You are on page 1of 8

The Neural Correlates of Emotional Memory in

Posttraumatic Stress Disorder


Kathryn Handwerger Brohawn, Reid Offringa, Danielle L. Pfaff, Katherine C. Hughes, and Lisa M. Shin
Background: Posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) is marked by intrusive, chronic, and distressing memories of highly emotional events.
Previous research has highlighted the role of the amygdala and its interactions with the hippocampus in mediating the effect of enhanced
memory for emotional information in healthy individuals. As the functional integrity of these regions may be compromised in PTSD, the
current study examined the neural correlates of emotional memory in PTSD.

Methods: We used functional magnetic resonance imaging and an event-related subsequent memory recognition paradigm to study
amygdala and hippocampus activation in 18 individuals with PTSD and 18 trauma-exposed non-PTSD control participants.

Results: Memory enhancement for negative, relative to neutral, pictures was found across all subjects, without significant differences
between groups. Relative to the trauma-exposed non-PTSD group, the PTSD group showed exaggerated amygdala activation during the
encoding of negative versus neutral pictures. This effect was even more pronounced when the analysis included data from only pictures that
were subsequently remembered 1 week later. In the PTSD group, degree of amygdala activation during the encoding of negative versus
neutral pictures was positively correlated with hippocampal activation and current PTSD symptom severity. The PTSD group also showed
exaggerated hippocampal activation in response to negative pictures that were remembered versus forgotten. Finally, hippocampal
activation associated with the successful encoding of negative relative to neutral pictures was significantly greater in the PTSD group.

Conclusions: Exaggerated amygdala activation during the encoding of emotionally negative stimuli in PTSD is related to symptom severity
and to hippocampal activation.

Key Words: Amygdala, emotion, hippocampus, magnetic reso- Importantly, amygdala activation has been shown to be in-
nance imaging, memory, neuroimaging, posttraumatic, stress creased in PTSD during the recollection of traumatic events and the
disorders viewing of trauma-unrelated emotionally negative stimuli (14 –19).
Furthermore, degree of amygdala activation is positively correlated
motionally arousing stimuli or events are typically remem- with PTSD symptom severity (e.g., 17,18,20,21). Given this exagger-

E bered better than neutral ones (1). This phenomenon can be


beneficial, as it helps us to remember sources of potential
danger and avoid them in the future. However, memories of highly
ated amygdala activation, individuals with PTSD may show en-
hanced memory for novel emotional stimuli. Indeed, this behav-
ioral finding has been documented in PTSD ([11,22–24]; but see
[25,26]).
emotional events can become intrusive, chronic, and extremely
distressing, as in the case of individuals with posttraumatic stress The effect of emotional arousal on memory may also depend on
disorder (PTSD). The mediating neuroanatomy of enhanced mem- amygdala-hippocampal interactions (5,27). Although some studies
ory for emotional information has been studied extensively in ro- have reported diminished hippocampal activation in PTSD (26,28 –
dents and healthy humans, but little is known about the neural 32), others have reported exaggerated hippocampal activation
correlates of this phenomenon in individuals with PTSD. (31,33–38). The direction of hippocampal abnormalities in PTSD
Neuroscience research has revealed that the effect of emotion may depend on the type of task and/or analysis performed (39).
on memory is mediated, at least in part, by activation in the amyg- Admon et al. (40) recently reported a delicate interplay between the
dala and by interactions between the amygdala and hippocampus amygdala and hippocampus in response to stressful life events.
(2–9). These two structures are anatomically connected, and effer- While prestress amygdala activation was associated with greater
ent projections from the amygdala to the hippocampus are symptoms after stress, such symptoms also depended on the de-
thought to play a role in the emotional modulation of memory gree of hippocampal plasticity following stress, highlighting the
(3,10). While the amygdala appears to be responsible for assigning need to study both structures in trauma-exposed individuals.
emotional significance to stimuli, the hippocampus assigns contex- Recently, subsequent memory paradigms have highlighted the
tual meaning to them (11). Basic research has highlighted the role role of the amygdala and hippocampus in the emotional modula-
of norepinephrine and glucocorticoids in mediating the effects of tion of memory in healthy humans (6,41). In these paradigms, brain
emotion on memory via their actions on these two structures activation during encoding is analyzed based on whether items are
(2,12,13). later remembered or forgotten (42,43). The term difference due to
memory (Dm) refers to greater brain activation for remembered
than forgotten items and is thought to reflect successful encoding
From the Department of Psychology (KHB, RO, DLP, KCH, LMS), Tufts Univer-
processes (44,45). In healthy individuals, amygdala and hippocam-
sity, Medford; Department of Psychiatry (KHB, KCH, LMS), Massachusetts
pal/parahippocampal activation are associated with successful
General Hospital and Harvard Medical School, Boston; and Schizophre-
nia and Bipolar Disorders Program (DLP), McLean Hospital, Belmont, subsequent memory of emotional relative to neutral stimuli (41,43).
Massachusetts. Whether the neural correlates of the successful subsequent mem-
Address correspondence to Kathryn Handwerger Brohawn, Ph.D., Tufts Uni- ory of emotional stimuli differ in individuals with PTSD is unclear.
versity, Department of Psychology, 490 Boston Avenue, Medford, MA To date, only two studies have used subsequent emotional
02155; E-mail: katiebrohawn@gmail.com. memory paradigms to examine amygdala and hippocampal activa-
Received Dec 8, 2009; revised Jul 14, 2010; accepted Jul 16, 2010. tion in PTSD. Neither study explored a direct functional relationship

0006-3223/$36.00 BIOL PSYCHIATRY 2010;68:1023–1030


doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.07.018 © 2010 Society of Biological Psychiatry
1024 BIOL PSYCHIATRY 2010;68:1023–1030 K. Handwerger Brohawn et al.

Table 1. Demographic and Psychometric Data

PTSD TENP Stats


Mean SD Range Mean SD Range t(34) p

Age 28.2 7.8 19–46 26.2 4.5 20–36 .914 .369


Education 15.0 2.3 10–18 16.8 1.6 14–20 2.765 .009
CAPS Current 57.7 15.8 34–85 3.6 5.9 0–24 13.620 ⬍.001
CAPS Lifetime 86.1 12.0 66–105 11.3 8.8 0–33 21.254 ⬍.001
BDI 15.3 12.0 0–41 1.6 1.7 0–5 4.810 ⬍.001
BDI, Beck Depression Inventory; CAPS, Clinician Administered PTSD Scale; PTSD, posttraumatic stress disorder;
TENP, trauma-exposed non-PTSD.

between these structures. Dickie et al. (46) examined subsequent or physical abuse, assault, and witnessing serious injury/death. Ac-
memory for fearful and neutral faces in PTSD. They reported a cording to the Clinician-Administered PTSD Scale for DSM-IV (CAPS)
significant positive correlation between PTSD symptom severity (49), 21 had current PTSD and 21 never had PTSD (TENP group).
and amygdala activation in response to successfully remembered Participants were right-handed (50) with no history of head injury,
fearful versus neutral faces. However, because all subjects had neurological disorders, or other major medical conditions. No par-
PTSD, whether those with PTSD differ from trauma-exposed indi- ticipants were pregnant or using psychotropic or cardiovascular
viduals without the disorder is unknown. More recently, Thomaes et medication at the time of study. Two PTSD participants were ex-
al. (37) used a subsequent memory paradigm to study nine individ- cluded from analyses due to excessive head movement during
uals with PTSD associated with childhood abuse and multiple co- scanning. Three TENP participants were excluded due to below-
morbid psychiatric disorders. Relative to the control group, the chance memory performance. One PTSD participant failed to return
PTSD group showed significantly greater hippocampal activation in for the memory test. The final sample consisted of 18 (3 male) in the
response to subsequently remembered deeply encoded negative PTSD group and 18 (6 male) in the TENP group (Table 1).
words compared with a low-level baseline (but not when compared The Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (51) was used to
with neutral words). However, this study had a small sample size, assess other Axis I disorders. Current comorbidity included major
used a trauma-unexposed control group, and reported no group depression (n ⫽ 4 PTSD, n ⫽ 1 TENP), panic disorder (n ⫽ 2 PTSD),
differences in the amygdala. and specific phobia (n ⫽ 1 PTSD). The Partners Healthcare System
In the current study, we examined the neural correlates of the (Boston, Massachusetts) Institutional Review Board approved this
emotional modulation of memory in PTSD. Specifically, we used study. Written informed consent was obtained from each partici-
functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to study amygdala pant.
and hippocampal function during the encoding of negative, com-
pared with neutral, pictures in trauma-exposed individuals with Materials
and without PTSD. A recognition memory test was administered Stimuli consisted of 80 negative, 80 neutral, and 80 positive
outside of the scanner 1 week later. We predicted that both groups pictures selected from the International Affective Picture System
would remember more negative pictures than neutral pictures, but (IAPS) (52). The negative and positive pictures did not differ on
given that PTSD is primarily a disorder of intrusive emotional mem- normative arousal ratings. Pictures within each type were divided
ories, we predicted that the PTSD group would remember more into two separate sets (A and B); one set was used for encoding and
negative versus neutral pictures than the trauma-exposed non- the other as foils in the subsequent recognition test. Sets were
PTSD (TENP) group. Additionally, we predicted that the PTSD group counterbalanced across subjects in each group and were matched
would show exaggerated amygdala and hippocampal activation for valence and arousal ratings within each picture type (negative,
during the encoding of negative versus neutral pictures, especially neutral, positive).
those that were subsequently remembered. Our prediction of ex-
aggerated amygdala activation in PTSD was based on similar previ- Experimental Design
ous findings in the literature (e.g., 21,47,48). Our prediction of Functional magnetic resonance imaging data were gathered
greater hippocampal activation in PTSD was based on a previous while participants viewed the stimuli, which were presented in an
finding (37) and on our reasoning that enhanced memory for neg- event-related paradigm via the stimulus-presentation program
ative versus neutral pictures in PTSD should be accompanied by MacStim (MacStim 3.2.1; Darby, White Ant Occasional Publishing;
greater hippocampal activation. Furthermore, based on the find- West Melbourne, Australia) using a Sharp Notevision6 (XG-NV6XU)
ings of subsequent memory studies (6,41), we predicted that fMRI LCD projector (Osaka, Japan). Each full-color PICT file was presented
signal changes in the amygdala and hippocampus would be posi- for 5 seconds followed by a fixation cross ranging in duration from 1
tively correlated, especially in the PTSD group. Finally, based on to 11 seconds. The duration of each of the two functional scans was
previous findings (17,18,20,21,46), we predicted that fMRI signal 8 minutes, 8 seconds. Immediately following the scan, participants
changes in the amygdala would be positively correlated with symp- completed the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI) to quantify de-
tom severity in the PTSD group. Due to limited prior evidence, we pressed mood (53).
had no predictions regarding a correlation between hippocampal One week later, participants returned to the laboratory. At this
activation and symptom severity. visit, they completed a surprise recognition memory test outside of
the scanner. Participants viewed all 240 pictures (sets A and B), only
Methods and Materials half of which they saw during fMRI scanning. Participants indicated
whether they had seen the picture during scanning (old) or
Participants whether it was a novel picture that they had not previously seen
Participants were 42 individuals who reported experiencing cri- (new). They also reported their confidence in each selection on a
terion A traumatic events, including motor vehicle accidents, sexual three-point scale. After completing the memory test, participants

www.sobp.org/journal
K. Handwerger Brohawn et al. BIOL PSYCHIATRY 2010;68:1023–1030 1025

again viewed the 120 pictures they originally saw during scanning ative Dm versus neutral Dm comparison showed the difference in
and rated each picture on 9-point Likert scales of arousal (1 ⫽ calm, BOLD signal between NegR ⫺ NegF and NeutR ⫺ NeutF.
9 ⫽ excited) and valence (1 ⫽ negative, 5 ⫽ neutral, 9 ⫽ positive). The negative versus neutral contrast ought to best probe the
role of the amygdala, as it compares different emotion conditions
and does not take subsequent memory into consideration. In the
fMRI Procedures
NegR versus NegF contrast (i.e., negative Dm), one would expect
Scans were obtained from a Symphony/Sonata 1.5 Tesla whole
primary involvement of the hippocampus, as the emotional va-
body scanner (Siemens Medical Systems, Iselin, New Jersey) with a
lence is held steady on both sides of the contrast, and the subse-
three-axis gradient head coil. After shimming procedures were per-
quent memory of stimuli is of key interest. The negative Dm versus
formed (54), high-resolution, three-dimensional, magnetization-
neutral Dm contrast assesses the role of emotion in the successful
prepared rapid acquisition gradient-echo scans (repetition time/
subsequent memory of stimuli and should reveal activation in both
echo time/flip angle ⫽ 2730 msec/3.39 msec/7°; slice thickness ⫽
the amygdala and hippocampus.
1.33 mm) were collected. Functional magnetic resonance imaging
For each of these contrasts, our general approach was the same.
(blood oxygen-level dependent [BOLD]) (55) images were acquired
First, we created a voxelwise contrast image for each subject. Next,
using a gradient echo T2*-weighted sequence (repetition time/
we submitted the contrast images for all subjects to a voxelwise,
echo time/flip angle ⫽ 2000 msec/40 msec/90°). Functional images
one-sample t test to assess the main effect of condition (collapsing
were collected in 26 coronal slices angled perpendicular to the
across groups). If significant activations were found in our a priori
anterior commissure-posterior commissure line (slice thickness ⫽ 4
regions of interest (amygdala and hippocampus), we used the
mm, skip ⫽ 1 mm; voxel size ⫽ 3.1 ⫻ 3.1 ⫻ 4 mm).
MarsBaR program (MRC Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit, Cam-
bridge, United Kingdom) to extract fMRI data from all voxels within
Behavioral Analysis the clusters of significantly activated voxels. Extracted data were
Hits (number of pictures correctly remembered 1 week later), then submitted to independent sample t tests (in SPSS; SPSS Inc,
misses, false alarms, correct rejections, and the sensitivity index (d’) Chicago, Illinois) to determine whether fMRI signal changes differed
for each picture type (negative, neutral, positive) were calculated between PTSD and TENP groups. This method of analysis (21) cre-
for each participant. The d’, a signal detection statistic, was included ates an unbiased functional region of interest (ROI) and thus per-
because it takes into account both hits and false alarms (d’ ⫽ Z[hit] mits the use of a .05 significance threshold for the subsequent
– Z[false alarm]). Hit rate and d’ data were analyzed using two (two-tailed) between-group comparisons on extracted data. This
separate 2(group: PTSD, TENP) ⫻ 3(picture-type: negative, neutral, method also benefits from the ability to use a larger sample size
positive) analyses of variance (ANOVAs). However, preliminary anal- when creating the functional ROI. Whole-brain voxelwise between-
ysis of behavioral data revealed no significant differences on any of group analyses were also conducted, and those results were nearly
the memory measures between positive and neutral pictures either identical to those reported herein. Additionally, whole-brain voxel-
between or within diagnostic groups. Additionally, positive pic- wise analyses confirmed that BOLD signal changes in our a priori
tures were reported as significantly less arousing than negative regions of interest were indeed in the same direction in both
pictures, despite initial matching of pictures based on IAPS nor- groups, thus further supporting the use of this method of analysis.
mative ratings. Given this, we limit our reported fMRI analyses to For all contrasts in which activation was found in both amygdala
comparisons between negative and neutral pictures only. Hit and hippocampus, bivariate correlations were run between the
rate and d’ data were then analyzed using two separate 2(group: extracted values from these regions within groups to test our hy-
PTSD, TENP) ⫻ 2(picture-type: negative, neutral) ANOVAs. potheses concerning the relationship between activation of these
two structures. Extracted values from the amygdala and hippocam-
fMRI Data Analysis pus were also tested for correlations with CAPS and BDI scores
Image preprocessing and statistical analyses were performed within groups.
using SPM2 (http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/;Wellcome Depart-
ment of Imaging Neuroscience, London, United Kingdom). Func- Statistics
tional data were motion-corrected, coregistered to subjects’ ana- The voxelwise statistical parametric maps resulting from the
tomical images, spatially normalized into a standard stereotaxic one-sample t tests (main effect of condition) were inspected for
space (Montreal Neurological Institute [MNI]), and spatially activations in a priori regions of interest. Given our strong, direc-
smoothed using a 4-mm Gaussian kernel. The BOLD responses were tional a priori hypotheses, we used a significance threshold of
modeled as events convolved with the canonical hemodynamic p ⬍ .001, uncorrected (z score ⱖ 3.09) for activations in these
response function in SPM2. For each condition (negative, neutral, regions. Because the procedure of correcting p values based on
and fixation), all trials were averaged to estimate BOLD responses. the region size is biased toward finding significance in relatively
Five main contrasts of interest were examined. In the negative small structures, we chose to employ the above-stated constant
versus neutral contrast, BOLD signal in response to all negative significance threshold (17,48). For regions about which we had
pictures was compared with that of all neutral pictures. In the neg- no a priori prediction, we used a more conservative constant
ative remembered (NegR) versus neutral remembered (NeutR) significance threshold of p ⬍ .00001, uncorrected (z score ⱖ
comparison, BOLD signal in response to negative pictures that were 4.27) (31,48).
subsequently remembered was compared with that of neutral pic-
tures that were subsequently remembered. The NegR versus nega- Results
tive forgotten (NegF) contrast (i.e., negative Dm) examined BOLD
signal in response to negative pictures that were subsequently Valence/Arousal Ratings
remembered relative to those that were forgotten. The NeutR ver- Two separate 2(group: PTSD, TENP) ⫻ 2(picture type: negative,
sus neutral forgotten (NeutF) contrast (i.e., neutral Dm) examined neutral) ANOVAs were used to analyze valence and arousal ratings.
BOLD signal in response to neutral pictures that were subsequently Valence. The main effect of picture type was significant
remembered relative to those that were forgotten. Lastly, the neg- [F (1,34) ⫽ 89.77, p ⬍ .001]. Negative pictures were rated signifi-

www.sobp.org/journal
1026 BIOL PSYCHIATRY 2010;68:1023–1030 K. Handwerger Brohawn et al.

Figure 1. The functional image on the left displays activation in the right amygdala (Montreal Neurological Institute ⫽ 26, ⫺2, ⫺18; z ⫽ 4.52) in the negative ⬎ neutral
condition collapsing across groups. The bar graph on the right shows magnetic resonance signal change in this region relative to fixation baseline by diagnostic
status. Fix, fixation baseline; Neg, negative; Neut, neutral; PTSD, posttraumatic stress disorder; TENP, trauma-exposed non-PTSD.

cantly lower in valence than neutral pictures. The main effect of fMRI Results
group and the picture type by group interaction were not signifi- Negative Versus Neutral. Collapsing across groups, the neg-
cant (p’s ⬎ .33). ative versus neutral comparison revealed significant activation in
Arousal. The main effect of picture type was significant both the amygdala and hippocampus bilaterally and in other un-
[F (1,34) ⫽ 109.45, p ⬍ .001]. Negative pictures were rated signifi- predicted regions (Table S1 in Supplement 1). Functional magnetic
cantly more arousing than neutral pictures. The main effect of resonance imaging data were extracted from significant functional
group and the picture type by group interaction were not signifi- activations (ROIs) in the amygdala and hippocampus. Independent
cant (p’s ⬎ .15). sample t tests on the differences scores of the extracted data re-
vealed significantly greater right amygdala activation in the PTSD
Memory Performance compared with the TENP group (Figure 1).
Two separate 2(group: PTSD, TENP) ⫻ 2(picture type: negative, In the PTSD group, significant positive correlations were found
neutral) ANOVAs were used to analyze hits and d’. With regard to between BOLD signal increases in the right hippocampus (MNI ⫽
hits, the main effect of picture type was significant [F (1,34) ⫽ 71.85, 22, ⫺28, ⫺6) and in both the left [r(16) ⫽ .564, p ⫽ .015: MNI ⫽ ⫺18,
p ⬍ .001]. Negative pictures were remembered significantly better ⫺4, ⫺14] and right [r(16) ⫽ .513, p ⫽ .029: MNI ⫽ 18, ⫺4, ⫺12;
than neutral pictures (Figure S1 in Supplement 1). The main effect Figure 2A] amygdala. In the TENP group, no such correlations were
of group and the picture type by group interaction were not signif- found between BOLD signal in the right hippocampus and either
icant (p’s ⬎ .25). The same pattern of results was found when the left [r(16) ⫽ .037, p ⫽ .883] or right [r(16) ⫽ ⫺.005, p ⫽ .983]
assessing d’: only the main effect of picture type was significant amygdala. Additionally, PTSD subjects’ current CAPS scores were
[F (1,34) ⫽ 9.299, p ⫽ .004] (all other p’s ⬎ .80). positively correlated with activation in the left [r(16) ⫽ .563, p ⫽
There were no significant correlations between memory mea- .015; Figure 2B] but not right [r(16) ⫽ .339, p ⫽ .169] amygdala.
sures (hits or d’ for either valence category) and CAPS or BDI scores. Analogous correlations were not found in the TENP group [left:

Figure 2. (A) Correlation between blood oxygen-level dependent activation in the right hippocampus (Montreal Neurological Institute [MNI] ⫽ 22, ⫺28, ⫺6)
and right amygdala (MNI ⫽ 18, ⫺4, ⫺12) in the negative ⬎ neutral contrast in the posttraumatic stress disorder group only (r ⫽ .513, p ⫽ .029). (B) Correlation
between blood oxygen-level dependent activation in the left amygdala (MNI ⫽ ⫺26, ⫺4, ⫺22) and current Clinician-Administered PTSD Scale for DSM-IV
scores in the negative ⬎ neutral contrast in the posttraumatic stress disorder group only (r ⫽ .563, p ⫽ .015). CAPS, Clinician-Administered PTSD Scale for
DSM-IV; PTSD, posttraumatic stress disorder.

www.sobp.org/journal
K. Handwerger Brohawn et al. BIOL PSYCHIATRY 2010;68:1023–1030 1027

Figure 3. (A) The functional image on the left displays activation in the right amygdala (Montreal Neurological Institute ⫽ 26, ⫺2, ⫺20; z ⫽ 4.31) in the
negative remembered ⬎ neutral remembered condition collapsing across groups. The bar graph on the right shows magnetic resonance signal change in this
region relative to fixation baseline by diagnostic status. (B) The functional image on the left displays activation in the left amygdala (Montreal Neurological
Institute ⫽ ⫺20, ⫺8, ⫺12; z ⫽ 4.46) in the negative remembered ⬎ neutral remembered condition collapsing across groups. The bar graph on the right shows
magnetic resonance signal change in this region relative to fixation baseline by diagnostic status. NegRvFix, negative remembered relative to fixation
baseline; NeutRvFix, neutral remembered relative to fixation baseline; PTSD, posttraumatic stress disorder; TENP, trauma-exposed non-PTSD.

r(16) ⫽ ⫺.340, p ⫽ .168; right: r(16) ⫽ ⫺.143, p ⫽ .573]. No Figure 4B] but not in the TENP group [r(14) ⫽ .062, p ⫽ .410].
significant correlations were found between BDI scores and Activation in the right hippocampus was positively correlated with
amygdala or hippocampal activation in either group. BDI scores in the PTSD group [r(16) ⫽ .512, p ⫽ .030].
Negative Remembered Versus Neutral Remembered. Col- Negative Dm Versus Neutral Dm. Collapsing across groups,
lapsing across groups, we found significantly greater activation in this analysis revealed significantly greater activation in the negative
response to subsequently remembered negative pictures than to Dm versus the neutral Dm in the amygdala bilaterally and the right
subsequently remembered neutral pictures in the left hippocam- hippocampus. Amygdala activation did not significantly differ be-
pus and bilateral amygdala. Extracted values in these functionally tween groups. The PTSD group had significantly greater hippocam-
defined ROIs were submitted to two separate independent sam- pal activation than the TENP group [t (32) ⫽ 2.211, p ⫽ .034; Figure
ple t tests. Activation in the right [t (34) ⫽ 2.122, p ⫽ .041] and left 5]. No significant correlations were found between activation in
[t (34) ⫽ 2.053, p ⫽ .048] amygdala was significantly greater in the either the amygdala or hippocampus and CAPS or BDI in either
PTSD group than the TENP group (Figure 3). In the PTSD group only, group.
a significant positive correlation was found between activation in All analyses that yielded significant findings were repeated after
the left hippocampus and the left amygdala [r(16) ⫽ .614, p ⫽ .007; temporarily removing the data of participants with current comor-
Figure 4A]. The analogous correlation was not found in the TENP bid major depression. All findings remained significant, with one
group [r (16) ⫽ .082, p ⫽ .746]. No significant correlations were exception: the p value for the between-group difference in the right
found between activation in these NegR versus NeutR ROIs and amygdala in the NegR versus NeutR contrast changed from .041 to
CAPS or BDI scores in the PTSD group. .075. Additionally, despite group differences in years of education,
Negative Remembered Versus Negative Forgotten. Col- there were no significant correlations between years of education
lapsing across groups, we found significantly greater activation in and amygdala or hippocampal activation in any of the ROIs defined
response to negative pictures that were subsequently remembered in the aforementioned contrasts in either group.
than to those that were forgotten in the right amygdala and hip-
pocampus bilaterally. No between-group differences in amygdala Discussion
activation were found. Right hippocampal activation was greater in
the PTSD group than in the TENP group [t (34) ⫽ 1.919, p ⫽ .064]. Our subsequent recognition memory paradigm revealed the
Activation in the right amygdala was positively correlated with expected memory enhancement for negative versus neutral pic-
current CAPS scores in the PTSD group [r(16) ⫽ .451, p ⫽ .030; tures after a 1-week retention interval. Contrary to our hypotheses,

www.sobp.org/journal
1028 BIOL PSYCHIATRY 2010;68:1023–1030 K. Handwerger Brohawn et al.

Figure 4. (A) Correlation between blood oxygen-level dependent activation in the left hippocampus (Montreal Neurological Institute [MNI] ⫽ ⫺18, ⫺28, ⫺6)
and left amygdala (MNI ⫽ ⫺20, ⫺8, ⫺12) in the negative remembered ⬎ neutral remembered condition in the posttraumatic stress disorder group only (r ⫽
.614, p ⫽ .007). (B) Correlation between blood oxygen-level dependent activation in the right amygdala (MNI ⫽ 22, 4, ⫺16) and current Clinician-Administered
PTSD Scale for DSM-IV score in the negative remembered ⬎ negative forgotten condition in the posttraumatic stress disorder group only (r ⫽ .451, p ⫽ .030).
CAPS, Clinician-Administered PTSD Scale for DSM-IV; PTSD, posttraumatic stress disorder.

this effect was not significantly greater in the PTSD group than the The degree of hippocampal activation in both the negative ver-
TENP group. This finding is contrary to previous reports of better sus neutral and negative remembered versus neutral remembered
memory for emotional versus neutral material in PTSD (11,22–24) contrasts did not differ between groups. Similarly, Thomaes et al.
but is consistent with the findings of Bremner et al. (26), who found (37) did not find enhanced hippocampal activation in response to
no evidence for enhanced memory for emotional versus neutral negative words in PTSD when using neutral pictures as a compari-
word pairs in PTSD. Our 1-week retention interval may not have son condition. However, our finding of a significant positive corre-
been long enough to reveal behavioral between-group differences lation between amygdala and hippocampal activation in these con-
(both groups remembered over 80% of the negative stimuli after 1 trasts only in the PTSD group supports the idea of exaggerated
week). Additionally, group differences in memory may be more functional connectivity between these two structures in PTSD. Ad-
likely to emerge when the stimuli are more directly related to par- ditionally, hippocampal activation was greater in the PTSD group
ticipants’ traumatic events. However, a lack of behavioral differ- than the TENP group when directly assessing remembered (vs.
ences between groups may actually make the between-group dif- forgotten) negative pictures.
ferences in brain activation more interpretable. Consistent with previous research (41), greater bilateral amyg-
Our findings of exaggerated amygdala activation in the PTSD dala activation was found in response to the negative Dm than to
group relative to the TENP group during the encoding of negative the neutral Dm in both groups pooled together. However, this
versus neutral pictures (especially those that were subsequently amygdala activation did not differ between diagnostic groups. This
remembered) add to the existing literature documenting exag- lack of a between-group difference may help account for the lack of
gerated amygdala responses in PTSD (e.g., [14,15,17]; for a re- a behavioral difference between groups. However, we did find
view see [56]). Additionally, our correlation between amygdala greater hippocampal activation in PTSD subjects than in TENP sub-
activation during the encoding of negative stimuli and symptom jects in response to the negative Dm versus neutral Dm contrast.
severity in the PTSD group parallels that of Dickie et al. (46). Positive Splitting subjects by diagnosis revealed that only the PTSD group
correlations between symptom severity and amygdala activation displayed significant hippocampal activation in response to this
have also been reported in previous studies (17,18,20,21). contrast. This finding lies in contrast to that of Dolcos et al. (41), who

Figure 5. The functional image on the left displays activation in the right hippocampus (Montreal Neurological Institute ⫽ 28, ⫺30, ⫺22; z ⫽ 3.46) in the
negative difference due to memory ⬎ neutral difference due to memory condition collapsing across groups. The bar graph on the right shows magnetic
resonance signal change in this region relative to fixation baseline by diagnostic status. NegRvFix, negative remembered relative to fixation baseline;
NeutRvFix, neutral remembered relative to fixation baseline; PTSD, posttraumatic stress disorder; TENP, trauma-exposed non-PTSD.

www.sobp.org/journal
K. Handwerger Brohawn et al. BIOL PSYCHIATRY 2010;68:1023–1030 1029

reported enhanced hippocampal activation in response to the Supplementary material cited in this article is available online.
emotional Dm versus neutral Dm in a sample of healthy individuals.
We were not able to replicate the Dm effect in our control group. 1. Christianson SA (1992): The Handbook of Emotion and Memory: Research
However, Dolcos et al. (41) used both positive and negative pictures and Theory. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
and a cued-recall task and a 45-minute retention interval, whereas the 2. McGaugh JL, Cahill L, Roozendaal B (1996): Involvement of the amyg-
current study used a recognition test and a 1-week retention interval. dala in memory storage: Interaction with other brain systems. Proc Natl
Acad Sci U S A 93:13508 –13514.
These paradigm differences may have accounted for differences in 3. McGaugh JL, McIntyre CK, Power AE (2002): Amygdala modulation of
results, as these two types of memory are believed to be mediated by memory consolidation: Interaction with other brain systems. Neurobiol
different regions within the medial temporal lobe, with recall requiring Learn Mem 78:539 –552.
more hippocampal involvement than recognition (57). 4. Strange BA, Dolan RJ (2004): Beta-adrenergic modulation of emotional
memory-evoked human amygdala and hippocampal responses. Proc
Natl Acad Sci U S A 101:11454 –11458.
Limitations and Conclusions 5. Richardson MP, Strange BA, Dolan RJ (2004): Encoding of emotional
In the current study, we excluded individuals who were taking memories depends on amygdala and hippocampus and their interac-
psychiatric medications and who had current comorbid disorders tions. Nat Neurosci 7:278 –285.
other than depression and anxiety disorders. While comorbid de- 6. Dolcos F, LaBar KS, Cabeza R (2005): Remembering one year later: Role of
the amygdala and the medial temporal lobe memory system in retriev-
pression in the PTSD group may be viewed as a limitation, given the ing emotional memories. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 102:2626 –2631.
high comorbidity rates between PTSD and depression (58), exclud- 7. van Stegeren AH, Goekoop R, Everaerd W, Scheltens P, Barkhof F, Kuijer
ing PTSD patients with depression would have severely compro- JP, Rombouts SA (2005): Noradrenaline mediates amygdala activation
mised external validity. Nearly all analyses remained significant in men and women during encoding of emotional material. Neuroimage
when data from participants with comorbid depression were tem- 24:898 –909.
porarily removed. In addition, in the PTSD group, amygdala activa- 8. van Stegeren AH (2008): The role of the noradrenergic system in emo-
tional memory. Acta Psychol 127:532–541.
tion was correlated with CAPS scores but not BDI scores. Neverthe-
9. Tsoory MM, Vouimba RM, Akirav I, Kavushansky A, Avital A, Richter-Levin
less, future studies of emotional memory in PTSD should employ G (2008): Amygdala modulation of memory-related processes in the
psychiatric control groups. It is also important to note that the hippocampus: Potential relevance to PTSD. Prog Brain Res 167:35–51.
restricted range of CAPS scores in the TENP group could explain the 10. Pikkarainen M, Ronkko S, Savander V, Insausti R, Pitkanen A (1999):
lack of significant correlations with BOLD signal changes in this Projections from the lateral, basal, and accessory basal nuclei of the
group. Finally, using the main effect of condition maps to define amygdala to the hippocampal formation in rat. J Comp Neurol 403:229 –
functional ROIs cannot reveal areas of functional activation in which 260.
11. Paunovic N, Lundh LG, Ost LG (2002): Attentional and memory bias for
the direction of effects differ between the PTSD and TENP groups. emotional information in crime victims with acute posttraumatic stress
However, whole-brain voxelwise analyses confirmed that BOLD sig- disorder (PTSD). J Anxiety Disord 16:675– 692.
nal changes in our a priori regions of interest were indeed in the 12. Cahill L, McGaugh JL (1998): Mechanisms of emotional arousal and
same direction in both groups, thus supporting use of this method. lasting declarative memory. Trends Neurosci 21:294 –299.
In conclusion, we document for the first time that amygdala 13. McGaugh JL (2000): Memory—a century of consolidation. Science 287:
activation related to the subsequent memory of negative, trauma- 248 –251.
14. Shin LM, McNally RJ, Kosslyn SM, Thompson WL, Rauch SL, Alpert NM, et
unrelated versus neutral pictures is exaggerated in PTSD. Addition- al. (1997): A positron emission tomographic study of symptom provo-
ally, enhanced hippocampal activation in PTSD is related to the cation in PTSD. Ann N Y Acad Sci 821:521–523.
successful encoding of negative pictures, and functional coupling 15. Liberzon I, Taylor SF, Amdur R, Jung TD, Chamberlain KR, Minoshima S,
between the amygdala and hippocampus during the encoding of et al. (1999): Brain activation in PTSD in response to trauma-related
negative pictures also appears exaggerated. Together, these find- stimuli. Biol Psychiatry 45:817– 826.
ings point toward an interaction between these two structures in 16. Pissiota A, Frans O, Fernandez M, von Knorring L, Fischer H, Fredrikson M
(2002): Neurofunctional correlates of posttraumatic stress disorder: A
the pathophysiology of this disorder. PET symptom provocation study. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 252:
That these functional abnormalities were not accompanied by 68 –75.
relatively enhanced memory for negative versus neutral stimuli 17. Shin LM, Orr SP, Carson MA, Rauch SL, Macklin ML, Lasko NB, et al. (2004):
suggests that fMRI measures may be more sensitive to group differ- Regional cerebral blood flow in amygdala and medial prefrontal cortex
ences than behavioral measures in this paradigm. Because our during traumatic imagery in male and female Vietnam veterans with
comparison group was exposed to criterion A trauma but did not PTSD. Arch Gen Psychiatry 61:168 –176.
18. Armony JL, Corbo V, Clement MH, Brunet A (2005): Amygdala response
have PTSD, trauma exposure cannot account for the group differ-
in patients with acute PTSD to masked and unmasked emotional facial
ences reported herein. Future studies using longitudinal or twin expressions. Am J Psychiatry 162:1961–1963.
designs should attempt to determine whether these functional 19. Liberzon I, Sripada CS (2008): The functional neuroanatomy of PTSD: A
abnormalities are acquired characteristics of PTSD or rather repre- critical review. Prog Brain Res 167:151–169.
sent risk factors for the development of PTSD after psychological 20. Protopopescu X, Pan H, Tuescher O, Cloitre M, Goldstein M, Engelien W,
trauma. et al. (2005): Differential time courses and specificity of amygdala activ-
ity in posttraumatic stress disorder subjects and normal control sub-
jects. Biol Psychiatry 57:464 – 473.
21. Rauch SL, Whalen PJ, Shin LM, McInerney SC, Macklin ML, Lasko NB, et al.
KHB was supported by a National Science Foundation graduate (2000): Exaggerated amygdala response to masked facial stimuli in
student fellowship. LMS received support from Tufts University and a posttraumatic stress disorder: A functional MRI study. Biol Psychiatry
National Alliance for Research on Schizophrenia and Depression 47:769 –776.
Young Investigator Award. 22. Vrana SR, Roodman A, Beckham JC (1995): Selective processing of trau-
We thank the individuals who served as research participants; ma-relevant words in posttraumatic stress disorder. J Anxiety Disord
9:515–530.
Doug Greve, Mary Foley, Clas Linnman, and Lawrence White for their 23. Amir N, McNally RJ, Wiegartz PS (1996): Implicit memory bias for threat
technical assistance; Caroline Davit for assistance with scanning and in posttraumatic stress disorder. Cognit Ther Res 20:625– 635.
participant recruitment; and Matt Nathanson, Lisanne Petracca, and 24. Moradi AR, Taghavi R, Neshat-Doost HT, Yule W, Dalgleish T (2000):
Elizabeth Hammond for pilot testing the paradigm. Memory bias for emotional information in children and adolescents

www.sobp.org/journal
1030 BIOL PSYCHIATRY 2010;68:1023–1030 K. Handwerger Brohawn et al.

with posttraumatic stress disorder: A preliminary study. J Anxiety Disord sition and hippocampal plasticity. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A
14:521–534. 106:14120 –14125.
25. McNally RJ, Amir N (1996): Perceptual implicit memory for trauma- 41. Dolcos F, LaBar KS, Cabeza R (2004): Interaction between the amygdala
related information in post-traumatic stress disorder. Cogn Emot 10: and the medial temporal lobe memory system predicts better memory
551–556. for emotional events. Neuron 42:855– 863.
26. Bremner JD, Vythilingam M, Vermetten E, Southwick SM, McGlashan T, 42. Wagner AD, Schacter DL, Rotte M, Koutstaal W, Maril A, Dale AM, et al.
Staib LH, et al. (2003): Neural correlates of declarative memory for emo- (1998): Building memories: Remembering and forgetting of verbal ex-
tionally valenced words in women with posttraumatic stress disorder periences as predicted by brain activity. Science 281:1188 –1191.
related to early childhood sexual abuse. Biol Psychiatry 53:879 – 889. 43. Brewer JB, Zhao Z, Desmond JE, Glover GH, Gabrieli JD (1998): Making
27. Phelps EA (2004): Human emotion and memory: Interactions of the memories: Brain activity that predicts how well visual experience will be
amygdala and hippocampal complex. Curr Opin Neurobiol 14:198 –202. remembered. Science 281:1185–1187.
28. Shin LM, McNally RJ, Kosslyn SM, Thompson WL, Rauch SL, Alpert NM, et 44. Paller KA, Kutas M, Mayes AR (1987): Neural correlates of encoding in an
al. (1999): Regional cerebral blood flow during script-driven imagery in incidental learning paradigm. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 67:
childhood sexual abuse-related PTSD: A PET investigation. Am J Psychi- 360 –371.
atry 156:575–584. 45. Paller KA, Wagner AD (2002): Observing the transformation of experi-
29. Bremner JD, Staib LH, Kaloupek D, Southwick SM, Soufer R, Charney DS ence into memory. Trends Cogn Sci 6:93–102.
(1999): Neural correlates of exposure to traumatic pictures and sound in 46. Dickie EW, Brunet A, Akerib V, Armony JL (2008): An fMRI investigation of
Vietnam combat veterans with and without posttraumatic stress disor- memory encoding in PTSD: Influence of symptom severity. Neuropsy-
der: A positron emission tomography study. Biol Psychiatry 45:806 – 816. chologia 46:1522–1531.
30. Bremner JD, Vythilingam M, Vermetten E, Southwick SM, McGlashan T, 47. Williams LM, Kemp AH, Felmingham K, Barton M, Olivieri G, Peduto A, et
Nazeer A, et al. (2003): MRI and PET study of deficits in hippocampal al. (2006): Trauma modulates amygdala and medial prefrontal re-
structure and function in women with childhood sexual abuse and sponses to consciously attended fear. Neuroimage 29:347–357.
posttraumatic stress disorder. Am J Psychiatry 160:924 –932. 48. Shin LM, Wright CI, Cannistraro PA, Wedig MM, McMullin K, Martis B, et
31. Shin LM, Shin PS, Heckers S, Krangel TS, Macklin ML, Orr SP, et al. (2004): al. (2005): A functional magnetic resonance imaging study of amygdala
Hippocampal function in posttraumatic stress disorder. Hippocampus
and medial prefrontal cortex responses to overtly presented fearful
14:292–300.
faces in posttraumatic stress disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry 62:273–281.
32. Astur RS, St. Germain SA, Tolin D, Ford J, Russell D, Stevens M (2006):
49. Blake DD, Weathers FW, Nagy LM, Kaloupek DG, Gusman FD, Charney
Hippocampus function predicts severity of post-traumatic stress disor-
DS, Keane TM (1995): The development of a Clinician-Administered
der. Cyberpsychol Behav 9:234 –240.
PTSD Scale. J Trauma Stress 8:75–90.
33. Semple WE, Goyer PF, McCormick R, Donovan B, Muzic RF Jr, Rugle L, et
50. Oldfield RC (1971): The assessment and analysis of handedness: The
al. (2000): Higher brain blood flow at amygdala and lower frontal cortex
blood flow in PTSD patients with comorbid cocaine and alcohol abuse Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia 9:97–113.
compared with normals. Psychiatry 63:65–74. 51. First M, Spitzer R, Gibbon M, Williams J (1997): Structured Clinical Inter-
34. Sachinvala N, Kling A, Suffin S, Lake R, Cohen M (2000): Increased re- view for DSM IV Axis 1 Disorders-Patient Edition. New York: State Psychi-
gional cerebral perfusion by 99mTc hexamethyl propylene amine atric Institute, Biometrics Research Department.
oxime single photon emission computed tomography in post-trau- 52. Lang PJ, Bradley MM, Cuthbert BN (1999): International Affective Picture
matic stress disorder. Mil Med 165:473– 479. System (IAPS): Instruction Manual and Affective Ratings Technical Report
35. Shin LM, Whalen PJ, Pitman RK, Bush G, Macklin ML, Lasko NB, et al. A-4. Gainsville, FL: University of Florida, Center for Research in Psycho-
(2001): An fMRI study of anterior cingulate function in posttraumatic physiology.
stress disorder. Biol Psychiatry 50:932–942. 53. Beck AT, Steer RA (1987): Manual for the Revised Beck Depression Inven-
36. Osuch EA, Benson B, Geraci M, Podell D, Herscovitch P, McCann UD, Post tory. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.
RM (2001): Regional cerebral blood flow correlated with flashback in- 54. Reese TG, Davis TL, Weisskoff RM (1995): Automated shimming at 1.5 T
tensity in patients with posttraumatic stress disorder. Biol Psychiatry using echo-planar image frequency maps. J Magn Reson Imaging 5:739 –
50:246 –253. 745.
37. Thomaes K, Dorrepaal E, Draijer NP, de Ruiter MB, Elzinga BM, van 55. Kwong KK, Belliveau JW, Chesler DA, Goldberg IE, Weisskoff RM, Ponce-
Balkom AJ, et al. (2009): Increased activation of the left hippocampus let BP, et al. (1992): Dynamic magnetic resonance imaging of human
region in complex PTSD during encoding and recognition of emotional brain activity during primary sensory stimulation. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A
words: A pilot study. Psychiatry Res 171:44 –53. 89:5675–5679.
38. Francati V, Vermetten E, Bremner JD (2007): Functional neuroimaging 56. Shin LM, Rauch SL, Pitman RK (2006): Amygdala, medial prefrontal cor-
studies in posttraumatic stress disorder: Review of current methods and tex, and hippocampal function in PTSD. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1071:67–79.
findings. Depress Anxiety 24:202–218. 57. Eichenbaum H, Yonelinas AR, Ranganath C (2007): The medial temporal
39. Shin LM, Liberzon I (2010): The neurocircuitry of fear, stress, and anxiety lobe and recognition memory. Annu Rev Neurosci 30:123–152.
disorders. Neuropsychopharmacology 35:169 –191. 58. Kessler RC, Sonnega A, Bromet E, Hughes M, Nelson CB (1995): Posttrau-
40. Admon R, Lubin G, Stern O, Rosenberg K, Sela L, Ben-Ami H, Hendler T matic stress disorder in the National Comorbidity Survey. Arch Gen
(2009): Human vulnerability to stress depends on amygdala’s predispo- Psychiatry 52:1048 –1060.

www.sobp.org/journal

You might also like