d (partner
Partner effect Partner effect effects effects
Actor effect (W–M) Actor effect (M–W) Actor effect Partner effect M–W) M–W)

  Estimate Estimate Estimate Estimate Estimate Estimate Z Z


a ** *
Appear. Enh. 0.011 0.581   0.569   −0.074 0.290 0.253 1.347 −1.414
a
Love & care 0.088 −0.067 0.023 −0.167 0.056 −0.008 −0.750 1.313
Intimacy Ind. Ia −0.172 0.451**  0.287 −0.105 0.057 0.173 1.611 −1.945
Intimacy Ind. 0.078 −0.344 0.034 −0.044 0.056 −0.194 −0.111 0.867
IIa
Direct guard. a 0.063 −0.286*  −0.264 0.018 −0.100 −0.134 −1.511 1.501
a
Positivity −0.169 0.132 0.084 0.279 −0.043 0.205 1.063 0.554
Opennessb −0.003 0.012 −0.006 −0.010 −0.004 0.001 −0.217 −1.524
Length of Rel.c −0.015   −0.090**    −0.052*    −2.473*   

Note: The entries to individual predictors are estimates obtained by the generalised least squares analysis with correlated errors and restricted maxi‐
mum likelihood estimation.
Abbreviations: Dyad Sat., Dyadic Satisfaction score; M, men; W, women.
a
Number of behavioural acts.
b
Time in seconds.
c
Time in months.

<0.1.
*<0.05.
**<0.01.

F I G U R E 4   Effects of one's own (actor) behaviour, partner's behaviour and covariates on the Dyadic Satisfaction of men.
Note: Entries to the individual variables are betas obtained in the APIM 2 using standard deviation for men only. W, women; M, men.
Statistically significant results are shown in bold
|
10       LINDOVÁ et al.

F I G U R E 5   Effects of one's own (actor) behaviour, partner's behaviour and covariates on the Dyadic Satisfaction of women.
Note: Entries to the individual variables are betas obtained in the APIM 2 using standard deviation for women only. W, women; M, men.
Statistically significant results are shown in bold

satisfaction. De Miguel and Buss (2011), on the other hand, recently on the other hand, was negatively related to their partner's satis‐
concluded in their self‐report based study that appearance enhance‐ faction, and the same trend, although only approaching the level of
ment is related to a higher relationship commitment. Our results in‐ statistical significance, was observed for partner's dyadic adjust‐
dicate that there exists not only an association between women's ment. This corresponds to a categorisation proposed by Miner et al.
appearance enhancement and their partners' dyadic satisfaction and (2009), where intimacy inducement is classified as a benefit‐provi‐
adjustment, but also a similarly strong association between such be‐ sioning (i.e. relationship satisfaction increasing) tactic, while direct
haviour and women's own dyadic satisfaction and adjustment. guarding is listed among cost‐inflicting mate retention strategies. A
The mate retention tactic of Appearance Enhancement was fol‐ contrast between a positive association of dyadic satisfaction with
lowed on a microlevel of non‐verbal behaviour as the frequency of benefit‐provisioning mate retention tactics (positive inducements
self‐touching of specific body parts, such as the torso and the thighs, and public signals of possession) and a negative association of dyadic
including manipulation with hair and clothing. In earlier studies, this satisfaction with cost‐inflicting mate retention tactics has also been
behaviour was usually studied in the context of mate choice and reported by other authors (such as Salkicevic et al., 2014). Although
mate attraction rather than mate retention. For instance, Moore we are not aware of any study focused specifically on the opposite
(1985) includes various types of touching one's own body, hair and effect of short versus long look in terms of mate retention effective‐
clothing among non‐verbal courtship signals used by women. One ness, studies from other areas of psychology do provide evidence
of the two main functions of courtship displays performed during about the different effects of short and long looks in interpersonal
relationship formation is to draw attention to one's own attractive contexts (Argyle, Lefebvre, & Cook, 1974; Bente, Eschenburg, &
characteristics (Givens, 1978), that is to promote or enhance one's Krämer, 2007). While a long gaze is associated with liking and ro‐
appearance. It seems that a similar pattern of behaviour is a rela‐ mantic interest from the perspective of both the actor and the per‐
tively frequent and efficient tactic to secure a preferred mate. ceiver (Exline, 1963; Grammer, Kruck, Juette, & Fink, 2000; Kleinke,
Among other mate retention behaviours, we found some evi‐ Bustos, Meeker, & Staneski, 1973; Kleinke, Meeker, & Fong, 1974;
dence of a positive association between women's intimacy induce‐ Renninger, Wade, & Grammer, 2004; Russo, 1975; but see Noller,
ment (by long looks at a partner) and dyadic adjustment in women 1980), a short gaze is perceived as less friendly and more assertive
themselves, as well as between women's intimacy inducement and (Bente et al., 2007). It functions as an instrument of visual monitoring
dyadic satisfaction of their male partners. Nevertheless, these re‐ that is likely to increase vigilance and may even be stressful for the
sults are somewhat specific for the scale used, because correlations actor himself or herself (Warm, Parasuraman, & Matthews, 2008).
between women's Intimacy Inducement I and either their own dyadic Contrary to our expectations, we found no significant association
satisfaction or their male partners' dyadic adjustment were non‐sig‐ between advanced intimacy inducement by touching a partner and
nificant. Women's guarding (monitoring) of a partner via short looks, the level of dyadic adjustment and satisfaction. Such an association
LINDOVÁ et al. |
      11

was expected, because Buss and Shackelford (1997) reported that similar in size to samples used in similar studies that worked with
to retain their mate, both husbands and wives report that they often microanalysis of behaviour (Farley, 2014; Gonzaga et al., 2001;
touch their mate when a competitor is in close proximity, and several Hall & Xing, 2015; Renninger et al., 2004; Vacharkulksemsuk et
other studies found a positive association between relationship sat‐ al., 2016; Van der Meij, Almela, Buunk, Almela, Buunk, Fawcett, &
isfaction and a positive physical touch (Margolin & Wampold, 1981; Salvador, 2011)—was still relatively small and may have failed to
Revenstorf, Hahlweg, Schindler, & Vogel, 1984). Absence of such an confirm the statistical significance of effects, which are of merely
association among our findings may be due to the laboratory set‐ a moderate strength. Aside from that, the use of one single experi‐
tings: partners may have refrained from touching one another be‐ menter who conducted all the interviews with all test couples may
cause they viewed such behaviour as neither quite appropriate and have led to some experimenter‐specific effects. We focused on
nor as pleasant as in more private settings. In our experiment, the the behavioural interaction between the partners and not on the
incidence of women touching their male partners was relatively low behaviour of participants towards the experimenter who was sup‐
(less than 1 touch per female partner) and the variability of data was posed to serve as standard stimulus who elicits in the participants
therefore likewise low. a specific interpretation of the situation. It is, however, possible
In contrast to our prediction, we also found no statistically sig‐ that the experimenter was more likely to trigger certain specific
nificant association between men's displays of love by head nods, behavioural patterns in the participating dyads than another ex‐
gestures, and smiles and dyadic satisfaction or adjustment in either perimenter would have done, and that with multiple experiment‐
partner. This association was expected based on Gonzaga et al. ers, the couple's behaviours would have been more varied. The
(2001) who suggest that an important function of love displays is generalisation potential of our results may thus be somewhat lim‐
to reassure a partner about one's commitment in situations where ited in this respect. Future studies should employ several inter‐
the partner may feel insecure. On the other hand, because the men viewers of varying attractiveness.
in our study were interacting with another female within the frame‐ Moreover, our study focused specifically on women's behaviour.
work of our experimental design, it is possible they felt no need to A similar study aimed at eliciting a mild perception of infidelity treat
reassure their partners of their commitment or were even unaware in men would highlight the gender specifics of mate retention be‐
that their partner might perceive the situation as a threat to their haviours. Our design had also prevented us from drawing any con‐
relationship. clusions about causal relationships. In particular, while it is possible
Furthermore, we were not able to confirm the frequently re‐ that some behavioural mate retention tactics do influence dyadic
ported association between positive/negative non‐verbal be‐ adjustment and satisfaction, it may also be the case that dyadic ad‐
haviours and relationship quality (Birchler, Clopton, & Adams, 1984; justment and satisfaction manifests itself through specific mate re‐
Gottman, Markman, & Notarius, 1977; Hooley & Hahlweg, 1989; tention behaviours. To address this issue, a longitudinal study would
Schaap & Jansen‐Nawas, 1989). It may be that relationship mainte‐ have to be performed that would allow for an observation of the
nance strategies in general, or positivity and openness in particu‐ effects of mate retention tactics employed in the initial phases and
lar, function better in a preventive and long‐term context and are then throughout the relationship on the long‐term progress of the
not employed in the specific situation of the presence of a potential relationship.
rival. Alternatively, one might hypothesise that these strategies can To conclude, this study yielded some evidence that non‐ver‐
be identified only on a verbal level, as was the case in earlier studies bal displays of several mate retention behaviours by women in
(Canary & Stafford, 1992), or that we did not choose suitable non‐ situations where both their partner and a potential female rival
verbal displays. are present are related to dyadic adjustment and satisfaction of
The main limitations of our design lie in the likely variability romantic couples. Our results highlight the role of non‐verbal
of participants' subjective perception and the emotions they ex‐ behaviour in mate retention, whereby two main patterns were
perienced during the study. Our intention was to create a situa‐ observed. First of all, our results point to the importance of attrac‐
tion which the female participants would perceive as a moderate tion and attractiveness in established relationships, that is under
infidelity threat, process it largely implicitly and correspondingly circumstances where—in contrast to mate‐choice contexts, where
respond on the level of non‐verbal, rather than verbal, behaviour. this is treated as being of key importance—the role of appearance
It was, however, likely that the perception of threat would to seems to be somewhat overlooked. Secondly, our study confirmed
some extent vary, which may also be why a response in terms of a hypothesis that looking at a partner can serve contrasting func‐
increased mate retention behaviours was not observed in all fe‐ tions depending on the length of the action, most likely spanning
male participants. Besides variations in female participants' mate from visual monitoring to intimacy inducement. In particular, we
value and their partners' level of commitment, another possible have shown that in the presence of a potential rival, women give
source of variance may have been linked to the experimenter's their partners different kinds of looks depending on whether the
non‐standardised clothing (Lennon, 1990). She tried to maintain a couple is well‐functioning or distressed.
certain style but had to adjust her attire depending on the changes Our results add to a growing body of knowledge on adaptive be‐
in outside and inside temperatures during the year. Another pos‐ havioural mechanisms known as mate retention tactics. These mech‐
sible limitation of our study is that our study sample—although anisms, such as appearance enhancement, intimacy inducement or
|
12       LINDOVÁ et al.

guarding of partner, though most likely not fully consciously per‐ Benshoof, L., & Thornhill, R. (1979). The evolution of monogamy and con‐
formed, seem to be consistently associated with relationship quality. cealed ovulation in humans. Journal of Social and Biological Structures,
2(2), 95–106. https​://doi.org/10.1016/0140-1750(79)90001-0
It is, however, also worth noting that our results show a somewhat
Bente, G., Eschenburg, F., & Krämer, N. C. (2007). Virtual gaze. A pilot
counterintuitive lack of significance of positive, loving and negative study on the effects of computer simulated gaze in avatar‐based con‐
behaviours for assessing relationship quality. These negative results, versations. In R. Shumaker (Ed.), Virtual Reality ICVR 2007. Lecture
as well as the whole pattern of findings, could be of special interest Notes in Computer Science, 4563. Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer. https​
://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-540-73335-5_21
to counsellors and clinicians who seek ways to identify relationship
Birchler, G. R., Clopton, P. L., & Adams, N. L. (1984). Marital conflict
dysfunction based on directly observable behavioural data. While resolution: Factors influencing concordance between partners and
on the one hand, we show that micro‐observation of behaviour trained coders. The American Journal of Family Therapy, 12(2), 15–28.
confirms the existence of some phenomena known from self‐report https​://doi.org/10.1080/01926​18840​8250166
Burgoon, J. K. (1991). Relational message interpretations of touch, con‐
studies, this level of analysis also hints at a substantially different
versational distance, and posture. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior,
picture of mate retention behaviour than verbal reporting on its own 15(4), 233–259. https​://doi.org/10.1007/BF009​86924​
tends to yield. Buss, D. M. (1987). Sex differences in human mate selection criteria:
An evolutionary perspective. In C. Crawford, M. Smith, & D. Krebs
(Eds.), Sociobiology and Psychology: Ideas, Issues, and Applications
AC K N OW L E D G E M E N T S (pp. 335–352). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Buss, D. M. (1988a). From vigilance to violence: Tactics of mate re‐
We would like to thank all the study participants. We are also in‐ tention. Ethology and Sociobiology, 9(5), 291–317. https​://doi.
debted to Barbara Husárová, Jana Pelechová, Kristyna Polášková, org/10.1016/0162-3095(88)90010-6
Jana Šelová, Monika Ubryová and Jan Vávra for their help with data Buss, D. M. (1988b). The evolution of human intrasexual competition:
Tactics of mate attraction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
collection. Our special thanks go to Lenka Martinec Nováková for
54(4), 616–628. https​://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.54.4.616
her statistical advice, Anna Pilátová for language editing, and to the Buss, D. M. (2006). The evolution of love. In R. J. Sternberg, & K. Weis
reviewers for their advice. (Eds.), The New Psychology of Love (pp. 65–85). New Haven, UK: Yale
University Press.
Buss, D. M., & Shackelford, T. K. (1997). From vigilance to violence: Mate reten‐
C O N FL I C T O F I N T E R E S T tion tactics in married couples. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
72(2), 346–361. https​://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.72.2.346
The authors declare no conflict of interest. Buunk, B. (1982). Anticipated sexual jealousy: Its relationship to selfes‐
teem, dependency, and reciprocity. Personality and Social Psychology
Bulletin, 8, 310–316. https​://doi.org/10.1177/01461​67282​0 82019
ORCID Buunk, B. P., Dijkstra, P., Fetchenhauer, D., & Kenrick, D. T. (2002). Age
and gender differences in mate selection criteria for various in‐
Jitka Lindová  https://orcid.org/0000-0001-8846-4552 volvement levels. Personal Relationships, 9(3), 271–278. https​://doi.
org/10.1111/1475-6811.00018​
Kateřina Klapilová  https://orcid.org/0000-0002-2972-0111
Canary, D. J., & Stafford, L. (1992). Relational maintenance strategies
Jan Havlíček  https://orcid.org/0000-0003-2374-7337 and equity in marriage. Communications Monographs, 59(3), 243–267.
https​://doi.org/10.1080/03637​75920​9376268
Canary, D. J., Stafford, L., & Semic, B. A. (2002). A panel study of the
associations between maintenance strategies and relational charac‐
REFERENCES
teristics. Journal of Marriage and Family, 64(2), 395–406. https​://doi.
Ackerman, R. A., Donnellan, M. B., & Kashy, D. A. (2011). Working with org/10.1111/j.1741-3737.2002.00395.x
dyadic data in studies of emerging adulthood: Specific recommen‐ de Miguel, A., & Buss, D. M. (2011). Mate retention tactics in Spain: Personality,
dations, general advice, and practical tips. In F. D. Fincham, & M. Cui sex differences, and relationship status. Journal of Personality, 79(3),
(Eds.), Romantic relationships in emerging adulthood (pp. 67–97). New 563–586. https​://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.2011.00698.x
York, NY: Cambridge University Press. Dindia, K., & Baxter, L. A. (1987). Strategies for maintaining and repairing
Ackerman, R. A., & Kenny, D. A. (2016). APIMPower: An interactive tool for marital relationships. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 4(2),
Actor‐Partner Interdependence Model power analysis [Computer software]. 143–158. https​://doi.org/10.1177/02654​07587​0 42003
Available from https​://robert-a-acker​man.shiny​apps.io/apimp​ower/ Ekman,P.,&Friesen,W.V.(1972).Handmovements.TheJournalofCommunication,
Argyle, M., Lefebvre, L., & Cook, M. (1974). The meaning of five patterns 22(4), 353–374. https​://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-2466.1972.tb001​63.x
of gaze. European Journal of Social Psychology, 4(2), 125–136. https​:// Exline, R. V. (1963). Explorations in the process of person perception: Visual
doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.24200​4 0202​ interaction in relation to competition, sex, and need for affiliation1. Journal
Bagner, D. M., & Eyberg, S. M. (2007). Parent–child interaction therapy for of Personality, 31(1), 1–20. https​://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.1963.
disruptive behavior in children with mental retardation: A randomized tb018​36.x
controlled trial. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 36(3), Farley, S. D. (2014). Nonverbal reactions to an attractive stranger: The role of
418–429. https​://doi.org/10.1080/15374​41070​1448448 mimicry in communicating preferred social distance. Journal of Nonverbal
Batres, C., Russell, R., Simpson, J. A., Campbell, L., Hansen, A. M., & Behavior, 38(2), 195–208. https​://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-014-0174-4
Cronk, L. (2018). Evidence that makeup is a false signal of sociosexu‐ Fisher, M., & Cox, A. (2010). Four strategies used during intrasexual com‐
ality. Personality and Individual Differences, 122, 148–154. https​://doi. petition for mates. Personal Relationships, 18(1), 20–38. https​://doi.
org/10.1016/j.paid.2017.10.023 org/10.1111/j.1475-6811.2010.01307.x
LINDOVÁ et al. |
      13

Fischer, A., & LaFrance, M. (2015). What drives the smile and the tear: Lindová, J., Little, A., Havlicek, J., Roberts, S. C., Rubešová, A., & Flegr, J.
Why women are more emotionally expressive than men. Emotion (2016). Effect of partnership status on preferences for facial self‐re‐
Review, 7(1), 22–29. https​://doi.org/10.1177/17540​73914​544406 semblance. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 869. https​://doi.org/10.3389/
Gangestad, S. W., & Simpson, J. A. (1990). Toward an evolutionary his‐ fpsyg.2016.00869​
tory of female socio‐sexual variation. Journal of Personality, 58, Lockard, J. S., & Adams, R. M. (1980). Courtship behaviors in public:
69–96. https​://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.1990.tb009​0 8.x Different age/sex roles. Ethology and Sociobiology, 1(3), 245–253.
Givens, D. B. (1978). The nonverbal basis of attraction: Flirtation, https​://doi.org/10.1016/0162-3095(80)90011-4
courtship, and seduction. Psychiatry, 41, 346–359. https​://doi. Lundström, J. N., & Jones‐Gotman, M. (2009). Romantic love modulates
org/10.1080/00332​747.1978.11023994 women's identification of men's body odors. Hormones and Behavior,
Gonzaga, G. C., Keltner, D., Londahl, E. A., & Smith, M. D. (2001). Love 55(2), 280–284. https​://doi.org/10.1016/j.yhbeh.2008.11.009
and the commitment problem in romantic relations and friend‐ Maner, J. K., Gailliot, M. T., & Miller, S. L. (2009). The implicit cognition
ship. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81(2), 247–262. of relationship maintenance: Inattention to attractive alternatives.
https​://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.81.2.247 Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45(1), 174–179. https​://doi.
Gottman, J. M., Markman, H., & Notarius, C. (1977). Topography of org/10.1016/j.jesp.2008.08.002
marital conflict– Sequential analysis of verbal and nonverbal be‐ Maner, J. K., Rouby, D. A., & Gonzaga, G. C. (2008). Automatic inatten‐
havior. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 39, 461–477. https​://doi. tion to attractive alternatives: The evolved psychology of relation‐
org/10.2307/350902 ship maintenance. Evolution and Human Behavior, 29(5), 343–349.
Grammer, K., Honda, M., Juette, A., & Schmitt, A.. (1999). Fuzziness of https​://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolh​umbeh​av.2008.04.003
nonverbal courtship communication unblurred by motion energy de‐ Margolin, G., & Wampold, B. E. (1981). Sequential‐analysis of conflict
tection. Journal of personality and social psychology, 77(3), 487. http:// and accord in distressed and non‐distressed marital partners. Journal
dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.77.3.487 of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 49(4), 554–567. https​://doi.
Grammer, K., Kruck, K., Juette, A., & Fink, B. (2000). Non‐verbal behav‐ org/10.1037/0022-006x.49.4.554
ior as courtship signals: The role of control and choice in selecting McCormick, N. B., & Jones, A. J. (1983). Gender differences in nonverbal
partners. Evolution and Human Behavior, 21(6), 371–390. https​://doi. flirtation. Journal of Sex Education and Therapy, 15(4), 271–282. https​
org/10.1016/S1090-5138(00)00053-2 ://doi.org/10.1080/01614​576.1989.11074969
Hall, J. A. (1984). Nonverbal sex differences: Communication accuracy and McMahon, C. A., Barnett, B., Kowalenko, N. M., & Tennant, C. C.
expressive style. Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins University Press. (2006). Maternal attachment state of mind moderates the im‐
Hall, J. A., & Xing, C. (2015). The verbal and nonverbal correlates of the pact of postnatal depression on infant attachment. Journal of
five flirting styles. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 39(1), 41–68. https​ Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47(7), 660–669. https​://doi.
://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-014-0199-8 org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2005.01547.x
Havlicek, J., Husarova, B., Rezacova, V., & Klapilova, K. (2011). Correlates Miller, R. S. (1997). Inattentive and contented: Relationship commitment
of extra‐dyadic sex in Czech heterosexual couples: Does sexual be‐ and attention to alternatives. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
havior of parents matter? Archives of Sexual Behavior, 40, 1153–1163. 73(4), 758–766. https​://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.73.4.758
https​://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-011-9869-3 Miner, E. J., Starratt, V. G., & Shackelford, T. K. (2009). It's not all
Hooley, J. M., & Hahlweg, K. (1989). Marital satisfaction and marital com‐ about her: Men's mate value and mate retention. Personality and
munication in German and English couples. Behavioral Assessment, Individual Differences, 47(3), 214–218. https​://doi.org/10.1016/j.
11, 119–133. paid.2009.03.002
Hurlbert, D. F., White, L. C., Powell, R. D., & Apt, C. (1993). Orgasm Moore, M. M. (1985). Nonverbal courtship patterns in women: Context
consistency training in the treatment of women reporting hy‐ and consequences. Ethology and Sociobiology, 6(4), 237–247. https​://
poactive sexual desire: An outcome comparison of women‐ doi.org/10.1016/0162-3095(85)90016-0
only groups and couples‐only groups. Journal of Behavior Moore, M. M. (1995). Courtship signaling and adolescents: Girls just
Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 24(1), 3–13. https​://doi. wanna have fun? Journal of Sex Research, 32(4), 319–328. https​://doi.
org/10.1016/0005-7916(93)90003-F org/10.1080/00224​49950​9551805
Karremans, J. C., Dotsch, R., & Corneille, O. (2011). Romantic relation‐ Noller, P. (1980). Gaze in married couples. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior,
ship status biases memory of faces of attractive opposite‐sex oth‐ 5(2), 115–129. https​://doi.org/10.1007/BF009​86514​
ers: Evidence from a reverse‐correlation paradigm. Cognition, 121(3), Renninger, L. A., Wade, T. J., & Grammer, K. (2004). Getting that female
422–426. https​://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogni​tion.2011.07.008 glance: Patterns and consequences of male nonverbal behavior in
Kenny, D. A., Kashy, D. A., Cook, W. L., & Simpson, J. A. (2006). Dyadic courtship contexts. Evolution and Human Behavior, 25(6), 416–431.
data analysis (Methodology in the social sciences). New York, NY: https​://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolh​umbeh​av.2004.08.006
Guilford. Revenstorf, D., Hahlweg, K., Schindler, L., & Vogel, B. (1984). Interaction
Kleinke, C. L., Bustos, A. A., Meeker, F. B., & Staneski, R. A. (1973). Effects of analysis of marital conflict. In K. Hahlweg, & N. S. Jacobson (Eds.),
self‐attributed and other‐attributed gaze on interpersonal evaluations Marital interaction: Analysis and modification (pp. 121–183). New York,
between males and females. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, NY: Guilford Press.
9(2), 154–163. https​://doi.org/10.1016/0022-1031(73)90007-3 Rusbult, C. E., Olsen, N., Davis, J. L., & Hannon, P. A. (2001). Commitment
Kleinke, C. L., Meeker, F. B., & La Fong, C. (1974). Effects of gaze, touch, and use of and relationship maintenance mechanisms. In J. H. Harvey, & A.
name on evaluation of “engaged” couples. Journal of Research in Personality, Wenzel (Eds.), Close romantic relationships: Maintenance and enhance‐
7(4), 368–373. https​://doi.org/10.1016/0092-6566(74)90058-0 ment (pp. 87–113). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Koranyi, N., & Rothermund, K. (2012). When the grass on the other Russo, N. F. (1975). Eye contact, interpersonal distance, and the equilib‐
side of the fence doesn't matter: Reciprocal romantic interest neu‐ rium theory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 31(3), 497–
tralizes attentional bias towards attractive alternatives. Journal 502. https​://doi.org/10.1037/h0076476
of Experimental Social Psychology, 48(1), 186–191. https​://doi. Salkicevic, S., Stanic, A. L., & Grabovac, M. T. (2014). Good mates re‐
org/10.1016/j.jesp.2011.06.012 tain us right: Investigating the relationship between mate retention
Lennon, S. J. (1990). Effects of clothing attractiveness on perceptions. strategies, mate value, and relationship satisfaction. Evolutionary
Home Economics Research Journal, 18(4), 303–310. https​://doi. Psychology, 12(5), 147470491401200. https​://doi.org/10.1177/14747​
org/10.1177/10777​27X90​01800403 04914​01200512
|
14       LINDOVÁ et al.

Schaap, C., & Jansen‐Nawas, C. (1989). Marital interaction, affect and Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 279(1726), 202–208. https​://doi.
conflict resolution. Sexual and Marital Therapy, 2(1), 35–51. https​:// org/10.1098/rspb.2011.0764
doi.org/10.1080/02674​65870​8 407736 Warm, J. S., Parasuraman, R., & Matthews, G. (2008). Vigilance requires
Spanier, G. B. (1976). Measuring dyadic adjustment: New scales for as‐ hard mental work and is stressful. Human Factors: the Journal of the
sessing the quality of marriage and similar dyads. Journal of Marriage Human Factors and Ergonomics Society, 50(3), 433–441. https​://doi.
and the Family, 38, 15–28. https​://doi.org/10.2307/350547 org/10.1518/00187​2008X​312152
Stafford, L. (2011). Measuring relationship maintenance behaviors: Weigel, D. J., & Ballard‐Reisch, D. S. (1999). How couples maintain mar‐
Critique and development of the revised relationship maintenance riages: A closer look at self and spouse influences upon the use of
behavior scale. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 28(2), maintenance behaviors in marriages. Family Relations, 48(3), 263–
278–303. https​://doi.org/10.1177/02654​07510​378125 269, https​://doi.org/10.2307/585635
Stafford, L., & Canary, D. J. (1991). Maintenance strategies and roman‐ Weigel, D. J., & Ballard‐Reisch, D. S. (2008). Relational maintenance,
tic relationship type, gender and relational characteristics. Journal satisfaction, and commitment in marriages: An actor‐partner anal‐
of Social and Personal Relationships, 8(2), 217–242. https​://doi. ysis. Journal of Family Communication, 8(3), 212–229. https​://doi.
org/10.1177/02654​07591​0 82004 org/10.1080/15267​43080​2182522
Vacharkulksemsuk, T., Reit, E., Khambatta, P., Eastwick, P. W., Finkel, E.
J., & Carney, D. R. (2016). Dominant, open nonverbal displays are at‐
tractive at zero‐acquaintance. Proceedings of the National Academy of
How to cite this article: Lindová J, Klapilová K, Johnson D,
Sciences, 113(15), 4009–4014. https​://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.15089​
32113​
Vobořilová A, Chlápková B, Havlíček J. Non‐verbal mate
Van de Mortel, T. F. (2008). Faking it: Social desirability response bias retention behaviour in women and its relation to couple's
in self‐report research. The Australian Journal of Advanced Nursing, relationship adjustment and satisfaction. Ethology.
25(4), 40. 2019;00:1–15. https​://doi.org/10.1111/eth.12949​
Van der Meij, L., Almela, M., Buunk, A. P., Fawcett, T. W., & Salvador,
A. (2011). Men with elevated testosterone levels show more affili‐
ative behaviours during interactions with women. Proceedings of the

APPENDIX 1 while the sum of behaviours Look around (unconcentrated, sweep‐


Behavioural catalogue ing, room‐directed look, lack of interest) and Head Shake (negative
Love and Care was calculated as the sum of the following behaviours: or dismissive shaking of the head) added negatively to this category.
Illustrator: Illustrating hand movement while speaking. Only behav‐ Openness was calculated as the sum of durations of the following
iour acts. Smile: A contraction of Musculus zygomaticus major. (We postures: Sitting up straight, arms by the body: Erect torso with arms
included all smiles because it was impossible to clearly identify ei‐ close to the upper body or resting on knees. Upper body faces the
ther the target of smile, partner or experimenter or the type of smile, experimenter or the partner. Leaning back, arms by the body: Body
that is Duchenne or non‐Duchenne. A new smile was coded for each leans back towards the backrest of the sofa, while the arms rest on
new peak in smiling.) Head nodding: Nodding to express assent to the lap. Only one arm can cross the body or one arm or hand can
partner. Head moves slightly up and down. touch the partner. Upper body can face the partner and the back is
Appearance Enhancement was calculated as the sum of the fol‐ not exposed to the partner. Body leans over the arm of the sofa: Body
lowing behaviours: Self‐Touch: Scratch, caress or touch to the torso, leans to the side with elbow on the arm of the sofa with shoulder
shoulders, arms, thighs, neck, hair or facial hair. Hair flip: Typically a distance from the partner at least 50cm. The respective hand can be
head toss, where one hand is used to throw hair away from the face used to support the head. Leaning back, arms stretched: Body leans
or to return hair to original position. Other forms of touching own back to the backrest of the sofa with the arms spread. One arm can
hair were also included. Primp: Activities such as ordering, smooth‐ rest on partners shoulder and the upper body can face the partner.
ing or rubbing of one's own clothing. Leaning forward: Elbows are on the knees while the hands are ei‐
Direct Guarding. Short Glance: Look at partner shorter than 2 s. ther be crossed or one hand supports the head. Sitting straight, arms
Intimacy Inducement I. Long Glance: Look at partner of 2  s and stretched: A straight posture with the arms spread.
longer. Upper Body Closed included the following body postures: Leaning
Intimacy Inducement II was calculated as the sum of the following back, crossed arms: The body leans back towards the backrest of the
Touching behaviour: Hand on Partner, Holding Hand/Arm, Patting sofa, while one arm is draped over the other, one arm crosses the
Partner, Stroke, Hugging/Embracing, Kiss, Arm around shoulders/on body, hands are held close to the face, or one hand holds the other
the back of the sofa, Intentional touch of a partners body part with arm's wrist. Upper body is oriented towards the experimenter. Sitting
legs hips or shoulders up, arms crossed: Erect torso with one arm crossing the other. The
Positivity. The count of Laughter (same as smile, but with open upper body faces either the experimenter or the partner.
mouth and producing sound) added positively to this category,
LINDOVÁ et al. |
      15

APPENDIX 2
Correlational analysis of predictors

Appearance Intimacy induce‐ Intimacy in‐ Direct Openness:


N = 94 Love and care enhancement ment I ducement II guarding Positivity Upper body

Love and care


Tau   −.065 .007 .083 .149* .067 .140
p   .393 .921 .306 .041 .360 .077
Appearance enhancement
Tau     −.017 .044 −.012 .151 −.005
p     .827 .614 .880 .051 .955
Intimacy inducement I
Tau       −.001 .043 .096 −.112
p       .994 .564 .199 .168
Intimacy inducement II
Tau         .049 .110 −.068
p         .557 .182 .448
Direct guarding
Tau           −.070 .092
p           .347 .251
Positivity
Tau             −.009
p             .910
Openness: Upper body
Tau              
p              

You might also like