You are on page 1of 15

Biological Psychology 127 (2017) 108–122

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Biological Psychology
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/biopsycho

Information Processing of the Rorschach's Traumatic Content Index in MARK


Trauma-exposed Adults: An Event Related Potential (ERP) Study

Gil Zukermana, , Esther Ben - Itzchaka,1, Leah Fosticka,2, Rinat Armony-Sivanb,3
a
Department of Communication Disorders, School of Health Sciences, Ariel University, Ariel, Israel
b
Department of Psychology, Ashkelon Academic College, Ashkelon, Israel

A R T I C L E I N F O A B S T R A C T

Keywords: PTSD elicits hypervigilance to trauma-related stimuli. Our novel research examined event-related potentials
Post traumatic stress disorder from Blood, Anatomy, and Morbid content derived from the Rorschach's traumatic content index (TCI).
Event related potentials Participants included: 16 with PTSD, 24 trauma-exposed without PTSD (non-PTSD), and 16 non-traumatized
P3 Controls. P3 oddball paradigms were used with TCI-derived Distractors and neutral Targets/Standards. We
Oddball distractor paradigm
predicted larger P3 amplitudes in the context of TCI-related Distractors among trauma-exposed participants.
Rorschach inkblot test
Significant interaction of Group and Distractor type was found for P3 amplitude. PTSD and non-PTSD groups
Traumatic content index
exhibited larger P3 amplitudes from Blood and Anatomy Distractors, and attenuated amplitudes from Morbid;
the reverse pattern was found among Controls. A late negative component was observed, denoting a significantly
larger area under the curve (AUC) among the PTSD group for Anatomy and Blood Distractors. Larger AUC's were
observed for Distractors among the PTSD group, and Targets among Controls. The findings concur with the
neurocircuitry model of PTSD and suggest impairment in cerebral suppression of attention to stimuli that may
have been perceptually primed with trauma.

1. Introduction positive component occurring around 300 ms after stimulus onset, has
been widely used to examine trauma-related changes in attention (for
An exposure to a life-threatening event is sometimes followed by an review, see Javanbakht, Liberzon, Amirsadri, Gjini, & Boutros, 2011;
elevation of psychological distress due to the development of such Johnson, Allana, Medlin, Harris, & Karl, 2013). The P3 is commonly
symptoms as intrusive memories, hyper-arousal, and avoidance of elicited by a three-stimuli oddball paradigm, in which the participants
trauma-related stimuli. These post-traumatic stress (PTS) symptoms are requested to respond to a low frequency target stimulus presented
are usually followed by a feeling of constant threat to the individual's amongst high frequency, repetitive standard stimuli and low frequency
well-being (Ehlers & Clark, 2000) and the perception of the environ- salient distractors they must ignore; the stimuli are referred to as
ment as unstable and dangerous (Janoff-Bulman, 1989). In some cases, Target, Standard, and Distractor stimuli. It has been previously reported
severe PTS symptom prevalence beyond one month may lead to the that when individuals with PTSD are presented with an oddball
clinical diagnosis of a post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) (DSM-5, paradigm that includes trauma-related Distractors, the P3 amplitude
American Psychiatric Association, 2013). in response to Targets and Distractors (also known as P3b and P3a,
respectively) is enhanced, while in the context of neutral (not trauma-
1.1. ERP studies in PTSD related) Distractors, the response to either Target and Distractor stimuli
is reduced (Karl, Malta, & Maercker, 2006; Javanbakht et al., 2011;
Event-related potential (ERP) studies have been used to compare Johnson et al., 2013). PTS symptom severity has also been associated
information-processing patterns of individuals diagnosed with PTSD to with P3 amplitudes (Lobo et al., 2015). These findings suggest that,
healthy controls (Felmingham, Bryant, Kendall, & Gordon, 2002; compared to healthy controls, individuals diagnosed with PTSD exhibit
Galletly, Clarc, McFarlane, & Weber, 2001; McFarlane, Weber, & Clark, attentional alterations: allocating more attention to stimuli perceived to
1993). Among several ERP components, the P3, a centro-parietal be threatening or trauma-related, while reducing attention to neutral


Corresponding author. Tel./Fax: +972 39765755; Mobile: +972 547702468.
E-mail addresses: gilzu@ariel.ac.il (G. Zukerman), benitze@ariel.ac.il (E.B.-. Itzchak), leah.fostick@ariel.ac.il (L. Fostick), rinatsivan@gmail.com (R. Armony-Sivan).
1
Tel./Fax: +972 39765755.
2
Tel./Fax: +972 39765755.
3
Tel./Fax: +972 86789273.

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2017.05.002
Received 10 July 2016; Received in revised form 1 April 2017; Accepted 1 May 2017
Available online 10 May 2017
0301-0511/ © 2017 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
G. Zukerman et al. Biological Psychology 127 (2017) 108–122

stimuli (Johnson et al., 2013; Karl et al., 2006). This pattern of cerebral history, and have been associated with selective attention to negative
response was conceptualized at the cognitive level by the “resource stimuli; this has been postulated to represent a general “negativity bias”
allocation” model of PTSD (Ehlers & Clark, 2000). According to this among the general population (Olofsson, Nordin, Sequeira, & Polich,
model, trauma facilitates the development of a “fear network”, leading 2008). Therefore, it is not clear whether this increase in cerebral
to an attentional bias to trauma-related stimuli at the expense of neutral response to negative stimuli can be attributed solely to PTSD.
stimuli. These assertions also align with the “neurocircuitry” model of A common denominator of previous hypotheses is that patients with
PTSD that associates PTSD-related information processing changes and PTSD may be hypervigilant to the conceptual aspects of threatening
interaction patterns of cerebral structures (Rauch, Shin, Whalen,- stimuli, in which association with the traumatic event is achieved via
& Pitman, 1998). According to this neurocognitive model, PTSD meaning (for example, when a rape victim encounters the word
involves impaired prefrontal cortex (PFC) top-down regulation of “helplessness”). An alternative approach suggests that trauma-exposed
hyper-responsivity within the amygdala, along with alterations in participants may be highly responsive to the perceptual properties of
hippocampal activity that lead to deficits in contextual conditioning stimuli (Ehlers et al., 2002), in which an association between stimuli
(Rauch, Shin, & Phelps, 2006). PFC deficit, particularly in the ventral/ and the traumatic event is made through perceptual priming (for
medial PFC, which is thought to impair suppression of attention to example, seeing headlights leads to accelerated heart rate because they
trauma-related stimuli, might be expressed by larger P3 amplitudes to were previously encountered during a nighttime head-on collision). The
trauma-related stimuli. Both models are supported by the clinical perceptual priming approach postulates that the traumatic experience
manifestation of PTSD that includes hypervigilance to trauma-related leads to data-driven processing (i.e. focusing on the perceptual and
stimuli, exaggerated startle response and concentration difficulties sensory impression of an event rather than on its meaning). This results
(DSM-5, APA, 2013). Additionally, although most studies have reported in a strong perceptual priming of the various stimuli encountered in
on significant P3 differences between participants diagnosed with PTSD temporal proximity to the traumatic experience (Christianson, 1992;
and either non-PTSD traumatized participants or Control subjects with Halligan, Clark, & Ehlers, 2002; Kindt, van den Hout, Arntz, & Drost,
no previous traumatic history (Johnson et al., 2013), recent research 2008; Van der Kolk & van der Hart, 1989; Wing Lun, 2008). These
findings suggest that P3 alterations might be found among individuals primed stimuli, when identified in the everyday environment, are then
with previous traumatic exposure even without meeting clinical criteria perceived as a warning signal of impending danger and may lead to a
for PTSD diagnosis (Kimble, Fleming, Bandy, & Zambetti, 2010). rapid, vivid recollection of the traumatic event, experienced as a
Most conceptualizations of PTSD characterize the condition as “flashbacks.” This approach is supported by behavioral research studies
hyper-responsivity to incoming stimuli that has been coupled with which indicate that, in comparison to the general population, trauma-
traumatic experience. However, the nature of these associations and the tized individuals who develop PTSD symptoms are more likely to
mechanism by which some inputs become “trauma-related stimuli” is associate stimuli characterized by salient perceptual properties with
still not clear. Previous research findings suggest that those with PTSD trauma-related information (Halligan et al., 2002; Kindt et al., 2008;
exhibit elevated cerebral responses to stimuli that are associated with Lin, Hofmann, Qian, & Li, 2015). Accordingly, war veterans with PTSD
their specific traumatic experience, such as combat- or earthquake- have been found to demonstrate heightened physiologic arousal,
related stimuli, but not to trauma-irrelevant stimuli (Attias, Bliech, expressed by higher skin conductance and elevated heart rate, when
Furman, & Zinger, 1996a; Attias, Bliech, & Gilat, 1996b; Stanford, perceiving Rorschach Inkblots as being related to autobiographical
Vasterling, Mathias, Constans, & Houston, 2001; Zhang, Kong, Han, images of combat trauma. The authors postulated that this heightened
Najam Ul Hasan, & Chen, 2014; Zhang, Kong, Hasan, Jackson, & Chen, physiologic arousal to the Rorschach inkblots corresponds to the
2015). Other authors have hypothesized that cognitive alterations development of intrusive symptoms that occur when encountering a
related to PTSD are not limited to increased attention to specific stimulus with perceptual similarity to those encountered at the time of
trauma-associated stimuli but to a general increased expectancy of trauma (Goldfinger, Amdur, & Liberson, 1998). Since the Rorschach is
threat, resulting in elevated sensitivity to threat-related cues considered primarily a perception test (Blatt, 1990), this finding
(Engelhard, de Jong, van den Hout, & van Overveld, 2009; Kimble suggests that perceptual properties of inkblots may trigger a condi-
et al., 2010). Though some research suggests that PTSD-related tioned response mediated by altered information processing patterns.
hypersensitivity to threat exists even at earlier, subliminal levels, this In summary, current cognitive and neurocircuitry models suggest
attentional bias to threat was postulated to mainly occur in later, post- that PTSD involves directing more attention to stimuli that were
recognition stages of information processing (Buckley, Blanchard,- previously associated with traumatic experience. This was believed by
& Neill, 2000). The hypothesis of general sensitivity to threat is also some authors to reflect a deficit in PFC top-down regulation of hyper-
supported by the expression of PTSD symptoms that frequently include reactivity within the amygdala to trauma-related stimuli (Rauch, Shin,
a constant search for a wide variety of threats in the everyday et al., 1998, Rauch, Shin, & Phelps, 2006). ERP research demonstrating
environment, beyond those related to the original trauma. However, larger P3 amplitudes to trauma-related stimuli among individuals with
ERP studies that have used threatening stimuli not directly related to PTSD supports this theory. While some authors have suggested that the
the participants’ traumatic experiences have found reduced threat association between incoming stimuli and traumatic experience is
processing, possibly due to an increased expectancy of threatening achieved via meaning (through direct association with a specific
information or, alternatively, as an adaptive response aimed at reducing traumatic experience) or via other conceptual properties of the stimuli
emotional arousal (Kimble, Batterink, Marks, Ross, & Fleming, 2012; (level of threat or level of negativity), others have suggested that the
MacNamara, Post, Kennedy, Rabinak, & Phan, 2013). association of stimuli with the traumatic event is made through
Some previous research, however, suggest that those with PTSD perceptual priming.
may possess a general sensitivity that is not limited to perceived threat.
Research findings demonstrating greater PTSD-related cerebral re- 1.2. The Rorschach inkblot test and PTSD
sponses to unpleasant/negative stimuli (not considered threatening or
trauma-related) have caused some authors to suggest the existence of a The Rorschach is a psychological test intended for evaluation of
PTSD-related hypervigilant pattern of information processing; such a perception along with cognitive function and personality characteristics
pattern, they suggest, is characterized by an elevated response to (Blatt, 1990; Exner, 2001). The Rorschach examines the individual's
negative emotional stimuli, regardless whether or whether not it perception of 10 inkblots printed on cards, and is widely used by clinical
contains threatening content (Blomhoff, Reinvang, & Malt, 1998; Lobo psychologists for assessment and intervention planning (Weiner & Greene,
et al., 2014, Saar-Ashkenazy et al., 2015). However, valence effects 2007). Exner (1974) developed a comprehensive system for analyzing
were also found in ERP studies among participants with no trauma responses to the Rorschach test, currently the most frequently used method

109
G. Zukerman et al. Biological Psychology 127 (2017) 108–122

for applying the Rorschach in research and clinical practice. An accumulat- associations (Viglione et al., 2012), the neural correlates of such
ing body of empirical evidence derived from research in recent years processes have never been explored by ERP analysis. The advantage
suggests the Rorschach is a valid tool for the assessment of the traumatic of utilizing ERP, in contrast to other neuroimaging techniques such as
experience, and can provide valuable information regarding traumatic fMRI and PET, is its ability to provide excellent time resolution
imagery and cognitive avoidance strategies among trauma-exposed indivi- especially suitable for examining rapid processing of potentially
duals (Brand, Armstrong, & Loewenstein, 2006; Brand, Armstrong, threatening stimuli (LeDoux, 2000).
Loewenstein, & McNary, 2009; Holaday, 2000; Katsounari & Jacobowitz, The neurocircuitry model proposes that PTSD is associated with an
2011; Opaas & Hartmann, 2013; Sloan, Arsenault, & Hilsenroth, 2002; impaired PFC suppression of attention to trauma-related stimuli (Rauch
Tibon, Rothschild, Appel, & Zeligman, 2011; Viglione, Towns, & - et al., 2006). This understanding is supported by ERP research
Lindshield, 2012). indicating an attentional bias toward threat/trauma-related stimuli
Based on Exner's comprehensive system, the analysis of the indivi- resulting in larger P3 amplitudes. If TCI-related contents indeed
dual's set of responses (the individual's “protocol”) on the Rorschach facilitate activation of traumatic imagery, as was previously suggested
provides several indicators of mental function. Some of these indices (Viglione et al., 2012), then the exposure of participants with trauma
are specifically associated with PTSD, including those that tap into history to such contents would result in P3 alteration. Consequently, we
proneness to psychopathology, hypervigilant processing style, thought hypothesized that:
disturbances and impairment in stress tolerance (Tibon et al., 2011;
Viglione et al., 2012). Among these indices, the traumatic content index 1. Compared to non-traumatized Controls, participants with PTSD
(TCI) is a constellation of several themes more frequently reported in would exhibit larger P3 amplitudes in the context of TCI-related
Rorschach protocols of individuals diagnosed with PTSD, and are Distractors.
considered related to personal traumatic history (Armstrong- 2. Exposure to TCI-related content would be followed by increased P3
& Lowenstein, 1990). The TCI consists of the number of Rorschach amplitudes in both the trauma-exposed groups (PTSD and non-
responses in which an individual identifies inkblot percepts as relating PTSD), compared to healthy control participants.
to one of following thematic categories: 1) blood (Blood), 2) internal 3. When exposed to TCI-related contents, participants with previous
organs such as liver, bones or intestines (Anatomy), 3) sexual organs or traumatic exposure would exhibit increased P3 amplitudes only in
sexual acts and behaviors (Sex), 4) objects described as torn, broken, response to Target and Distractor stimuli. If such hypothesis were to
ruined, dead or attributed with dysphoric feeling (Morbid) and 5) be confirmed, it may suggest the existence of increased cerebral
aggressive acts (Aggressive Movement). The TCI is calculated by the reactivity to either target realted/trauma related stimuli among
sum of TCI responses divided by the total number of responses in a trauma-exposed participants, along with intact ability to discard
Rorschach protocol (Tibon et al., 2011). non-relevant (Standard) stimuli.
Over the past 25 years, an extensive body of research has shown that,
compared to individuals with no traumatic history, the Rorschach protocols In addition to the formal hypotheses delineated above, we also
of participants who report diverse interpersonal and non-interpersonal considered the potential effects of TCI categories on responsivity. Since
traumatic experiences are characterized by elevated frequency of the five the TCI consists of five distinct content categories, it is possible that
themes of the TCI (Armstrong & Lowenstein, 1990; Burch, 1993; Goldfinger Distractors from various categories of the TCI facilitate different
et al., 1998; Kamphuis, Kugeares, & Finn, 2000; Min, Lee, Kim, & Sim, 2011; cerebral responses. Accordingly, the current study examined participant
Opaas & Hartmann, 2013; Smith, Chang, Kochinski, Patz, & Nowinski, 2010; cerebral response by using three oddball paradigms, each containing
Sloan, Arsenault, Hilsenroth, Handler, & Harvill, 1996; Van der Distractors from only one of the Anatomy, Blood, and Morbid TCI
Kolk & Ducey, 1989; for review see Viglione et al., 2012). It has been categories. Due to the novelty of the current research, we had no
suggested that the ambiguous nature of the Rorschach promotes the preliminary hypothesis regarding the nature of cerebral response
breakthrough to consciousness of traumatic imagery. According to this associated with each of the TCI categories; however, by using this
idea, the appearance of traumatic content in the individual's response experimental design, we intended that the current study not only shed
reflects perception of the inkblot as related to specific themes associated light on potential mechanisms by which TCI-related stimuli affects
with their traumatic experience through meaning (Viglione et al., 2012). For information processing, but also whether this mechanism differs in
example, a response indicating perception of Blood content may be related response to various TCI categories.
to a previous trauma by being a likely symbol of danger and injury to the
human body experienced at the traumatic situation, while a response 2. Materials and methods
indicating Morbid content such as “rotten apple” may represent a sense of
damaged or injured self-esteem and feeling of incompleteness related to the 2.1. Participants
traumatic event (Meloy & Genoco, 1997; Viglione et al., 2012). However,
the high frequency with which TCI related themes are reported on Overall, the total sample consisted of 56 participants (mean
Rorschach protocols by individuals with PTSD may be explained in other age = 24.20; SD = 2.39). Trauma-exposed participants were recruited
ways than trauma-related association; for example, Morbid responses can through local university advertisement boards and flyers posted at the
indicate, according to Rorschach conceptualization, negative feelings university's student counseling center and various local medical centers,
toward oneself that may stem from general feelings of inadequacy and requesting individuals who have experienced severe negative life events
blame, or even be a reaction to being tested in a psychological setting. to participate in our study. Forty participants (26 females) reported a
previous traumatic history that included at least one traumatic event in
1.3. This study accordance with DSM-5 “criterion A” for PTSD diagnosis (APA, 2013).
These participants were divided into two groups. The first group, the
This study proposed to examine the electrophysiological response of “PTSD” group, consisted of 16 participants (12 females) who met clinical
trauma-exposed participants to an oddball paradigm in which criteria for PTSD and had scores of at least 40 or greater on the Clinician-
Distractor stimuli consisted of two-word phrases derived from the TCI Administered PTSD Scale (CAPS) (mean = 50.67; SD = 9.53); a CAPS
(Armstrong & Lowenstein, 1990). Target and Standard stimuli were score of 40 and greater is postulated to indicate moderate to severe PTSD
comprised of two-word neutral phrases derived from neutral content symptoms (MacNamara et al., 2013). The second group, the “non-PTSD”
categories of the Rorschach. While it has been postulated that the group, included 24 participants (14 females) that had been exposed to
identification of a Rorschach inkblot with traumatic content represents one or more trauma-related events, but did not meet clinical criteria for
an activation of traumatic imagery via semantic or meaning-based the diagnosis of PTSD at the time of testing. The average CAPS score for

110
G. Zukerman et al. Biological Psychology 127 (2017) 108–122

21 subjects of this group (mean = 26.00; SD = 9.92) showed subthres- that relate to one of three subscales: intrusion (re-experiencing),
hold PTSD (Weathers, Keane, & Davidson, 2001); three additional sub- avoidance and numbing, and hyper-arousal. The CAPS has demon-
jects with no CAPS score were included in this group based on their PDS strated high reliability and validity ratings across various studies (Pupo
(Posttraumatic Diagnostic Scale) score that indicated a subthreshold, et al., 2011; Weathers et al., 2001). This measure was administered only
mild level of PTSD. The average PDS score (mean = 10.21; SD = 7.11) in the trauma-exposed groups (PTSD and non-PTSD).
for all 24 “non-PTSD” participants indicated a subthreshold, mild PTSD
(McCarthy, 2008). 2.2.4. Beck Depression Inventory-II (BDI-II)
The Control group consisted of 16 university students (14 females), The BDI-II is a 21-item self-report measure assessing cognitive,
recruited through local university advertisements, with no previous affective and behavioral outcomes of depression (BDI-II, Beck,
traumatic history. None of the participants reported any medical Steer, & Brown, 1996) The BDI is widely used and has high reliability
problems, being in psychological or psychiatric treatment or the use and validity as a measure of depression (Beck et al., 1996). This
of any medication. All participants provided signed informed consent measure was administered to all the participants in the study.
and were compensated for their time. The study was approved by the
university ethics committee. 2.2.5. Other medical or psychological/psychiatric conditions
Finally, participants from all groups (PTSD, non-PTSD and Control)
2.2. Measures and procedures were asked whether they suffer from any major health problems, were
taking prescribed medication (including psychiatric medication) or
All questionnaire administration and interviews were performed by were receiving psychotherapy.
a licensed clinical psychologist who was trained to evaluate PTSD
symptoms. 2.3. Oddball paradigm with Rorschach's traumatic content index
Distractors
2.2.1. Trauma History Questionnaire (THQ)
The THQ is a 24-item self-report questionnaire developed to All participants’ brain activity was recorded via EEG while perform-
measure exposure to potentially traumatic events included in the “A1 ing these paradigms. The oddball paradigms used in this study
criteria” of DSM-IV for PTSD and acute stress disorder (Hooper, contained 250 two-word phrases of visual stimuli of three Trial
Stockton, Krupnick, & Green, 2011). The events may include crime- Types: 1) Target stimuli consisting of a specific neutral phrase (e.g.
related events (e.g. robbery, mugging), general disaster and trauma “silver fish”) that related to Rorschach's Animal content category; these
(e.g. injury, natural disaster, witnessing death), and unwanted physical stimuli appeared 50 times (20% of all stimuli shown in the task),
and sexual experiences. For each of the items, the participant indicates presented randomly, and participants were asked to press a joystick
whether or not he or she experienced the event, and if so, the number of button when detecting such a phrase; 2) Distractor stimuli consisting of
times and approximate age(s) of occurrence. The THQ has been widely 10 different phrases related to three of Exner's content categories
used to measure previous traumatic history among clinical (PTSD) and included in the TCI (Anatomy, Blood, or Morbid); each phrase was
non-clinical (non-PTSD) samples. Moderate to high test-retest reliability repeated five times resulting in 50 phrases (20% of all stimuli)
and validity have been reported (Hooper et al., 2011). In the current presented randomly; and 3) Standard (non-target) stimuli consisting
study, a trauma history score was calculated by adding the number of of 30 different phrases of domestic items related to Rorschach's
traumatic events reported by the participant. Thus, a higher score Household content category (Exner, 2001), such as “desk lamp”, “wall
indicates a higher rate of traumatic event exposure. This measure was clock” or “kitchen cupboard”; each phrase was repeated five times,
administered to all the participants in the study. resulting in 150 phrases presented randomly (60% of all stimuli).
Each participant was presented with three experimental paradigms
2.2.2. Posttraumatic Diagnostic Scale (PDS) that were identical except for the type of Distractor stimuli: Anatomy,
The PDS is a 49-item self-rating scale developed to assess PTSD Blood, and Morbid. Since each TCI content category may have a
diagnosis according to DSM-IV criteria (Foa, 1995). The first section of different effect on cerebral response, the experimental design included
the questionnaire includes a short checklist that identifies potentially three runs, each employing Distractors from a different TCI category.
traumatizing events experienced by the respondent. Next, in the case of The Anatomy paradigm included Distractors that were comprised of
more than one traumatic experience, the participant is asked to choose phrases related to skeletal, muscular, and internal anatomy such as
one single traumatic experience “that currently bothers you the most” “two lungs”, “pair of ribs”, or “brain tissue.” The Blood paradigm
(major event). Then, with regard to the major event, the participant is included Distractors that were comprised of phrases related to blood
asked to use a 4-point scale to rate the frequency with which he or she content such as “red blood”, “fresh blood”, or “a pond of blood.” The
has experienced each one of 17 PTSD symptoms over the previous two Morbid paradigm included Distractors that were comprised of phrases
weeks; the 17 items measure thought intrusion (re-experiencing), related to dead objects (“a baby's body”), destroyed, ruined, spoiled,
avoidance and arousal symptoms. Finally, a PTSD symptom severity damaged or broken objects (“broken toy”) or objects attributed with a
score is calculated; a cutoff score of 11 and above has been suggested to dysphoric feeling (“depressed woman”). The content category classifi-
reflect a moderate level of PTSD (McCarthy, 2008). Previous research cation for the phrases used as stimuli was evaluated separately by two
findings have demonstrated high internal consistency and test-retest licensed clinical psychologists trained in Exner's comprehensive system
reliability as well as a satisfactory agreement with PTSD-related of Rorschach scoring; inter-rater reliability was high (Kappa = 0.90).
diagnostic interviews such as the Structural Clinical Interview for Fig. 1 presents the experimental paradigm. At this study, we used three
DSM Disorders - SCID (Foa, Cashman, Jaycox, & Perry, 1997). This paradigms, each relating to different TCI content category (Anatomy/
measure was administered to all the participants in the study. Blood/Morbid), though the TCI contains five content categories.
Distractors from the two additional content categories (Sex and
2.2.3. Clinician-Administered PTSD Scale (CAPS) Aggressive movement) were not used mainly due to time considera-
.The CAPS is a semi-structured interview for PTSD diagnosis tions; each paradigm took as long as eight minutes and we estimated
according to DSM-IV criteria (Blake et al., 1995). The CAPS is the using the all five categories would reduce the participant interest
considered a “gold standard” in PTSD assessment. It provides informa- and motivation in completing the task.
tion regarding the intensity and frequency of PTSD symptoms in
response to traumatic experience. A total severity score is calculated 2.3.1. Distractors: evaluating emotional valence and threat level
by adding the intensity and frequency scores of 17 4-point scale items Since PTSD has previously been associated with increased respon-

111
G. Zukerman et al. Biological Psychology 127 (2017) 108–122

Fig. 1. Experimental paradigm. Each subject participated in three experimental paradigms that included Target stimuli (animal-related content such as “silver fish”), Distractor stimuli
(either Anatomy, Blood or Morbid related content) and Standard stimuli (two-word phrases of household-related content). Subjects were asked to press a button on a joystick when they
identified the Target stimuli. The order of the three experimental paradigms was counterbalanced across participants.

sivity to negative emotional stimuli (Blomhoff et al., 1998; Saar- 300 ms and interstimulus fixed intervals were 1700 ms, thus, stimuli
Ashkenazy et al., 2015) as well as increased responsivity to threatening were presented every 2 s.
stimuli (Engelhard et al., 2009; Kimble et al., 2012), we evaluated: 1) Participants were seated in a comfortable armchair, told that they
the emotional valence of the Distractor stimuli used in this study and 2) would see all kinds of phrases on the computer monitor and were asked
the perceived level of threat of Distractors in this study. to press a button on a joystick when they identified the Target stimuli
In order to evaluate the emotional valence of the Distractor stimuli, (“silver fish”). The computer monitor was located 100 cm from the
a separate group of 49 age-matched undergraduate students who did participant's head.
not participate in the ERP portion of the study (46 females, mean
age = 22) were asked to rate Distractor phrases on a nine-point Likert
scale from 1 (“exceptionally negative”) to 9 (“exceptionally positive”). 2.4. Electrophysiological testing and measures
Participants’ responses were analyzed with a repeated measure analysis
of variance (ANOVA) with three content categories (Anatomy, Blood, The EEG was recorded in a room free of noise and electromagnetic
Morbid) containing 10 phrases in each category (3 × 10). The results fields. Continuous EEG was recorded using a Micromed SD 64 channel
indicated significantly different negative ratings by content category, F system and a Neuroscan 64 channel elastic cap with electrode locations
(2, 26) = 240.36, p < .00, ηp2 = 0.91. Bonferroni post hoc tests re- based on the 10/20 system. All electrodes were referenced to an
vealed that phrases in the Morbid category were perceived as signifi- electrode located at the tip of the nose. A ground electrode was placed
cantly more negative (M = 2.58, SE = 0.05) than phrases from the on the right mastoid. A vertical electrooculogram (EOG) was recorded
Anatomy (M = 3.98, SE = 0.07) and Blood (M = 3.35, SE = 0.09) using two electrodes, one located above and one below the right eye.
categories. Significant differences were also found between the Blood The impedance measure for each electrode was always below 5 kΩ.
and Anatomy categories, p < .00, denoting that anatomy Distractors Raw data were continuously recorded with a 16-bit A/D and a band
were perceived as more negative then Distractors at the Blood category. pass filter of 0.15–463 Hz and a sampling rate of 1024 Hz.
In order to evaluate the perceived level of threat of Distractor All data were analyzed using BPM software (Brain Performance
stimuli, an additional 24 age-matched undergraduates students who did Measurement: Orgil Medical Equipment, Inc.). EEG Recordings were
not participate in the ERP procedure (23 females, mean age = 22) were segmented into time intervals that were time-locked to the stimuli, and
asked to rate the Distractor phrases on a 6-point Likert scale from 0 extended from 200 ms pre-stimulus to 2000 ms post-stimulus. An eye
(“not threatening”) to 5 (“very threatening”). Participant responses movement correction procedure was performed offline using an eye
were analyzed with a repeated measure analysis of variance (ANOVA) movement correction algorithm. The algorithm detects an epoch with
with three content categories (Anatomy, Blood, Morbid) containing 10 ocular artifacts by comparing the signals recorded from above and
phrases in each category (3 × 10). The results indicated significantly below the eye. In a second step, a correction is performed for each
different threat ratings by content category, F(2, 22) = 17.30, p < .00, record and each channel separately. Additionally, EEG signals were
ηp2 = 0.61. Bonferroni post hoc tests revealed that Blood (M = 3.19, visually scored on a high resolution computer monitor, and portions of
SE = 0.19) and Morbid (M = 3.15, SE = 0.11) Distractors were rated the data containing eye movement, muscle movement or other sources
as significantly more threatening than Anatomy Distractors (M = 2.07, of artifact were removed. Baseline correction for each trial was
SE = 0.17). No significant differences were found between Blood and performed using the 200 ms prior to stimulus onset for each channel
Morbid Distractors. separately. Amplitude rejection had been applied to the data prior to
Target, Standard, and Distractor stimuli were presented in random averaging. Records with amplitude higher than 250 microvolts (even in
order. The order of the three experimental paradigms (Anatomy, Blood, a single channel) were excluded. For each participant, all trials were
and Morbid) was counterbalanced across participants. Chi-square averaged per stimulus type (Target, Distractor, and Standard). In the
analysis indicated no significant group differences in paradigm pre- final stage of averaging, the data were filtered using a 6 Hz low-pass
sentation order, χ2(4, 56) = 0.64, p > .05). Stimuli were presented as filter.
white letters on a black background computer screen. Times New Global field power (GFP) was calculated across all 12 electrodes,
Roman font size 88 letters were used. Each stimulus duration was Groups (PTSD, non-PTSD, Control), Paradigm type (Anatomy, Blood,

112
G. Zukerman et al. Biological Psychology 127 (2017) 108–122

Morbid) and Trial type (Target, Distractor, Standard). The GFP was that were divided between two location factors. Given that the P3
calculated by the root mean square (RMS) of the grand average response of different locations may represent divergent cognitive
waveform (Murry, Brunet, & Michel, 2008) thus obtaining positive processes (Katayama & Polich, 1998), we assigned each electrode to a
components that indicate the strength of the potential being recoded. four-level Midline Site factor denoting Prefrontal (PF1, PFZ, PF2),
The GFP provides a reference–independent measure of response Frontal (F3, Fz, F4), Central (C3, Cz, C4) and Parietal (P3, Pz, P4) Sites.
strength (Murry et al., 2008). Reviewing the GFP identified two main Additionally, since the oddball paradigms used in this study included
components. The first component was identified as P3, denoting a two-word phrases, and based on a well-established concept that the left
positive deflection at mid-latency time intervals and a second, late hemisphere is associated with more efficient processing of verbal
negative component (LNC). The P3 component was quantified in terms information (Dickson & Federmeier, 2014; Selpien et al., 2015), elec-
of peak amplitudes (maximum positive amplitude from baseline in trodes were also divided into a three-level Side Site factor denoting Left
microvolts) and latency (time interval from stimulus onset to peak (PF1, F3, C3, P3), Middle (PFz, Fz, Cz, Pz) and Right (PF2, F4, C4, P4)
amplitude in milliseconds) between 300 and 750 ms. The second sites. This categorization of the electrodes into two factors is in
component was evident between 950 and 1500 ms with no defined accordance with previous research (Kimble, Kaloupek, Kaufman,-
peak. Thus, we calculated the area under the curve (AUC) (Luck, 2014) & Deldin, 2000).
by advancing along the time axes between the two time points (1 ms All analyses were performed with and without gender and depres-
differentiates each successive sampling at a sampling rate of 1 KHz). sion levels as covariates. Gender was selected as a covariate factor since
The AUC is defined by the area between the curve and a baseline level, numerous studies have reported that females are more likely to develop
which was calculated by using the more positive amplitude of two time intense PTS symptoms, as well as meet criteria for PTSD (for review, see
points (950 and 1500 ms). The AUC measurement, expressed in Tolin & Foa, 2006). Depression was selected as a covariate factor since
μv × ms units, has been associated with total activity of the underlying comorbidity between PTSD and major depressive disorder is common
neural substrate (Pratt, Mittelman, Bleich, & Laufer, 2004). Fig. 2 (Stander, Thomsen, & Highfill-McRoy, 2014) and lower P3 amplitudes
presents the GFP. The GFP, derived from the mean square, produced have been reported among depressed individuals (Iv, Zhao, Gong,
positive values for both positive and negative components. Thus, Chen, & Miao, 2010). In all analyses, no significant effects for either
although the GFP values for the second component were positive, it is of the covariates were found. Therefore, results are reported only for
presented in its initial negative format. analyses with no covariates.

3. Results
2.5. Statistical analysis
3.1. Participant characteristics
Analyses were conducted by using SPSS version 21 (Armonk, N.Y.;
IBM Corp., 2013). For comparison of demographics and behavioral No significant gender differences were found between groups, χ2(2,
assessment measures (THQ, PDS, BDI) one-way analysis of variance N = 56) = 4.14, p > .05, in spite of female predominance. No sig-
(ANOVA) with Group (PTSD, non-PTSD, Controls) as an independent nificant group differences in age were found. On the PDS, the majority
variable was used. An independent t-test was used to track group of the participants in trauma-exposed groups (PTSD and non-PTSD)
differences (PTSD vs non-PTSD) on the CAPS assessment. A non- reported motor vehicle accidents (62.5%) as their major traumatic
parametric Chi-square test was conducted to compare gender differ- experience, 12.5% war or combat related traumatic experiences, 5.0%
ences among the three groups. sexual assaults and 5.0% the loss of a family member. Most participants
P3 amplitude and latency as well as AUC comparisons were described a history of multiple types of trauma. The distribution of
conducted by a repeated measure analysis of variance (ANOVA) with types of “main traumatic event” as well as types of overall traumatic
Group (PTSD, non-PTSD, Control) as a between-factor and Paradigm event distribution, as reported on the PDS, is presented in Table 1. No
type (Anatomy, Blood, Morbid) X Trial type (Target, Distractor, significant statistical differences were found in the distribution of the
Standard) X Electrode Midline site (Prefrontal, Frontal, Central, main traumatic event type between the PTSD and non-PTSD groups, χ2
Parietal) X Electrode Side site (Left, Midline, Right) as within-factors. (6, N = 40) = 4.47, p > .05.
Greenhouse Geisser corrections were applied as necessary for violations Data for age, THQ, PDS, and BDI indices are presented in Table 2. A
of sphericity, and partial eta square (ηp2 ) served as an estimate for effect significant effect of Group on the level of previous traumatic exposure
size of the ANOVA's. Bonferroni post hoc tests have been used to in the THQ was found. Bonferroni post hoc tests revealed significantly
examine group differences, while contrast analysis was used to examine higher rates of previous traumatic exposure among participants in the
interaction effects for the P3 component analysis. Data were obtained PTSD and non-PTSD groups. No significant difference in trauma history
from 12 electrodes (PF1, PFz, PF2, F3, Fz, F4, C3, Cz, C4, P3, Pz, P4) levels between PTSD and non-PTSD groups, as reported at the THQ, was
observed. A significant effect of Group on the level of post-traumatic
symptoms, as indicated by the PDS, was found. As expected, Bonferroni
post hoc tests have indicated that, in comparison to the Control group
who reported no traumatic symptoms, PTSD and non-PTSD group
participants reported significantly higher levels of traumatic symptoms.

Table 1
Trauma type distribution among 40 subjects with traumatic history.

Trauma type Major traumatic event Any traumatic event

Motor vehicle accident 25 (62.5%) 26 (65%)


War- or combat-related 5 (12.5%) 17 (42.5%)
experience
Sexual assault 2 (5%) 5 (12.5%)
Loss of family member 2 (5%) 3 (7.5%)
Fig. 2. Grand average global field power. GFP was calculated by the root mean square Other 6 (15%) 19 (47%)
(RMS) of the grand averages waveform resulting in two components that were identified
as positive P3 component and LNC, respectively. Note: Trauma type distribution as reported at the posttraumatic diagnostic scale (PDS).

113
G. Zukerman et al. Biological Psychology 127 (2017) 108–122

Table 2
Traumatic history and symptoms reported among all groups.

Control non-PTSD PTSD

Mean (SD) n Mean (SD) n Mean (SD) n F/t p

Age 23 1.96 15 24.63 2.48 24 24.69 2.38 16 2.57 ns


THQ scores 0 0 16 5.00 1.87 22 4.81 2 16 40.59 < 0.01
PDS scores 0 0 16 10.21 7.11 24 21.25 8.14 16 44.37 < 0.01
Total CAPS scores 26 9.92 21 50.75 9.22 16 −7.96 < 0.01
BDI scores 1.07 1.38 15 6.75 7.9 24 9.00 7.73 15 5.66 < 0.01

Note: THQ = Trauma History Questionnaire, PDS = Posttraumatic Diagnostic Scale, CAPS = Clinician Administered PTSD Scale, BDI = Beck Depression Inventory.

In accordance with PDS norms (McCarthy, 2008) the levels of traumatic amplitudes for the Anatomy and Blood paradigms compared to the
symptoms matched “mild” and “moderate to severe” levels of traumatic Morbid paradigm, while the Control group was characterized by larger
symptoms among the non-PTSD and PTSD groups, respectively. The P3 amplitudes for the Morbid paradigm compared to the Anatomy and
difference in the level of traumatic symptoms reported by the PTSD and Blood paradigms. The results of the analysis of P3 amplitude and
the non-PTSD groups was also statistically significant, indicating a latency are provided below.
higher level of traumatic symptoms among the PTSD group. Addition-
ally, a significant difference in reported levels of depression, as
3.2.1.1. P3 amplitude. A Group (PTSD, non-PTSD, Control) X Paradigm
indicated by the BDI, was found, with higher levels of depressive
Type (Anatomy, Blood, Morbid) X Trial Type (Target, Distractor,
symptoms reported among those with PTSD, as compared to non-PTSD
Standard) X Electrode Midline Site (Prefrontal, Frontal, Central,
and Control participants. Nevertheless, the average score of each group
Parietal) X Electrode Side Site (Left, Middle, Right) repeated
indicated non-depression (BDI score < 13) (Beck et al., 1996, Dozois,
measures ANOVA was conducted in order to assess P3 peak
Dobson, & Ahnberg, 1998; Kendall, Hollon, Beck, Hamenn, & Ingram,
amplitude levels.
1987; Whisman & Richardson, 2015).
Results indicated a significant Group X Paradigm Type crossover
interaction, F(3.43, 91.11) = 3.19, p < .05, ηp2 = 0.11. A secondary
3.2. ERP component analysis contrast analysis revealed that the combined effect of Group and
Paradigm Type was significantly different for the Anatomy and Blood
3.2.1. P3 amplitude and latency paradigms, compared to the Morbid paradigm, F(2, 53) 5.22, p < .01,
The grand averages of the ERP waveforms can be seen in Fig. 3 for ηp2 = 0.17; F(2, 53) = 3.59, p < .05, ηp2 = 0.12 , respectively. This points
Target stimuli and Fig. 4 for Distractor and Standard stimuli. Visual out differences in cerebral activation patterns in response to the various
inspection of the waveforms suggests that, across trials, the trauma- paradigms between study groups: participants in the PTSD and non-
exposed groups (PTSD and non-PTSD) were characterized by larger P3 PTSD groups exhibited higher P3 amplitudes for the Anatomy and the

Fig. 3. Grand average ERP's for electrodes Fz and Cz in response to Target stimuli across the three experimental paradigms (Anatomy, Blood, Morbid) among the three study groups
(PTSD, non-PTSD, C ontrol). Shaded areas correspond to the time window for P3 (300–750 ms) and late negative component (LNC) (950–1500 ms).

114
G. Zukerman et al. Biological Psychology 127 (2017) 108–122

Fig. 4. Grand average ERP's for electrodes Fz and Cz in response to Distractor (a) and Standard (b) stimuli for the three experimental paradigms (Anatomy, Blood, Morbid) among the
three study groups (PTSD, non- PTSD and Control) Shaded areas correspond to the time window for P3 (300–750 ms) and late negative component (LNC) (950–1500 ms).

Blood paradigms, in comparison to the Morbid paradigm. In contrast, Additionally, a significant effect of Electrode Side Site was found, F
Control participants exhibited lower amplitudes for the Anatomy and (1.40, 74.63) = 34.34, p < .00, ηp2 = 0.39. Bonferroni post hoc testing
Blood paradigms in comparison to the Morbid paradigm. Fig. 5 revealed a significant difference in amplitudes elicited by electrodes at
illustrates this disordinal interaction pattern between Group and the Left (M = 7.79, SE = 0.43) in comparison to the Right (M = 7.14,
Paradigm Type on P3 amplitude across Trial Type (Target, Distractor, SE = 0.39, p < .00). No other significant main effects were found for
Standard), averaged for all 12 electrodes. No other significant main peak amplitude.
effects or interactions involving the Group factor were found. Table 3
displays the group averages for the P3 analysis.
3.2.1.2. P3 latency. A Group (PTSD, non-PTSD, Control) X Paradigm
A significant main effect of Trial Type was found F(1.54, 81.91)
Type (Anatomy, Blood, Morbid) X Trial type (Target, Distractor,
= 58.13, p < .01, ηp2 = 0.52 . Follow-up Bonferroni post hoc tests
Standard) X Electrode Midline Site (Prefrontal, Frontal, Central,
revealed a significant difference in amplitudes elicited by Target stimuli
Parietal) X Electrode Side Site (Left, Middle, Right) repeated
(M = 10.82, SE = 0.65) in comparison to Distractor (M = 6.40,
measures ANOVA was conducted in order to assess differences in P3
SE = 0.43, p < .00) and Standard stimuli (M = 5.40, SE = 0.42;
peak latencies.
p < .00). The difference between amplitudes elicited by Distractor
A significant main effect for Midline Site was observed, F(1.65,
and Standard stimuli was also significant, p < .05.
87.47) = 10.35, p < .01, ηp2 = 0.16 . Bonferroni post hoc tests have

115
G. Zukerman et al. Biological Psychology 127 (2017) 108–122

For the Anatomy Paradigm, a significant Group X Trial interaction


was observed F(3.01, 79.88) = 3.73, p < .05, ηp2 = 0.12 . To probe the
interaction, repeated measure ANOVA's were conducted for each Trial
Type. Results indicated a significant effect of Group on AUC values for
only the Distractor Trial Type F(2, 53) = 3.35, p < .05, ηp2 = 0.11.
Bonferroni post hoc tests revealed significantly larger AUC values for
the PTSD (M = 2412.78, SE = 231.54) compared to the Control group
(M = 1566.51, SE = 231.54, p < .05). No other main effects of
interactions involving the Group Factor were observed. Fig. 6 illustrates
the AUC values for the three experimental paradigms (Anatomy, Blood,
Morbid) as function of Trial Type (Target, Distractor, Standard) for the
three study groups (PTSD, non-PTSD, Control), averaged for all 12
electrodes.
Fig. 5. P3 amplitudes for Group (PTSD, non-PTSD, Control) and Paradigm Type For the Blood Paradigm, a significant effect of Group was observed F
(Anantomy, Blood, M orbid) averaged for 12 electrodes. Means and standard errors of (2, 53) = 3.37, p < .05, ηp2 = 0.11. Bonferroni post hoc tests revealed
means are presented. significantly larger AUC values for the PTSD (M = 2034.75,
SE = 176.15) as compared to the non-PTSD (M = 1472.40,
indicated that this effect was due to significantly longer latencies at SE = 143.82, p < .05). Additionally, a significant Trial Type and
Prefrontal sites (M = 495.32, SE = 6.70) in comparison to Frontal Group interaction was observed F(3.47, 92.19) = 4.11, p < .01,
(M = 483. 97, SE = 6.78, p < .01), Central (M = 481.93, SE = 6.64, ηp2 = 0.13. In order to probe the interaction, repeated measures
p < .01) and Parietal (M = 481.55, SE = 6.43, p < .05) Sites. No ANOVA's analyses, separate for each Trial Type (Target, Distractor,
other significant effects of Paradigm Type, Trial Type, or Electrode Side Standard) were conducted. For Target Trial Type, a significant effect of
Site on latency time were observed. Group was observed F(2, 53) = 3.60, p < .05, ηp2 = 0.12 . Bonferroni
post hoc tests revealed significantly smaller AUC values for the non-
PTSD group (M = 1659.48, SE = 212.54) as compared to the Control
3.2.2. Late negative component (LNC) AUC
group (M = 2546.53, SE = 260.30, p < .05). For Distractor Trial type,
A Group (PTSD, non-PTSD, Control) X Paradigm Type (Anatomy,
a significant effect of Group was observed F(2, 53) = 5.00, p = .01,
Blood, Morbid) X Trial type (Target, Distractor, Standard) X Electrode
ηp2 = 0.16 . Bonferroni post hoc tests revealed significantly larger AUC
Midline Site (Prefrontal, Frontal, Central, Parietal) X Electrode Side Site
values for the PTSD group (M = 2575.98, SE = 236.73) compared to
(Left, Middle, Right) repeated measures ANOVA was conducted in order
the non-PTSD (M = 1643.88, SE = 193.29, p < .01) and Control
to assess differences in the AUC at the 950–1500 ms time points. (See
(M = 1775.33, SE = 236.73, p < .05) groups. No significant main
Figs. 3 and 4 for grand averages of ERP waveforms).
effects or interactions involving the Group factor were observed for
A significant two-way interaction of Group X Trial Type was
Standard Trial Type (Fig. 6).
observed, F(4, 102) = 6.61, p, < .01, ηp2 = 0.21. Additionally, a four-
For the Morbid Paradigm, a significant Group X Trial Type X
way interaction of Group X Paradigm type X Trial type X Electrode Side
Electrode Side interaction was observed F(4.75, 125.96) = 3.17,
was found, F(7.79, 206.55) = 2.00, p = .05, ηp2 = 0.07. No other
p < .05, ηp2 = 0.11. Probing the interaction by separate repeated
significant main effects or interactions involving the Group factor were
measures ANOVA's for each Trial Type have indicated a significant
found.
Group X Electrode Side interaction for Target Trial Type F(2.55, 66.82)
In order to probe the four-way interaction, Group (PTSD, non-PTSD,
= 4.47, p < .01, ηp2 = 0.14 . Probing this interaction by repeated
Control) X Trial type (Target, Distractor, Standard) X Electrode Side
measures ANOVA's, separated for each side, indicated a significant
(Left, Middle, Right) repeated measures ANOVA's were conducted,
effect of Group only for Right Side electrodes F(2, 53) = 3.18, p = .05,
separately for each Paradigm Type (Anatomy, Blood, Morbid).

Table 3
Means and standard errors of P300 amplitude (μV) and late negative component (LNC) area under the curve (μV*ms) values for the study groups across all paradigms and trial types.

PTSD Non-PTSD Control

Mean SE Mean SE Mean SE

P300
Anatomy Target 12.37 1.26 10.82 1.03 11.04 1.26
Distractor 6.13 1.00 7.05 0.82 5.10 1.00
Standard 5.28 0.85 5.34 0.69 4.62 0.85
Blood Target 11.83 1.58 9.25 1.29 10.55 1.58
Distractor 7.08 1.17 7.60 0.95 6.65 1.17
Standard 6.69 1.02 5.87 0.83 4.03 1.01
Morbid Target 9.68 1.48 9.28 1.20 12.54 1.48
Distractor 4.32 1.23 6.17 1.00 7.46 1.23
Standard 5.31 0.86 5.61 0.70 5.84 0.86
LNC
Anatomy Target 2195.93 268.46 1963.34 239.17 2486.35 268.46
Distractor 2412.78 231.54 1944.48 189.05 1566.51 231.54
Standard 1232.13 151.46 1240.05 123.66 1175.05 151.46
Blood Target 2162.08 260.30 1659.48 212.54 2546.53 260.30
Distractor 2575.98 236.73 1643.88 193.29 1775.33 236.73
Standard 1366.21 159.53 1113.85 130.25 1271.69 159.53
Morbid Target 2268.73 242.07 1798.68 197.64 2382.37 242.07
Distractor 2027.85 239.17 1876.22 195.28 1750.50 239.17
Standard 1298.07 238.30 1154.52 194.57 1666.70 238.30

Note: All values are averaged for 12 electrodes.

116
G. Zukerman et al. Biological Psychology 127 (2017) 108–122

In summary, our findings indicate that for the Anatomy and Blood
paradigms, when subjects observed Distractors with Anatomy (“a pair
of ribs”) or Blood (“red blood”) contents, the PTSD group responded
with significantly larger AUC values compared to the non-PTSD group.
For the Blood and Morbid paradigms, the response to neutral Target
stimuli was significantly larger among Control subjects, compared to
the non-PTSD group (although this effect was restricted to the Right
Side electrodes and only for the Morbid paradigm). Table 3 displays
group averages for the LNC analysis. Beyond Paradigm Type, our
findings also showed a significantly smaller AUC among non-PTSD
participants for Target Trials, compared to Controls, and larger AUC
values among the PTSD group compared to Controls in response to
Distractor Trials.
A significant main effect of Trial type was observed F(2, 52)
= 87.10, p < .05, ηp2 = 0.77. Bonferroni post hoc tests revealed larger
AUC values for Targets (M = 2153.78, SE = 94.14) as compared to
AUC values elicited in response to Distractors (M = 1939.00,
SE = 92.80, p < .05) as well as Standard stimuli (M = 1275.70,
SE = 78.69, p < .01). The difference in AUC values for Distractors
and Standard Trials was also significant (p < .01).
Finally, a significant main effect of Midline Electrode Site on AUC
vales was observed F(1.34, 71.05) = 5.42, p < .05, ηp2 = 0.09.
Bonferroni post hoc tests revealed larger AUC values at the Prefrontal
Electrode Site (M = 1888.64, SE = 94.83) as compared to AUC values
at Frontal Site (M = 1733.70, SE = 74.34, p < .01). No other sig-
nificant main effects or interactions involving the Group factor were
observed.

4. Discussion

This is the first ERP study to examine the cerebral response to


Rorschach's Traumatic Content Index among a sample of trauma-
exposed participants diagnosed with PTSD, trauma-exposed partici-
pants not meeting clinical criteria for PTSD, and Control participants
unexposed to trauma. Our study found that trauma-exposed partici-
pants (PTSD and non-PTSD) exhibited different cerebral activation
patterns in the presence of Anatomy, Blood and Morbid contents, than
did Control participants. This finding indicates that, when faced with
some TCI-related traumatic themes (Anatomy and Blood), trauma-
exposed participant ERP's will exhibit increased P3 amplitudes.

5. Allocation of attention

Fig. 6. Late negative component area under the curve (AUC) for each of the experimental Larger P3 amplitudes have been postulated to indicate an allocation
paradigms Anatomy (a), Blood (b), Morbid (c), as function of Trail Type (Target, of greater attention (Polich & McIssac, 1994). Thus, our findings
Distractor, Standard) for the three study groups (PTSD, non-PTSD and control), averaged indicate an allocation of more attention to some TCI-related stimuli
for all 12 electrodes. (* p < 0.05). For the Morbid paradigm, significant effect was among trauma-exposed participants. This concurs with the resource
restricted to the Right Side electrodes. Means and standard errors of means are displayed.
allocation model of PTSD (Ehlers & Clark, 2000) as well as with the
neurocircuitry model of PTSD, denoting a deficit in PFC suppression of
attention to trauma-related stimuli (Rauch et al., 1998, Rauch et al.,
ηp2 = 0.11. Bonferroni post hoc tests revealed significantly smaller AUC 2006). Specifically, the results indicate that, in comparison to Control
values for the non-PTSD group (M = 1679.36, SE = 204.37) compared participants, trauma-exposed participants demonstrated an attentional
to the Control (M = 2469.20, SE = 250.30, p = .05) group (Fig. 6). bias toward Distractor phrases related to Anatomy and Blood, while
We also probed the two-way Group X Trial Type interaction by their reaction to phrases related to torn, broken, or dysphoria-related
separate repeated measures ANOVA's for each Trial type. Results objects (Morbid content) was characterized by reduced attention. Thus,
indicated a significant effect of Group on AUC values for Targets F(2, the finding regarding Anatomy and Blood content supports our first
53) = 4.65, p < .05, ηp2 = 0.15. Bonferroni post hoc tests revealed hypothesis (i.e. participants with PTSD would exhibit larger P3
significantly smaller AUC for the non-PTSD (M = 1807.17, amplitudes in the context of TCI-related Distractors compared to non-
SE = 141.20) compared to Control participants (M = 2471.75, traumatized Controls), while the Morbid content finding does not.
SE = 172.94). Results also indicated a significant effect of Group on The elevated P3 response in both trauma-exposed groups (PTSD and
AUC values for Distractors F(2, 53) = 4.20, p < .05, ηp2 = 0.13. non-PTSD) to Anatomy and Blood contents (but not Morbid) also
Bonferroni post hoc tests revealed significantly larger AUC values for supports our second hypothesis, which indicated that exposure to
the PTSD group (M = 2338.87, SE = 168.45) compared to the Controls TCI-related content would be followed by increased P3 amplitudes in
(M = 1697.44, SE = 168.45). A marginally significant difference be- both the trauma-exposed groups (PTSD and non-PTSD), compared to
tween the PTSD compared to the non-PTSD group (M = 1821.53, the Control participants. These findings concur with the growing
SE = 137.54, p = .06) was also observed. literature suggesting that attentional bias after an exposure to trauma

117
G. Zukerman et al. Biological Psychology 127 (2017) 108–122

are also found among participant that do not meet clinical criteria for relates to a general sense of inadequacy (Exner, 2001; Hartmann,
PTSD (Kimble et al., 2010; Thomas, Goegan, Newman, & Arndt, 2013) Halvorsen, & Wang, 2013; Meyer & Viglione, 2008). Thus, it is possible
that these differences may have triggered larger P3 in response to
6. General hypervigilance pattern Anatomy and Blood than to Morbid Distractors. Notwithstanding,
Distractors were not rated according to their level of perceptual/
In contrast to previous findings (Karl et al., 2006; Johnson et al., conceptual information within this study; furthermore, other explana-
2013) and to our third hypothesis (i.e. that participants with previous tions can be suggested for the current findings. For example, it is
traumatic exposure would exhibit increased P3 amplitudes only in possible that the Anatomy and Blood Distractors were more concep-
response to Target and Distractor stimuli), our findings showed that an tually related to the content of the participants’ primary traumatic
increase in P3 amplitude to Anatomy and Blood content among trauma- event (Zhang et al., 2015), the vast majority of which were motor
exposed participants (PTSD and non-PTSD) was evident for all Trial vehicle accidents or war/combat. Therefore, it is our recommendation
types (Target, Distractor, Standard). This finding may be characterized that perceptual and conceptual levels of incoming stimuli on informa-
as a general hypervigilant pattern, and may suggest impaired stimulus tion processing among trauma-exposed individuals should be explored
filtering. A recent magnetoencephalography (MEG) study of partici- in future studies, in light of this novel finding.
pants with PTSD found dorsolateral prefrontal-related hyperactivity to
standard stimuli in the presence of threatening Distractors (Herz et al., 8. Late negative component
2016). While the Herz study's results seem to provide some support for
the current findings, further research is needed to examine a possible This study's findings extend beyond the P3 component. Although we
pattern of general hypervigilance among trauma-exposed individuals. had no preliminary hypothesis regarding possible effects of trauma
In summary, our ERP findings indicate that there is an attentional exposure on later ERP components, evaluation of the grand averages of
bias to the Anatomy and Blood TCI subjects among trauma-exposed ERP waveforms and the resulting statistical analysis have revealed that
participants, thus complementing the cognitive resource allocation the elevated P3 response of the PTSD group to Anatomy and Blood
model (Ehlers & Clark, 2000) as well as the neurocognitive neurocir- Distractors was followed by a significantly larger AUC values in the
cuitry model (Rauch et al., 1998, Rauch et al., 2006) of PTSD. The LNC. In the LNC results, participants with PTSD were characterized by
findings support the explanation that the relatively high frequency of increased responsivity to Distractors derived from the TCI, while the
TCI responses in the Rorschach protocols of trauma-exposed individuals Controls showed increased responsivity to the Target (neutral) stimuli.
can be explained, at least in part, by activation of the individual's Specifically, participants with PTSD were characterized by increased
traumatic imagery. This mechanism may be restricted to Anatomy and response to Distractors with Anatomy and Blood contents, as compared
Blood contents, since the cerebral response to Morbid content among to Control participants. This pattern of response in a late component has
the trauma-exposed groups was attenuated. not been previously documented.
Providing further context to this pattern is a study involving an
7. Responsivity to different TCI categories emotional Stroop task that suggested its finding of an increased late
(626–726 ms) negativity indicated an association between emotional
Beyond the above findings, this study also showed that trauma- arousal and late attentional processes that enroll higher order cognitive
exposed participants had higher cerebral responsivity to Anatomy and control mechanisms (Feroz, Liecht, Steinmann, Andreou, & Mulert,
Blood Distractors (“a pair of ribs” or “a drop of blood”, respectively) 2016). It is possible that in the present study, Anatomy and Blood
along with attenuated responses to Morbid Distractors (“a baby's Distractors elicited higher levels of arousal among participants in the
body”). These differences can be explained neither by differences in PTSD group. Other ERP studies have related LNC to sensitivity to the
level of perceived threat among Distractors of the three paradigms number of items that are stored in working memory, and an index of
(Kimble et al., 2010) nor by PTSD-related sensitivity to negative filtering efficiency (i.e. the ability to exclude non-relevant items from
emotional valance of the stimulus (Blomhoff et al., 1998; Saar-Askenazy working memory storage) (Qi, Ding, & Li, 2014; Vogel & Machizawa,
et al., 2015). This position is informed by our study findings from two 2004). Highly Trait Anxious (HTA) individuals that showed large
separate groups of age-matched undergraduate students who were amplitudes of LNC were suggested to allocate excessive working
asked to rate the threat and negativity levels of the TCI phrases. These memory storage to threatening Distractors, indicating inefficient filter-
groups rated the Anatomy phrases as significantly less threatening than ing ability (Stout, Shackman, & Larson, 2013). This might also be the
Blood and Morbid phrases, while rating Blood and Anatomy as less case in this study, indicating that PTSD participants who associated
negative than Morbid phrases Anatomy and Blood contents with traumatic experience through
A higher cerebral responsivity to Anatomy and Blood Distractors perceptual priming required more working memory storage and elicited
might be related to trauma-exposed participants’ high responsiveness to larger late negativity. This should be further explored in future studies.
stimuli that bear perceptual similarities to stimuli encountered during Notwithstanding the above, this study's results should be viewed
the trauma itself. These perceptual properties acquire the status of a within the context of the study design, and the previous hypothesis’
warning signal through perceptual priming (Ehlers et al., 2002; Ehlers, relevance to the current findings should be considered with caution. In
Hackmann, & Michael, 2004). It has also been suggested that partici- the current study, an oddball paradigm was used, the LNC was reported
pants exposed to trauma focus on the central perceptual properties of at later time intervals than in previous studies, and was calculated as a
an object rather than on its meaning due to narrow attention during the measure of the AUC; in previous studies, other ERP paradigms were
traumatic experience (Christianson, 1992; Ehlers & Clark, 2000; Ehlers used and the late negativity was calculated as mean amplitude (Feroz
et al., 2004; Sundermann, Hauschildt, & Ehlers, 2013; Van der et al., 2013; Stout et al., 2013).
Kolk & Fisler, 1995; Wing Lun, 2008). It might be that Anatomy and Taken together, the findings for both P3 and LNC suggest a PTSD-
Blood Distractors used in the current study contained more perceptual related information processing pattern denoting increased responsivity
information (like “smelly skull” or “red blood”) than Morbid Distractors to Anatomy and Blood Distractors in mid-latency time intervals
that contained more affective and symbolic data (“depressed woman”, followed by later increased cerebral response. These findings may point
“broken toy”). These possible differences between TCI categories are to impaired PFC top-down regulation influence on information proces-
also acknowledged by the Rorschach interpretive conceptualization in sing. Another possibility is that these findings indicate a “vigilance-
which Anatomy and Blood content is thought to reflect preoccupation avoidance” pattern of information processing among individuals with
with concrete bodily injury (Bohm, 1958; Meyer & Viglione, 2008) PTSD: the increased P3 amplitude may signify an initial vigilance to
while Morbid content is thought to symbolize wounded self-image and cues that were perceptually primed and associated with danger,

118
G. Zukerman et al. Biological Psychology 127 (2017) 108–122

followed by elevation of the late negativity indicating an effort to avoid possible effects of arousal should be evident among Controls as well
or suppress the processing of emotional stimuli. Such a response pattern as traumatized participants. However, the current finding indicates
(Mathews, 1990; Mogg & Bradley, 1998) is supported by a number of significant differences between trauma-exposed and Control partici-
behavioral studies indicating that, among participants with High Trait pants. Moreover, previous arousal research focused on evaluating
Anxiety or anxiety disorders, there is a significant initial heightened responsivity to validated visual stimuli (Rozenkrants & Polich, 2008),
vigilance to threatening stimuli at the first 500 ms after stimulus while our stimuli were phrases adopted from the Rorschach, possessing
presentation followed by avoidance of the aversive stimuli at very late no previous norms. Nonetheless, arousal, as well as emotional valence
latencies of approximately 1500 ms after stimulus onset (Amir, and level of perceived threat, should be further evaluated; we intend to
Foa, & Coles, 1998; Hermans, Vansteenwegen, & Eelen, 1999; Mogg, evaluate TCI-related emotional arousal levels, and their possible effect
Bradley, Miles, & Dixon, 2004; Rohner, 2002). In summary, this study's on ERP's, in future studies.
findings suggest that information processing alterations among partici- The null effect of Trial type (Target, Distractor, Standard) in this
pants with PTSD may extend beyond the P3 time interval, as indicated study may indicate a general, nonspecific hypervigilant information
by the LNC alterations. Future studies should examine whether the processing pattern among those with PTSD. However, another possibi-
increase in LNC is due to hyper-responsivity to trauma-related stimuli lity is that the present study's sample was underpowered to detect
or reflects an attempt to avoid processing of such stimuli. subtle effects of stimulus type. Moreover, only Blood, Anatomy and
Morbid categories from the TCI were analyzed in our study. Therefore,
9. Study limitations future research should use a larger sample size and also examine the
other two TCI categories (Sex and Aggressive Movement) to enable
To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study examining the additional comparisons.
cerebral response to phrases from Rorschach's Traumatic Contents
Index among participants whose levels of PTSD symptoms have been 10. Conclusions
validated by self-report questionnaires and semi-structured interviews.
Notwithstanding, the current study has several limitations. The present study findings support the previously suggested hypoth-
First, the generalizability of this study might be limited since all esis that the perception of Rorschach inkblots as containing traumatic
participants were young students, healthy, with minimal comorbidity contents represents a breakthrough of intrusive traumatic imagery
and “moderate to severe” level of PTSD. Despite these factors, the fact among those with PTSD (Viglione et al., 2012). Our findings also
that our results were obtained even among this selective subset of generate additional hypotheses that should be explored in future
participants indicates the strength of the findings. Nevertheless, having studies. One such hypothesis is that intrusive memories are elicited
a greater representation of the full range of ages and severity levels by perceptual properties of the inkblots primed by the traumatic
(including participants with extremely severe PTSD symptoms) would experience. Another hypothesis is that phrases with Morbid content
have increased the generalizability of our findings. Moreover, since no contains less perceptual information and that the higher frequency of
formal mental health evaluation was conducted, we cannot exclude the Morbid responses observed in the Rorschach protocols of individuals
possible effects of undetected mental health problems on our findings. with PTSD may stem from another mechanism.
However, considering the fact that our participants were mainly young, Previous research has mainly focused on the conceptual or semantic
high-functioning individuals, we theorize that the levels of the possible associations between the traumatic experience and the processing of
effects would be minor. incoming stimuli in a current setting, those with PTSD have been thought
A second limitation may be the higher representation of females as to allocate attention to stimuli that were conceptually related to trauma
well as an imbalanced gender ratio in some of the groups (Females through meaning via explicit memory representations. Cognitive-based
made up: 12 of 16 PTSD, 14 of the 24 non-PTSD, and 14 of the 16 intervention therapies for PTSD, such as Cognitive Processing Therapy
Control group members). Interestingly, a higher rate of females (Reisick, Monson, & Chard, 2007) are, in fact, based on the premise that
characterizes other related studies (Felmingham et al., 2002; Lobo PTSD develops due to changes in the meaning of previous cognitions
et al., 2014). Regarding gender ratio: our analyses indicated no following traumatic event exposure, such as a change from a thought that
significant differences in male/female ratios between the study groups, “the world is a just place in which people get what they deserve” to “the
and no significant effect of gender as a covariate in any of the analyses. world is an unpredictable, chaotic environment” (Ehlers & Clark, 2000;
Previous research also indicated negligible effects of gender on P3 Janoff-Bulman, 1989). However, it is our contention that, in accordance
characteristics (Rozenkrants & Polich, 2008). While we conclude that with an accumulating body of research in recent years, our findings suggest
the imbalance in male/female ratio had no major effect on results, we the presence of another mechanism of action in PTSD: one in which
suggest that, to some extent, this study findings may be more applicable attention is elevated when encountering stimuli that have been perceptually
to women and to trauma-exposed individuals whose posttraumatic primed, organized in implicit memory representations, and associated with
symptom levels meets clinical criteria for PTSD. impending danger. If confirmed in future studies, this may have significant
Additionally, as noted previously, the absence of data on arousal implications for PTSD treatment methodologies: this newly identified
levels of Distractors is a possible study limitation. While Distractors mechanism may be resilient to standard psychological intervention techni-
were evaluated for emotional valence and threat levels, and those ques that focus solely on altering conceptual, semantic-based cognition.
analyses demonstrate the improbability of the heightened responsivity Development of novel methods to reduce perceptually generated
to Anatomy and Blood Distractors (compared to the Morbid Distractors) PTSDsymptoms may be warranted.
due to those factors, we did not rate Distractors according to their
arousal level. Arousal may play an under-recognized role in the Acknowledgement
heightened responsivity observed among different categories of TCI
Distractors used in this study. Even so, we believe that such possible The authors would like to thank Shira Chana Bienstock for her
arousal differences cannot fully explain variances in cerebral response thorough editorial and scientific review of this manuscript. The authors
within study groups. Arousal level effects have been mainly reported would also like to thank Mirit Mazor Hadad for her help in evaluating
among non-disordered participants (Olofsson et al., 2008); thus, the Rorschach phrases that were used in this study.

Appendix A. Appendix

Table 1A

119
G. Zukerman et al. Biological Psychology 127 (2017) 108–122

Table 1A
Trauma content index distractor phrase examples.

Anatomy distractors Blood distractors Morbid distractors

Smelly scull A pond of blood Split throat


Exposed intestine Streaming blood Wounded soldier
Brain tissue Splashing blood Leg amputee
Skeleton muscle Congealed blood Decaying body
A pair of ribs A bloody fluid Depressed women
Spinal cord Flowing blood Rotten apple
Liver lobe Umbilical blood A baby's body
Gall bladder Spilled blood Injured bear
Tail bone Fresh blood A torn curtain
A pair of lungs Red blood Broken toy

traumatic stress disorder: Phenomenology, theory and therapy. Memory, 12(4),


403–415. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09658210444000025.
Engelhard, I. M., de Jong, P. J., van den Hout, M. A., & van Overveld, M. (2009).
References Expectancy bias and persistence of posttraumatic stress. Behaviour Research and
Therapy, 47(10), 887–892. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2009.06.017.
Exner, J. E. (1974). The Rorschach: A comprehensive system, Vol. 1. New York: Wiley.
American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental Exner, J. E. (2001). A Rorschach workbook for the comprehensive system (5th ed). Asheville,
disorders (5th ed). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association doi: 10.1176/ NC: Rorschach Workshops.
appi.books.9780890425596. Felmingham, K. L., Bryant, R. A., Kendall, C., & Gordon, E. (2002). Event related potential
Amir, N., Foa, E. B., & Coles, M. E. (1998). Automatic activation and strategic avoidance dysfunction in posttraumatic stress disorder: The role of numbing. Psychiatry
of threat-relevant information in social phobia. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, Research, 109(2), 171–179. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0165-1781(02)00003-3.
107(2), 285–290. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.107.2.285. Feroz, F. S., Liecht, G., Steinmann, S., Andreou, C., & Mulert, C. (2016). The time course
Armstrong, J. G., & Lowenstein, R. J. (1990). Characteristics of patients with multiple of activity within the dorsal and rostral-ventral anterior cingulate cortex in the
personality and dissociative disorders on psychological testing. Journal of Nervous and emotional Stroop task. Brain Topography, Advanced online publication.. http://dx.doi.
Mental Disorders, 178(7), 448–454. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/00005053- org/10.1007/s10548-016-0521-3.
199007000-00006. Foa, E. B. (1995). Posttraumatic stress diagnostic scale manual. Minneapolis, MN: National
Attias, J., Bliech, A., Furman, V., & Zinger, Y. (1996a). Event-related potentials in post- Computer Systems Inc.
traumatic stress disorder of combat origin. Biological Psychiatry, 40(5), 373–381. Foa, E. B., Cashman, L., Jaycox, L., & Perry, K. (1997). The validation of a self-report
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0006-3223(95)00419-X. measure of posttraumatic stress disorder: The posttraumatic diagnostic scale.
Attias, J., Bliech, A., & Gilat, S. (1996b). Classification of veterans with post-traumatic Psychological Assessment, 9(4), 445–451. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/1040-3590.9.4.
stress disorder using visual brain evoked P3s to traumatic stimuli. British Journal of 445.
Psychiatry, 168, 110–115. http://dx.doi.org/10.1192/bjp.168.1.110. Galletly, C., Clarc, C. R., McFarlane, A. C., & Weber, D. C. (2001). Working memory in
Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., & Brown, G. K. (1996). Manual for the beck depression inventory-II. posttraumatic stress disorder – an event-related potential study. Journal of Traumatic
San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation. Stress, 14(2), 295–309. http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/A:1011112917797.
Blake, D. D., Weathers, F. W., Nagy, L. M., Kaloupek, D. G., Gusman, F. D., Charney, D. S., Goldfinger, D. A., Amdur, R. L., & Liberson, I. (1998). Rorschach patterns of response in
et al. (1995). The development of a clinician-administered PTSD scale. Journal of Vietnam veterans with posttraumatic stress disorder versus combat and normal
Traumatic Stress, 8, 75–90. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/jts.2490080106. controls. Depression and Anxiety, 8(3), 104–111 doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1520-6394(1998)
Blatt, S. J. (1990). The Rorschach: A test of perception or an evaluation of representation. 8:3 < 104::AID-DA2 > 3.0.CO;2-Y.
Journal of Personality Assessment, 55(3 & 4), 394–416. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/ Halligan, S. L., Clark, D. M., & Ehlers, A. (2002). Cognitive processing, memory, and the
s15327752jpa5503&4_1. development of PTSD symptoms: Two experimental analogue studies. Journal of
Blomhoff, S., Reinvang, I., & Malt, U. F. (1998). Event-related potentials to stimuli with Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 33(2), 73–89. http://dx.doi.org/10.
emotional impact in posttraumatic stress disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 44(10), 1016/S0005-7916(02)00014-9.
1045–1053. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0006-3223(98)00058-4. Hartmann, E., Halvorsen, M., & Wang, C. E. A. (2013). Rorschach variables and
Bohm, E. (1958). A textbook in Rorschach test diagnosis: For psychologists, physicians and dysfunctional attitudes as measures of depressive vulnerability: A 9-year follow-up
teachers. New York and London: Grune and Stratton. study of individuals with different histories of major depressive disorder. Journal of
Brand, B. L., Armstrong, J. G., & Loewenstein, R. J. (2006). Psychological assessment of Personality Assessment, 95, 26–37. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00223891.2012.
patients with dissociative identity disorder. Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 29, 713881.
145–168. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psc.2005.10.014. Hermans, D., Vansteenwegen, D., & Eelen, P. (1999). Eye movement registration as a
Brand, B. L., Armstrong, J. G., Loewenstein, R. J., & McNary, S. W. (2009). Personality continuous index of attention deployment: Data from a group of spider anxious
differences on the Rorschach of dissociative identity disorder, borderline personality students. Cognition and Emotion, 13, 419–434. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/
disorder and psychotic inpatients. Psychological Trauma: Theory, Research, Practice 026999399379249.
and Policy, 1(3), 188–205. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0016561. Herz, N., Reuveni, I., Goldstein, A., Peri, T., Schreiber, S., Harpaz, Y., et al. (2016). Neural
Buckley, T. C., Blanchard, E. B., & Neill, W. T. (2000). Information processing and PTSD: correlates of attention bias in posttraumatic stress disorder. Clinical Neurophysiology,
A review of the empirical literature. Clinical Psychology Review, 20(8), 1041–1065. 127(10), 3268–3276. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.clinph.2016.07.016.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0272-7358(99)00030-6. Holaday, M. (2000). Rorschach protocols from children and adolescents diagnosed with
Burch, R. F. (1993). Assessment of post-traumatic stress disorder among Vietnam posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Personality Assessment, 75(1), 143–157.
veterans: A Rorschach study. Dissertation Abstracts International, 54(5-B), 2742. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/S15327752JPA7501_10.
Christianson, S. A. (1992). Remembering emotional events: Potential mechanisms. In S. Hooper, L. M., Stockton, P., Krupnick, J. L., & Green, B. L. (2011). Development, use and
A. Christianson (Ed.), The handbook of emotional memory: Research and theory.. psychometric properties of the trauma history questionnaire. Journal of Loss and
Hillsdale, N.J: Erlbaum. Trauma: International Perspectives on Stress and Coping, 16, 258–283. http://dx.doi.
Dickson, D. S., & Federmeier, K. D. (2014). Hemispheric differences in orthographic and org/10.1080/15325024.2011.572035.
semantic processing as revealed by event-related potentials. Neuropsychologia, 64, Iv, J., Zhao, L., Gong, J., Chen, C., & Miao, D. (2010). Event related potential based
230–239. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.09.037. evidence of cognitive dysfunction in patients during the first episode of depression
Dozois, D. J. A., Dobson, K. S., & Ahnberg, J. L. (1998). A psychometric evaluation of the using a novelty oddball task. Psychiatry Research, 182(1), 58–66. http://dx.doi.org/
Beck Depression Inventory-II. Psychological Assessment, 10(2), 83–89. http://dx.doi. 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2010.02.005.
org/10.1037/1040-3590.10.2.83. Janoff-Bulman, R. (1989). Assumptive worlds and the stress of traumatic events:
Ehlers, A., & Clark, D. M. (2000). A cognitive model of posttraumatic stress disorder. Application of the schema constructs. Social Cognition, 7(2), 113–136. http://dx.doi.
Behaviour Research and Therapy, 38(4), 319–345. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0005- org/10.1521/soco.1989.7.2.113.
7967(99)00123-0. Javanbakht, A., Liberzon, I., Amirsadri, A., Gjini, K., & Boutros, N. N. (2011). Event-
Ehlers, A., Hackmann, A., Steil, R., Clohessy, S., Wenninger, K., & Winter, H. (2002). The related potential studies of post-traumatic stress disorder: A critical review and
nature of intrusive memories after trauma: The warning signal hypothesis. Behaviour synthesis. Biology of Mood & Anxiety Disorders, 1(5), 1–12. http://dx.doi.org/10.
Research and Therapy, 40(9), 995–1002. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0005-7967(01) 1186/2045-5380-1-5.
00077-8. Johnson, J. D., Allana, T. N., Medlin, M. D., Harris, E. W., & Karl, A. (2013). Meta-analytic
Ehlers, A., Hackmann, A., & Michael, T. (2004). Intrusive re-experiencing in post- review of P3 components in posttraumatic stress disorder and clinical utility. Clinical

120
G. Zukerman et al. Biological Psychology 127 (2017) 108–122

EEG and Neuroscience, 44(2), 112–134. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/ and passive conditions. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 17, 25–34. http://
1550059412469742. dx.doi.org/10.1016/0167-8760(94)90052-3.
Karl, A., Malta, L. S., & Maercker, A. (2006). Meta-analytic review of event-related Pupo, M. C., Jorge, M. R., Schoedl, A. F., Bressan, R. A., Andreoli, S. B., Mello, M. F., et al.
potential studies of post-traumatic stress disorder. Biological Psychology, 71(2), (2011). The accuracy of the clinician-administered PTSD scale (CAPS) to identify
123–147. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2005.03.004. PTSD cases in victims of urban violence. Psychiatry Research, 185(1–2), 157–160.
Katayama, J., & Polich, J. (1998). Stimulus context determines P3a and P3b. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2009.11.006.
Psychophysiology, 35, 23–33. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1469-8986.3510023. Pratt, H., Mittelman, N., Bleich, N., & Laufer, I. (2004). Auditory middle-latency
Katsounari, I., & Jacobowitz, J. (2011). A comparative analysis of MMPI and Rorschach components to fusion of speech elements forming an auditory object. Clinical
findings assessing combat-related PTSD in Vietnam veterans. Psychology, 2(4), Neurophysiology, 115, 1083–1089. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.clinph.2003.12.004.
335–341. http://dx.doi.org/10.4236/psych.2011.24053. Qi, S., Ding, C., & Li, H. (2014). Neural correlates of inefficient filtering of emotionally
Kamphuis, J. H., Kugeares, S. L., & Finn, S. E. (2000). Rorschach correlates of sexual neutral distractors from working memory in trait anxiety. Cognitive, Affective and
abuse: Trauma content and aggression indexes. Journal of Personality Assessment, Behavioral Brain Neuroscience, 14, 253–265. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13415-013-
75(2), 212–224. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/S15327752JPA7502_3. 0203-5.
Kendall, P. C., Hollon, S. D., Beck, A. T., Hamenn, C. L., & Ingram, R. E. (1987). Issues and Rauch, S. L., Shin, L. M., Whalen, P. J., & Pitman, R. K. (1998). Neuroimaging and the
recommendations regarding use of the Beck depression inventory. Cognitive Therapy neuroanatomy of post traumatic stress disorder. CNS Spectrum, 3(suppl 2), 30–41.
and Research, 11(3), 289–299. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF01186280. Rauch, S. L., Shin, L. M., & Phelps, E. A. (2006). Neurocircuitry models of posttraumatic
Kimble, M., Batterink, L., Marks, E., Ross, C., & Fleming, K. (2012). Negative expectancies stress disorder and extinction: Human neuroimaging research- past, present and
in posttraumatic stress disorder: Neurophysiological (N400) and behavioral evidence. future. Biological Psychiatry, 60, 376–382. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.
Journal of Psychiatric Research, 46(7), 849–855. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j. 2006.06.004.
jpsychires.2012.03.023. Reisick, P. A., Monson, C. M., & Chard, K. M. (2007). Cognitive processing therapy:
Kimble, M. O., Fleming, K., Bandy, C., & Zambetti, A. (2010). Attention to novel and Veterans/military version. Washington DC: Department of Veterans Affairs.
target stimuli in trauma survivors. Psychiatry Research, 178(3), 501–506. http://dx. Rohner, J. C. (2002). The time-course of visual threat processing: High trait anxious
doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2009.10.009. individuals eventually avert their gaze from angry faces. Cognition and Emotion,
Kimble, M., Kaloupek, D., Kaufman, M., & Deldin, P. (2000). Stimulus novelty 16(6), 837–844. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02699930143000572.
differentially affects attentional allocation in PTSD. Psychiatry Research, 47(10), Rozenkrants, B., & Polich, J. (2008). Affective ERP processing in visual oddball task;
880–890. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0006-3223(99)00258-9. Arousal, valence and gender. Clinical Neurophysiology, 119(10), 2260–2265. http://
Kindt, M., van den Hout, M., Arntz, A., & Drost, J. (2008). The influence of data-driven dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.clinph.2008.07.213.
versus conceptually-driven processing on the development of PTSD-like symptoms. Saar-Ashkenazy, R., Shalev, H., Kanthak, M. K., Guez, J., Freidman, A., & Cohen, J. E.
Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 39(4), 546–557. http://dx. (2015). Altered processing of visual emotional stimuli in posttraumatic stress
doi.org/10.1016/j.jbtep.2007.12.003. disorder: An event-related potential study. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 233(2),
LeDoux, J. E. (2000). Emotion circuits in the brain. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 23, 165–174. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.pscychresns.2015.05.015.
155–184. http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev.neuro.23.1.155. Selpien, H., Siebert, C., Genc, E., Beste, C., Faustmann, P. M., Gunturkun, O., et al. (2015).
Lin, M., Hofmann, S. G., Qian, M., & Li, S. (2015). Enhanced association between Left dominance for language perception starts in the extrastriate cortex: An ERP and
perceptual stimuli and trauma-related information in individuals with posttraumatic sLORETA study. Behavioral Brain Research, 291, 325–333. http://dx.doi.org/10.
stress disorder symptoms. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 46, 1016/j.bbr.2015.05.050.
202–207. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jbtep.2014.10.008. Sloan, P., Arsenault, L., & Hilsenroth, M. (2002). Use of the Rorschach in the assessment
Lobo, I., David, I. A., Figueira, I., Campagnoli, R. R., Volchan, E., Pereira, M. G., et al. of war-related stress in military personnel. Rorschachiana, 25, 86–122. http://dx.doi.
(2014). Brain reactivity to unpleasant stimuli is associated with severity of org/10.1027/1192-5604.25.1.86.
posttraumatic stress symptoms. Biological Psychology, 103, 233–241. http://dx.doi. Sloan, P., Arsenault, L., Hilsenroth, M., Handler, L., & Harvill, L. (1996). Rorschach
org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2014.09.002. measures of posttraumatic stress in Persian Gulf War veterans: A three-year follow-up
Lobo, I., Portugal, L. C., Figueira, I., Volchan, E., David, I., Pereira, M. G., et al. (2015). study. Journal of Personality Assessment, 66, 54–64. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/
EEG correlates of the severity of posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms: A s15327752jpa6601_4.
systematic review of the dimensional PTSD literature. Journal of Affective Disorders, Smith, S. R., Chang, J., Kochinski, S., Patz, S., & Nowinski, L. A. (2010). Initial validity of
183, 210–220. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2015.05.015. the Logical Rorschach in the assessment of trauma. Journal of Personality Assessment,
Luck, S. J. (2014). An introduction to event related potentials technique. Cambridge: MIT 92(3), 222–231. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00223891003670174.
press. Stander, V. A., Thomsen, C. J., & Highfill-McRoy, R. M. (2014). Etiology of depression
MacNamara, A., Post, D., Kennedy, A. E., Rabinak, C. A., & Phan, K. L. (2013). comorbidity in combat-related PTSD: A review of the literature. Clinical Psychology
Electrocortical processing of social signals of threat in combat-related post-traumatic Review, 34(2), 87–98. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2013.12.002.
stress disorder. Biological Psychology, 94(2), 441–449. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j. Stanford, M. S., Vasterling, J. J., Mathias, C. W., Constans, J. I., & Houston, R. J. (2001).
biopsycho.2013.08.009. Impact of threat relevance on P3 event-related potentials in combat-related post-
Mathews, A. (1990). Why worry? The cognitive function of anxiety. Behaviour Research traumatic stress disorder. Psychiatry Research, 102(2), 125–137. http://dx.doi.org/
and Therapy, 28(6), 455–468. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0005-7967(90)90132-3. 10.1016/S0165-1781(01)00236-0.
McCarthy, S. (2008). Post-Traumatic Stress Diagnostic Scale (PDS). Occupational Medicine, Stout, D. M., Shackman, A. J., & Larson, C. L. (2013). Failure to filter: Anxious individuals
58(5), 379. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/occmed/kqn062. show inefficient gating of threat from working memory. Frontiers in Human
McFarlane, A. C., Weber, D. L., & Clark, C. R. (1993). Abnormal stimulus processing in Neuroscience, 7, 58. http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2013.00058.
posttraumatic stress disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 34(5), 311–320. http://dx.doi. Sundermann, O., Hauschildt, M., & Ehlers, A. (2013). Perceptual processing during
org/10.1016/0006-3223(93)90088-U. trauma, priming and development of intrusive memories. Journal of Behavior Therapy
Meyer, G. J., & Viglione, D. J. (2008). An introduction to Rorschach assessment. In R. P. and Experimental Psychiatry, 44(2), 213–220. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jbtep.
Archer, & S. R. Smith (Eds.), Personality Assessment (pp. 281–336). New York, NY: 2012.10.001.
Routledge. Thomas, C. L., Goegan, L. D., Newman, K. R., & Arndt, J. E. (2013). Attention to threat
Min, S. K., Lee, C. H., Kim, J. Y., & Sim, E. J. (2011). Posttraumatic stress disorder in images in individuals with clinical and subthreshold symptoms of post traumatic
former ‘comfort women’. Israeli Journal of Psychiatry and Related Sciences, 48, stress disorder. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 27, 445–447. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/
161–169. j.janxdis.2013.05.
Mogg, K., & Bradley, B. P. (1998). A cognitive-motivational analysis of anxiety. Behaviour Tibon, S., Rothschild, L., Appel, L., & Zeligman, R. (2011). Assessing effects of national
Research and Therapy, 36(9), 809–848. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0005-7967(98) trauma on adaptive functioning of mentally healthy adults: An exploratory Rorschach
00063-1. study. Psychology, 2(9), 953–960. http://dx.doi.org/10.4236/psych.2011.29144.
Mogg, K., Bradley, B., Miles, F., & Dixon, R. (2004). Brief report: Time course of Tolin, D. F., & Foa, E. B. (2006). Sex differences in trauma and posttraumatic stress
attentional bias for threat scenes: Testing the vigilance-avoidance hypothesis. disorder: A quantitative review of 25 years of research. Psychological Bulletin, 132(6),
Cognition and Emotion, 18(5), 689–700. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/ 959–992. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.132.6.959.
02699930341000158. Van der Kolk, B. A., & Ducey, C. P. (1989). The psychological processing of traumatic
Meloy, J. R., & Genoco, C. B. (1997). Borderline psychopath. In J. F. Murry, C. B. Genoco, experience: Rorschach patterns in PTSD. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 2(3), 259–274.
M. W. Acklin, & J. R. Meloy (Eds.), Contemporary Rorschach Interpretation (pp. 139– http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF00976231.
155). New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum. Van der Kolk, B. A., & Fisler, R. (1995). Dissociation and the fragmentary nature of
Murry, M. M., Brunet, D., & Michel, C. M. (2008). Topographic ERP analyses: A step-by- traumatic memories: Overview and exploratory study. Journal of Traumatic Stress,
step tutorial review. Brain Topography, 20(4), 249–264. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/ 8(4), 505–525. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/jts.2490080402.
s10548-008-0054-5. Van der Kolk, B. A., & van der Hart, O. (1989). Pierre Janet and the breakdown of
Olofsson, J. K., Nordin, S., Sequeira, H., & Polich, J. (2008). Affective picture processing: adaptation in psychological trauma. American Journal of Psychiatry, 146(12),
An integrative review of ERP findings. Biological Psychology, 77(3), 247–265. http:// 1530–1540. http://dx.doi.org/10.1176/ajp.146.12.1530.
dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2007.11.006. Vogel, E. K., & Machizawa, M. G. (2004). Neural activity predicts individual differences in
Opaas, M., & Hartmann, E. (2013). Rorschach assessment of traumatized refugees: An visual working memory capacity. Nature, 428, 748–751. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/
exploratory factor analysis. Journal of Personality Assessment, 95(5), 457–470. http:// nature02447.
dx.doi.org/10.1080/00223891.2013.781030. Weathers, F. W., Keane, T. M., & Davidson, J. R. (2001). Clinician-administered PTSD
Polich, J., & McIssac, H. K. (1994). Comparison of auditory P300 habituation from active scale: A review of the first ten years of research. Depression and Anxiety, 13(3),

121
G. Zukerman et al. Biological Psychology 127 (2017) 108–122

132–156. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/da.1029. Rorschach inkblot test to assess post-traumatic conditions. Psychological Injury and
Weiner, I. B., & Greene, R. L. (2007). Handbook of personality assessment. Hoboken, NJ: Law, 5(2), 135–144. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s12207-012-9128-5.
John Wiley & Sons. Zhang, Y., Kong, F., Han, L., Najam Ul Hasan, A., & Chen, H. (2014). Attention bias in
Whisman, M. A., & Richardson, E. D. (2015). Normative data on the Beck depression earthquake-exposed survivors: An event-related potential study. International Journal
inventory – second edition (BDI-II) in college students. Journal of Clinical Psychology, of Psychophysiology, 94(3), 358–364. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2014.09.
71(9), 898–907. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/jclp.22188. 004.
Wing Lun, L. M. (2008). A cognitive model of peritraumatic dissociation. Psychology and Zhang, Y., Kong, F., Hasan, A. N., Jackson, T., & Chen, H. (2015). Recognition memory
Psychotherapy: Theory, Research and Practice, 81(Pt 3), 297–307. http://dx.doi.org/ bias in earthquake-exposed survivors: A behavioral and ERP study.
10.1348/147608308X304484. Neuropsychobiology, 71(2), 70–75. http://dx.doi.org/10.1159/000369023.
Viglione, D. J., Towns, B., & Lindshield, D. (2012). Understanding and using the

122

You might also like