You are on page 1of 10

J Child Fam Stud

DOI 10.1007/s10826-014-0029-y

ORIGINAL PAPER

Parental Sensitivity Towards Toddlers and Infant Siblings


Predicting Toddler Sharing and Compliance
Sheila R. van Berkel • Marleen G. Groeneveld • Judi Mesman •

Joyce J. Endendijk • Elizabeth T. Hallers-Haalboom •


Lotte D. van der Pol • Marian J. Bakermans-Kranenburg

Ó Springer Science+Business Media New York 2014

Abstract Children with younger brothers or sisters are Introduction


exposed to parenting directed towards themselves as well
as parenting directed towards their siblings. We examined Differences in the socio-emotional development of two
the hypothesis that mothers’ and fathers’ sensitive parent- siblings within a family have been explained by non-shared
ing towards their second-borns predicts compliance and family environmental factors such as birth order, peer
sharing behavior in their firstborns, over and above their relations, and differential parenting (Knafo et al. 2011;
parenting towards their firstborns. In a sample of 388 Ragan et al. 2012). When children have younger brothers
families with a toddler and infant, parental sensitivity, child or sisters, they are not only exposed to parenting directed
sharing behavior, and child compliance were observed towards themselves, but also to parenting directed towards
during two different home visits, one with father and one their siblings. Parenting directed towards a sibling can
with mother present. The results showed that toddlers influence child behavior through several processes, for
shared more with their younger siblings and showed more example by influencing the general atmosphere at home
compliance when their fathers were more sensitive towards (Feinberg and Hetherington 2001), or trough rivalry over
them, but only if fathers showed low sensitivity towards the the love and attention of a parent (Volling et al. 2010). To
younger siblings. We suggest two explanations: toddlers understand toddler behavior in relation to mothers’ and
may show more positive behavior to ensure continuation of fathers’ parenting it may be helpful to take into account
their favored position, or they may compensate for the lack mothers’ and fathers’ parenting towards the infant sibling.
of fathers’ sensitivity towards the younger siblings. Our Sharing and compliant behaviors emerge around
study highlights the importance of the broader family 24 months of age and are two empirically related important
context of parenting for child socio-emotional indicators of social-behavioral development that have been
development. found to be related to each other (Knafo and Plomin 2006;
Kochanska and Aksan 2006; Vreeke and Van der Mark
Keywords Siblings  Fathers  Sensitivity  Prosocial 2003). In infancy and toddlerhood, children need prompts
development  Compliance from their parents to be able to follow social rules (situa-
tional compliance), but when approaching preschool age,
children start to internalize parental rules and begin to
show committed compliance (Kochanska et al. 2001). The
development of self-regulation represents an important
developmental task relevant to both committed compliance
S. R. van Berkel  M. G. Groeneveld  J. Mesman (&)  and prosocial behavior (Kochanska and Aksan 2006). Self-
J. J. Endendijk  E. T. Hallers-Haalboom  regulation allows for the internalization of rules and social
L. D. van der Pol  M. J. Bakermans-Kranenburg norms, which in turn enables compliance and willingness
Faculty of Social and Behavioral Sciences, Centre for Child and
to share. Thus, both prosocial behavior and compliance
Family Studies, Institute of Education and Child Studies, Leiden
University, PO Box 9555, 2300 RB Leiden, The Netherlands during toddlerhood are considered expressions of the
e-mail: mesmanj@fsw.leidenuniv.nl development of self-regulation. Positive parenting in the

123
J Child Fam Stud

form of sensitive discipline, warmth, and support predicts interactions between dyads within the family also influence
more prosocial behavior, including sharing, and more the behaviors of all individual family members (Minuchin
compliance in children (e.g., Feldman and Klein 2003; 1985; Volling et al. 2009). Following the premise of family-
Kiang et al. 2004; Kochanska and Aksan 2006) through system theories, child behavior could be influenced not only
processes of modeling other-oriented behavior (Hastings by sensitivity of both parents towards the child itself or by
et al. 2007; Van IJzendoorn 1997; Van IJzendoorn et al. interactions between mother’s and father’s sensitivity, but
2010), and increasing children’s willingness to cooperate also by parental sensitivity towards a sibling, or any differ-
with the parent (Ainsworth et al. 1978; Kochanska and ence between parental sensitivity towards the sibling and
Aksan 2006). towards the child itself (Reiss et al. 1995). Previous studies
Although most studies on the effects of parenting on the on parenting and siblings’ development have focused pri-
development of sharing or compliance include only mothers, marily on differential parenting, examining how a parent
there are studies indicating a similar influence of fathers’ responds to one sibling compared to the other (e.g., Blandon
parenting on children’s prosocial behavior (e.g. Sturgess and Volling 2008). Several studies have shown that differ-
et al. 2001) and compliance (e.g., Kochanska and Kim 2012). ential parenting is related to less prosocial behavior and
There is even some evidence that the influence of fathers may compliance, and more externalizing behavior in the less
be stronger than that of mothers (Blandon and Volling 2008; favored sibling (Asbury et al. 2003; Blandon and Volling
Volling and Belsky 1992). In one study, facilitative parent- 2008; Caspi et al. 2004; Mullineaux et al. 2009).
ing of fathers, but not mothers, was related to prosocial Only few studies have investigated the effect of par-
interactions between siblings (Volling and Belsky 1992). In a enting towards a child on the behavior of this child’s sib-
second study, toddlers’ compliance was related to gentle ling (Feinberg et al. 2001; Reiss et al. 1995). One study
guidance by fathers, while no direct effect of maternal gentle found that negative parenting towards a sibling was asso-
guidance was found (Blandon and Volling 2008). The ciated with positive behavioral outcomes in adolescents
influence of fathers on child behavior may be especially (Reiss et al. 1995). In another study, adolescents had the
important for the older sibling. It has been argued that when least externalizing problems when they received high lev-
second-born children are infants and need more care than els of positive parenting themselves, while their siblings
their older siblings, fathers’ parenting becomes especially received low levels of positive parenting. This is described
important for the firstborn children (Volling 2012). Since as the ‘‘sibling barricade’’, a process through which par-
previous findings are inconsistent and fathers may be espe- enting towards a child has an opposite effect on the
cially important for the firstborn child in the period after the behavioral outcomes of the sibling, while controlling for
birth of a second child, when mothers tend to spend more the parenting towards the sibling (Feinberg et al. 2000;
time with the infant (Volling 2012), further research is nee- Reiss et al. 1995). An explanation for this effect is that
ded on the relation between fathers’ sensitivity and toddler children perceive to be better off than their siblings irre-
behaviors in families with younger siblings. spective of the parenting they receive themselves. This
In addition to the direct effects of each parent on child sibling barricade may also play a role with younger chil-
behavior, interactions between fathers’ and mothers’ sensi- dren. Especially when older siblings experience a change in
tivity may influence child behavior (Volling et al. 2006). parenting behavior following the birth of a younger sibling,
Previous studies have shown contradictory results concern- they may be inclined to compare parenting towards them-
ing the interaction between fathers’ and mothers’ parenting selves to parenting towards their younger sibling (Volling
behavior. In a study with toddlers and their older siblings, the 2012), and may therefore be especially affected by how
relation between fathers’ gentle guidance and child com- their younger sibling is parented. However, other studies
pliance was moderated by gentle guidance of the mothers indicate that rivalry between siblings over positive par-
(Volling et al. 2006). More gentle guidance by fathers was enting can also result in positive child outcomes (Fearon
related to more compliance of their children, but only if et al. 2006; Fearon et al. 2010; Knafo 2009). It appears that
mothers were low in their use of gentle guidance. In a rep- both negative and positive behaviors can occur in response
lication study, however, the opposite effect was found, with to evaluations of how a sibling is being parented, and both
more gentle guidance of fathers being only related to more can be strategies to gain more parental attention (Belsky
child compliance when mothers were high in their use of 1997), which is consistent with an evolutionary view on the
gentle guidance (Blandon and Volling 2008). These findings competition over caregiving resources (Fearon et al. 2010).
emphasize the importance of examining within-family pro- In the current study we investigated mothers’ and fathers’
cesses and parenting behaviors of both fathers and mothers sensitivity towards toddlers and their younger siblings in
when investigating the development of child behavior. relation to the toddlers’ sharing behavior and compliance.
According to family-system theories, family members do Our study examines issues raised by family-system theories,
not only influence each other in direct interactions, but which state that interactions between all dyads within the

123
J Child Fam Stud

family influence child behavior as well as (the effects of) Method


interactions between other dyads (Minuchin 1985; Volling
et al. 2009). For example, preschoolers’ situational compli- Participants
ance has been related to more maternal gentle guidance only
when fathers displayed high levels of gentle guidance, This study is part of the longitudinal study Boys will be
whereas no relation was found between maternal guidance Boys? examining the influence of gender-differentiated
and child compliance when fathers displayed low levels of socialization on the socio-emotional development of boys
gentle guidance (Blandon and Volling 2008). In addition, and girls in the first years of life. This paper reports on data
effects of differential parenting on child behavior have been from the first wave. Families with two children in the
found to differ depending on the quality of the parenting a Western region of the Netherlands were selected from
child actually receives. Fathers’ differential gentle guidance municipality records. Families were eligible for participa-
was related to low levels of compliance of the less favored tion if the second-born child was around 12 months of age
child only when fathers’ gentle guidance directed towards and the firstborn child was between 2.5 and 3.5 years old at
this child was low, while if the less favored child received the time of recruitment. Exclusion criteria were single
still rather high levels of paternal gentle guidance this child parenthood, severe physical or intellectual handicaps of
displayed high levels of compliance (Blandon and Volling parent or child, and parents being born outside the
2008). This suggests that differential parenting may only Netherlands or not speaking the Dutch language. Between
lead to negative child behavior in the less favored child in April 2010 and May 2011 eligible families were invited by
case of low quality parenting. Therefore, we examined two mail to participate; 31 % (n = 390) of the 1,249 families
interaction effects: the interaction between paternal and agreed to participate. The participating families did not
maternal sensitivity, and the interaction between parental differ from the non-participating families on age, educa-
sensitivity towards the oldest child and parental sensitivity tional level of both parents, and degree of urbanization of
towards the youngest child, in combination with the main the place of residence (all ps [ .10). For the current paper,
effect of parenting. one family with missing data for sensitivity of the mother
Differential parenting is relevant for children’s social and one family with missing data of both visits for sharing
development given that it has been found to predict com- behavior of the firstborn child were excluded, resulting in a
pliance, externalizing behaviors, and prosocial behaviors final sample of 388 families. Two families consisted of the
(e.g., Blandon and Volling 2008; Caspi et al. 2004; Mul- biological mother of the children and a stepfather, while
lineaux et al. 2009). Most studies found that differential the other 386 families (99.5 %) consisted of two biological
parenting leads to less compliance and prosocial behavior, parents. The distribution of family constellations was as
and more externalizing behavior in the less favored sibling, follows: 107 boy–boy (28 %), 91 girl–girl (23 %), 97
although there is also some evidence that it may lead to boy–girl (25 %), and 93 girl–boy (24 %).
more prosocial behavior in the less favored sibling (Knafo At the time of the first visit toddlers were, on average
2009). Differential parenting has also been related to more 36.2 months old (SD = 3.6) and their younger siblings
social problems in both siblings, the favored as well as less were, on average, 12.0 months old (SD = .2). The mean
favored (Meunier et al. 2013). In early childhood, sharing age difference between siblings was 23.7 months
with siblings and compliance are central aspects of social (SD = 3.6). Mothers were aged between 23 and 46 years
development within the family context (Knafo and Plomin (M = 33.9, SD = 4.0) and fathers were between 26 and
2006; Kochanska and Aksan 2006), so differential sensi- 63 years of age (M = 36.8, SD = 5.1). Most participating
tivity during toddlerhood may be especially relevant for the parents were married or had a cohabitation agreement or
development of these behaviors. Previous studies have registered partnership (93 %), and the remaining 7 % lived
indeed found that sharing and compliance are affected by together without any kind of registered agreement. With
differential parenting in preschoolers and during middle regard to educational level, most of the mothers (76 %) and
childhood (i.e. Knafo 2009; Volling et al. 2006). fathers (79 %) had a high educational level (academic or
We expected that more parental sensitivity towards their higher vocational schooling).
toddlers is positively related to toddlers’ sharing behavior
and compliance. In addition, we expected, based on pre- Procedure
vious research concerning differential parenting, that the
positive relation between parental sensitivity towards their Each family was visited twice within about 2 weeks, once
toddlers and toddlers’ sharing behavior and compliance is for observation of the mother and the two children and
stronger if parents are less sensitive towards their youngest once for observation of the father and the two children. The
children, because these toddlers would be less jealous of order of father and mother visits was counterbalanced.
their younger sibling. After the two visits families received a gift of 30 Euros and

123
J Child Fam Stud

small presents for the children. Before each home visit both noncompliance could range from 0 to a maximum of 24
parents were asked to individually complete a set of events. The two observations of compliance for each child
questionnaires. During the home visits parent–child inter- within the same family (once with the mother present,
actions and sibling interactions were filmed, and children once with the father present) were coded by different
and parents completed computer tasks. All visits were coders to guarantee independence of the ratings. Interob-
conducted by pairs of trained graduate or undergraduate server reliability was good, with a mean intraclass corre-
students. Informed consent was obtained from all partici- lation (single rater, absolute agreement) for all pairs of the
pating families. Ethical approval for this research was nine coders of .97 (range .92–1.00). To prevent coder drift
provided by the Research Ethics Committee of the Institute regular meetings with coders were organized. In order to
of Education and Child Studies of Leiden University. generate a measure for compliance the inverse sum scores
of noncompliance were computed, with a score of 0 rep-
Measures resenting complete compliance (i.e., no events of non-
compliance) during the don’t touch task, and scores below
Sharing Behavior 0 representing progressively lower levels of compliance.
Because, toddlers’ compliance during the two visits were
Toddlers received a small box of raisins (a common chil- significantly correlated (r = .36, p \ .01) and there were
dren’s treat in the Netherlands) and were instructed by the no mean-level differences between visits (p = .31) com-
experimenter to share these with their younger siblings. bined mean scores for compliance were computed. For
The sharing task was administered during both the father two families observations of one visit were missing, for
and mother visits. Parents were present during the task and these families the data of the other visit was used as the
were free to interfere if they considered this necessary. The best estimate of child compliance.
task was filmed and the number of treats shared with the
younger sibling was counted. Treats shared with or by the Parental Sensitivity
parent were not counted; when a toddler took treats back
from the younger sibling these were subtracted from the Each dyad (one parent, one child) received a bag with toys
total number of shared treats. Parents within the same and was invited to play with the toys for 8 min. The Sen-
family were coded by different coders to guarantee inde- sitivity scale from the fourth edition of the Emotional
pendency among ratings. Interobserver reliability between Availability Scales (EAS; Biringen 2008) was used to
all pairs of seven independent coders was good with a measure parental sensitivity during free play. The scale is
mean intraclass correlation (single rater, absolute agree- divided into seven subscales; (1) affect, (2) clarity of per-
ment) of .95 (range .86–1.00). Sharing behavior was sig- ceptions and appropriate responsiveness, (3) awareness of
nificantly correlated between visits (r = .30, p \ .01) and timing, (4) flexibility, variety, and creativity in modes of
showed no mean-level differences between visits play or interaction with parent, (5) acceptance in speech,
(p = .71). We therefore computed a combined mean score (6) amount of interaction, and (7) conflict situations. The
for toddlers’ sharing behavior. The number of treats shared first two subscales are coded on a seven-point Likert scale
ranged from not sharing any treats to giving all the treats to and the other subscales are coded on a three-point Likert
the younger sibling (n = 0–30). scale with a potential score range of 7–29. The sixth author,
who is an experienced coder of parent–child interactions,
Compliance completed the online training provided by Zeneyp Biringen
and then trained a team of coders. During the team training
Toddlers’ compliance was measured in a 4-min disci- some alterations were made to improve intercoder agree-
plinary don’t context (Kochanska et al. 2001). The parent ment, for more information about these alterations see
was asked to put a set of attractive toys on the floor in Hallers-Haalboom et al. (2014). Dyads within the same
front of both children, but to make sure the children did family were coded by different coders (i.e., four coders per
not play with or touch the toys. After 2 min, both siblings family) to guarantee independency among ratings. Inter-
were allowed to play only with an unattractive stuffed coder reliability for the total sensitivity score for all pairs
animal for another 2 min. Noncompliance was coded with of the seven coders was adequate, with a mean intraclass
an event-based coding system. An event was coded when correlation coefficient (absolute agreement) of .81 (range
the child reached towards or touched the prohibited .73–.92). During the coding process, the first 100 video-
toys after the parent explained that the child was not tapes were coded twice independently by separate coders
allowed to touch them. If a child reached or touched the and regular meetings were organized to prevent coder drift.
toys more than once within 10 s this was coded as one Sensitivity, compliance, and sharing behavior were coded
event of noncompliance. Within the 4 min of the task, by different coders.

123
J Child Fam Stud

Table 1 Summary of means, standard deviations and correlations for toddlers’ sharing behavior, compliance, and age, and parental sensitivity
towards both siblings
1 2 3 4 5 6 M SD

1. Toddler’s sharing behavior 9.4 3.9


2. Toddler’s compliance .09 -6.4 4.2
3. Toddler’s age .07 .13* 3.0 .3
4. Fathers’ sensitivity to toddler .12* .03 .01 24.1 2.9
5. Mothers’ sensitivity to toddler -.01 -.03 .03 .19** 24.9 2.8
6. Fathers’ sensitivity to sibling .07 -.06 -.06 .37** .16** 22.6 3.6
7. Mothers’ sensitivity to sibling .06 -.01 .11* .06 .31** .25** 24.0 3.1
Note: the negative mean of compliance indicates that on average children showed 6.5 events of non-compliance during the don’t touch task
* p \ .05, ** p \ .01

Data Analysis in Table 1. No significant correlation between toddler


compliance and sharing behavior was found. There was a
All measures were inspected for possible outliers that were positive correlation between paternal sensitivity and tod-
defined as values more than 3.29 SD below or above the mean dlers’ sharing behavior indicating that more sensitive
(Tabachnick and Fidell 2012). Outliers were winsorized to fathers had toddlers who shared more. For both sharing
make them no more extreme than the most extreme value that behavior and compliance no associations were found with
fell within the accepted range conform a normal distribution maternal sensitivity towards either child, or with parental
(Tabachnick and Fidell 2012). Sharing behavior was posi- sensitivity towards the younger sibling. Sensitivity was
tively skewed, and a square root transformation was used for positively correlated between parents and between siblings,
analyses (Tabachnick and Fidell 2012). All other measures and mothers were more sensitive towards the younger
were normally distributed. We chose not to use difference siblings when their older siblings were older. Mothers’
scores (subtracting sensitivity towards the toddler from sensitivity towards the younger siblings ranged from 14 to
sensitivity towards the sibling), because of systematic dif- 29 (M = 24.0, SD = 3.1) and fathers’ sensitivity ranged
ferences in parental sensitivity towards their toddlers and from 11 to 29 (M = 22.6, SD = 3.6). Sensitivity towards the
their 1-year-olds (see below). Most parents were more sen- toddlers ranged from 16 to 29 for both parents (mothers:
sitive towards their toddlers than towards their 1-year-olds M = 24.9, SD = 2.8; fathers: M = 24.1, SD = 2.9). Sen-
(mothers 68 %; fathers 70 %), which is likely to be due to sitivity was higher towards the toddlers than towards the
age-related differences in the children’s developmental youngest siblings for both fathers, t(387) = 8.00, p \ .01,
stages (Hallers-Haalboom et al. 2014). To assess main and d = .81, and mothers, t(387) = 5.35, p \ .01, d = .54.
interaction effects of parental sensitivity towards both sib- Compared to mothers, fathers were less sensitive towards
lings on toddlers’ sharing behavior and compliance, hierar- their toddlers, t(387) = 4.58, p \ .01, d = .41, and the
chical regression analyses were conducted. In the first step younger siblings: t(387) = 6.73, p \ .01, d = .61.
age of the toddler, gender of both children, and parental For sharing behavior, the hierarchical regression ana-
sensitivity towards both children were entered. In the second lysis showed a main effect for paternal sensitivity to the
step two-way interactions between parental sensitivity toddler (Table 2). Toddlers shared more with their siblings
towards the toddler and towards the sibling and between when their fathers were more sensitive towards them.
paternal and maternal sensitivity were entered. Finally non- Moreover, the interaction between paternal sensitivity
significant interactions were deleted from the model. Sibling towards the toddler and paternal sensitivity towards the
gender composition was also examined as a potential mod- younger sibling was significant. Simple slopes analyses
erator, but it did not affect the relations reported in this paper, (Aiken and West 1991) revealed a significant relation
and was deleted from the model. Variables were centered between paternal sensitivity and sharing only for toddlers
before the computation of interaction terms. whose fathers showed relatively low sensitivity towards the
younger siblings, while no significant relation was found
for toddlers whose fathers showed high sensitivity towards
Results the younger siblings (Fig. 1). Other predictors were not
significant, including child characteristics, maternal sensi-
The means, standard deviations and bivariate correlations tivity towards the toddler, the main effects of sensitivity of
of the predictors and the outcome measures are presented both parents towards the younger sibling, and both the

123
J Child Fam Stud

Table 2 Parental sensitivity Sharing Compliance


towards both siblings in relation
2
to toddler’s sharing and B SEB b R B SEB b R2
compliance
Step 1 .04 .06
Toddler’s age .14 .11 .07 .17 .06 .14**
Toddler’s gender .12 .06 .10 1.57 .42 .19**
Sibling’s gender .08 .06 .07 -.03 .43 -.00
Fathers’ sensitivity to toddler .03 .01 .12* .07 .08 .05
Mothers’ sensitivity to toddler -.01 .01 -.05 -.03 .08 -.03
Note: beta’s derived from the Fathers’ sensitivity to sibling .00 .01 .01 -.13 .07 -.11
final model. T refers to toddler Mothers’ sensitivity to sibling .01 .01 .05 -.01 .07 -.01
and S refers to the younger Step 2 .04 .07
sibling
Fathers’ sensitivity T * fathers’ sensitivity S -.01 .00 -.10* -.05 .02 -.14**
* p \ .05, ** p \ .01

4.5 2
.18, p < .05
.01, p = .95 0
Fathers' Fathers'

Toddlers' compliance
4 sensitivity sensitivity
-2
Toddlers' sharing

towards towards
younger younger
sibling -4 sibling
3.5
- SD - SD
-6
.19, p < .01 + SD + SD

-8
3 -.07, p = .36

-10

2.5 -12
16 18 20 22 24 26 28 16 18 20 22 24 26 28

Fathers' sensitivity towards toddler Fathers' sensitivity towards toddler

Fig. 1 The association between fathers’ sensitivity towards the Fig. 2 The association between fathers’ sensitivity towards the
toddler and toddler sharing behavior by fathers’ sensitivity towards toddler and toddler compliance by fathers’ sensitivity towards the
the younger sibling younger sibling

interactions between maternal and paternal sensitivity for toddlers whose fathers showed high sensitivity towards
towards the toddler, and maternal sensitivity towards the the younger siblings (Fig. 2). This implies that toddlers
toddler and maternal sensitivity towards the younger showed more compliance when their fathers were more
sibling. sensitive towards them, but only if fathers showed lower
For compliance, the multiple regression analysis levels of sensitivity towards their younger siblings.
revealed significant main effects for age and gender
(Table 2), with older toddlers and girls showing more
compliance. Neither the main effects of parental sensitivity Discussion
nor the interaction between paternal and maternal sensi-
tivity were significant. Again, the interaction between Our results showed that toddlers share more with their
paternal sensitivity towards the toddler and paternal sen- younger siblings and exhibit more compliance when their
sitivity towards the younger sibling contributed signifi- fathers were more sensitive towards them, but only if
cantly to the prediction of compliance. To further examine fathers showed low sensitivity towards the younger sibling.
the significant interaction effect, simple slopes analyses The moderation effect, indicating that toddlers who
were conducted (Aiken and West 1991). Similar to the exhibited most sharing behavior and compliance had
results for sharing, we found a significant relation between fathers who showed high levels of sensitivity towards the
paternal sensitivity and compliance only for the toddlers toddlers themselves and low levels of sensitivity toward the
whose fathers showed relatively low sensitivity towards the younger siblings, confirms the assumption of family-
younger siblings, whereas no significant relation was found systems theories that interactions between dyads within the

123
J Child Fam Stud

family influence other dyadic interactions within the family between paternal sensitivity and prosocial sibling interac-
which in turn influence child outcomes (Minuchin 1985; tions, whereas no relation for maternal sensitivity was
Volling et al. 2009). In addition, it supports the idea of found (Volling and Belsky 1992). The differences between
social comparison between siblings (Feinberg et al. 2000; the effects of paternal and maternal sensitivity on child
Volling et al. 2010). Three-month-old infants already dif- behavior may be related to the differences in how fathers
ferentiate between prosocial and antisocial actions of oth- and mother interact with their children, especially during
ers and are able to evaluate and compare behaviors of free play. Fathers initiate more physical and rough-and-
others (Hamlin and Wynn 2011). By the age of 6 months tumble play than mothers do (Volling et al. 2002). In rats,
infants feel jealous when their parents direct their attention such rough-and-tumble play has been related to the
towards another child (Hart 2010). By (unconsciously) development of social and emotional competences (Pellis
noting the level of sensitivity the younger sibling receives and Pellis 2007). It has been proposed that a similar rela-
in comparison with the level of sensitivity the toddler tion between rough-and-tumble play and more social skills
receives, the toddler might feel favored by the parent. and emotional understanding could exists in humans,
Previous studies have indicated that toddlers’ behavior is because during this more physical play it is necessary to
sensitive to parenting towards younger siblings and that monitor emotional expressions of a playmate in order to
differentiated parenting is related to more positive behavior assure that the game is still enjoyed by the other (Pellegrini
in the favored sibling (Blandon and Volling 2008; Fearon and Smith 1998).
et al. 2010; Knafo 2009). Compliance was not related to sharing, which may be
Social comparison influences social development due to the different interaction partners in the two settings.
through sibling rivalry and fear over losing parental Compliance was observed during parent–child interaction,
attention (Boyle et al. 2004). As a consequence the favored while sharing was observed during a child-sibling inter-
toddler may exhibit more preferred behavior, i.e. more action. In addition, this result shows that sharing in the
prosocial behavior and more compliance, to ensure con- presence of a parent is not necessarily influenced by
tinuation of its favored position (Fearon et al. 2006). compliance to parental rules.
Another explanation could be that in the case of sharing This study has some limitations. The sample consisted
behavior, toddlers may compensate for the lack of fathers’ predominantly of highly educated parents, which may
sensitivity towards the younger sibling by exhibiting more influence the generalizability of the results. Although, the
prosocial behavior towards their younger siblings. Eight- high educational level of our sample is comparable to
month-old infants are sensitive to the level of fairness of educational levels of samples of other studies including
actions towards others, and from the age of 19 months both parents, often from convenience samples (Blandon
toddlers are able to share altruistically and adapt their and Volling 2008; Verhoeven et al. 2010), it is important
prosocial acts to how the other behaved or was treated for future research to include lower-educated parents.
before (Hamlin et al. 2011; Schmidt and Sommerville Another limitation lies in the observational measure of
2011). Toddlers shared more with a person who was vic- sensitivity. Sensitivity was observed during a free-play
timized before than with a person who was helped before situation with pre-selected toys. Although free-play is
the sharing task (Hamlin et al. 2011). The experience of frequently used for observation of parental sensitivity (e.g.
being favored over a younger sibling could be experienced Kiang et al. 2004), it could be that this is not a naturalistic
as unfair by the toddler, which may lead to more prosocial situation for all parents. This could also explain why
behavior towards the younger sibling. However, since our fathers were found to be less sensitive than mothers, since
data is correlational, it could be that toddlers’ sharing this kind of play may be closer to daily-life experiences for
behavior and compliance increased their fathers’ sensitivity mother–child interactions than for father-child interactions
towards them (Carlo et al. 2010; Combs-Ronto et al. 2009). (Volling et al. 2002). Furthermore, the use of a play situ-
The finding that mothers were more sensitive than ation limits the number of observations of parental sensi-
fathers is in line with findings of previous studies (Lovas tivity to a child’s distress, while parental responses to a
2005; Volling et al. 2002). Mothers are often the primary child’s distress are seen as a central concept of sensitive
caregiver and therefore they are more familiar with the parenting (Bowlby 1982; Mesman et al. 2012; Out et al.
signals of the child which may make them more sensitive 2010). Indeed, some studies only found a relation between
than fathers, see Hallers-Haalboom et al. (2014). In con- parental sensitivity towards child distress and child out-
trast to paternal sensitivity, maternal sensitivity was not comes, while no relation was found between parental
related to toddlers’ behavior. This difference between sensitivity to child nondistress and behavioral outcomes
fathers and mothers is consistent with findings from pre- (Leerkes et al. 2009). This could explain why no direct
vious studies (Volling and Belsky 1992; Volling et al. effect of sensitivity on sharing or compliance was found.
2006). A study with preschoolers showed a relation Therefore, future studies could focus more on the relation

123
J Child Fam Stud

between parental sensitivity to distress and child behavior. Asbury, K., Dunn, J. F., Pike, A., & Plomin, R. (2003). Nonshared
Finally, we only used the number of shared treats as a environmental influences on individual differences in early
behavioral development: A monozygotic twin differences study.
measure for sharing, and we did not observe any other Child Development, 74, 933–943. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.
aspect of the child’s sharing behavior or the behavior of the 00577.
younger sibling or the parent during the sharing task. Belsky, J. (1997). Variation in susceptibility to environmental
Although the number of shared treats measure seems to be influence: An evolutionary argument. An International Journal
for the Advancement of Psychological Theory, 8, 182–186.
useful in revealing relations between parenting and sharing doi:10.1207/s15327965pli0803_3.
behavior, observing the actual behavior of each family Biringen, Z. (2008). The Emotional Availability (EA) Scales, 4th
member present during a sharing episode could provide edition. Infancy/Early Childhood Version (child age: 0–5 years).
more information on processes between siblings and Retrieved from www.emotionalavailability.com.
Blandon, A. Y., & Volling, B. L. (2008). Parental gentle guidance and
parental interference. children’s compliance within the family: A replication study.
In conclusion, compliance and sharing behavior in tod- Journal of Family Psychology, 22, 355–366. doi:10.1037/0893-
dlers are related to the parenting they receive in combina- 3200.22.3.355.
tion with the parenting directed towards their younger Bowlby, J. (1982). Attachment and loss: Vol. 1. Attachment. New
York, NY: Basic Books.
siblings, which they observe. Fathers’ sensitivity was pos- Boyle, M. H., Jenkins, J. M., Georgiades, K., Cairney, J., Duku, E., &
itively related to toddlers’ sharing behavior and compliance Racine, Y. A. (2004). Differential-maternal parenting behavior:
when fathers were not so sensitive towards the younger Estimating within and between family effects. Child Develop-
siblings. This could be explained by toddlers showing ment, 75, 1457–1476. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00751.x.
Carlo, G., Mestre, M. V., Samper, P., Tur, A., & Armenta, B. E.
positive behavior to ensure continuation of their favored (2010). The longitudinal relations among dimensions of parent-
position or, in the case of sharing, they may compensate for ing styles, sympathy, prosocial moral reasoning, and prosocial
the lack of fathers’ sensitivity towards the younger siblings. behaviors. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 35,
Our study contributes to the growing body of research 116–124. doi:10.1177/0165025410375921.
Caspi, A., Moffitt, T. E., Morgan, J., Rutter, M., Taylor, A., Kim-
indicating that not only one-to-one parenting shapes child Cohen, J., et al. (2004). Maternal expressed emotion predicts
socio-emotional development, but also observed parenting children’s antisocial behavior problems: Using monozygotic-
towards siblings. This is one of the first studies that exam- twin differences to identify environmental effects on behavioral
ined interactions between parenting towards two children, development. Developmental Psychology, 40, 149–161. doi:10.
1037/0012-1649.40.2.149.
instead of using the difference in parenting towards two Combs-Ronto, L. A., Olson, S. L., Lunkenheimer, E. S., & Sameroff,
children, as a predictor of child behavior. This enabled us to A. J. (2009). Interactions between maternal parenting and
document the interplay between dyadic interactions within children’s early disruptive behavior: Bidirectional associations
the family, revealing that parenting towards a child and its across the transition from preschool to school entry. Journal of
Abnormal Child Psychology, 37, 1151–1163. doi:10.1007/
sibling are related to child behavior. In addition, while most s10802-009-9332-2.
studies have focused on preschoolers or school-aged chil- Fearon, R. M., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & Van IJzendoorn, M. H.
dren, we focused on toddlers to investigate the influence of (2010). Jealousy and attachment. The case of twins. In S. L. Hart &
parenting towards two children during a developmental M. Legerstee (Eds.), Handbook of jealousy: Theory, research, and
multidisciplinary approaches (pp. 362–386). New York: Wiley.
period in which compliance and prosocial behavior first Fearon, R. M., Van IJzendoorn, M. H., Fonagy, P., Bakermans-
emerge. Our results are in line with family-systems theories Kranenburg, M. J., Schuengel, C., & Bokhorst, C. L. (2006). In
(Minuchin 1985; Volling et al. 2009) and confirm that the search of shared and nonshared environmental factors in security
interplay between different dyadic interactions within a of attachment: A behavior-genetic study of the association
between sensitivity and attachment security. Developmental
family are related to child outcomes. Doing so, our findings Psychology, 42, 1026–1040. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.42.6.1026.
highlight the significance of the broader family context for Feinberg, M., & Hetherington, E. M. (2001). Differential parenting as
child development. a within-family variable. Journal of Family Psychology, 15,
22–37. doi:10.1037//0893-3200.15.1.22.
Acknowledgments This research was supported by a European Feinberg, M., Neiderhiser, J., Howe, G., & Hetherington, E. M.
Research Council Starting Grant awarded to Judi Mesman (Project # (2001). Adolescent, parent, and observer perceptions of parent-
240885). Marian J. Bakermans-Kranenburg was supported by the ing: Genetic and environmental influences on shared and distinct
Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research (VICI Grant perceptions. Child Development, 72, 1266–1284. doi:10.1111/
453-09-003). 1467-8624.00346.
Feinberg, M. E., Neiderhiser, J. M., Simmens, S., Reiss, D., &
Hetherington, E. M. (2000). Sibling comparison of differential
parental treatment in adolescence: Gender, self-esteem, and emo-
References tionality as mediators of the parenting-adjustment association. Child
Development, 71, 1611–1628. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00252.
Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and Feldman, R., & Klein, P. S. (2003). Toddlers’ self-regulated
interpreting interactions. Newbury Park, CA: Sage. compliance to mothers, caregivers, and fathers: Implications
Ainsworth, M., Blehar, M. C., Waters, E., & Wall, S. (1978). Patterns for theories of socialization. Developmental Psychology, 39,
of attachment. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. 680–692. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.39.4.680.

123
J Child Fam Stud

Hallers-Haalboom, E. T., Mesman, J., Groeneveld, M. G., Endendijk, Mullineaux, P. Y., Deater-Deckard, K., Petrill, S. A., & Thompson,
J. J., Van Berkel, S. R., Van der Pol, L. D., et al. (2014). A. L. (2009). Parenting and child behaviour problems: A
Mothers, fathers, sons and daughters: Parental sensitivity in longitudinal analysis of non-shared environment. Infant and
families with two children. Journal of Family Psychology, 28, Child Development, 18, 133–148. doi:10.1002/ICD.593.
138–147. doi:10.1037/a0036004. Out, D., Pieper, S., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., Zeskind, P. S., &
Hamlin, J. K., & Wynn, K. (2011). Young infants prefer prosocial to Van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2010). Intended sensitive and harsh
antisocial others. Cognitive Development, 26, 30–39. doi:10. caregiving responses to infant crying: The role of cry pitch and
1016/j.cogdev.2010.09.001. perceived urgency in an adult twin sample. Child Abuse and
Hamlin, J. K., Wynn, K., Bloom, P., & Mahajan, N. (2011). How Neglect, 34, 863–873. doi:10.1016/j.chiabu.2010.05.003.
infants and toddlers react to antisocial others. PNAS, 108, Pellegrini, A. D., & Smith, P. K. (1998). Physical activity play: the nature
19931–19936. doi:10.1073/pnas.1110306108. and function of a neglected aspect of play. Child Development, 69,
Hart, S. L. (2010). The ontogenesis of jealousy in the first year of life: 577–598. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.1998.tb06226.x.
A theory of jealousy as a biologically-based dimension of Pellis, S. M., & Pellis, V. C. (2007). Rough-and-tumble play and the
temperament. In M. Legerstee & S. Hart (Eds.), Handbook of development of the social brain. Current Directions in Psycho-
jealousy. Theory, research, and multidisciplinary approaches logical Science, 16, 95–98. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.
(pp. 387–417). New York: Wiley. 00483.x.
Hastings, P. D., Utendale, W. T., & Sullivan, C. (2007). The Ragan, C. M., Loken, E., Stifter, C. A., & Cavigelli, S. A. (2012).
socialization of prosocial development. In J. E. Grusec & P. Within-litter variance in early rat pup–mother interactions and
D. Hastings (Eds.), Handbook of socialization; Theory and adult offspring responses to novelty. Development Psychobiol-
research (pp. 638–664). New York: Guilford Publications. ogy, 54, 199–206. doi:10.1002/dev.20581.
Kiang, L., Moreno, A. J., & Robinson, J. L. (2004). Maternal Reiss, D., Hetherington, E. M., Plomin, R., Howe, G. W., Simmens,
preconceptions about parenting predict child temperament, S. J., Henderson, S. H., et al. (1995). Genetic questions for
maternal sensitivity, and children’s empathy. Developmental environmental studies. Differential parenting and psychopathol-
Psychology, 40, 1081–1092. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.40.6.1081. ogy in adolescence. Archives of General Psychiatry, 52,
Knafo, A. (2009). Prosocial development: The intertwined roles of 925–936. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.1995.03950230039007.
children’s genetics and their parental environment. Invited Schmidt, M. F. H., & Sommerville, J. A. (2011). Fairness expecta-
address. Denver, CO: The Society for Research in Child tions and altruistic sharing in 15-month-old human infants. PLoS
Development Biennial Meeting. One, 6, 1–7. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0023223.
Knafo, A., Israel, S., & Ebstein, R. P. (2011). Heritability of Sturgess, W., Dunn, J., & Davies, L. (2001). Young children’s
children’s prosocial behavior and differential susceptibility to perceptions of their relationships with family members: Links
parenting by variation in the dopamine receptor D4 gene. with family setting, friendships, and adjustment. International
Development and Psychopathology, 23, 53–67. doi:10.1017/ Journal of Behavioral Development, 25, 521–529. doi:10.1080/
S0954579410000647. 01650250042000500.
Knafo, A., & Plomin, R. (2006). Parental discipline and affection and Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2012). Using multivariate
children’s prosocial behavior: Genetic and environmental links. statistics (6th ed.). New York: Harper Collins.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 90, 147–164. Van IJzendoorn, M. H. (1997). Attachment, emergent morality and
doi:10.1037/0022-3514.90.1.147. aggression: Toward a developmental socioemotional model of
Kochanska, G., & Aksan, N. (2006). Children’s conscience and self- antisocial behaviour. International Journal of Behavioral Devel-
regulation. Journal of Personality, 74, 1587–1617. doi:10.1111/ opment, 21, 703–727. doi:10.1080/016502597384631.
j.1467-6494.2006.00421.x. Van IJzendoorn, M. H., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., Pannebakker,
Kochanska, G., Coy, K. C., & Murray, K. T. (2001). The development F., & Out, D. (2010). In defence of situational morality: Genetic,
of self-regulation in the first four years of life. Child Develop- dispositional and situational determinants of children’s donating
ment, 72, 1091–1111. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00336. to charity. Journal of Moral Education, 39, 1–20. doi:10.1080/
Kochanska, G., & Kim, S. (2012). Toward a new understanding of legacy 03057240903528535.
of early attachments for future antisocial trajectories: Evidence from Verhoeven, M., Junger, M., Van Aken, C., Dekovic, M., & Van Aken,
two longitudinal studies. Development and Psychopathology, 24, M. A. G. (2010). Parenting and children’s externalizing behav-
783–806. doi:10.1017/S0954579412000375. ior: Bidirectionality during toddlerhood. Journal of Applied
Leerkes, E. M., Blankson, A. N., & O’Brien, M. (2009). Differential Developmental Psychology, 31, 93–105. doi:10.1016/j.appdev.
effects of maternal sensitivity to infant distress and nondistress 2009.09.002.
on social-emotional functioning. Child Development, 80, Volling, B. L. (2012). Family transitions following the birth of a
762–775. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01296.x. sibling: An empirical review of changes in the firstborn’s
Lovas, G. S. (2005). Gender and patterns of emotional availability in adjustment. Psychological Bulletin, 138, 497–528. doi:10.1037/
mother-toddler and father-toddler dyads. Infant Mental Health a0026921.
Journal, 26, 327–353. doi:10.1002/imhj.20056. Volling, B. L., & Belsky, J. (1992). The contribution of mother–child
Mesman, J., Oster, H., & Camras, L. (2012). Parental sensitivity to and father–child relationships to the quality of sibling interac-
infant distress: What do discrete negative emotions have to do tion: A longitudinal study. Child Development, 63, 1209–1222.
with it? Attachment & Human Development, 14, 337–348. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.1992.tb01690.x.
doi:10.1080/14616734.2012.691649. Volling, B. L., Blandon, A. Y., & Gorvine, B. J. (2006). Maternal and
Meunier, J. C., Boyle, M., O’Connor, T. G., & Jenkins, J. M. (2013). paternal gentle guidance and young children’s compliance from
Multilevel mediation: Cumulative contextual risk, maternal a within-family perspective. Journal of Family Psychology, 20,
differential treatment, and children’s behavior within families. 514–525. doi:10.1037/0893-3200.20.3.514.
Child Development, 84, 1594–1615. doi:10.1111/cdev.12066. Volling, B. L., Kennedy, D. E., & Jackey, L. M. H. (2010). The
Minuchin, P. (1985). Families and individual development: Provoca- development of sibling jealousy. In M. Legerstee & S. Hart
tions from the field of family therapy. Child Development, 56, (Eds.), Handbook of jealousy. Theory, research, and multidis-
289–302. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.ep7251588. ciplinary approaches (pp. 387–417). New York: Wiley.

123
J Child Fam Stud

Volling, B. L., Kolak, A. M., & Blandon, A. Y. (2009). Family competence: Predictors of parent–infant attachment and emerg-
subsystems and children’s self-regulation. In S. L. Olson & A. ing self-regulation. Journal of Family Psychology, 16, 447–465.
Sameroff (Eds.), Regulatory processes in the development of doi:10.1037//0893-3200.16.4.447.
childhood behavior problems: Biological, behavioral, and Vreeke, G. J., & Van der Mark, I. L. (2003). Empathy, an integrative
social-ecological perspectives (pp. 238–257). New York: Cam- model. New Ideas in Psychology, 21, 177–207. doi:10.1016/j.
bridge University Press. newideapsych.2003.09.003.
Volling, B. L., McElwain, N. L., Notaro, P. C., & Herrera, C. (2002).
Parents’ emotional availability and infant emotional

123

You might also like