You are on page 1of 3

F O C U S O N P S Y C H I AT R I C D I S O R D E R S C O M M E N TA R Y

Psychiatric distress in animals versus


animal models of psychiatric distress
Robert M Sapolsky
The realm of human uniqueness steadily shrinks; reflecting this, other primates suffer from states closer to
© 2016 Nature America, Inc., part of Springer Nature. All rights reserved.

depression or anxiety than ‘depressive-like’ or ‘anxiety-like behavior’. Nonetheless, there remain psychiatric domains
unique to humans. Appreciating these continuities and discontinuities must inform the choice of neurobiological
approach used in studying any animal model of psychiatric disorders. More fundamentally, the continuities reveal
how aspects of psychiatric malaise run deeper than our species’ history.

Primatology research suggests that other human, as one “we are the only species that…” This includes a characteristic profile of activa-
primates suffer from crippling depression or after another has fallen. As first documented tion in the frontal cortex and ventral striatum,
anxiety, implying that these diseases’ roots pre- by Jane Goodall with her chimpanzees in frequent glancing at the corridor not taken and
date human history. At the same time, some Gombe, Tanzania, we are not the only species a greater likelihood of persevering with a first
realms of psychiatry remain uniquely human. that makes and uses tools. Instead, such mate- corridor choice in the future4. Moreover, rats
Recognizing the similarities and dissimilarities rial culture is widespread among other spe- show temperamental differences in degree of
between us and other primates is essential in cies; moreover, some species utilize multiple optimism, in that some tend to interpret an
studying animal models of psychiatric disease. tools in sequential manners and make, store or ambiguous cue as a reward contingency, oth-
A cornerstone of evolution is the continu- transport tools in anticipation of future use1. ers as a punishment contingency5.
ity of traits across species as a result of shared We are most definitely no longer the only Rodents also show elements of empathy and
ancestry. Such continuity is the reason why we, animal that kills members of its own species. proactive compassion. A mouse’s pain thresh-
chimps and bonobos share more than 98% of Instead, other animals inflict lethal harm on old will be lowered when in the presence of
our DNA or why we and warthogs both have others in ways that can be highly strategic. another mouse in pain, but this only occurs if
four limbs and two nostrils; it is the reason for Chimps, for example, will kill each other in the other mouse is a cagemate6. As in humans,
our vague and distant relatedness to both T. an organized manner that can involve the use the circle of such shared pain is expanded to
rex and crabgrass. of physical objects to do harm. Moreover, they include strangers if the glucocorticoid stress-
This continuity obviously extends to ner- are capable of something resembling genocide, response to the presence of a stranger is phar-
vous systems; if nothing else, if you want to in which individuals are killed, not for who macologically blocked7. Moreover, rats will
accurately predict a neuron’s resting poten- they are, but for what category they belong to work proactively to release a distressed cage-
tial, whether it is from a human or an Aplysia, (i.e., males of a neighboring group)2. As pio- mate from a restrainer and will even forgo a
you’re going to have to relearn the Nernst equa- neered by Frans de Waal and expanded by oth- desirable food reward to do so8. In addition,
tion (or was it the Goldman equation?). And, ers, we are not the only species that shows the pair-bonded prairie voles will increase their
of course, it extends to the principle output of rudiments of a range of prosocial behaviors, rate of licking and grooming of a mate who is
the nervous system, namely, behavior and the including reconciliation, empathy, third-party stressed, but not of a stranger who is stressed;
internal life that can be inferred from it. consolation, an aversion to inequality and a this behavior is dependent upon oxytocin
sense of justice (c.f. 3). actions in the anterior cingulate cortex9.
The narrowing range of human Many attributes once thought to be human- Finally, on a level that initially seems play-
uniqueness specific even need to be extended to rodents. ful but is ultimately profound, we are not even
Behavioral continuity has repeatedly forced a For example, consider a rat in an arena leading the only species that enjoys being tickled. Apes
narrowing of the definition of what it is to be to a number of corridors, each giving a particu- tickle each other in naturalistic settings, trig-
lar food reward after a time delay. Upon enter- gering rapid exhalations and vocalizations
Robert M. Sapolsky is in the Departments of ing a corridor and hearing a tone that indicates that are similar to human laughter. Moreover,
Biological Sciences, Neurology and Neurosurgery, the duration of the delay, it can choose to stay “tickle me” is a phrase learned early by apes in
Stanford University, Stanford, California, USA; or switch to another corridor. When rats forgo various symbolic language studies (as a stu-
and the Institute of Primate Research, National a reward with a moderate delay, in order to dent, I worked on one such project and spent a
Museums of Kenya, Nairobi, Kenya. choose a corridor that turns out to require an large percentage of a summer being requested
e-mail: sapolsky@stanford.edu. even longer wait, they show evidence of ‘regret’. to do so by a chimp). Remarkably, manual

NATURE NEUROSCIENCE VOLUME 19 | NUMBER 11 | NOVEMBER 2016 1387


C O M M E N TA R Y

be a scenario that would occur with a clarity an array of neotenized behaviors as a juvenile
never observed in actual field observations: a (for example, continued attempts to nurse).
low-ranking male baboon stumbles upon and Upon Flo’s death of old age in 1972, Flint was
predates a highly prized prey item, a young unable to function independently, withdrew
gazelle. Were this to have happened in the from all social activities, stayed near her body
middle of the troop, he would barely have continuously and died within a month15.
managed a bite before being displaced by a Thus, other primates respond to loss with
higher-ranking male. Instead, he is at a dis- behavioral and/or endocrine indices of dis-
tance from everyone else and has some time tress; most respond with increased seeking
to eat before a higher-ranking male arrives. of social support and affiliation; a substantial
A clean, quantitative index of anxiety would minority respond instead with a prolonged
be how often the baboon would stop eating period of social withdrawal and maladaptive
to look back over his shoulder to see who is behavior; a tiny subset are pathologically, even
approaching; in contrast, an index of depres- fatally, incapacitated by loss. The similarities
sion of the learned helplessness type would be to human grief, reactive depression and severe
if his displacement from the kill would seem major depression are striking. Moreover, stud-
so inevitable that he walks away before anyone ies of captive primates demonstrate neurobio-
© 2016 Nature America, Inc., part of Springer Nature. All rights reserved.

is even close, perhaps even before taking a bite. logical underpinnings to these affective states
While such clarity is rare, nonhuman pri- that are the same as with human depression
mates in their natural habitats do display (for example, gene–environment interactions
‘depression-like’ and ‘anxiety-like’ behavior concerning vulnerability and the serotonin
Figure 1 Affective experiences on a continuum (see Fig. 1). The former is most common after transporter16). Appropriately, primatologists
with humans; subjects are olive baboons (Papio
the death of particular group members. Among who study such ‘comparative thanatology’
anubis) living in the Serengeti ecosystem of
East Africa. Top: an adolescent male who had Chacma baboons, for example, the predation of typically transition from thinking that they are
spent much of the previous months closely an adult female produces a month-long increase examining ‘grieving behavior’ or ‘depression-
following an adolescent female (arrow), in entirely in glucocorticoid levels among her close rela- like behavior’ to, instead, ‘grief ’ and ‘depres-
unsuccessful attempts to be groomed by her. tives (but not among unrelated individuals). sion’ in a closely related species.
Bottom: an adult male who, one hour before, had Moreover, such relatives seek social support, We and other primates are also on a con-
been toppled from the alpha position, having lost
initiating grooming at a higher frequency and tinuum when it comes to anxiety. There are
a fight and being forced to give a subordinating
gesture to the beta male.
with a larger number of grooming partners11. broad similarities in the circumstances of
Less adaptive responses to the death of a ambiguity about threat that provoke it (for
somatosensory stimulation—tickling—by relative are seen as well. One that has long example, proximity of an aggressive alpha
humans induces a positive affective state in intrigued and puzzled primatologists occurs in male for a monkey or of a hostile boss for a
rats, elicits a characteristic chirping vocaliza- chimpanzees and various monkey species (for human). There is also continuity in terms of
tion in the 50 kHz range that is also elicited by example, macaques and baboons). Specifically, the behavioral stereotypies that occur: an anx-
sex, appetitive social situations and dopamine when an infant dies, the mother will often ious monkey, for example, will show bursts of
agonists, and is dependent upon mesolimbic carry and groom the corpse for a long period repetitive self-grooming and scratching, teeth
dopamine activation10. Moreover, such tick- afterward; among Japanese macaques, for grinding, nose wiping, assuming of a vigilant
ling makes rats more optimistic in the ambigu- example, nearly 30% of females show this stance, slapping at the ground or purposeless
ous cue test just described5. behavior when an infant under a month of age breaking of branches or pulling up of grass;
If we prick them, do they not bleed? If we dies12. The behavior is typically accompanied the similarities to displacement behaviors in
tickle them, do they not do something that by decreased feeding and by social withdrawal anxious humans is striking.
is on a phylogenetic continuum with laugh- by the female, such as decreased grooming, There is also a continuum of the physiol-
ing? What are we to make of continuity of contact or other social interactions. Some pri- ogy underlying anxiety; for example, as in
behavior and affect among us and other ani- matologists have proposed that the behavior is humans, the frequency of anxiety-related
mals when it comes to mental illness? At one adaptive, insofar as it would prevent the fatal behaviors among wild baboons increases
extreme, psychiatric disorders can be viewed abandonment of an infant who is transiently in dramatically when benzodiazepine recep-
as so intrinsically human that any similarities an unresponsive state, rather than dead. This tors are blocked with a b-carboline receptor
in other species constitute merely a model. At seems an implausible selective force, in that antagonist. Moreover, endogenous benzodi-
the other, some other species can be viewed mothers will carry infant corpses for astonish- azepine tone varies with social setting: that
as potentially suffering from psychiatric disor- ingly long periods of time—numerous species b-carboline effect is most pronounced in low-
ders as surely as do humans. To approach this show corpse carrying on the order of weeks to ranking baboons but only in troops in which
issue, it is worth examining what the homologs months, during which time corpses decay or such subordinate animals are subject to high
of certain types of psychiatric distress look like mummify13,14. rates of displacement aggression at the teeth
in other primates navigating the vicissitudes of Moreover, primates in their natural habitats of dominant individuals17. Furthermore, indi-
life in their natural habitats. show pathological reactions to loss. Probably vidual differences in anxiety-related behavior
the best documented is the case of Flint, an can be long lasting in other primates; male
Mood disorders and anxiety in free-living eight-year-old chimpanzee studied from birth baboons differ dramatically as to how read-
primates by Goodall. Flint was the son of Flo, a matri- ily they perceive neutral social situations to be
What might anxiety or depression look like arch in the group, and as her final living child, threatening (as assessed by how close a higher
in another primate in the wild? Here would he was atypically dependent on her, showing ranking male, in a nonthreatening state, has

1388 VOLUME 19 | NUMBER 11 | NOVEMBER 2016 NATURE NEUROSCIENCE


C O M M E N TA R Y

to be in order to disrupt a subject’s ongoing if there are primate homologs of Capgras tion or socioeconomic status in increasing
behavior). These temperamental differences delusion (the belief that loved ones have incidences of certain types of mental illness.
are stable over years, and a propensity toward been replaced by imposters), acrotomophilia There are also obviously human-specific ele-
reacting to the neutral presence of a rival as (sexual arousal by amputees) or Stendhal ments to the far too common skepticism in the
if he is threatening is highly predictive of ele- syndrome (ecstatic agitation and autonomic lay public about the biological bases of mental
vated basal glucocorticoid levels18. arousal when first experiencing artwork in illness. This is the view of so many who have
Thus, there is a striking continuum between Florence). All species are unique, but some been spared the reality of, say, depression, that
humans and other primates in their natural are unique in particularly unique ways. it is merely manipulation, self-indulgence or
habitats when it comes to mood and anxiety What is one to make of animal models for lack of willpower—some sort of neurochemi-
disorders. Of course, at the same time, the psychiatric disorders that seem deeply specific cal depletion of Calvinist gumption. What
qualitative differences between humans and to humans? In such cases, it is appropriate to these studies of wild primates show is that
other primates are enormous. A chimp may cautiously study well-defined, narrow and the biological roots of pathological sadness or
feel a malignant sadness because of the death reductive endpoints that model isolated pieces anxiety run much deeper than the inventions,
of her child; a human may do so because of the of the disorder. This is the world of endophe- neuroses and artifices of human life.
death of a child in a distant refugee camp, in notypes, signal transduction pathways, epi-
COMPETING FINANCIAL INTERESTS
a novel, or because of a Mahlerian song cycle genetic modification of gene regulation and so
The author declares no competing financial interests.
about the death of children. A baboon may be on; the enormous and exciting progress with
© 2016 Nature America, Inc., part of Springer Nature. All rights reserved.

made anxious by the proximity of an alpha such approaches is highlighted in many of the Reprints and permissions information is available
male; a human by thoughts of mortality or the papers in this issue. online at http://www.nature.com/reprints/index.html.
I.R.S. or by wondering if their research will be What is one to make of the similarities
replicated. Even the motoric stereotypies that between some cases of psychiatric disease 1. Shumaker, R.W., Walkup, K.R. & Beck, B.B. Animal
can be shared with us by other primates have in humans and psychiatric disease and dis- Tool Behavior, the Use and Manufacture of Tools by
Animals. (Johns Hopkins University Press, 2011).
striking human-specific features. Consider tress in other primates? One direction that 2. Wrangham, R.W. & Glowacki, L. Hum. Nat. 23, 5–29
the first-person account of a tic by a character I often find to be unfruitful is questioning (2012).
3. Brosnan, S.F. & de Waal, F.B.M. Science 346, 314–
with Tourette’s syndrome in Jonathan Letham’s whether, say, a ‘depressed’ nonhuman pri- 319 (2014).
novel, Motherless Brooklyn19: mate is really feeling the same things as a 4. Steiner, A.P. & Redish, A.D. Nat. Neurosci. 17, 995–
“I left the notebook beside the [bag of fast depressed human. This readily turns into a 1002 (2014).
5. Rygula, R., Pluta, H. & Popik, P. PLoS One 7, e51959
food hamburgers] on the seat between us and debate floating somewhere between Skinner’s (2012).
slapped the compartment door shut again, famed, dogmatic assertion that “emotions are 6. Langford, D.J. et al. Science 312, 1967–1970 (2006).
then delivered six redundant slaps to the same fictional causes to which we attribute animal 7. Martin, L.J. et al. Curr. Biol. 25, 326–332 (2015).
8. Ben-Ami Bartal, I., Decety, J. & Mason, P. Science
spot to ventilate my brain’s pressure by repro- behavior”20 and college dorm discussions of 334, 1427–1430 (2011).
ducing the hollow thump I’d liked. Six was a whether someone else sees the color blue the 9. Burkett, J.P. et al. Science 351, 375–378 (2016).
10. Panksepp, J. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 35, 1791–
lucky number tonight, six burgers, six forty- same way that you do. When a highly depen- 1804 (2011).
five. So six slaps.” dent chimpanzee sits alone by the corpse 11. Engh, A.L. et al. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 273, 707–712
Moreover, there is a wide range of psychiat- of his mother for so long that he starves to (2006).
12. Sugiyama, Y. et al. Anthropol. Sci. 117, 113–119 (2009).
ric maladies for which there is no nonhuman death, the similarities with the human experi- 13. Fashing, P.J. et al. Am. J. Primatol. 73, 405–409
primate equivalent, and this is not surprising. ence far outweigh the differences. (2011).
Psychotic thought disorder and florid hal- This strengthens the validity of some animal 14. Biro, D. et al. Curr. Biol. 20, 351–352 (2010).
15. Goodall, J. The Chimpanzees of Gombe: Patterns of
lucinations are not particularly compatible ‘models’ in psychiatry research while, at the Behavior. (Harvard University Press, 1986).
with long-term survival in a wild primate and same time, often questioning their ethicality. 16. Lesch, K.P. Curr. Top. Behav. Neurosci. 7, 251–280
(2011).
likewise for a manic state of grandiosity, if it But the continuity raises a deeper point. Many 17. Sapolsky, R.M. & Share, L.J. PLoS Biol. 2, E106–
involves repeated challenges to a predator or aspects of psychiatry research focus, appropri- E113 (2004).
high-ranking individual. Nonhuman primate ately, on issues specific to the human condi- 18. Sapolsky, R. & Ray, J. Am. J. Primatol. 8, l–13 (1989).
19. Letham, J. Motherless Brooklyn. (Vintage, N.Y., 2000).
anorexics will leave few copies of their genes. tion—for example, cross-cultural differences 20. Skinner, B.F. Science and Human Behavior. (Simon &
And furthermore, I would be mighty surprised in symptomatology or the roles of urbaniza- Schuster, NY, 1953).

NATURE NEUROSCIENCE VOLUME 19 | NUMBER 11 | NOVEMBER 2016 1389

You might also like