You are on page 1of 13

Received: 3 January 2018 Revised: 11 June 2018 Accepted: 12 June 2018

DOI: 10.1111/joss.12459
Journal of
Sensory Studies
ORIGINAL ARTICLE

Temporal dominance of sensations, emotions, and temporal


liking measured in a bar for two similar wines using a multi-sip
approach
Ana P. Silva1,2 | Hans-Peter Voss2,3 | Hannelize van Zyl4 | Tim Hogg2,5 | Cees de Graaf1 |
Manuela Pintado2 | Gerry Jager1

1
Division of Human Nutrition, Wageningen
University, Wageningen, The Netherlands Abstract
2
CBQF – Centro de Biotecnologia e Química Eating and drinking are dynamic processes where both sensations and emotions might evolve or
Fina – Laboratório Associado, Escola Superior change over time during multiple bites/sips. However, most previous studies have measured
de Biotecnologia, Rua Arquiteto Lobão Vital,
food-evoked emotions statically, that is, at a fixed time point after consumption and using a sin-
Universidade Católica Portuguesa, Porto,
Portugal gle bite/sip approach. This study aimed to explore the sensitivity of temporal dominance of sen-
3
VossID, Almere, The Netherlands sations (TDS), of emotions (TDE), and temporal liking (TL), using a multi-sip approach, to
4
HEINEKEN Global Innovation and Research, differentiate between two comparable tasting wines. A glass of wine, in an appropriate con-
Zoeterwoude, The Netherlands sumption context, a bar, was served to 69 consumers, in two different sessions. It was shown
5
Centro de Excelência da Vinha e do Vinho, that TDS and TDE captured small differences between equally liked wines. Wines were distin-
Universidade de Trás-os-Montes e Alto Douro, guishable during consumption, based on the dominance of basic sensations such as acid, bitter,
Vila Real, Portugal
and dry, rather than aromatic sensations and based on three emotions pleased, comforted, and
Correspondence
relaxed. These emotions were dominant in both wines and in all stages of consumption but dif-
Ana Patricia Silva, Division of Human
Nutrition, Wageningen University, Stippeneng fered in the dominance rates. So, the impact of wine consumption on emotions was more uni-
4, 6703 HD, Wageningen, The Netherlands. form during consumption while new sensations became dominant during drinking.
Email: apsilva51@gmail.com
Present address
Ana Patricia Silva, Eurofins SAM Sensory &
Practical implications
Marketing Spain, Calle Ausiàs Marc 148-150, The method tested in this study showed a sensitivity level sufficient to capture subtle but signif-
08013 Barcelona, Spain. icant differences between similar, equally liked wines. Wines tested have a major difference in
Funding information wine-making process, that is, one of the wines had a particular wood aging processing in new
Fundação para a Ciência e a Tecnologia, Grant/
oak barrels conferring specific flavors and associated costs. For the wine industry, the method
Award Numbers: SFRH/BD/85152/2012,
UID/Multi/50016/2013 can be particularly useful to understand to which extent consumers perceive differences in sen-
sations and emotions, in a blind tasting, to investigate if increased costs of production are
acceptable and justified. For other food products, the method can be useful to use during prod-
uct development stage, when the aim is to differentiate prototypes with subtle differences in
ingredients composition and associated costs. Knowing when certain sensations and emotions
occur during consumption might help to create successful products in the market. Further
research using different food or beverages is however necessary to assure its validity.

1 | I N T RO D UC T I O N however, more than sensory experiences, they are also emotional


ones (Bisogni et al., 2007; Gibson, 2006; Piqueras-Fiszman & Jaeger,
Wine has a strong cultural and social connection with Portugal and 2016). Measuring food-evoked emotions has been shown to be a rele-
therefore an emotional connotation (Mora & Moscarola, 2010; Silva vant tool to complement sensory and liking measurements when aim-
et al., 2017). Wine producers and sensory researchers consider flavor ing at differentiating between products of the same category (Gutjar
to be one of the main criteria for wine choice by consumers (Lockshin, et al., 2015; Ng, Chaya, & Hort, 2013; Spinelli, Masi, Zoboli, Prescott, &
Jarvis, d'Hauteville, & Perrouty, 2006). Eating and drinking are, Monteleone, 2015). However, the emotion component of wine

J Sens Stud. 2018;e12459. wileyonlinelibrary.com/journal/joss © 2018 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. 1 of 13


https://doi.org/10.1111/joss.12459
2 of 13 Journal of SILVA ET AL.
Sensory Studies
consumption is less explored in the literature. It has been shown that Recently, researchers coupled temporal liking (TL), that is, liking
consumers perceive emotional benefits of drinking wine that are measured at several points throughout the tasting period, to TDS
partly derived from the sensory experience of wine flavor (Barrena & measurements. Different approaches were tested aiming to determine
Sanchez, 2009; Ferrarini et al., 2010; Mora & Moscarola, 2010; Silva which sensations are dominant when liking of a product increases or
et al., 2016). Recently, Danner, Johnson, Ristic, Meiselman, and decreases. The temporal drivers of liking method was developed using
Bastian (2017), Niimi, Danner, Li, Bossan, and Bastian (2017), Danner data from TL and TDS collected separately in two independent ses-
et al. (2016), and Jiang, Niimi, Ristic, and Bastian (2016) actually mea- sions (Thomas, Visalli, Cordelle, & Schlich, 2015). In a follow-up study,
sured emotions evoked by different wines in the sensory lab and in another method was developed, alternated temporal drivers of liking
different contexts of consumption. These studies used questionnaires (Thomas, van der Stelt, Prokop, Lawlor, & Schlich, 2016). In this
and rating scales to measure the intensity of emotions, which were method, TL was alternated with TDS in the same session, that is, for
measured statically, that is, at one point after tasting and using a small each sip TDS was performed followed by a liking measurement that
sample of wine. This means that the emotional experience of wine appeared on the screen immediately after completion of the TDS. In
tasting was summarized in an overall single score. Emotions are, how- other study, consumers performed TDS and TL simultaneously using
ever, defined as a continuous process and are recursive, according to the same data acquisition screen (Schlich, 2015; Thomas, Chambault,
contemporary appraisal theories (Moors, Ellsworth, Scherer, & Frijda, et al., 2015). Additionally, in some TDS studies a multi-sip approach
2013). Therefore, using a dynamic approach to measure emotions was applied showing an added value compared to a single intake eval-
might be important because it could reveal part of the emotion pro- uation (Galmarini, Loiseau, et al., 2016; Galmarini, Visalli, et al., 2016;
cess that would remain hidden with more static methods (Jager, Thomas et al., 2016; Zorn, Alcaire, Vidal, Giménez, & Ares, 2014).
2016). For instance, static measurements do not reveal in which stage Indeed, tasting a full-portion is becoming more and more important in
of consumption negative emotions occurred, or what is the first emo- sensory and consumer research, as tasting a small sample is limited in
tion response immediately after the first sip. Knowing which emotions the amount of information given to study the experience of consump-
are perceived in the different stages of consumption is important as tion, and does not translate to consumers' behavior in daily life. In
there are key moments of consumption experience that will be stored order to study the consumption experience not only the amount
in memory and seem to be important in explaining choice and con- tasted play a role, but also the context where the experience occurs
sumption (Robinson, Blissett, & Higgs, 2011). For instance, a yogurt (Jaeger et al., 2017). There are many external contextual conditions
that provoked a pleasant sensation at the end was remembered as sig- that can influence consumer perception (sensory or emotional) and
nificantly more enjoyable than one in which the pleasure was more behavior, such as music, videoclips, lightning, and social interaction
concentrated at the beginning (Robinson et al., 2011). Regarding wine, (Sester et al., 2013; Stafford, Fernandes, & Agobiani, 2012; Quartier,
it is not known how emotions evolve and change during the consump- Vanrie, & Van Cleempoel, 2014; Wang & Spence, 2015; Wansink,
tion experience and how this information can be used to differentiate 2004). These factors had mostly been studied independently but in
wines. consumption experience they may not be independent and may inter-
Temporal dominance of sensations (TDS) is a method that cap- act in an integrative way, and therefore ecologically valid research
tures how dominant sensations simultaneously evolve over time should take into account products, consumers, and the environment
(Pineau et al., 2009) and has been used to determine how wine sensa- (Jaeger & Porcherot, 2017; Köster, 2009). This present study took
tions evolve during consumption using a single or a multi-sip approach place in a wine bar, a natural and appropriate place and one of the
(Galmarini, Loiseau, Visalli, & Schlich, 2016; Galmarini, Visalli, & social settings where wine is consumed (Silva et al., 2017).
Schlich, 2016; Meillon et al., 2010; Meillon, Urbano, & Schlich, 2009; Combining the measurement of dominant emotions and sensa-
Sokolowsky & Fischer, 2012; Sokolowsky, Rosenberger, & Fischer, tions and TL during consumption experience in a bar might be a signif-
2015). TDS determines dominant sensations, which are defined as the icant contribution to the characterization of products of this type and
ones that trigger most attention at a given moment during consump- might help to better understand the choice of wine. As such, the study
tion (Pineau et al., 2009). TDS seems to be a suitable method to use would extend the palette of methods available in consumer science
with consumers as dominance is recorded, and usually not intensity, and marketing. The role of understanding emotions in the product
and therefore, training is not required (Di Monaco, Su, Masi, & Cavella, development process is often focused on products that are similar in
2014), as long as consumers are familiar with the attributes used. nature, an example of a potentially application would be to decide
Based on the concept of temporal dominance, Jager et al. (2014) which of a number of prototypes developed fits a desirable concept
evaluated how food-evoked emotions simultaneously evolve over (van Zyl, 2016).
time, replacing sensations by emotions, and developing the temporal The aim of this study was thus to explore the sensitivity of TDS,
dominance of emotions (TDE). The authors showed that TDS and TDE TDE, and TL, to differentiate two similar tasting wines. The protocol
discriminated among five different plain and flavored chocolates, used in this study was first introduced by Schlich (2015) and Thomas,
showing that the temporal evolution of sensations and emotions were Chambault, et al. (2015) to evaluate TDS and TL simultaneously. In
related. For flavored chocolates, mainly flavor attributes and positive our study, the referred protocol was extended to include TDE and
emotions were dominant whereas for plain chocolates, textural and TL. Data were collected by combining the approaches of TDE + TL
taste attributes were dominant and accompanied by more negative and TDS + TL with the same consumers in two different sessions. The
emotions. To the best of our knowledge, none of the published stud- study was performed in a real context of consumption, a bar, using a
ies have measured the TDE evoked by wine consumption. multi-sip approach, simulating a natural situation of wine
SILVA ET AL. Journal of 3 of 13
Sensory Studies
consumption, which increases the validity of the measurements on the procedure described by Lawless and Heymann (2010). Participants
the prediction of real-life consumption behavior (Jaeger et al., 2017; were presented with three samples of wine and had to identify the
Köster, 2003; Meiselman, 2013). sample that differed from the other two. Wines were coded with a
three-digit code, served in tulip-shaped dark glasses at 10  C, each
containing 20 ml of wine. Results of the triangle test showed 21 cor-
2 | MATERIALS AND METHODS rect and 26 incorrect answers, which was according to a binomial test
indicated that the wines were perceived as not significantly different
2.1 | Wines (p > .05). It can be argued that the result is on the edge of significance.
This however indicates that the wines are not easily discriminated by
Two commercial Protected Designation of Origin (PDO) white wines
consumers, making them suitable for the aim of this study.
of 2015 from the Dão region in Portugal were studied, hereafter
named DV and CB. Wines were from the same wine producer and
both had 13% alcohol (vol/vol). The description of the wines and a 2.3 | Attribute selection
basic chemical characterization is presented in Table 1. The wines The selection of the sensory and the emotion attributes is a key pro-
were selected because of their closeness in intrinsic attributes, that is, cedure for a TDS/TDE measurement as it determines the response
grape variety and wine-making process. The composition regarding range of consumers. Following the guidelines of Pineau et al. (2012), a
grape variety and amount is slightly different, although the major vari- list of 10 attributes for each measurement, that is, 10 for sensations
ety is the same for both wines: DV contains 72% Encruzado, 18% and 10 for emotions was defined. Subsequently, it was assured that
Bical, and 10% Verdelho and CB contains 80% Encruzado and 20% Ver- the attributes selected were used in everyday language so that con-
delho. Regarding the wine-making process, 20% of the wine CB is fer- sumers would easily understand the meaning of the attributes
mented and matured for 6 months in oak barrels, whereas the presented.
fermentation of the wine DV occurs mainly in stainless steel tanks. Sensory attributes were defined by the internal sensory panel of
Most extrinsic elements of these wines are very different, namely the the company that produce the wines, which are well trained panelists
label, bottle, and price but those were not the scope of the study. that taste wines on a daily basis. These attributes were validated with
Wines were presented blind and served at 10–12  C in tulip-shaped a consumer panel in a single session described below. Seven regular
glasses, each containing 80 ml of wine. This type of glasses was cho- white wine consumers (four men, three women, aged between 27 and
sen because it allows consumers to smell the aroma of the wine in the 56 years old) were recruited from Catholic University in Porto to par-
center of the glass at a lower alcohol concentration while drinking ticipate in a sensory evaluation session. These consumers did not par-
(Arakawa et al., 2015). ticipate in any other part of the study. The session took part in a
comfortable room of the university and lasted one and a half hours.
For each sample, consumers tasted the wines blinded and wrote down
2.2 | Preliminary test: Triangle test
the attribute names and/or descriptions that were perceived in flavor
To know how comparable the wines were, a triangle test was per- (overall or notes). Then, in a group discussion, the 10 most relevant
formed with 47 wine consumers (not participating in the temporal attributes for the sensory characterization of both wines were chosen
study), aiming to know whether the wines were perceived as signifi- based on consensus among consumers (see Table 2).
cantly different or not. Consumers (23 men, 24 women, aged between Emotion attributes related to wine consumption were adopted
26 and 57 years) were recruited using a database of the Catholic Uni- from our previous qualitative study in which 25 terms were compiled
versity in Porto, and were regular white wine consumers, which for as the most representative of emotion associations of wine consump-
this study was defined as ranging from drinking one glass of white tion for a group of Portuguese and Dutch consumers (Silva et al.,
wine per week to two glasses a day. The test was performed in indi- 2016). To select the 10 most relevant (out of the 25) emotion attri-
vidual booths at the sensory facilities of the university, and followed butes, a survey was performed with 426 Portuguese wine consumers

TABLE 1 Characterization of the Dão wines from Portugal used in the study

Vinification and maturation Ethanol % TA RS Terpenes Esters


Wine Type/year Grapes descriptions (vol/vol) (g/L) pH (g/L) (μg/L) (mg/L)
Duque de Viseu (DV) Dão PDO 72% Encruzado, Vinification: Stainless steel tanks. 13 5.53 3.35 <2 109.6 21
white 2015 18% Bical, Maturation: The majority in
10% Verdelho stainless steel tanks and a
small part in French oaks.
Encruzado Colheita Dão PDO 80% Encruzado, Vinification: Encruzado 13 6.39 3.16 3.3 191.7 27.6
Branco (CB) white 2015 20% Verdelho must was partly fermented
in French oak barrels.
The remainder was vinified
in stainless steel tanks.
Maturation: 20% aged about 6 months
in used French oak barrels and 80%
in stainless steel tanks.

TA = total acidity expressed in g/L as tartaric acid equivalent; RS = residual sugars.


4 of 13 Journal of SILVA ET AL.
Sensory Studies
TABLE 2 List of sensory and emotion attributes translated from average of 30 min. An informed consent form was signed by each par-
Portuguese to English and its description ticipant before the start of the study and all were compensated for
Portuguese English Description their participation. This study was submitted and positively evaluated
Sensory 
Acido Acid Vinegar by the Medical Ethical Committee of Wageningen University, The
attributes

Alcool Alcohol Warmth sensation Netherlands.
Maçã/pêra Apple/pear White fruits as apple
and pear
2.5 | General procedure and experimental design
Amargo Bitter Coffee without sugar
Lima/Limão Citrus Grapefruit, orange, lime, This study took place in the wine bar, Prova (Porto, Portugal, https://
lemon, and lemon prova.com.pt/) in two sessions performed on two consecutive week
grass
days. The bar is a specific place to taste wine. The decoration is mod-
Seco Dry That leaves your mouth
dry ern and allusive to wine, with stone walls and dark comfortable wood
Minerais Mineral Mineral water tables and chairs and low artificial light conditions. There was ambient
Doce Sweet Sugar low volume music and the temperature inside was 22–23 C. Smoking
Folhas verdes/ Vegetal Chives, hay cut, balm, is not allowed. During the course of the study, extreme care was
vegetais and moss taken to maintain the same environment conditions to avoid bias.
Madeira Woody Wood, oak Consumers performed individually using a laptop while sitting
Emotion Divertido/a Amused Diverted, showing that around tables. Data were collected using TimeSens software Version
attributes something is funny
0.93.122.904 (INRA, Dijon, France). To ensure that consumers under-
Comfortado/a Comforted Consolation, feeling
good stood and could identify the different attributes, the list of sensory
Desiludido/a Disappointed When your and emotion attributes (Table 2), as well as their descriptions, was
expectations are not sent to all consumers via e-mail two days before the first session and
fulfilled, frustration
they were asked to read it carefully. This list was also available during
Energético/a Energetic Having or showing
energy the experiment. The study design was balanced across consumers for
Livre Free Not limited or the order of the wines´ presentation as well as for the type of session
controlled, (sensations or emotions). Attributes were presented in random order
unrestrained, and
disinhibited across consumers but remained the same for a given consumer during
Completo/a Fulfilled Satisfaction, completed the evaluation to avoid order effects (Pineau et al., 2012). The proto-
Alegre/feliz Happy Joy or glad, col was an extension of those presented by Schlich (2015) and Jager
contentment, and
et al. (2014) and consisted of coupling TDS or TDE with a liking scale
delighted
while drinking a glass of wine. As an example, a print screen of each
Agradado/a Pleased Pleasure
session is presented in Figure 1. Before starting the first session (day
Relaxado/a Relaxed Relieved from tension
or anxiety, without 1), each consumer received an individual explanation and demonstra-
worries
tion on how to use the program. Consumers performed TDS + TL or
Triste Sad Sorrow or unhappiness
TDE + TL with a glass of water (dummy sample) to familiarize them-
selves with the computer program and the methodology. Following
(136 men and 290 women, aged between 18 and 70 years old). Con- this, the real experiment started. Each consumer had to perform either
sumers were asked to imagine a situation where they were drinking the TDS + TL or the TDE + TL while drinking a glass of the first wine.
wine and to rate-all-that-apply (Ares et al., 2014) in each emotion The specific instructions are described in the next section. After fin-
term to what extent they perceived a change in that emotion after ishing the first wine, consumers were instructed to rinse their mouth
consumption. The rating scale used was: much less, less, slightly less,
with water and eat some crackers. Then they were forced by the com-
slightly more, more, much more, and not applicable/no change. Based
puter directed protocol to wait at least four minutes before proceed-
on the frequencies of the responses for each emotion, 10 terms were
ing and starting the evaluation of the second wine. The second test,
selected to be used in this study (data not published yet) (see Table 2).

2.4 | Participants
Eighty Portuguese consumers, 41 men and 39 women, 39 aged
between 20 and 35 years and 41 between 36 and 62 years, were
recruited via a consumer research company. Participants were all
moderate alcohol consumers, and drink on average 1.7 glasses of
white wine per occasion, ranging from 3 to 4 times per week to one
time per fortnight. They had a normal ability to taste and smell (self-
reported) and had no previous training in sensory evaluation. Partici-
pants attended two sessions on two different days, each lasting an FIGURE 1 Example of a print screen of each session
SILVA ET AL. Journal of 5 of 13
Sensory Studies
was similar with the first one, performing TDS + TL or TDE + TL while 2.7.1 | Consumers performance
drinking the second wine. In the second session (day 2) consumers fol- Consumers performance between sessions and wines were compared
lowed the same procedure as the first day. In summary, sessions of through selected indicators (Lepage et al., 2014; Rodrigues et al.,
TDS + TL and TDE + TL were not mixed, so in 1 day consumers only 2016), namely: average number of attributes used, average of time
performed one type of session, either TDS + TL for both wines or elapsed until the first attribute selection, average of total duration of
TDE + TL for both wines. the evaluation and average number of sips. For TL, the indicators used
were: average number of clicks in the liking scale, average time
elapsed until the first liking score and average of range, that is, the
2.6 | Measurements: TDS + TL and TDE + TL average of the difference between the highest and the lowest scores.
The same procedure described below was followed for both TDS + TL Analysis of Variance (ANOVA) was used to compare the average
or TDE + TL measurements but sessions occurred on different days. scores of continuous variables, per wine and per session, and chi-
The 10 selected attributes were presented at the same time on a square (Χ 2) test was used to compare categorical variables per wine
computer screen and consumers were instructed to click the “START” and per session.
button as soon as they drank the first sip of wine. Then they had to
select the attribute that was dominant, that is, the attribute that cap- 2.7.2 | Dominance rates of sensations and emotions:
tured their attention most at a given moment in time (Pineau et al.,
TDS/TDE curves and difference curves
2009). While drinking, consumers were free to select the same attri- TDS and TDE data are commonly represented by dominance curves in

bute several times or never use an attribute. The sequence of domi- which the dominance rate of each attribute is represented over time.

nant attributes was recorded for each wine and for each consumer as The dominance rate is calculated by dividing the number of citations

was the duration of dominance (not intensity). The duration of domi- an attribute is given at a certain moment by the number of judges

nance corresponds to the time passed between the selection of one times the number of replications (Pineau et al., 2009). All the values

attribute and the next attribute chosen. For each new sip, each person are then compared to a significance level to differentiate results that
are due to chance from results that come from agreement among par-
had to click the button “NEW SIP” and then continue to evaluate the
ticipants. The chance level is the dominance rate that an attribute can
dominant attributes. The button “NEW SIP” recorded the time at
obtain by chance and the significance level is calculated based on a
which consumers drank a new sip. Consumers were asked to drink at
binomial test (Pineau et al., 2009). An attribute is considered dominant
least four sips. They could finish the glass or not and the time they
when it is above the significance level. If the attribute is between
needed to drink it was not controlled. When they finished the glass of
chance and significance level, it can be considered as having a ten-
wine and no longer perceived any attribute as dominant, they were
dency toward dominance. TDS and TDE difference curves were built
instructed to click on the button “STOP.”
to compare the wines. The difference curves are the result of sub-
While drinking each glass of wine, consumers performed a TL task
tracting dominance rates at each time point from one wine to the
simultaneously with the TDS or TDE task. A 9-point Liking scale rang-
other, and are plotted when the difference is significantly different
ing from “Dislike extremely” to “Like extremely” was shown on the
from zero.
same screen as the dominance attributes (Schlich, 2015; Thomas,
Chambault, et al., 2015). For each wine, consumers were instructed to
2.7.3 | ANOVA and MANOVA of dominance durations of
score liking at least once, although they were free to select liking attributes
scores whenever they wanted throughout the tasting, and to change
Data analysis followed the procedure described by Galmarini, Visalli,
the liking score every time they perceived a change in liking.
et al. (2016). The average of the total duration of dominance of attri-
butes was calculated from the total duration over which an attribute
was dominant for each participant. Attribute duration was analyzed
2.7 | Data analysis
using ANOVA, with wine (DV or CB), sips (first, second, and last) as
Data were analyzed using TimeSens software, IBM SPSS Statistics for fixed factors and consumers as random factors. Attributes with signifi-
Windows, Version 22.0. Armonk, NY: IBM Corp. and Microsoft Excel cant effects or interaction (wine × sip) where further analyzed using
2013/XLSTAT (XLSTAT Version 2015.5.01.23234, Addinsoft, Inc., ANOVA by wine. A Multivariate Analysis of Variance (MANOVA) test
Brooklyn, NY). For the analysis, three sips were considered: the first, was performed to evaluate if there was a multivariate wine effect.
the second, and the last sip. The first sip represents the beginning of The MANOVA F-value was obtained according to the Hotelling-
the drinking experience and the first contact with the wine; the sec- Lawley statistic (Peltier, Visalli, & Schlich, 2015).
ond sip represents the continuity of the drinking experience and the
last sip represents the end of the drinking experience, so the last sen- 2.7.4 | Temporal liking
sations and emotions evoked by wine. Sixty-nine consumers per- TL was recorded per sip, per wine, and per session (sensations and
formed the test as required and behaved in a similar way, for example, emotions). Liking was analyzed using a mixed model ANOVA, with
they took at least three sips of each wine. The 11 participants that did wine (DV, CB), sips (first, second, and last), and session (sensations,
not comply with the protocol had their data excluded from all emotions) as fixed factors and consumers, and all interactions including
analyses. it, as random factors.
6 of 13 Journal of SILVA ET AL.
Sensory Studies

3 | RESULTS reaching the highest dominance rate of 25% in the middle of the eval-
uation period.
Regarding wine, DV acid was dominant from the beginning reach-
3.1 | Consumers performance
ing a peak of 31% of dominance rate, and then decreased until no
In Table 3, a comparison of consumers´ performance in both sessions dominance around three quarters of the evaluation time. After that
is shown using different indicators. Results showed that on average time, dry reached a small peak of dominance (15%) followed by alco-
consumers used half of the attributes (5 out of 10) to describe sensa- hol (15%). Additionally, sweet had a dominance rate of 23% but only
tions and emotions, and clicked six times to evaluate liking while at the very beginning of the first sip. The second sip was characterized
drinking. The time elapsed for the first attribute selection was on aver- by the dominance of acid in the beginning (23%), evolving to alcohol in
age 16 s, and for the first liking evaluation was 34 s; this corresponds the middle, that reached also 23% of dominance rate. Sweet was dom-
to 7 and 14% of the total duration of the test, respectively. On aver- inant at the same time as alcohol but with a lower dominance rate of
age consumers took five sips of the wines in the 4 min in which they 17%, evolving to dry also with 17% of dominance rate. At the end of
were performing the test, which means they evaluated each sip in less the second sip, acid was again dominant reaching 21% of dominance
than a minute. rate. On the last sip, acid was first dominant with 25% of dominance
For all indicators, no differences between sessions and between rate evolving to alcohol, which reached the highest dominance rate of
wines were found (all p > .05), showing that the response of con- 23% at the middle period of evaluation, very similarly as in the second
sumers to the experiment was consistent between sessions and sip. However, afterwards, two new attributes were dominant for the
wines. first time in this wine. In the middle vegetal reached 16% of domi-
nance rate and in the very end bitter with 17% of dominance.
Regarding emotions, the same three emotions were dominant in
3.2 | TDS and TDE curves both wines, and all were dominant in each of the three sips. In the first
sip of wine CB, pleased was highly dominant from the beginning until
In Figure 2, the TDS and TDE curves show the dominant attributes
the end, starting and ending with a 30% of dominance rate and reach-
per sip and wine over time. For wine CB, at first sip acid had the high-
ing a peak of 36% before the halfway through of the evaluation period.
est dominant rate, starting in the very beginning of the evaluation
Comforted showed an increase in dominance rate during the evaluation
with 30% of dominance rate, and slowly decreasing to no dominance
period reaching a rate of 19% by the end. Relaxed showed the lowest
at the end of the sip. Toward the end of the evaluation, alcohol
dominance rate of all, at 17% in the beginning and becoming dominant
became dominant reaching 18% of dominance rate. The first dominant
again around three quarters of the way throughout the evaluation
sensation of the second sip was alcohol, reaching its highest domi-
period and staying this way until the end. With the second sip, pleased
nance rate of 20% in the beginning of the evaluation, and keeping a
was dominant from the beginning until the end reaching the highest
lower dominance rate in the second half of this sip. The second sensa-
rate at the end with 30%. Comforted was also dominant from the begin-
tion dry became dominant (18%) over a limited time period, between a ning until the end reaching the highest dominance rate at the beginning
fifth and half of the evaluation time. In the same period, bitter also with 27%. Relaxed was dominant during the whole sip as well, but with
started to become dominant, increasing its dominance rate, reaching lower rates than pleased and comforted, reaching its peak of 26% in the
the highest rate (21%) near the end of the evaluation. At the begin- middle of the period under evaluation. The last sip was characterized
ning of the last sip, alcohol, bitter, and dry were all dominant but alco- by the high rate of dominance of relaxed during the whole sip, with the
hol reached the highest level, a peak of 23%, decreasing to a lower highest value in the beginning with 33%. Around a fifth of the evalua-
rate between the second half and the end. Bitter was dominant during tion period, pleased was dominant with a rate of 20%, becoming domi-
all evaluation periods, except for the middle part. From a fifth of the nant again at the end of the sip. Comforted, was only dominant at the
evaluation time until the end, dry was the most dominant attribute middle of the evaluation period with a rate of 20%. Regarding wine

TABLE 3 Indicators of consumers performance per session and per wine (average and standard deviation)

CB DV
Wine
Session Sensations Emotions Sensations Emotions
Temporal dominance indicators
Number of used attributes 4.7  2 4.8  2 4.7  1 4.9  2
Time elapsed for the first attribute selection (in seconds) 17  15 17  10 13  10 16  10
Temporal liking indicators
Number of clicks in liking scores 5.7  3 6.5  4 5.7  3 5.7  3
Time elapsed for the first liking score selection (in seconds) 36  38 33  29 31  27 35  26
Range of liking scores (max–min) 0.9  0.8 1.1  0.9 1.0  0.9 1.0  0.9
Number of sips 5.7  2 5.8  3 5.4  2 5.4  2
Total duration of the test (in seconds) 244  126 233  129 240  119 228  120

No significant differences among wines evaluation for each session were found (p > .05).
SILVA ET AL. Journal of 7 of 13
Sensory Studies

FIGURE 2 Temporal dominance of sensations and emotions for the first, second and last sip of the wines. Legend: above the significance level
dominant attributes are shown; below the significance level a grey shadow is hiding attributes that are not significantly dominant

DV, during the first sip, it was observed that pleased started with the Overall, a greater number of sensory attributes (six) achieved
highest dominance rate of 36%, decreasing gradually along the evalua- dominance compared to the number of emotions that were dominant
tion period but always remaining dominant. Until the middle of the (three).
evaluation, comforted and relaxed were both dominant with rates
around 17%. In the middle of the evaluation, relaxed increased its domi-
3.3 | TDS and TDE difference curves
nance rate to 26% and comforted decreased. In the second sip, pleased
started with 29% of dominance rate and after a fifth of the evaluation Figure 3 depicts the difference curves of dominant sensations and
time was quite constant in its dominance rate (21%). Comforted reached emotions between wines per sip. Longer and higher differences in
a peak of dominance of 22% just before the middle of the evaluation dominance rates of the sensory attributes were observed in the sec-
period. Relaxed was dominant during the whole evaluation period, with ond and last sips when compared to the first sip. In the first sip, differ-
a higher dominance rate of 24% in the beginning and decreasing ences were observed only in the very beginning in which sweet in the
slightly until the end. In the last sip, pleased was dominant during the wine DV was very briefly more dominant than the wine CB, which
whole sip, reaching a peak rate of 27% after a quarter of the evaluation instead had a higher dominance rate for dry. In the second sip, the
period. Relaxed became dominant after a fifth of the evaluation time, wine DV had a higher and shorter dominance rate for acid in the
increasing until the middle stage to a rate of 24% and remaining domi- beginning followed by sweet. For a short time, the wine CB showed a
nant until the end. Comforted was only dominant at the very beginning higher dominance rate for dry at the beginning and an almost continu-
of this sip, with 17% of dominance rate. ous higher dominance rate for bitter throughout this sip. In the last
8 of 13 Journal of SILVA ET AL.
Sensory Studies
sip, wine DV had again a higher dominance rate for acid at the begin- wine level [F (1,68) = 6.19; p = .03], meaning that the total duration
ning and, for a brief period at the middle, for vegetal. The wine CB had of bitter was significantly higher for wine CB compared to wine DV
a higher dominance rate of alcohol in the beginning but for a very (4.9 versus 2.1 s). However, no significant interaction between wines
short period. Bitter was higher in dominance until the middle of this and sip were found for any of the sensory attributes, meaning that the
sip and dry showed the highest and most continuous dominance rate. duration of sensations within each sip was the same in both wines.
Regarding differences in the dominance rate of emotions, it was Regarding emotion attributes, a wine effect was observed in ful-
observed that in the first sip of wine DV, relaxed showed a higher filled [F (1,68) = 5.11; p = .02], meaning that the total duration of
dominance rate in the middle whereas wine CB had a higher domi- dominance of fulfilled was significantly higher for wine DV than for
nance rate for pleased. In the second sip, the wine CB had a higher wine CB (2.3 versus 1.0 s). There was an interaction between wine
dominance rate of comforted in the beginning and middle time of eval- and sip in energetic [(F (2,136) = 3.31; p = .04]. ANOVA per wine
uation and, for a very short period, a higher dominance rate for (Table 5) showed significant differences between wines, that is a sig-
pleased. In the last sip, wine CB had a higher dominance rate of nificant sip effect only in wine DV [F (2,136) = 3.70; p = .03], and that
relaxed, followed for a short time by comforted, and wine DV had a the dominance of energetic was decreasing significantly along the sips
higher dominance rate of pleased in the middle of the evaluation (first sip 5.9 s, second sip 1.9 s, and last sip 0.9 s).
period. Total duration of dominance of sensory and emotion attributes
It was observed that the number of attributes with differences in per wine and per sip showed no significant differences (all p-values
dominance is greater in the last sip compared to the first sip, that is, >.05).
two sensory attributes in the first sip versus five attributes in the last
sip, and two emotion attributes in the first sip versus three in the
3.5 | Temporal liking
last sip.
The ANOVA model for TL showed a significant wine effect
[F (1,204) = 4.80; p = .03], a significant sip effect [F (2,204) = 4.86;
3.4 | Differences between wines based on durations
p = .009], and a significant session effect [F (1,204) = 10.39;
of dominance of attributes p = .002]. This means that on average: the wine DV was globally more
MANOVA test showed that there is no multivariate wine effect for liked than the wine CB, liking increased with the number of sips and
sensations or emotions. ANOVA results of Table 4 showed that acid liking scores were rated higher in the emotion session compared to
had a significant effect at sip level [F (2,136) = 5.94; p = .03], as it was the sensations session. The model did not show significant interac-
dominant for longer within the first sip. ANOVA per wine (Table 5) tions between the factors, which means that the session and sip
showed significant differences between wines, that is a sip effect only effects on liking was the same for both wines. In Figure 4, the mean
in wine CB [F (2,207) = 6.12; p = .003], and that the dominance of liking scores per wine, sip, and session are graphically presented. Pair-
acid was significantly higher in the first sip. Bitter had an effect at the wise comparisons between sips per session for each wine, showed

FIGURE 3 Difference curves for dominant duration of sensations and emotions for the first, second and last sip of the wines
SILVA ET AL. Journal of 9 of 13
Sensory Studies
TABLE 4 ANOVA results for the total duration of dominance per attribute for wines and sips. Mean values and standard errors (in seconds)

Attributes F-wines CB DV F-sip First sip Second sip Last sip F-wines × sip
Sensory Acid 0.39 4.1  1 4.8  1 5.94 7.1 a,b
2 3.4  0.5
a
2.8  0.4
b
0.23
Alcohol 0.28 5.7  1 6.6  1 1.65 4.2  1 7.5  1 6.7  2 0.35
Apple/pear 0.65 1.6  0.5 2.2  0.5 1.88 2.8  1 2.0  1 1.0  0.5 2.04
Bitter 6.19 4.9a  1 2.1a  0.4 0.50 2.9  1 4.3  1 3.4  1 0.70
Citrus 0.03 2.9  1 2.7  1 0.27 3.2  1 3.0  1 2.3  1 1.47
Dry 0.26 4.9  1 4.3  1 0.51 5.0  1 5.0  1 3.7  1 0.34
Mineral 1.44 1.6  0.5 3.4  1 1.01 3.8  2 2.6  1 1.2  0.4 0.70
Sweet 0.13 3.9  1 4.3  1 0.01 4.1  1 4.2  1 4.0  2 2.51
Vegetal 1.39 1.5  0.4 2.3  0.5 0.96 2.1  1 2.3  1 1.3  0.3 0.63
Woody 0.13 2.2  1 1.8  1 0.93 1.8  1 3.0  1 1.3  0.4 0.60
All sensations 1.09 33.3  3 34.5  2 1.39 36.9  3 37.3  3 27.6  3 1.04
Emotions Amused 0.39 1.4  0.4 1.8  0.4 1.08 1.7  0.5 2.0  0.6 1.0  0.3 0.70
Comforted 0.00 5.7  1 5.8  1 0.81 5.7  0.9 6.8  1.2 4.7  1.2 0.70
Disappointed 0.02 1.0  0.3 1.1  0.3 0.41 1.3  0.4 1.0  0.4 0.9  0.4 0.66
Energetic 3.50 1.2  0.3 2.9  1 2.60 3.4  1.2 1.9  0.5 1.0  0.3 3.32
Free 0.17 2.3  0.4 2.7  1 0.39 1.9  0.6 2.4  0.6 3.0  1.2 1.37
Fulfilled 5.11 1a  0.2 2.3a  1 1.61 0.9  0.3 2.1  0.6 2.0  0.5 0.42
Happy 0.06 2.1  1 2.0  0.4 0.77 1.8  0.5 2.6  0.6 1.7  0.4 0.13
Pleased 0.11 7.0  1 6.6  1 0.93 7.8  1.2 7.1  1.1 5.5  1.3 0.73
Relaxed 0.52 5.7  1 6.7  1 0.47 6.5  1.7 6.9  1.3 5.2  0.8 0.01
Sad 0.90 0.7  0.3 0.3  0.2 0.18 0.66  0.4 0.6  0.3 0.4  0.3 0.98
All emotions 1.07 28.3  2 32.2  2 0.89 31.9  2.4 33.3  2.3 25.5  2.8 0.93

Significant F-values are highlighted in bold (p ≤ .05). Means with the same uppercase letters are significantly different (p ≤ .05).

TABLE 5 ANOVA results for the duration of dominance of significant attributes for each wine over the different sips. Mean values and standard
errors (in seconds)
DV CB
Wine
Attributes F-sip First sip Second sip Last sip F-sip First sip Second sip Last sip
Acid 2.56 7.0a  2 4.2  0.9 3.2a  0.6 6.12 7.2a,b  2 2.6a  0.9 2.5b  0.9
Energetic 3.70 5.9 a,b
2 1.9  0.7
a
0.9  0.3
b
0.63 1.0  0.4 1.8  0.8 1.0  0.5

Significant F-values are highlighted in bold (p ≤ .05). Means with the same uppercase letters are significantly different (p ≤ .05).

significant differences which are marked in Figure 4. From the graph, was not significant in both sessions (p > .05), however in the emotion
it can be observed that the wine effect was larger in the emotion ses- session there was a marginally significant trend (p ≤ .1).
sion compared to the sensory session. The difference between wines

Temporal liking 4 | DI SCU SSION


7.5
Temporal dominance of sensations and emotions and temporal liking
c
7.0 d measured using a multi-sip approach showed that two similar tasting
7.0 6.9
Liking - 9 point scale

a wines were equally liked along the drinking process but each demon-
6.7 6.7 c
b 6.6 strated small differences in sensory and emotion profiles. New sensa-
6.5 6.5 6.5
d
6.5 6.4 6.4 tions become dominant while drinking, whereas the same emotions
6.3
a were dominant in all stages of consumption. These findings are dis-
6.1

6.0 cussed in more detail below.


Besides vegetal, that was dominant at the end of the tasting
period for wine DV, none of the other aromatic descriptors, such as
5.5
F S L F S L F S L F S L apple/pear, citrus, mineral, and woody, were dominant at any time
CB_Sensations DV_Sensations CB_Emotions DV_Emotions during consumption. Dominant sensations were the basic taste
descriptors, acid, sweet, bitter, and sensations characteristic of wine
FIGURE 4 Representation of the mean liking scores of the wines per
sip (F - first, S - second, L - last) and per session. Values with the same in general, such as alcohol and dry. This is not surprising because the
uppercase letters are significantly different (p ≤ .05) consumers in this study were not trained in sensory descriptive
10 of 13 Journal of SILVA ET AL.
Sensory Studies
analysis and therefore rely on common sensory words to describe of wine, liking increased. This is hedonic escalation and is likely to
their sensations (Hopfer & Heymann, 2014; Mueller, Lockshin, Salt- occur in palatable food or beverages with complex combinations of
man, & Blanford, 2010). However, these descriptors are those that flavors (Crolic & Janiszewski, 2016). As hedonic escalation occurred
can differentiate between wines and that allow us to understand con- with the number of sips, namely: liking increased and more differences
sumers´ perception and evaluation. From a chemical analysis, the wine in dominance were observed, our findings suggest the importance of
CB showed a higher concentration of terpenes than did DV, but it is using a multi-sip approach to characterize and differentiate between
unlikely that this is related to the perceived bitterness. Terpenes are similar wines. To a certain extent, this follows the assumption that
fruit-derived compounds affecting wine aroma and quality signifi- first impressions based on a single evaluation, such as single sip, can
cantly (Sokolowsky & Fischer, 2012) and might be oxidized during oak be poor predictors of preference and choice (Köster, Couronne, Léon,
aging altering the type of aroma they confer (Marais, 1983). Bitterness Lévy, & Marcelino, 2003). However, as the objective of this study was
is one of the major modalities to characterize wine taste and is elicited not to compare static and dynamic responses, the authors suggest
primarily by phenolic compounds and ethanol (Noble, 1994). that further research is necessary for validation. Conversely, emotion
Untrained consumers might easily confuse astringency with bitterness profile of both wines was more uniform over time, in a sense that the
and both attributes are conferred by phenolic compounds that are same three emotions were dominant in all stages of consumption. This
extracted during wood aging. For the same reason, it was also might be different for other wines but no other studies were yet pub-
expected that woody sensation would be more dominant in the wine lished, so a comparison is not possible. However in chocolates, TDE
CB, as shown by Nunes et al. (2017), however woody was never a showed that it is possible that new emotions evolved at the middle
dominant sensation. It could also have happened that the aromatic stage of consumption, such as calm, aggressive, and energetic (Jager
sensations might be perceived by consumers, but are not the most et al., 2014).
dominant. An interesting method to complement the TDS is the new Several factors should be considered to understand the differ-
method of Temporal Check-all-that-apply, as several attributes can be ences in the development of the dynamic sensory versus dynamic
selected at the same time and relevant attributes for describing sam- emotion profiles: the high similarity between wines, differences in the
ples might have low dominance rates which did not reach significance perception process of sensory attributes versus emotion responses,
(Ares et al., 2015). the influence of contextual factors on the evaluations and the specific
Wines with different flavor characteristics influenced consumers´ list of emotion terms used.
on liking and elicited emotions when measured statically (Danner Regarding the wines, our results suggest that in the absence of a
et al., 2016; Jiang et al., 2016; Niimi et al., 2017). In this study, per- large difference in flavor in equally liked wines, the same emotions
formed using TDE, three dominant emotions characterized the con- caught the most attention of consumers for the two wines, and similar
sumption of both wines: relaxed, pleased, and comforted, confirming emotion profiles evolved in qualitative terms. Additionally, in sensory
the similarity, in qualitative evaluation, between the wines. evaluation consumers are focused on the product tasting through a
Relaxed and pleased are associated with wine consumption in gen- rational thinking and a conscious monitored process. Conversely,
eral (Charters & Pettigrew, 2008), and although differences in domi- emotion responses are intrinsic to the person, more subjective, and
nance of these emotions were perceived, its meaning was difficult to elicited without much reflection. So more an intuitive thought process
translate to practice as relaxed was dominant in the beginning of the (Kahneman, 2003) and therefore more difficult to report than sensory
wine DV and more pleased at the end of consumption, and the other evaluation (Thomson, 2016). The atmosphere of the place of wine
way around for the wine CB. Of relevance to distinguish the wines, is consumption has a great effect on emotions evoked (Edwards,
that during the middle and final stages of consumption, comforted was Hartwell, & Giboreau, 2016; Schrieks et al., 2014). It could be that the
more dominant in wine CB than with wine DV (Figure 3). According to similarity between emotion profiles of the wines that evolved while
wine experts (enologists and wine makers with whom the results were drinking was a response of the drinking experience itself, that is,
discussed), the comforted feelings being more dominant in wine CB including the emotions evoked by the context of consumption, thus
might be translating some particular differences between the wines not permitting the highlighting of huge differences evoked by the
due to the maturation process in oak barrels which consumers could wines. Based on TL measurements, the wines were not distinguishable
not translate based on the sensory attributes but expressed that dif- but showed differences in the dominance of emotions. Previous
ferences in the dominance of emotions. Oak aging softens wines and research performed in blind conditions in real contexts of consump-
gives it a more rounded mouthfeel and those aromas that it does con- tion or immersive contexts, showed that better liked wines evoked
fer are often low notes, being consistent with these observations. more intense positive emotions, whereas equally liked wines only dif-
Sensory profiles evolved faster than emotion profiles, in a sense fer in one emotion, surprised (Danner et al., 2016; Jiang et al., 2016).
that new sensations became dominant at different stages of wine con- Among three equally liked fruit variants of an alcoholic aperitif
sumption. This is likely to be related to the flavor development over consumed during a meal in a restaurant Porcherot, Petit, Giboreau,
time, which in beverages like wine is recognized to vary as a function Gaudreau, and Cayeux (2015) using a static method for measuring
of time, usually getting better over the course of the sensory experi- emotions did not find any significant difference in short-term emotion
ence (Lawless & Heymann, 2010) and highlights the relevance of using responses. So, it seems that in real contexts of consumption, emotion
a multi-sip approach. At the end of the drinking experience, more dif- profiles of similar products equally liked are small. In the current study,
ferences of dominance of sensations were perceived. At the same using temporal measurement of emotions of equally liked wines, the
time as different sensations became dominant with each additional sip whole emotion experience of drinking a glass of wine was revealed,
SILVA ET AL. Journal of 11 of 13
Sensory Studies
which is not achievable using static measurements. However, further computer or tablet. If this were the case, it could be that older con-
research is needed to understand the impact of the context of con- sumers might have more difficulties performing the test.
sumption in the emotion response. Of relevance is also the number of attributes to use in temporal
There are at least two more factors that might have affected measurements which should be restricted to 10 (Pineau et al., 2012).
reported emotion responses including the psychological state of par- Therefore, participants are forced to choose within the attributes pre-
ticipants at the moment of consumption (Di Muro & Murray, 2012) sented, which influences the results in a certain direction (Jager et al.,
and previous experiences of white wine consumption (Desmet & 2014). This shows how difficult and important it is to use the right
Schifferstein, 2008), but none of these was evaluated. words for a specific food or beverage. Although we selected appropri-
Wines were discriminated based on liking during the emotion ses- ated attributes for the wines characterization, does not mean that
sion, which did not happen during the sensation session. This high- other attributes, excluded from the lists presented on this study but
lights the relevance of the measurement of emotions to distinguish also associated to wine consumption, would not contribute for a bet-
similar wines. Wines were better liked during the emotion session ter differentiation between the wines. For example, other emotions
compared to the sensation session, showing that there was an impact wine related, such as contented, frustrated, and warm, might contribute
of measuring liking simultaneously with different tasks. Coupling TL to to better discrimination between the wines in terms of emotion pro-

TDE was for the first time measured and coupling TL with TDS is a file. A way to overcome this limitation, and include more terms in tem-

very recent practice (Galmarini, Loiseau, et al., 2016; Schlich, 2015; poral dominance measurements, could be to group terms in

Thomas et al., 2016). Prescott, Lee, and Kim (2011) found that overall dimensions. This idea could be tested in further research.
The protocol used in this study assumed that an attribute remain
liking rates decreased when consumers were asked to rate their liking
dominant until another attribute is selected, which means that is not
after doing an analytical task of rating the intensity of sensory attri-
considering “no dominant” periods during consumption, that might
butes of beverages. The measurements themselves are fundamentally
occur when consumers do not perceive anything, are distracted or
different, in that temporal dominance is qualitative and intensity rating
hesitate in attribute selection. This limitation can be weakened with a
is quantitative. Therefore, it could be that the focus of the evaluation
different protocol design and further studies could test the extent of
during the liking measurements is different, that is, when evaluating
this limitation.
sensory attributes consumers were focused on the wines whereas to
Food product development is in need of use research methods
report emotions they were focused on themselves. This might explain
able to accommodate dynamic changes in experiences that occur over
why liking is higher when coupled with emotion measurement. The
time (Schifferstein, 2015), and sufficiently sensitive to capture subtle
trend-wise better discrimination between wines noted when liking
differences among products (van Zyl, 2016), therefore it would cer-
and emotion measurements were coupled, compared to when liking
tainly be valuable to explore TDE further to confirm its validity and
and sensation measurements were coupled, is a topic that deserves to
reliability.
be explored to see if it is a more general phenomenon.
TDE is an explicit emotion measurement in which only the con-
scious part of the emotional response—the subjective feeling—is cap-
4.1 | Limitations and further research tured (Köster & Mojet, 2015). This is to a certain extent a limitation

Despite individual instructions and a practice session, 14% of the par- inherent to explicit emotion measurements as only emotional associa-

ticipants did not perform the task as expected. It could be that they tions that consumers are aware of are reported. However, explicit

did not properly understand the task proposed, as some participants, methods are the most prominent used to measure food-evoked emo-
tions (Lagast, Gellynck, Schouteten, De Herdt, & De Steur, 2017) and
for example, alternated to take a sip with the selection of one attri-
even if not all words represent a “true” emotion but an emotional
bute during the whole test. Consumers do not like tasks that are com-
association (Cardello & Jaeger, 2016), they are measurable and cer-
plex in nature and this showed that the protocol used is complex to
tainly relevant for consumer research.
perform with consumers and that there is a need to simplify the pro-
cedure and/or a more effective practicing prior to the measurements.
We added additional complexity to the procedure as we did not
5 | CONC LU SION
instruct on the number of sips that each consumer would drink nor
defined a specific time between each sip, as we thought this proce- This study aimed to explore the sensitivity of temporal dominance of
dure would be more in line with natural behavior. In other studies, sensations and emotions and temporal liking to differentiate between
where the number of sips and time between sips was controlled, this two similar tasting wines, using a multi-sip approach and a realistic con-
difficulty was not reported (Zorn et al., 2014). As also proposed by sumption context, a bar. It was shown that using TDS and TDE small
Thomas et al. (2016), in further research using this method, the num- differences between equally liked wines were captured, showing the
ber of sips should be limited and within specific time. We think that sensitivity of the method to distinguish similar wines, and giving a
on one hand, this will help consumers perform the test and, on the broader understanding of consumers´ perception of wines. The impact
other hand, will facilitate the analysis of the results creating a bal- of wine consumption on emotions was more uniform along consump-
anced data set and limiting variation. Additionally, it is not likely that tion experience whereas the impact of sensations changed in the differ-
the success of consumer´s performance is related to education level, ent stages of consumption, especially toward the end of the drinking
but it could be associated with the ability and practice to use a process. This highlights the importance of using a multi-sip approach, to
12 of 13 Journal of SILVA ET AL.
Sensory Studies
differentiate between similar products, as more differences were consumers' expectations, liking, emotions and willingness to pay for
observed as the number of sips consumed increased. In competitive Australian white wines. Food Research International, 99, 263–274.
Danner, L., Ristic, R., Johnson, T. E., Meiselman, H. L., Hoek, A. C.,
markets, this study is relevant for the food and beverages industry Jeffery, D. W., & Bastian, S. E. (2016). Context and wine quality effects
which are interested in testing subtle differences among prototypes of on consumers' mood, emotions, liking and willingness to pay for
products or between competitors. If the products are very different Australian Shiraz wines. Food Research International., 89, 254–265.
Desmet, P. M., & Schifferstein, H. N. (2008). Sources of positive and nega-
from one another, those same differences will be picked up no matter tive emotions in food experience. Appetite, 50(2), 290–301.
what the method, and therefore might not tell us so much about its Di Monaco, R., Su, C., Masi, P., & Cavella, S. (2014). Temporal dominance
sensitivity. To create food or beverages with high emotion impact and of sensations: A review. Trends in Food Science & Technology, 38(2),
104–112.
to have a broader understanding of the emotion perception of similar
Di Muro, F., & Murray, K. B. (2012). An arousal regulation explanation of
products, TDE, combined with TDS and TL seems to be promising and mood effects on consumer choice. Journal of Consumer Research, 39(3),
suitable for that purpose. 574–584.
Edwards, J. S., Hartwell, H. J., & Giboreau, A. (2016). Emotions studied in
context: The role of the eating environment, in Meiselman, Herbert L.
(ed), Emotion Measurement, 377–403. Woodhead Publishing.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS Ferrarini, R., Carbognin, C., Casarotti, E., Nicolis, E., Nencini, A., &
Meneghini, A. (2010). The emotional response to wine consumption.
The authors gratefully acknowledge Sogrape Vinhos S.A. and Sense
Food Quality and Preference, 21(7), 720–725.
Test for their support to perform this study, the graduate school Galmarini, M., Visalli, M., & Schlich, P. (2016). Advances in representation
VLAG from Wageningen University and the Portuguese Foundation and analysis of mono and multi-intake temporal dominance of sensa-
for Science and Technology (FCT) for the PhD grant of A. P. Silva (Ref. tions data. Food Quality and Preference, 56, 247–255.
Galmarini, M. V., Loiseau, A. L., Visalli, M., & Schlich, P. (2016). Use of
SFRH/BD/85152/2012). Also thanks to the owners of the wine bar multi-intake temporal dominance of sensations (TDS) to evaluate the
Prova and to the consumers for their enthusiastic participation. A spe- influence of cheese on wine perception. Journal of Food Science,
cial thanks to the team that assisted during study performance: Ana 81(10), S2566–S2577.
Gibson, E. L. (2006). Emotional influences on food choice: Sensory, physio-
Amaro, Beatriz Rocha, and Dr. Hans-Peter Voss. In addition, the logical and psychological pathways. Physiology & Behavior, 89(1), 53–61.
authors thank Michel Visalli for the support with TimeSens manage- Gutjar, S., de Graaf, C., Kooijman, V., de Wijk, R. A., Nys, A., ter
ment and Arnaud Thomas for the valuable comments on this paper. Horst, G. J., & Jager, G. (2015). The role of emotions in food choice
and liking. Food Research International, 76, 216–223.
This work was supported by National Funds from FCT through project
Hopfer, H., & Heymann, H. (2014). Judging wine quality: Do we need
UID/Multi/50016/2013. experts, consumers or trained panelists? Food Quality and Preference,
32, 221–233.
Jaeger, S. R., Hort, J., Porcherot, C., Ares, G., Pecore, S., & MacFie, H. J. H.
ORCID
(2017). Future directions in sensory and consumer science: Four per-
Ana P. Silva http://orcid.org/0000-0002-3262-7479 spectives and audience voting. Food Quality and Preference Part B, 56,
301–309. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.foodqual.2016.03.006
Jaeger, S. R., & Porcherot, C. (2017). Consumption context in consumer
RE FE R ENC E S research: Methodological perspectives. Current Opinion in Food Science,
15, 30–37. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cofs.2017.05.001
Arakawa, T., Iitania, K., Wang, X., Kajiro, T., Toma, K., Yano, K., & Jager, G. (2016). Short-term time structure of food-related emotions: Mea-
Mitsubayashia, K. (2015). Sniffer-camera for imaging of ethanol vapori- suring dynamics of responses, in Meiselman, Herbert L. (ed), Emotion
zation from wine: Effect of wine glass shape. Analyst, 140(148), Measurement, 273–298. Woodhead Publishing.
2881–2886. https://doi.org/10.1039/c4an02390k Jager, G., Schlich, P., Tijssen, I., Yao, J., Visalli, M., De Graaf, C., &
Ares, G., Bruzzone, F., Vidal, L., Cadena, R. S., Giménez, A., Pineau, B., … Stieger, M. (2014). Temporal dominance of emotions: Measuring
Jaeger, S. R. (2014). Evaluation of a rating-based variant of dynamics of food-related emotions during consumption. Food Quality
check-all-that-apply questions: Rate-all-that-apply (RATA). Food Qual- and Preference, 37, 87–99.
ity and Preference, 36, 87–95. Jiang, W., Niimi, J., Ristic, R., & Bastian, S. E. P. (2016). The effects of
Ares, G., Jaeger, S. R., Antúnez, L., Vidal, L., Giménez, A., Coste, B., … immersive context and wine flavor on consumer wine flavor percep-
Castura, J. C. (2015). Comparison of TCATA and TDS for dynamic sen- tion and emotions elicited. American Journal of Enology and Viticulture,
sory characterization of food products. Food Research International, 78, ajev-2016.
148–158. Kahneman, D. (2003). A perspective on judgment and choice: Mapping
Barrena, R., & Sanchez, M. (2009). Connecting product attributes with bounded rationality. American Psychologist, 58(9), 697–720.
emotional benefits: Analysis of a Mediterranean product across con- Köster, E. (2003). The psychology of food choice: Some often encountered
sumer age segments. British Food Journal, 111(2), 120–137. fallacies. Food Quality and Preference, 14(5), 359–373.
Bisogni, C. A., Falk, L. W., Madore, E., Blake, C. E., Jastran, M., Sobal, J., & Köster, E. (2009). Diversity in the determinants of food choice: A psycho-
Devine, C. M. (2007). Dimensions of everyday eating and drinking epi- logical perspective. Food Quality and Preference, 20(2), 70–82.
sodes. Appetite, 48(2), 218–231. Köster, E., Couronne, T., Léon, F., Lévy, C., & Marcelino, A. (2003). Repeat-
Cardello, A. V., & Jaeger, S. R. (2016). Measurement of consumer product ability in hedonic sensory measurement: A conceptual exploration.
emotions using questionnaires, in Meiselman, Herbert L. (ed), Emotion Food Quality and Preference, 14(2), 165–176.
measurement, 165–200. Woodhead Publishing. Köster, E., & Mojet, J. (2015). From mood to food and from food to mood:
Charters, S., & Pettigrew, S. (2008). Why do people drink wine? A A psychological perspective on the measurement of food-related emo-
consumer-focused exploration. Journal of Food Products Marketing, tions in consumer research. Food Research International, 76, 180–191.
14(3), 13–32. https://doi.org/10.1080/10454440801985894 Lagast, S., Gellynck, X., Schouteten, J. J., De Herdt, V., & De Steur, H. (2017).
Crolic, C., & Janiszewski, C. (2016). Hedonic escalation: When food just Review: Consumers' emotions elicited by food: A systematic review of
tastes better and better. Journal of Consumer Research, 43(3), 388–406. explicit and implicit methods. Trends in Food Science & Technology (Part
https://doi.org/10.1093/jcr/ucw032 A), 69, 172–189. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tifs.2017.09.006
Danner, L., Johnson, T. E., Ristic, R., Meiselman, H. L., & Bastian, S. E. Lawless, H. T., & Heymann, H. (2010). Sensory evaluation of food: Principles
(2017). “I like the sound of that!” Wine descriptions influence and practices. Springer Science & Business Media, New York, USA.
SILVA ET AL. Journal of 13 of 13
Sensory Studies
Lepage, M., Neville, T., Rytz, A., Schlich, P., Martin, N., & Pineau, N. (2014). Schifferstein, H. N. (2015). Employing consumer research for creating new
Panel performance for temporal dominance of sensations. Food Quality and engaging food experiences in a changing world. Current Opinion in
and Preference, 38, 24–29. Food Science, 3, 27–32.
Lockshin, L., Jarvis, W., d'Hauteville, F., & Perrouty, J.-P. (2006). Using simula- Schlich, P. (2015). Temporal dominance of sensation paired with temporal
tions from discrete choice experiments to measure consumer sensitivity liking. Workshop presentation at the 11th Pangborn Sensory Science Sym-
to brand, region, price. and awards in wine choice. Food Quality and Prefer- posium, Gothenburg, Sweden.
ence, 17(3–4), 166–178. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.foodqual.2005.03.009 Schrieks, I. C., Stafleu, A., Kallen, V. L., Grootjen, M., Witkamp, R. F., &
Marais, J. (1983). Terpenes in the aroma of grapes and wines: A review. Hendriks, H. F. (2014). The biphasic effects of moderate alcohol consump-
South African Journal of Enology and Viticulture, 4(2), 49–58. tion with a meal on ambiance-induced mood and autonomic nervous sys-
Meillon, S., Urbano, C., & Schlich, P. (2009). Contribution of the temporal tem balance: A randomized crossover trial. PLoS One, 9(1), e86199.
dominance of sensations (TDS) method to the sensory description of Sester, C., Deroy, O., Sutan, A., Galia, F., Desmarchelier, J.-F.,
subtle differences in partially dealcoholized red wines. Food Quality Valentin, D., & Dacremont, C. (2013). “Having a drink in a bar”: An
and Preference, 20(7), 490–499. immersive approach to explore the effects of context on drink choice.
Meillon, S., Viala, D., Medel, M., Urbano, C., Guillot, G., & Schlich, P. Food Quality and Preference, 28(1), 23–31.
(2010). Impact of partial alcohol reduction in Syrah wine on perceived Silva, A. P., Jager, G., van Bommel, R., van Zyl, H., Voss, H.-P., Hogg, T., …
complexity and temporality of sensations and link with preference. de Graaf, C. (2016). Functional or emotional? How Dutch and Portu-
Food Quality and Preference, 21(7), 732–740. guese conceptualise beer, wine and non-alcoholic beer consumption.
Meiselman, H. L. (2013). The future in sensory/consumer research: Evolv- Food Quality and Preference, 49, 54–65.
ing to a better science. Food Quality and Preference, 27(2), 208–214. Silva, A. P., Jager, G., van Zyl, H., Voss, H.-P., Pintado, M., Hogg, T., & de
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.foodqual.2012.03.002 Graaf, C. (2017). Cheers, Proost, Saúde: Cultural, contextual and psy-
Moors, A., Ellsworth, P. C., Scherer, K. R., & Frijda, N. H. (2013). Appraisal chological factors of wine and beer consumption in Portugal and in the
theories of emotion: State of the art and future development. Emotion Netherlands. Critical Reviews in Food Science and Nutrition, 57(7),
Review, 5(2), 119–124. 1340–1349.
Mora, P., & Moscarola, J. (2010). Representations of the emotions associ- Sokolowsky, M., & Fischer, U. (2012). Evaluation of bitterness in white wine
ated with a wine purchasing or consumption experience. International applying descriptive analysis, time-intensity analysis, and temporal domi-
Journal of Consumer Studies, 34(6), 674–683. nance of sensations analysis. Analytica Chimica Acta, 732, 46–52.
Mueller, S., Lockshin, L., Saltman, Y., & Blanford, J. (2010). Message on a Sokolowsky, M., Rosenberger, A., & Fischer, U. (2015). Sensory impact of
bottle: The relative influence of wine back label information on wine skin contact on white wines characterized by descriptive analysis,
choice. Food Quality and Preference, 21(1), 22–32. time–intensity analysis and temporal dominance of sensations analysis.
Ng, M., Chaya, C., & Hort, J. (2013). The influence of sensory and packag- Food Quality and Preference, 39, 285–297. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
ing cues on both liking and emotional, abstract and functional concep- foodqual.2014.07.002
tualisations. Food Quality and Preference, 29(2), 146–156. Spinelli, S., Masi, C., Zoboli, G. P., Prescott, J., & Monteleone, E. (2015). Emo-
Niimi, J., Danner, L., Li, L., Bossan, H., & Bastian, S. E. (2017). Wine con- tional responses to branded and unbranded foods. Food Quality and
sumers' subjective responses to wine mouthfeel and understanding of Preference, 42, 1–11. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.foodqual.2014.12.009
wine body. Food Research International., 99, 115–122. Stafford, L. D., Fernandes, M., & Agobiani, E. (2012). Effects of noise and
Noble, A. C. (1994). Bitterness in wine. Physiology & Behavior, 56(6), distraction on alcohol perception. Food Quality and Preference, 24(1),
1251–1255. https://doi.org/10.1016/0031-9384(94)90373-5 218–224. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.foodqual.2011.10.012
Nunes, P., Muxagata, S., Cosme, F., Nunes, F. M., Correia, A. C., & Thomas, A., Chambault, M., Dreyfuss, L., Gilbert, C., Hegyi, A., Henneberg, S.,
Jordão, A. M. (2017). Effect of oak wood barrel capacity and utilization … Schlich, P. (2015). Temporal drivers of liking in 6 European countries.
time on phenolic and sensorial profile evolution of an Encruzado white Advances in data collection and analysis. Poster presentation at the 11th
wine. Journal of the Science of Food and Agriculture, 97(14), Pangborn Sensory Science Symposium, Gothenburg, Sweden.
4847–4856. https://doi.org/10.1002/jsfa.8355 Thomas, A., Visalli, M., Cordelle, S., & Schlich, P. (2015). Temporal drivers
Peltier, C., Visalli, M., & Schlich, P. (2015). Canonical variate analysis of of liking. Food Quality and Preference, 40, 365–375.
sensory profiling data. Journal of Sensory Studies, 30(4), 316–328. Thomas, A., van der Stelt, A., Prokop, J., Lawlor, J., & Schlich, P. (2016).
https://doi.org/10.1111/joss.12160 Alternating temporal dominance of sensations and liking scales during
Pineau, N., de Bouillé, A. G., Lepage, M., Lenfant, F., Schlich, P., the intake of a full portion of an oral nutritional supplement. Food Qual-
Martin, N., & Rytz, A. (2012). Temporal dominance of sensations: What ity and Preference, 53, 159–167.
is a good attribute list? Food Quality and Preference, 26(2), 159–165. Thomson, D. M. (2016). Conceptual profiling, in Meiselman, Herbert L.
Pineau, N., Schlich, P., Cordelle, S., Mathonnière, C., Issanchou, S., (ed), Emotion Measurement, 239–272. Woodhead Publishing.
Imbert, A., … Köster, E. (2009). Temporal dominance of sensations: van Zyl, H. (2016). Emotion in Beverages, in Meiselman, Herbert L. (ed),
Construction of the TDS curves and comparison with time–intensity. Emotion Measurement, 473–499. Woodhead Publishing.
Food Quality and Preference, 20(6), 450–455. Wang, Q., & Spence, C. (2015). Assessing the effect of musical congruency
Piqueras-Fiszman, B., & Jaeger, S. R. (2016). Consumer segmentation as a on wine tasting in a live performance setting. Perception, 6(3),
means to investigate emotional associations to meals. Appetite, 105, 2041669515593027.
249–258. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2016.05.034 Wansink, B. (2004). Environmental factors that increase the food intake
Porcherot, C., Petit, E., Giboreau, A., Gaudreau, N., & Cayeux, I. (2015). Mea- and consumption volume of unknowing consumers. Annual Review of
surement of self-reported affective feelings when an aperitif is con- Nutrition, 24, 455–479.
sumed in an ecological setting. Food Quality and Preference, 39, 277–284. Zorn, S., Alcaire, F., Vidal, L., Giménez, A., & Ares, G. (2014). Application of
Prescott, J., Lee, S. M., & Kim, K.-O. (2011). Analytic approaches to evaluation multiple-sip temporal dominance of sensations to the evaluation of
modify hedonic responses. Food Quality and Preference, 22(4), 391–393. sweeteners. Food Quality and Preference, 36, 135–143. https://doi.
Quartier, K., Vanrie, J., & Van Cleempoel, K. (2014). As real as it gets: What org/10.1016/j.foodqual.2014.04.003
role does lighting have on consumer's perception of atmosphere, emo-
tions and behaviour? Journal of Environmental Psychology, 39, 32–39.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2014.04.005
How to cite this article: Silva AP, Voss H-P, van Zyl H, et al.
Robinson, E., Blissett, J., & Higgs, S. (2011). Peak and end effects on remem-
bered enjoyment of eating in low and high restrained eaters. Appetite, Temporal dominance of sensations, emotions, and temporal
57(1), 207–212. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2011.04.022 liking measured in a bar for two similar wines using a multi-sip
Rodrigues, J. F., de Souza, V. R., Lima, R. R., Carneiro, J. d. D. S., approach. J Sens Stud. 2018;e12459. https://doi.org/10.1111/
Nunes, C. A., & Pinheiro, A. C. M. (2016). Temporal dominance of sen-
joss.12459
sations (TDS) panel behavior: A preliminary study with chocolate. Food
Quality and Preference, 54, 51–57.

You might also like