You are on page 1of 29

Review of General Psychology Copyright 1997 by the Educational Publishing Foundation

1997, Vol. L No. 2, 198-226 1089-268O/97/S3.0O

Early Development of Executive Function:


A Problem-Solving Framework
Philip David Zelazo Alice Carter and J. Steven Reznick
University of Toronto Yale University

Douglas Frye
University of Pennsylvania
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Executive function (EF) accounts have now been offered for several disorders with
childhood onset (e.g., attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder, autism, early-treated
phenylketonuria), and EF has been linked to the development of numerous abilities
(e.g., attention, rule use, theory of mind). However, efforts to explain behavior in terms
of EF have been hampered by an inadequate characterization of EF itself. What is the
function that is accomplished by EF? The present analysis attempts to ground the
construct of EF in an account of problem solving and thereby to integrate temporally
and functionally distinct aspects of EF within a coherent framework. According to this
problem-solving framework, EF is a macroconstruct that spans 4 phases of problem
solving (representation, planning, execution, and evaluation). When analyzed into
subfunctions, macroconstructs such as EF permit the integration of findings from
disparate content domains, which are often studied in isolation from the broader context
of reasoning and action. A review of the literature on the early development of EF
reveals converging evidence for domain-general changes in all aspects of EF.

Within the relatively new domain of develop- Pennington & OzonorT, 1996,fora review), and
mental neuropsychology (Spreen, Risser, & executive function has been linked to the
Edgell, 1995), there is considerable interest in development of numerous psychological pro-
the ontogeny of executive function, a construct cesses, including attention, rule use, working
that is often used to characterize a diverse set of memory, and theory of mind (e.g., Diamond &
processes impaired by damage to prefrontal Taylor, 1996; Lyon & Krasnegor, 1996). The
cortex in adults (e.g., Stuss & Benson, 1986). construct of executive function thus promises to
Executive function accounts have now been explain important aspects of typical and atypical
offered for several psychiatric conditions with development in terms that point toward neuro-
childhood onset (e.g., autism, Gilles de la logical localization. Unfortunately, however,
Tourette Syndrome, attention-deficit/hyperactiv- efforts to explain behavior via executive func-
ity disorder, obsessive-compulsive disorder; see tion have been hampered by an inadequate
characterization of executive function itself.
Philip David Zelazo, Department of Psychology, Univer-
Prefrontal cortical damage produces impair-
sity of Toronto, Toronto, Ontario, Canada; Alice Carter and ments on a wide variety of tasks, including the
J. Steven Reznick, Department of Psychology, Yale Wisconsin Card Sort Task (the WCST; e.g.,
University; Douglas Frye, Graduate School of Education, Milner, 1964), the Stroop Color-Word Task
University of Pennsylvania.
We would like to thank Janet Boseovski, Josephine
(Perret, 1974; Stroop, 1935), tower tasks (e.g.,
Rieser, Sophie Jacques, Natasha Kirkham, Stephanie the Tower of London; Shallice, 1982), numerous
Lowell, and Margo Scharer for their assistance with the memory tasks (e.g., tasks assessing source
preparation of this article. S. Jacques and J. Fueser also memory and memory for temporal order;
provided extremely constructive comments on an earlier
version. Milner, Petrides, & Smith, 1985; Shimamura,
Correspondence concerning this article should be ad- Janowsky, & Squire, 1991), and the Porteus
dressed to Philip David Zelazo, Department of Psychology, Maze (Porteus, 1950), among many others (see
University of Toronto, 100 Saint George Street, Toronto, Stuss, Eskes, & Foster, 1994, for a review of
Ontario, Canada M5S 3G3. Electronic mail should he sent
via Internet to zelazo @psych.uloronto.ca. recent neuropsychological work). Often, descrip-

198
EXECUTIVE FUNCTION 199

tions of executive function consist of heteroge- Executive Function


neous lists of processes presumed to underlie
performance on these tasks (e.g., Levin et al., Definition o/Executive
1991; Shute & Huertas, 1990; Stuss & Benson,
1984). The term executive can be understood both in
the sense of one who issues commands by virtue
A number of authors have recently proposed
of rank in a hierarchy (as in chief executive
unified descriptions of prefrontal impairments
officer [CEO]) and in the sense of one who
that emphasize basic processes such as inhibi-
carries out those commands (as in the executor
tion (e.g., Barkley, 1997; Dempster, 1992,1995) of a will). Disagreement concerning how best to
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

or working memory (e.g., Kimberg & Farah, characterize executive function may depend in
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

1993), but it remains unclear whether these part on which sense of the term is taken to be
unified descriptions can account for the full primary. Some authors (e.g., Baddeley, 1986;
range of observed impairments (Stuss et al., Goldstein & Scheerer, 1941; Shallice, 1988)
1994). Moreover, it seems unlikely that basic emphasize the role of a supervisory, reflective
processes will suffice to explain the way in agent, whereas other authors have stressed the
which different aspects of executive function executor sense of the term and focused instead
interact to accomplish the sophisticated metacog- on the production of action (e.g., Brown, 1987;
nitive functions subserved by prefrontal cortex Goldberg, 1987). Stuss and Benson's (1986,
(Perecman, 1987; Stuss & Benson, 1986). What p. 204) description of executive function as
is needed is a characterization of the complex involving "anticipation, goal establishment,
processes attributed to executive function that planning, response trials, monitoring of results,
captures their diversity without simply listing and use of feedback" seems to combine these
them. senses. However, we are still left with a list of
The current article attempts to ground the partially overlapping abilities that are not
obviously arranged in a coherent fashion (cf.
construct of executive function in an account of
Stuss & Benson, 1984, p. 23).
problem solving, which is much more tractable
than executive function from a psychological
perspective (cf. Holyoak, 1990). In the follow- Definition o/Function
ing sections, a functional framework for the
study of executive function is proposed based on As Luria noted in his 1973 book, the term
the temporal phases of problem solving (i.e., the function in this context refers to a complex
steps that are required to get from recognition of activity with a constant outcome that is capable
a problem to its solution). This problem-solving of being effected in a variety of ways (see also
framework serves to organize the diverse Bernstein, 1967). The function of communica-
aspects of executive function, illustrating the tion provides an example: Suppose you have a
way in which they can be fitted into a single message that you wish to communicate to
overarching structure. In addition to providing a another person. You can do so by speaking it,
coherent characterization of executive function, using sign language, or writing it. Moreover,
the framework permits us to review an extensive you can write it with your dominant hand or
literature on the normal development of execu- with your other hand (though perhaps less well).
tive function in preschool children. We focus on In any case, although the form of the communi-
the preschool period, which spans the transition cation changes, the function of communication,
from infancy to the school-age years, because and even the message that is communicated,
this period is marked by major changes in the remain the same.
control of thought and action (Zelazo & Jacques, Two general characteristics of complex func-
1997). The review reveals a convergence of tions are worth noting. First, with complex
findings that is bound to be overlooked in the functions such as communication or locomo-
absence of a broad framework like the one tion, there are many degrees of freedom—many
advanced here. In light of the literature review, means to the same end. Second, complex
we evaluate several theories of executive functions are inherently hierarchical in nature;
function and its development. the function of communication, for example,
depends on subfunctions such as motor control.
200 ZELAZO, CARTER, REZNICK, AND FRYE

To a large extent, the task of characterizing a


complex function is a matter of describing its Problem Representation
hierarchical structure, characterizing its various
subfunctions, and organizing these subfunctions
around their constant common outcome.
Can sense be made of the diverse abilities
attributed to executive function and light thereby Planning
be shed on the control of thought and action?
One approach is to consider the common
outcome of executive function. Consistent with
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

earlier proposals (including those of Luria,


1980, and Shallice, 1988), the outcome of Execution:
executive function is taken to be the solution of
a problem. In the next section, we present a Intending/Rule Use
problem-solving framework for studying execu-
tive function that is designed to integrate
temporally and functionally distinct aspects of
executive function. As we shall see, whether or Evaluation:
not a problem is solved is relatively straightfor- Error Detection/Correction
ward to assess. However, when a problem
remains unsolved, it is often difficult to deter- Figure 1. A problem-solving framework that identifies
mine where in the process of problem-solving four temporally distinct phases of executive function.
performance breaks down.

A Problem-Solving Framework Applied to work (see Figure 1). The framework involves
Executive Function four temporally and functionally distinct steps
(plus substeps). To solve a problem, one must
Problem solving has been the subject of first construct a problem space (i.e., a representa-
considerable theoretical analysis since the start tion of the problem and possible solutions).
of psychology as a scientific enterprise. Dewey Work in the Gestalt tradition (e.g., Duncker,
(1933/1985) placed it at the center of his 1945; Kohler, 1917/1959; Maier, 1932) empha-
account of how we think, and it has remained a sized the importance of the problem representa-
central topic in cognitive psychology. Cyber- tion phase of problem solving. According to the
netic theory (Weiner, 1948), hierarchical models Gestalt approach, problem solving consists
of action control (e.g., Miller, Galanter, & primarily in restructuring one's representation
Pribram, 1960; Powers, 1973), and early infor- of a problem (i.e., insight). Once one attains the
mation-processing approaches (e.g., Newell, right problem representation (or set or Einstel-
Shaw, & Simon, 1958) have contributed the lung), the rest of the steps follow straightaway.
theoretical and methodological elements that In contrast, information-processing approaches
form the basis of most contemporary research have emphasized the second phase, planning.
on problem solving. Newell and Simon (1972) conceptualized prob-
According to Newell and Simon (1972), "A lem solving as search in a problem space for a
person is confronted with a problem when he promising plan. Planning strategies such as
wants something and does not know immedi- means-end analysis are often used to constrain
ately what series of actions he can perform to get this search, and an important aspect of planning
it" (p. 72). Many authors have attempted to is the selection of a plan from among alterna-
delineate the separate steps or phases that lead tives. Another aspect of planning is sequencing
from recognition of a problem to its solution actions in time. For Newell and Simon (1972),
(e.g., Bransford & Stein, 1993; Hayes, 1989; planning was the crux of problem solving, with
Kagan, 1966; Polya, 1945; Scholnick & Fried- a satisfactory plan yielding a problem solution.
man, 1987), and there is considerable overlap In the present framework, after one has
among these accounts. Our own analysis of selected a plan, the execution phase of problem
problem solving suggests the following frame- solving begins: In order for a plan to be
EXECUTIVE FUNCTION 201

executed, one must (a) keep the plan in mind Many of these tasks have not previously been
long enough for it to guide one's thought or considered relevant to executive function.
action and (b) actually carry out the prescribed
behavior. Keeping a plan in mind to control
behavior is referred to as intending; translating a
Executive Function as a Macroconstruct
plan into action is rule use. There is consider- The present framework differs from other
able evidence for abulic dissociations between recent approaches to executive function in that it
intending and rule use (i.e., cases where an treats executive function as a macroconstruct
intention is not translated into action; see Luria, that captures the way in which subfunctions of
1959a; Teuber, 1964; Zelazo, Frye, & Rapus,
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

executive function work together to accomplish


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

1996; Zelazo & Reznick, 1991). This prompts the higher-order function of problem solution.
us to consider intending and rule use as separate Aspects of executive function are organized
subphases of execution. around four temporally and functionally distinct
Finally, if one keeps a plan in mind and phases of problem solving, making it possible to
actually uses the plan to guide behavior, then the distinguish between inflexibility at each of the
final phase of problem solving may occur. In this four phases. Conceptualizing executive function
phase, one evaluates one's action and deter- as a macroconstruct acknowledges the inherent
mines whether a solution has been obtained. complexity of the self-regulative processes
Evaluation involves both error detection and involved in problem solving and permits us to
error correction (cf. Konow & Pribram, 1970), proceed with the difficult task of mapping out
the latter of which entails revision of one of the the interactions among subfunctions.
previous phases of problem solving. In contrast, recent approaches to executive
This framework allows us to capture, in an function have tended to emphasize particular
organized fashion, what we take to be the main subfunctions. For example, some authors have
aspects of executive function. Different aspects suggested that executive function necessarily
correspond to one or the other (or both) of the involves resistance to interference from auto-
senses of the term executive mentioned above matic (or prepotent) responses (e.g., Barkley,
(e.g., planning corresponds primarily to the 1996, p. 313) and that most or all executive-
CEO sense of the term). Normally, errors in function impairments derive from primary
executive function tasks (e.g., the WCST) could impairments in inhibition (Barkley, 1997; Demp-
be attributed to any number of processes, ster, 1993). Failures to inhibit responding (i.e.,
including an inflexible representation of the instances of perseveration) are certainly indica-
problem space (akin to functional fixedness; tive of executive function difficulties. However,
Duncker, 1945), the persistent use of a particular from the perspective of the problem-solving
plan (cf. Luchins, 1942), failures of rule use framework, basic processes such as inhibition
(Zelazo et al., 1996), or inability to benefit from are too simple by themselves to provide an
error information (Konow & Pribram, 1970). A adequate characterization of the complex strate-
major advantage of the problem-solving frame- gic and metacognitive processes involved in
work is that it allows researchers to locate lapses executive function.
of executive function fairly precisely in a As an example, consider the difficulty that
temporal sequence of problem-solving phases. 3-year-old children have making causal predic-
Several authors have developed tasks that are tions about unusual events. Frye, Zelazo,
amenable to this type of problem-solving Brooks, and Samuels (1996) asked children to
analysis, and these tasks are likely to prove predict the path of a marble in a ramp. In one
useful in understanding executive function in condition, the ramp had two input holes at the
disorders in which it is implicated (Delis, top and two output holes at the bottom, and
Squire, Bihrle, & Massman, 1992; Levine, marbles either rolled straight down the ramp or
Stuss, & Milberg, 1995). However, there are they rolled across it, depending on the ramp's
also a number of more traditional tasks from the configuration. Children in this condition dis-
standard cognitive and developmental litera- played a strong, perseverative tendency to say
tures that are focused on particular phases of that the marble would roll straight down
problem solving and are therefore interpretable regardless of the configuration of the ramp. One
in terms of the problem-solving framework. might plausibly propose that this task taps
202 ZELAZO, CARTER, REZNICK, AND FRYE

executive function and that children were unable that do not involve interference, and it often
to inhibit a well-established expectation for operates alongside of automatic processes in
marbles to roll straight down inclined planes. familiar situations in which habitual strategies
However, children's performance in another suffice.
condition reveals the inadequacy of this ac- One way to derive an accurate assessment of
count. When only a single input hole is used, executive function would be to create a task that
and the ramp is arranged so that the marble will is a "pure" executive function task and is
roll across, 3-year-olds accurately predict the sensitive only to variations in executive func-
behavior of the marble. Presumably, problem tion. However, this is probably impossible to do:
solving in both conditions involves executive Any task that taps executive function will be
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

function, but the likelihood of observing an "contaminated" by automatic processes work-


interference effect depends at least in part on ing either in concert with, or in opposition to,
aspects of executive function other than the executive function. The alternative advocated
inhibition of a prepotent expectation. By itself, by Maier (1936) and Jacoby (1991) is to
the construct of inhibition fails to capture the capitalize on this contamination and compare a
difference between conditions. facilitation condition to an opposition condition.
Another feature of the problem-solving frame- By considering these conditions together, one
work is that it recognizes that executive function may derive estimates of executive function.
is required even in situations that do not demand Consider, for example, the dimensional change
resistance to interference (e.g., it is required for card sort (see Figure 2; Zelazo et al., 1996), in
planning in novel situations). This feature is which children are first told to sort test cards by
compatible with Maier's (e.g., 1932, 1937) one dimension (e.g., color) and then instructed
suggestion that human behavior always reflects to switch and to sort cards by the other
the joint operation of two qualitatively different dimension. On the preswitch phase, executive
processes, which he called learning and reason- function and automatic process will work
ing. Learning is a relatively automatic process together, so performance will be relatively
that "is reproductive in nature and makes for insensitive to individual and group differences
stereotyped behavior" (Maier, 1932, p. 71). in executive function. That is, over trials,
Reasoning, on the other hand, is intentional and children can sort successfully either because
is usually seen in the goal-directed integration of they are controlling their behavior on each trial
separately learned behaviors. Problem solving or because they are relying on relatively
normally depends on both types of process, and automatic processes. As it turns out, both 3- and
it is difficult to create "pure" tests of reasoning 5-year-olds almost always pass this phase of the
(cf. Jacoby, 1991). However, the relative contri- task (e.g., Zelazo et al., 1996). On the other
bution of learning and reasoning to behavior in a hand, on the postswitch phase, executive
particular situation can be estimated by compar- function and automatic processes are put into
ing performance in a facilitation condition in
which the processes work together towards a
common solution, versus performance in an
opposition condition in which the processes are Target Cards
put into conflict (see also Maier, 1936).
Recently, an elaboration of this logic has been
successfully used by Jacoby (1991) in his Test Cards
process-dissociation procedure.
The opposition or interference conditions are
useful because they challenge controlled pro-
cesses and are therefore more sensitive than
facilitation conditions or neutral (novel) condi-
tions to endogenous or induced variations in
controlled processing. It is perhaps because of Figure 2. Stimuli from the dimensional change card sort,
this sensitivity that researchers have highlighted in which children are firs: told to sort test cards with target
cards according to one dimension (e.g., color) and then told
the importance of inhibition. However, execu- to sort the cards by the other dimension (e.g., shape; Zelazo
tive function also operates in novel situations et al., 1996).
EXECUTIVE FUNCTION 203

opposition. One might imagine that the task now tions between various aspects of executive
becomes an executive function task; indeed, function, which are particularly informative
3-year-olds usually fail this phase by perseverat- regarding the course of typical and atypical
ing on the first dimension despite being told the development (cf. Mundy, 1995), and which
postswitch rules on every trial. However, this support the analysis of executive function into
phase of the task is not a pure executive function subfunctions.
task and it is likely to underestimate children's
executive function. What is needed to estimate
the contribution of executive function to card Research on Executive Function in Early
sorting is both the pre- and the postswitch Childhood
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

phases: both the facilitation condition and the


opposition condition. Performance in the facili- In this section, the problem solving frame-
tation condition reflects executive function plus work is used to organize the literature on the
automatic processing (Pf = EF + AP). Perfor- development of executive function in early
mance in the opposition condition reflects childhood (roughly, between 2 and 5 years).
executive function minus automatic processing Studies are reviewed that correspond to each
(Po — EF — AP). By obtaining values for Pf and phase in the framework. Many experimental
Po, and combining variations of the previously paradigms (e.g., the WCST) simultaneously
mentioned equations through simple algebra, assess various aspects of executive function and
one can derive estimates of EF and AP (see could potentially be reviewed under the rubric
Jacoby, 1991). of more than one of our four phases of problem
solving. However, we have made an effort to
The basic structure of this logic should suffice restrict our review to just those findings that can
to support the following important points: (a) be clearly interpreted in terms of only one of our
executive function contributes to performance four phases. After examining specific phases of
not only in interference problems, but also in problem solving in isolation, it will be possible
problems in which executive function works in to consider them collectively in relation to the
conceit with other more automatic processes, macroconstruct of executive function.
(b) interference paradigms are more sensitive
than facilitation conditions to individual and
group variations in executive function, but they Problem Representation
underestimate executive function (because they
correspond to EF — AP), and (c) accurate In order to solve a problem, one must first
estimates of EF will require comparison of both represent the problem in a way that is conducive
interference and facilitation conditions. These to solving it. That is, one must create an
points should be kept in mind when considering adequate problem space (Keren, 1984). In the
studies of executive function in children. Gestalt tradition, problem representation was
Different studies reveal more or less executive often assessed using insight problems that
function in children at different ages in part demand that the problem representation be
because the tasks used vary in the extent to restructured (e.g., Duncker, 1945). Similar work
which they put executive function into opposi- with children would be informative, but, to our
tion with automatic processes (e.g., consider the knowledge, only one such study has been
subsequent section on children's integration as conducted, and unfortunately, the particular
assessed by H. H. Kendler and Kendler, 1962, problem used was too difficult for preschoolers
versus Crisafi & Brown, 1986). Although there (Antonietti & Nava, 1995). However, the
have been no deliberate efforts yet to derive restructuring of representations has been as-
estimates of executive function in children using sessed in the perceptual domain, in which
the process-dissociation logic (Jacoby, 1991; several studies have used ambiguous figures to
Maier, 1936), some developmental studies have examine the creation and modification of
been designed to permit the comparison be- perceptual sets.
tween opposition and facilitation conditions Problem representation or set is also closely
(e.g., Gerstadt, Hong, & Diamond, 1994; related to selective attention. In order to achieve
Wellman, Fabricius, & Sophian, 1985; Zelazo et a representation of a problem (or an interpreta-
al., 1996). Most of these studies reveal dissocia- tion of a figure), one must attend selectively to
204 ZELAZO, CARTER, REZNICK, AND FRYE

some aspects of a situation and ignore others information (e.g., disambiguated figures) and
(i.e., filtering). The top-down influence of were shown the ambiguous figures again. About
problem representation on filtering has recently half of the children failed to reverse in this final
been demonstrated by Folk and his colleagues phase, and those who did reverse did so
(e.g., Folk, Remington, & Johnston, 1992) who considerably less often than is typical of adults
found that the ease of ignoring distracters in a comparable test. The authors report that
depends on their conceptual relevance to the there was a nonsignificant trend for the 4-year-
target items. Distracters outside of a partici- olds to reverse more often than the 3-year-olds.
pants' attentional control settings (or set) are One interpretation of this trend is that the older
ignored relatively easily. Problem representation children are less likely to perseverate on an
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

is also related to the flexibility of attentional sets initial representation of an ambiguous stimulus.
(studied under the rubric of priming; Enns & Selective attention. Both filtering (i.e., ignor-
Cameron, 1987). Finally, recent work by DeLoa- ing distracters) and priming (i.e., flexibility of
che (e.g., 1987) on children's understanding of attentional sets) have been studied by Enns and
scale models explores representational flexibil- Cameron (1987) in children at 4 and 7 years of
ity more directly. age, but, unfortunately, the task that these
Set. The influence of age on the creation of authors used seems to involve too many phases
perceptual sets has been studied extensively. of problem solving to be easily interpreted
Reese (1963a) showed children between 3 and 8 within our framework. A more easily interpreted
years of age a series of pictures of animals or task (for our purposes) was used by Humphrey
people and then showed them Bugelski and (1982), who manipulated the relation between
Alampay's (1961) ambiguous rat-man figure distracters and the focal aspects of a test of
and asked them to identify it. Even the youngest verbal recall and recognition memory. Three
children seemed to acquire a set to perceive the types of distracter were used: environmental
ambiguous figure as a rat in the animal condi- distracters (a mirror placed near the child during
tion, but there was no evidence that preschool test), simple task-internal distracters (irrelevant
children induced the relevant set from the stimulus characteristics that could be ignored by
human pictures. Subsequent studies in which the a simple decision rule based on one stimulus
pictures of people were labeled as such also feature, e.g., "Ignore all the large ones"), and
failed to demonstrate set in this condition complex task-internal distracters (irrelevant
(Reese, 1963b). One possible interpretation of stimulus characteristics that could be ignored by
these findings is that children have difficulty a more complex decision rule based on two
overcoming a bias to represent the ambiguous features, e.g., "Ignore the large ones and/or the
figure as a rat. Perhaps the dnimal condition ones that differ in number"). Preschoolers (4 to
capitalized on children's a priori bias whereas 5 years) were impaired by all types of distracter,
the human condition required representational kindergartners (no age provided) by simple and
flexibility. In any case, West and Abravanel complex distracters, and second graders by
(1972) succeeded in inducing a human set in complex distracters. It was not until the fourth
4- and 5-year-olds. These authors required grade that children were successful in the
children to identify and classify the set-inducing presence of complex distracters. By the onset of
pictures before the critical ambiguous figure was the school-age period (kindergarten), then,
presented, a manipulation that may have af- children were able to ignore external distracters.
fected the potency of children's a priori The use of more complex selective attention
representational bias to see the picture as a rat. strategies was not revealed until later. Hum-
In another paradigm, Rock and his colleagues phrey argued that the ability to inhibit responses
(Rock, Gopnik, & Hall, 1994) asked 3- and to the complex distracters requires "the use of
4-year-old children to name one of three an intersection or a hierarchy of rules to be
ambiguous figures, including the rat-man fig- discriminated from target information. . ."
ure. During an initial 60-second inspection (p. 737).
period, no child spontaneously reversed his or Understanding the dual nature of 3-dimen-
her perception. Children were then informed sional representations, DeLoache (1987) de-
about the alternative interpretation using a veloped a scale-model task to study preschool-
graded series of increasingly explicit pieces of ers' ability to understand and use representations
EXECUTIVE FUNCTION 205

of the world to constrain their behavior. Sophian, 1984). These studies provide converg-
Children at 2.5 and 3 years of age watched a toy ing evidence that children's search becomes less
being hidden in a 3-dimensionaI scale model of redundant, more exhaustive, and generally more
a room and were then required to use this systematic between about 3 and 5 years of age.
information to find an analogous toy that had Unfortunately, however, systematicity does not
been hidden in the corresponding place in the seem to be a sufficient criterion to establish that
room itself. On 77% of trials, 3-year-olds found behavior has been planned. Perceptually driven
the toy, whereas 2.5-year-olds only did so on strategies such as sighting the next destination
15% of trials. However, when asked to retrieve (Wellman, Fabricius, & Sophian, 1985) could
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

the small toy in the model, children at both ages lead to orderly behavior in the absence of
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

were successful (83%-88% correct). DeLoache planning. Another, more convincing approach
interpreted these results in terms of a dual- has been to examine planful search in well-
orientation hypothesis: to understand and use a defined problem spaces, such as that denned by
symbolic model, children need to think about it the Tower of Hanoi (TOH; Piaget, 1976). In the
both as an object and as a symbol. Younger TOH, planning is clearly required to solve the
children tend to treat the model as a thing problem posed by the experimental situation.
itself—that is, to view it from its most salient Other types of search have also been studied,
orientation. A considerable amount of research including route planning and logical search.
now supports this hypothesis. For example, Finally, planning has been studied in paradigms
DeLoache and Burns (1993) reported that that require children to integrate component
increasing the salience of the model as a behaviors to achieve a goal. In all of these
3-dimensional object (by giving children time to contexts, efforts have been made to develop
play with it first) impaired 3-year-olds' perfor- tasks that are relatively pure measures of
mance on the task. On the other hand, planning in order to minimize difficulties that
decreasing the salience (by putting it behind a may be attributed to other phases of problem
window) improved performance in 2.5-year- solving such as the execution of plans (e.g.,
olds. Problem solving in this situation seems to Hudson, Shapiro, & Sosa, 1995; Klahr &
break down at the initial phase of problem Robinson, 1981). Furthermore, precautions have
solving (i.e., representation). been taken to reduce the risk that success on
Summary. The evidence reviewed reveals planning tasks could result from the use of
age-related changes in children's performance perceptual strategies or other techniques that
on perceptual set induction, selective attention, guide behavior one step at a time (see Wellman
and the flexible use of scale models to guide et al., 1985, for a discussion).
search. Preschool children often appear to Search in a well-defined problem space.
represent situations in ways that are inflexible Most research on planning in well-defined
and unlikely to support successful problem problems (i.e., those that involve completely
solving. This finding is compatible with the specified initial states, end states, and operators;
hypothesis that problem representation is an Newell & Simon, 1972) has used the Tower of
important aspect of executive function that is Hanoi and its variants (e.g., Bidell & Fischer,
sensitive to developmental variation. 1994; Byrnes & Spitz, 1977; Klahr & Robinson,
1981; Piaget, 1976). In the original Hanoi Tower
Planning task (Piaget, 1976), participants were presented
with three pegs on which were placed a certain
The second phase of problem solving is number of disks of variable size. The task
planning. Planning has long been a topic of requires participants to rearrange the disks into a
interest to developmental psychologists (e.g., predetermined goal configuration under the
Friedman, Scholnick, & Cocking, 1987). One of constraints that only one disk can be moved at a
the simplest approaches to the study of planning time and that a larger disk can never be placed
in preschoolers has been to note the extent to on a smaller disk. Klahr and Robinson (1981)
which children are systematic in unstructured modified the task by using upside-down cans
situations (e.g., Goodman, 1981; Gottschalk, instead of disks, thereby inverting one of the
Bryden, & Rabinovitch, 1964; Potter & Levy, arbitrary rules, "Don't put a smaller can on top
1968; Wellman, Somerville, Revelle, Haake, & of a larger one," and embodying it as an
206 ZELAZO, CARTER, REZNICK, AND FRYE

inviolable task constraint. These authors also More recently, Klahr (1985) examined 3.5- to
required children to describe their solution plans 5.5-year-olds' problem-solving strategies on the
without actually executing them. The difficulty Dog-Cat-Mouse Task, a task with ambiguous
of the task was manipulated by varying the subgoal ordering. Children were presented with
number of moves (one to seven) required for a square apparatus that contained grooves along
solution and the type of problem (i.e., tower- all four sides and along one of the diagonals.
ending, in which cans must be stacked, or The objective of the task was to move a dog, a
flat-ending, in which each can must be placed on cat, and a mouse to the corners containing their
a particular peg). Early work on the original respective foods (i.e., a bone, a fish, and a piece
TOH (Piaget, 1976) revealed poor performance of cheese), subject to three constraints: (a) only
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

even among school-age children. Using their one animal could be moved at a time, (b) only
modified version, however, Klahr and Robinson one animal could occupy any given corner at a
(1981) found that 4-year-olds typically did well time, and (c) animals always had to land on a
on two- and three-move problems, whereas corner. Klahr found a low correlation between
5-year-olds usually did well on four-move age and proportion correct. Even 4-year-olds
problems, and 6-year-olds did well on six-move avoided useless double moves, looked ahead
problems. Thus, clear age differences emerged about two to three moves (i.e., evidence of
in the length of the problems that children could planning), and generally used a hill-climbing
pass. In addition, problem type influenced technique (i.e., always move towards the goal)
performance at all ages: Tower-ending problems rather than random trial and error. Hill climbing
were solved more easily than flat-ending prob- is planful, but it is less sophisticated than
lems, possibly because flat-ending problems means-end analysis, and it is susceptible to local
have ambiguous subgoal ordering (see also maxima (i.e., children have difficulty tempo-
Klahr, 1985). Analysis of children's pass-fail rarily moving away from the goal as a step
patterns in different versions of the task toward solving the problem).
indicates that there are changes in problem Event planning. The TOH and the Dog-Cat-
analysis and depth of search (i.e., the number of Mouse Task are both well-defined problems
anticipated moves that must be remembered) with clearly defined problem spaces. The tasks
that correspond to improvements in planning are computationally tractable, but they are less
during the preschool years. ecologically valid than most tasks designed to
The fact that children were not required to assess event planning. Hudson et al. (1995)
execute their plan (i.e., the fact that this was a argued that children's initial planning skills are
pure planning task; Klahr & Robinson, 1981, likely to be observed in the context of familiar,
p. 119) might account for why children did so as opposed to unfamiliar, events (because
well compared to other investigations of TOH children can draw on their generalized event
performance (Byrnes & Spitz, 1977; Piaget, representations or scripts). These authors exam-
1976). On the other hand, children's perfor- ined 3-, 4- and 5-year-olds' ability to construct
mance in Klahr and Robinson's study was worse and provide verbal plans for familiar events.
than children's performance in an earlier study In order to differentiate children's ability to
(Klahr, 1978) in which the experimenter pro- plan familiar events (e.g., going to the beach)
vided visual feedback by actually executing the from their general ability to describe events,
moves that the child proposed. In that earlier children's verbal plans were compared to the
study, half of the 3-year-olds could solve two-move verbal descriptions of children who were simply
problems, most 4-year-olds solved four-move asked to report a script of the same events. Older
problems, and all 5-year-olds solved the five- children's scripts and plans were more different
move problems. In addition, children's perfor- from one another than younger children's. For
mance in Klahr and Robinson's study was worse example, 5-year-olds reported more activities
than the performance of children in a study by occurring at the onset of a plan than a script,
Welsh (1991), who tested 3- to 12-year-old which perhaps reflects the fact that planning is
children on the original TOH task. In Welsh's preparatory in nature, while script reporting is
study, children executed the moves themselves not. The finding that 3-year-olds were less likely
and therefore received both visual and motoric than 5-year-olds to adjust their reports when
feedback about the adequacy of their plans. planning was required indicates that the younger
EXECUTIVE FUNCTION 207

children had difficulty planning even in the then allowed them to maneuver through it.
context of real-world scenarios. Children at 25 months, but not 21 months,
Mazes and route planning. Mazes, particu- selected the correct side. Younger children
larly the Porteus maze (Porteus, 1950, 1959), frequently perseverated to the same side across
have long been used to study route planning. To repeated trials.
minimize errors (i.e., to avoid blind alleys), one Blades and Spencer (1987) required children
should locate the starting position and the end between the ages of 3 and 6 years to use a map to
position (goal) and plan a path (i.e., look ahead) plan a route through a maze in order to avoid
prior to entering the maze. In a study involving roadblocks positioned at various strategic points
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

18- to 36-month-olds, Brockman (1977) used along the maze. Although all children could
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

various slot mazes with slotted templates and a trace the correct path with their fingers on the
nonremovable stylus. On the easiest two-turn map, children in the two youngest age groups
maze, 18-month-olds successfully completed (3-11 and 4-6) did not use the map effectively
only 20% of trials. The majority of trials were to plan a correct route. As in Reiser et al.'s study
completed correctly by 24-month-olds for the (1982) with 2-year-olds, the performance of
two-turn maze, but they nonetheless made more these younger children was hindered by perse-
errors on this maze than the older children (30- veration. Out of 48 children in the younger two
and 36-month-olds). Tn addition, 24-month-olds age groups, 10 exhibited perseverative response
failed to complete the majority of five-turn and tendencies, such as always turning left at a
six-turn mazes. Thus, the results suggest that junction, regardless of the side on which the
age changes in route planning occur between the roadblock was located. The older age groups, on
ages of 24 and 36 months of age, especially on the other hand, performed better than would be
more complex maze problems. Brockman men- expected by chance.
tions that children were given assistance if they Fabricius (1988) has likewise found evidence
reentered the same blind alley three times. for the development of route planning in
Although she did not report data on the relative preschoolers. In that study, 4- and 5-year-olds
frequency of this type of perseverative response were required to plan a route in order to retrieve
pattern, perseverative tendencies may interfere items from several locations and subsequently
with planning. Support for this suggestion bring them to a final endpoint. Forward-search
comes from observations reported by Wertlieb planning was indexed by choosing a route in
and Rose (1979) in a study involving 3-, 4-, and which one did not have to backtrack. The results
5-year-old children who were required to indicated that 5-year-olds engaged in forward
navigate a large-scale maze. These authors search, whereas 4-year-olds used only a rudimen-
found that younger children made more errors tary form of forward search. In addition, there
during maze learning than did older children. were age changes in spontaneous self-correc-
Often these errors were perseverative (i.e., they tion.
involved repeatedly entering the same blind In one study by Fabricius, Wellman, and
alleys). Moreover, younger children were less Sophian (reported in Wellman et al., 1985), 3-,
likely than older children to change routes when 3.5-, 4.5-, and 5.5-year-olds' performance on
the maze conditions were rearranged. Rose various route-selection tasks was assessed to
(1980) has speculated that these findings reflect determine the relative contributions of planning
maturation of a functional system that involves and sighting (i.e., using proximity cues in a
prefrontal cortex in addition to subcortical sequential fashion) to route selection. The tasks
structures. differed in whether the shortest route could be
Large-scale mazes usually provide a better selected using (a) planning alone, (b) proximity
measure of learning than of planning (because it alone, (c) planning in conjunction with proxim-
is difficult or impossible to look ahead), but ity, or (d) planning as opposed to proximity.
planning is clearly emphasized when partici- When planning alone was required in order to
pants are provided with a map or an aerial view select the shortest route, only the 3.5-year-olds
of a large-scale maze. Reiser, Doxsey, McCar- and older children selected the planned route
rell, and Brooks (1982) showed toddlers an more often than chance. However, only the
aerial view of a simple maze (i.e., one barrier 5.5-year-olds selected a route based on planning
around which children must make a detour) and when it conflicted with proximity cues. These
208 ZELAZO, CARTER, REZNICK, AND FRYE

findings suggest that the tendency to plan may In Hullian integration tasks, children are first
be disrupted when other competing perceptual taught two subgoal-subgoal segments (A leads
strategies are put into opposition with planning. to B; X leads to Y). Then they are taught one
Logical search. In some search tasks, plan- subgoal-goal segment (B leads to G). In a final
ning involves making inferences regarding test phase they are asked to attain the goal (G)
which of several locations is the best (or only) given a choice between A and X. To achieve the
place to search. DeLoache and Brown (1984) goal, children must infer that A allows them to
reported that when searching for a missing toy, obtain B, which in turn allows them to obtain G
older 2-year-olds, but not younger 2-year-olds, (i.e., they must combine the A-B and B-G to
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

are likely to infer plausible new locations rather form A-B-G). In a series of studies, the
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

than simply to search in places where they have Kendlers (see T. S. Kendler & Kendler, 1967,
found the toy in the past. Sophian and for a review) presented children with an
Somerville (1988; Experiment 3) explicitly automated apparatus that had three panels (i.e.,
addressed reasoning about the possible locations left, center, and right). In the first phase of
of hidden objects. These authors created a learning, children were shown that pressing a
situation in which children had to infer from an button on one panel produced a marble (e.g., the
experimenter's behavior which of four locations A-B segment) while pressing a button on the
might possibly contain a toy. Two of the four other panel produced a ball-bearing (e.g., the
locations were logical possibilities. Before X-Y segment). Then, in the B-G segment,
children were allowed to search, the experi- children were taught that one of the two objects
menter removed one of the logically possible (e.g., the marble) could be used to retrieve a
locations. If children had been able to consider reward from the center panel. Finally, in the test
both possible locations simultaneously, they phase, all three panels were exposed. Children
would be in a position to infer the correct had to infer that they could obtain the correct B
location. Otherwise, if they had only focused on object (e.g., the marble) by pressing the
one of the two possible locations, they would appropriate A button. In contrast to their earlier
search correctly on half of the trials (on work (H. H. Kendler & Kendler, 1956), even
average). In contrast to 2.5- and 3-year-olds, kindergartners had considerable difficulty on
4-year-olds searched correctly more often than this integration task.
would be expected based on random respond- A number of authors have criticized the
ing. Thus, it was not until 4 years of age that Kendlers' automated box task, arguing that
there was reliable evidence of inferential aspects of the task that are irrelevant to
planning in this task. inferential integration obscured preschoolers'
Sequencing and integration of component ability to combine separately learned behaviors.
behaviors. The importance of integration as an In support of these claims, slight modifications
index of planning has long been noted (e.g., to the original Kendler box, such as substituting
Baldwin, 1897; Piaget, 1952). However, the best drawers for buttons in the A-B and X-Y
paradigm for the investigation of integration is segments (Hewson, 1978), or otherwise modify-
perhaps the one created by Maier (e.g., see ing the apparatus and instructions (Halliday,
Maier, 1936), and modified by Hull (1935), in 1977), have elicited better performance by
which participants (rats or children) are taught preschool children.
separate behavioral sequences and subsequently Crisafi and Brown (1986) devised a set of
required to combine them in a novel way to integration tasks involving familiar objects and
achieve a goal. Maier found marked changes by compared 2- to 4-year-olds' performance on
6 years of age in children's ability to integrate these new tasks with performance on the
learned experiences. In more recent work (e.g., original Kendler box. In the first study, Crisafi
Crisafi & Brown, 1986; Hewson, 1978; H. H. and Brown tested 2-year-olds' ability to com-
Kendler & Kendler, 1956), integration tasks bine separately learned behaviors using three
have undergone considerable modification, but analogous tasks. The easiest task consisted of
they have retained the basic logic of the Hullian familiar objects and separate associations that
version (for a discussion of important differ- capitalized on the objects5 familiar functions. In
ences between various integration tasks, see the A-B and X-Y phases, children were taught
T. S. Kendler & Kendler, 1967). that a piggy bank and a purse contained pennies
EXECUTIVE FUNCTION 209

and dimes, respectively. For the B-G segment, Three-year-olds were less likely than older
they were taught that a gum-ball machine would children to hide the object in the corner location,
dispense a gum-ball only if the correct coin (a even though when they did hide the object there,
penny) were used. The majority of 2-year-olds they were more likely to find it than when they
did well on the two tasks involving familiar hid it elsewhere. There were no group differ-
objects. On the other hand, the vast majority of ences in the tendency to search at the corner
4-year-olds performed poorly on a modified locations on nonmemory control trials.
version of the Kendler-box task unless it was Young children might fail to plan for any
presented to children after the easier tasks. number of reasons. For example, they may not
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Together with other findings (e.g., Hewson, appreciate the importance of planning (e.g.,
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

1978), Crisafi & Brown's (1986) results indicate Flavell, Freidrichs, & Hoyt, 1970), they may
that integration in preschoolers depends on the have difficulties implementing a strategy for
familiarity of the objects in the tasks. When remembering, or they may have selected a plan
familiar objects were used, even 2-year-olds that turned out to be ineffective. However,
were able to combine separately learned behav- studies on planning to remember suggest that
iors. On the other hand, it is not before the age of children first begin to plan for retrieval toward
5 years (Hewson, 1978) or even 7 years (T. S. the end of the preschool period.
Kendler & Kendler, 1967) that the majority of Social planning. Planning is not only impor-
children exhibit integration on tasks involving tant for acting in the physical world, and for
unfamiliar objects and relations among objects. regulating one's own behavior, but also for
This discrepancy in performance illustrates that negotiating the many social interactions in
preschoolers' ability to plan sequences of which it is necessary to understand and influ-
separately learned behaviors is fairly fragile. ence the behavior of others. The large body of
Planning to remember. In an effort to literature on children's theory of mind (e.g.,
examine the role of planning in memory, Astington, Harris, & Olson, 1988; Frye &
Rogoff, Newcombe, and Kagan (1974) showed Moore, 1991; Lewis & Mitchell, 1994) indicates
4-, 6-, and 8-year-olds 40 pictures and told them that there are major changes between the ages of
that they would be asked to remember these about 3 and 5 years in children's appreciation of
pictures after either a few minutes, 1 day, or 1 their own and other people's mental states.
week. The time to study each picture was Recently, it has been proposed that the develop-
self-paced by the children. The authors hypoth- ment of executive function may account for
esized that children who could plan should age-related changes in performance on theory-of-
adjust their study time depending on the length mind problems (Harris, 1993; Russell, Mauth-
of the delay period. They found that 8-year-olds ner, Sharpe, & Tidswell, 1991). This conclusion
were more likely than 4- and 6-year-olds to has typically been drawn from studies of
make appropriate adjustments. There are a deception, which has been examined exten-
number of possible explanations of 4- and sively within theory-of-mind research (e.g.,
6-year-olds' failure to adjust (e.g., difficulty Chandler, Fritz, & Hala, 1989; Sodian, Taylor,
using strategies, lack of metamemory skills), but Harris, & Perner, 1991). Deception is a type of
it is clear that 8-year-olds modified their social planning in which one must adopt a plan
behavior in anticipation of a subsequent re- of action to produce a false belief in another.
trieval. In one study of strategic deception, Russell et
In a more recent study, Heisel and Ritter al. (1991) presented 3- and 4-year-olds with two
(1981; Experiment 1) tested 3-, 5-, 7- and boxes that had windows on them. The windows
9-year-old children on a memory-for-location faced the child and revealed the contents of the
task. They presented children with a 12 X 12 boxes. On each trial, one of the boxes was
array of identical containers. Children were baited, and the child was instructed to tell the
asked to hide an object in one of the containers experimenter (who could not see the contents of
in such a way "that will be easy to find the boxes) where to look. The experimenter
[remember] later" (p. 353). The authors rea- searched where instructed by the child and the
soned that planful children should hide the child received the contents of the other box.
object in the corner locations because these were Thus, this was a zero-sum competitive game in
the most distinctive (and easiest to remember). which the successful strategy was to deceive the
210 ZELAZO, CARTER, REZNICK, AND FRYE

experimenter by pointing to the incorrect, planning to remember) probably continues to


unbaked box. Despite repeated failures to develop well into the school-age years.
receive the rewards, 3-year-olds (like autistic
children, but unlike 4-year-olds) continued to Execution
point to the baited box. In a striking display of
extensive perseveration, many 3-year-olds perse- Execution involves keeping a plan in mind
verated for the full 20 trials (but see also and translating that plan into action. The first
Samuels, Brooks, & Frye, 1996). Later studies process may be referred to as intending, whereas
(Hughes & Russell, 1993; Russell, Jarrold, & the second has been studied under the rubric of
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Potel, 1994) demonstrated that the difficulties rule use (Zelazo & Jacques, 1997). The finding
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

that 3-year-olds have in this task are executive that 3-year-olds can sometimes verbalize a new
function difficulties rather than difficulties in plan even while perseverating on an old one
deception per se. The social aspect of deception (Zelazo et al., 1996) supports the theoretical
can be separated from the executive function distinction between these subphases of execu-
requirements by removing the opponent from tion.
the task. Children were merely required to point Intending. Intending per se has received
to a visibly empty box rather than a baited one, very little attention from developmental psy-
whereupon they were given the concealed prize. chologists. One way to assess intending is in the
Russell et al. (1994) found that 3-year-olds still context of prospective memory (i.e., remember-
perseverated extensively even in this version ing to do something). However, prospective
(see Hughes & Russell, 1993, for similar memory has not been studied in preschoolers
findings with adolescents with autism). (see Ceci & Bronfenbrenner, 1985, for work
Brooks, Frye, and Samuels (1996) attempted with school-age children). Another approach to
to determine which aspect of executive function intending is to examine children's ability to
sustain attention over time. Sustained attention
was responsible for 3-year-olds' poor perfor-
seems necessary for keeping a plan activated so
mance on deception tasks. It was reasoned that
that it can control behavior. The standard
3-year-olds might have difficulty selecting an
sustained-attention task is the continuous perfor-
appropriate plan, or they might have difficulty mance task (CPT), originally created by Ros-
executing a plan when it is appropriate to do so. vold, Mirsky, Sarason, Brasome, and Beck
To distinguish between these possibilities, the (1956). In this task, participants are presented
authors showed children demonstrations of two with a series of items and given instructions to
already-formulated plans, one deceptive and one press a key when a target letter appears.
not, and asked children to select the one that Although most of the existing CPT tasks either
would trick another person. The children did not rely on the recognition of letters or numbers or
have to execute the deceptive response them- are otherwise inappropriate for use with pre-
selves. Although 4-year-olds were able to schoolers, Weissberg, Ruff, and Lawson (1990)
choose the deceptive plan, 3-year-olds could recently managed to use CPT tasks with
not. These results suggest that there are children as young as 3.5 years. In one task,
pronounced changes in preschoolers' planning children were required to push a button in
in the social realm, as in the realm of objects. response to a bell that rang after varying
Moreover, young children's difficulties in choos- intervals. Errors of commission and omission
ing a plan occur even when the need for declined from the first 10 trials to the second 10,
formulating and executing the plan is removed. a finding that the authors interpreted in terms of
Summary. The studies reviewed in this the "relative immaturity of attention and organi-
section span a variety of methods that research- zation" (p. 61) in preschoolers.
ers have used to assess planning abilities in A second study in the same paper (Weissberg
children. Research on planning in well-defined et al., 1990) used a simplified visual reaction
problems, route planning, logical search, plan- time (RT) task. Children at 2.5, 3.5, and 4.5
ning to remember, and social planning converge years of age were told to push a button in
to indicate that there are marked changes response to the presentation of a rabbit on a TV
between 3 and 5 years of age in children's ability monitor. There was a significant decrease in RT
and inclination to plan, but that planning (e.g., from 2.5 to 4.5 years, along with decreasing
EXECUTIVE FUNCTION 211

numbers of omission errors. Based on analyses were rewarded for correct responding. In one
that eliminated children who needed reminders condition, children were told, "Remember,
of what to do, the authors argued that the inside things go here, outside things go there.
changes in performance were not due solely to Look, here's something that goes outside, which
increased understanding of instructions, but box does it go in?" and then given a reward for
rather reflected increases in the temporal span of correct responding. Despite these aids and
goal-directed attention. incentives, analyses of children's errors revealed
Rule use. Rule use is the crux of execution, a tendency to repeat responses: when they erred,
when plans are translated into action. As Zelazo it usually involved putting a card into the box in
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

and Jacques (1997) note, the action plans that which they had put a card on the previous trial.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

underlie intentional actions are necessarily These results resemble those described by Luria
conditional: One does not simply act; one acts (1961) for the one-light task in that 2.5-year-
when it is appropriate to do so. Perhaps the most olds seemed to understand the task and the rules,
straightforward strategy for studying this aspect and actually started to use the rules, but were
of execution is the rule-use paradigm (see susceptible to perseverative errors.
Zelazo & Jacques, 1997, for a review). Tn any The rule use of 3- to 5-year-olds has been
rule-use task, children are presented with assessed using the dimensional change card sort
explicit conditional statements and required to (see Figure 2; see Zelazo & Frye, in press, for a
use them to guide their behavior. In one seminal review). This task puts two pairs of rules into
set of studies (Luria, 1961), children were opposition. Frye, Zelazo, and Palfai (1995,
instructed to squeeze a rubber bulb whenever a Experiment 2) presented children with colored
light appeared. In contrast to 3-year-olds, who shapes that would be sorted differently if one
were usually successful at this task, 2-year-olds were sorting by color or by shape. Children were
either made no response, or responded persevera- first required to sort the cards by one dimension
tively even in the absence of the light. A more (e.g., color—"All the red ones go here, but all
complex task involved a two-light discrimina- the blue ones go there. If it's red, put it here, but
tion with one excitatory and one inhibitory rule. if it's blue, put it there."), and then by the other
Children were instructed to squeeze the bulb dimension (e.g., shape). Three-year-olds were
when a red light came on, but to refrain from able to use the first set of rules that they were
squeezing when a green light was illuminated. given. However, in contrast to 5-year-olds, they
On this task, it was not until about 5 years of age could not switch between rule pairs. Despite
that children succeeded. being told to switch and told the new rules on
Luria's work has been difficult to evaluate due each trial, they systematically perseverated on
to the lack of detailed descriptions of methods the preswitch rules during the postswitch phase.
and results, but recent work from a similar Other studies have explored the nature of
theoretical perspective has supported and ex- 3-year-olds' perseveration on the dimensional
tended his observations concerning rule use. change card sort. Results of these studies
Zelazo and Reznick (1991, Experiment 1) indicate that the phenomenon is extremely
investigated rule use in 2.5- to 3-year-olds using robust and occurs in a wide range of situations
a card sort in which children were presented with a wide variety of task materials. For
with a pair of ad hoc rules (e.g., "If it's example, 3-year-olds perseverate even after a
something found inside the house, then put it single preswitch trial (Zelazo et al., 1996,
here. If it's something found outside the house, Experiment 2). These authors also found that
then put it there") and then asked to use these perseveration occurs even when verbal as
rules to separate 10 test cards. 2.5-year-olds opposed to manual responses are required.
failed to use the rules despite possessing Figurines were attached to each of the boxes and
knowledge about the cards. A subsequent study the boxes were labeled as Ernie's box and Big
(Zelazo, Reznick, & Piflon, 1995, Experiments 1 Bird's box. Then, 3-year-olds were told rules
& 2) found that 2.5-year-olds had difficulty even such as, "If it's red, then you have to put it over
when (a) cards were labeled by the correct rule's here in Ernie's box; blue ones go there in Big
antecedent on every trial (e.g., "Here's some- Bird's box." On each trial, children were asked,
thing that goes inside the house"), (b) children "Whose box does this go in?" Nearly all
were reminded of the rules, and (c) children 3-year-olds failed to switch. Zelazo and Frye (in
212 ZELAZO, CARTER, REZNICK, AND FRYE

press) attributed 3-year-olds' failure on this task sponse pattern. Whenever individuals must
to an inability to use a higher-order rule for make a decision that contravenes their bias, they
selecting between incompatible pairs of rules need to employ an embedded rule (i.e., "If I'm
(e.g., "If playing shape, then if blue rabbit, then doing this unusual task, then if I see the word
put it here"). red in blue ink, then say 'blue' "). The control
Some support for the notion that 3-year-olds condition, on the other hand, is comparable to a
have difficulty integrating incompatible pairs of simpler rule use task because it only requires the
rules into a single system of rules via a use a pair of rules (i.e., "If I see the color blue,
higher-order rule comes from the finding that then say 'blue'; if I see the color red, then say
'red' "). The crucial difference between the
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

3-year-olds seem to know both the pre- and the


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

postswitch rules, even though they fail to switch experimental condition and the control condi-
between them. Zelazo et al. (1996, Experiment tion is that in the control condition, the rules do
4) presented 3-year-olds with a dimensional not conflict with a preexisting pair of rules.
change card sort in which verbal responses were Together, the two conditions permit use of the
required. However, after the postswitch trials, process-dissociation procedure to estimate con-
children were given six additional trials that trolled processing (Lindsay & Jacoby, 1994).
assessed separately their knowledge of the School-age children's performance on the
postswitch rules and their ability to use that Stroop task has been assessed in a number of
knowledge. On each trial, children were first studies (e.g., Comalli, Wapner, & Werner, 1962;
asked, "Whose box do the (e.g., red ones) Rand, Wapner, Werner, & McFarland, 1963; see
go in, in the (e-g-, color) game? Whose MacLeod, 1991, for a review), but the task is
box do the (e.g., blue ones) go in?" In inappropriate for children who cannot read.
response to each knowledge question, children However, Gerstadt et al. (1994) recently devel-
had to name the appropriate box. Further, oped a Stroop-like task (based on the verbal
children were told, "Play the (e.g., color) conflict task used by Passler, Isaac, & Hynd,
game," and asked the rule use question, "Whose 1985) that was suitable for use with younger
box does this go in, in the (e-g-, color) children (although the task was too difficult for
game?" Nearly all of the children who persever- 3-year-olds, so they were not tested). Children
ated in the card sort itself answered the between 3.5 and 7 years of age were instructed
knowledge questions correctly, but then pro- to say the word night when shown a line
ceeded to answer the use questions incorrectly drawing of a sun and the word day when shown
(i.e., according to the preswitch rules). This a line drawing of a moon and stars. Children in a
abulic dissociation between knowledge and its control condition were instructed to respond
use supports the analysis of executive function "day" to one abstract design and "night" to
into temporally and functionally separate as- another There were significant age-related
pects. Furthermore, because 3-year-olds appear improvements in performance in the experimen-
to have difficulty using conscious information to tal condition, particularly between 3.5 and 4.5
control their behavior, the dissociation raises years of age. Children tended to respond more
general questions about the relation between rapidly on later trials than on earlier ones, and
consciousness and cognitive control. children (especially younger children) did better
Another rule use task that puts incompatible on trials in which they took longer to respond
rules into opposition is the Stroop task (Stroop, (cf. Tikhomirov, 1978). The most common error
1935), in which participants are presented with patterns could all be described as perseverative
color words (e.g., red) printed in nonmatching (e.g., alternating between saying "day" and
colored ink (e.g., blue ink), and required to "night"; saying "day" to a drawing of a sun).
name the color of the ink. A control condition Whereas rule-use tasks like those just de-
requires naming of color patches or of color scribed require children to comprehend and to
words printed in black ink. As Zelazo and follow arbitrary verbal instructions, imitation
Jacques (1997) noted, the experimental condi- tasks provide children with a visual model of the
tion of the Stroop task is comparable to the actions to be performed. In an early paper,
dimensional change card sort because it requires Pintner (1915) reported norms for the Knox
that children use a pair of rules that is Cubes Test, which requires children to imitate
incompatible with their default, canonical re- touching a set of blocks in sequence. The largest
EXECUTIVE FUNCTION 213

improvement on this task occurred between 3 themselves and behaved in a way that could be
years (6% correct) and 7 years (63% correct). described as perseverative.
On this task, children do not need to plan, but Rule use has also been studied in the context
only to remember and use the available of compliance with commands. For example,
information to guide their behavior. More Kopp and her colleagues (e.g., Vaughn, Kopp, &
recently, Alp (1994) used an imitation sorting Krakow, 1984) used a task in which a raisin was
task, in which children were asked to reproduce hidden in front of the child, and the child was
toy-sorting patterns that were demonstrated by required to inhibit responding during a delay
the experimenter. There were regular increases phase, and then retrieve the raisin. The capacity
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

across the ages tested (12 to 36 months) in the for self-control in this kind of task, and the
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

length of the patterns that children reproduced. stability of it across time and situations,
Some imitation tasks have been designed develops in the first few years of life. Kopp
specifically to contain an interference compo- (1982) described the following sequence: emer-
nent, thereby increasing the complexity of the gence of "impulse control" in the second year,
rules that must be used. Reed, Pien, and "self-regulation" at 36 months, and further
Rothbart (1984) developed a version of the changes in self-regulation during preschool
children's game Simon Says for use with period, including "adoption of contingency
preschoolers. In the original game, one person rules that guide behavior irrespective of situ-
(the model) issues commands (e.g., "Clap your ational pressures" (p. 200). As might be
hands") while concurrently performing the expected from a variety of theoretical perspec-
behavior. Children must obey the command tives (e.g., Luria, 1959a; Zelazo & Frye, in
only if the model prefaces the command with press), changes in self-control are related to
the words "Simon says. . . . " Otherwise, they various aspects of intellectual functioning,
must inhibit both their tendency to obey the especially language (Vaughn et al., 1984). Other
commands and their tendency to imitate the researchers have also noted increases in compli-
behavior of the model. In Reed et al.'s study, ance and in performance on delay tasks between
children in two age groups (40- to 44-months the ages of 2 and 4 (see Pressley, Reynolds,
and 45- to 49-months) were instructed to obey Stark, and Gettinger, 1983, for a review). For
the instructions of one stuffed animal and inhibit example, Schneider-Rosen and Wenz-Gross
responding to the instructions of another animal. (1990) found changes between 24 and 30
Unlike the original Simon Says task, the months on several measures of compliance,
inhibition trials were concretely marked by the including compliance in a situation in which the
presence of cue (i.e., one of the animals). child was instructed to persevere in attempting
Nonetheless, children still performed poorly. to solve a problem that was too difficult.
Children in the younger age group inhibited Summary. Evidence from diverse studies
their responses on only 4.7 out of 20 inhibition suggests that by 3 years of age, children acquire
trials. Older children inhibited their responses the ability to represent and use a pair of arbitrary
on 8.3 inhibition trials (see Zelazo & Jacques, rules. Sustained attention as assessed by continu-
1997, for further discussion). ous performance tasks continues to improve
Diamond and Taylor (1996) recently reported after 3 years of age, but in many situations even
data from a study using Luria's (1980) tapping 3-year-olds follow arbitrary rules and behave
task. Children aged 3.5 to 7 years were required systematically across trials. However, 3-year-
to follow a pair of verbal rules ("If the olds are likely to perseverate if they are required
experimenter taps once, then you tap twice; if to shift from one set of rules to an incompatible
the experimenter taps twice, then you tap set, or if they are required to use rules that
once"). Considerable improvements in speed conflict with their a priori biases (as in the
and accuracy were observed over the age range Stroop task). Thus, interference conditions are
tested, but most of this improvement occurred sensitive to age-related changes in rule use. By
by 6 years of age. The most common error comparing interference conditions (i.e., where
pattern was to tap in a consistent way (e.g., executive function and automatic processes are
always once) regardless of what the experi- put into opposition) to facilitation conditions
menter did. Thus, as in Gerstadt et al.'s (1994) (i.e., where executive function and automatic
Stroop-like task, children simplified the task for processes work together) it should be possible to
214 ZELAZO, CARTER, REZNICK, AND FRYE

derive estimates of the influence of executive the goal and pursuing a different direction
function on children's behavior at different ages suggested by the activity). In the following
and in different situations. Abulic dissociations sections, we bring data from these sources to
between knowledge of rules and the use of these bear on an analysis of the detection and
rules raise interesting questions about the nature correction components of evaluation in the
of cognitive control and suggest that the context of problem solving.
integration of simple rules via higher-order rules Termination when goal achieved. In many
may be an important, and heretofore ignored, situations, termination occurs naturally when a
influence on executive function. goal is achieved because the desire that gave rise
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

to the goal has been satisfied. However, in some


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Evaluation situations, this is not the case, and it is in these


situations that children's evaluation undergoes
Executive function is deemed successful development. Bullock and Ltitkenhaus (1988)
when it leads to the solution of a given problem presented children with a goal (e.g., creating a
or when an intention is realized. However, it is tower identical to a model). Children below 24
not always enough simply to have reached the months often lost track of the goal and failed to
goal state: the recognition that a goal has been stop the activity once the given end point had
attained is itself an important component of been reached. Likewise, Luria (1961) reported
executive function. A corollary skill is to be able that in the one-light bulb-pressing task, 2.5-year-
to detect when a goal has not been attained (or olds who were told to press the bulb when the
when a subgoal does not allow progress toward light comes on commenced pressing appropri-
the goal), and to formulate and carry out a ately, but continued to press even after the light
remedy for the error. Successful error correction was extinguished.
could lead to a revision of the original plan, a Error detection. Ohlsson (1996) draws a
change in how the plan is executed, or a distinction between objective errors and subjec-
modification of the goal or intention. tive errors. Objective errors are defined from a
Especially for children, who usually have less third-person perspective and they entail knowl-
experience in a given domain than do adults, edge of the consequences of every action.
error correction plays a central role in everyday Subjective errors are discrepancies between an
reasoning and action. When considered under intention (or belief) and an outcome. Theories
the aegis of evaluation, the development of this of how human beings monitor and control their
aspect of executive function is of particular own behavior (e.g., theories of executive
interest because it sheds light on the develop- function) need to take the point of view of the
ment of metacognition. Ultimately, errors are problem solver, thus entailing a focus on
also instructive in learning how to learn, as they subjective errors.
provide information on the relative efficacy of The notion that children compare the results
one's strategies (e.g., Baldwin, 1897; DeLoache, of their actions to an internal standard is central
Sugarman, & Brown, 1985). to developmental theory. For Baldwin (1894),
In trying to understand evaluation as an the discrepancy between the infant's intention to
aspect of executive function, many theorists imitate and the infant's actual imitation allows
have found it useful to make a distinction them to learn a novel fact or skill, as well as to
between error detection and error correction. begin to develop a sense of self as agent. For
Research with normal adults (e.g., Ohlsson, Piaget (1952), acts of assimilation or accommo-
1996) and patients (e.g., Konow & Pribram, dation that fail to produce a state of equilibrium
1970) distinguishes these two processes as drive the infant to further exploration. Kagan
logically separate and functionally dissociable (1981) posits a similar mechanism to explain
phases of problem solving. Developmental work why 18-20-month-olds are upset when asked to
also underscores the importance of the distinc- imitate a complex set of actions that is clearly
tion, and it leads to the inspection of several beyond their competence.
aspects of evaluation that are sometimes seem- By the third year of life, children are able to
ingly taken for granted in studies of adults, such detect errors in the performance of other people.
as recognition of the necessity of monitoring Bullock and Lutkenhaus (1988) gave children
progress towards a goal (rather than abandoning between the ages of 15 and 35 months two
EXECUTIVE FUNCTION 215

block-arrangement tasks and one blackboard- an increase in the proportion of children


cleaning task. The end-points of the tasks were attaining a proper seriation of the cups. In terms
either described to the children (blackboard of the problem-solving framework, children at
task) or presented visually (block-arranging all ages may have detected errors, but older
tasks). Bullock and Liitkenhaus (1988) found children were better able to correct them,
that 18- and 24-month-old children could perhaps in part because of increased representa-
reliably distinguish between correctly and incor- tional flexibility in the service of error correc-
rectly-built towers, even when they themselves tion.
failed to build the towers correctly. Moreover, Research on children's learning in situations
even 17-month-olds gave evidence of monitor-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

with an uncertain outcome also explicitly


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

ing their own performance qualitatively (e.g., by addresses error correction (e.g., Stevenson &
showing care about aligning blocks on a tower). Zigler, 1958). For example, Stevenson and Weir
However, it is not until 26 months that children (1961) studied preschoolers and young adults in
consistently monitored their progress towards a a simple operant task with a partial reinforce-
goal (e.g., creating a tower identical to a model). ment schedule. These authors used an apparatus
The extent to which children monitor their in which participants must press one of three
problem-solving behavior may be more likely knobs when a light is illuminated. If they press
for younger than for older children to depend on the correct knob, a marble rolls into a container.
the degree to which the goal is intrinsic or In this task, 3-year-old children tended to repeat
desired by the child (cf. DeLoache et al., 1985), their previous response following both rein-
as opposed to arbitrary or provided by the forced and nonreinforced trials, whereas chil-
experimenter. Similar evidence of error detec- dren 5 years and older were more likely to
tion and performance evaluation in preschoolers change their responses. The authors attributed
was reported by Gelman and Meek (1983), who the older children's behavior to the testing of the
found that 2.5-year-olds could detect errors in a hypothesis that the rewards followed a pattern
puppet's counting, despite making these errors (i.e., the older children were more likely to try to
themselves on other trials (but see Briars & maximize the reward). Only the younger group's
Siegler, 1984; Frye, Braisby, Lowe, Maroudas, action could be easily modeled in stimulus-
&Nicholls, 1989). response terms. This interpretation is consonant
Error correction. In many situations, the with numerous suggestions regarding the devel-
ability to detect one's errors seems to precede opment of mediation (H. H. Kendler & Kendler,
the ability to correct them, both logically and 1962; Luria, 1959a; Maier, 1936; Vygotsky,
developmentally. Let us now turn to the 1962/1934; White, 1965) and prediction hypoth-
development of error correction. DeLoache et eses as against response sets (e.g., position
al. (1985) investigated children's spontaneous preferences; Tumblin & Gholson, 1981). In any
seriation of five nested cups and found no case, however, the findings are important for our
difference in the number of errors made between purposes because they illustrate an age-related
18 and 42 months: approximately half of all insensitivity to error information (i.e., 3-year-
manipulations were errors. Moreover, at every olds' behavior can be described as win-or-lose-
age, the number of attempts to rectify these stay insofar as they stayed with their response
errors was approximately equivalent (on aver- regardless of whether they were rewarded).
age, children tried to correct 61% of errors). Stevenson and Weir's (1961) results have
However, there was a significant change in been confirmed and extended by several studies.
children's correction strategies in this age range. Weir (1964) used the same apparatus but
Whereas younger children often tried to force differing conditions of reinforcement (i.e., the
cups together, older children noticed their correct knob paid off 33% or 66% of the time).
errors, began to take a dual orientation to the Again, preschoolers (3-5 years) tended to repeat
cups (i.e., viewing them as smaller than some previous responses regardless of whether they
cups and bigger than others), and proceeded to were correct. Several other studies have used
put a smaller cup into a base cup, and then treat somewhat different paradigms to explore the
the base cup as a smaller cup to be put inside a strategic use of error information in the
larger one. Not surprisingly, an age-related preschool years. Kessen and Kessen (1961)
increase in the use of this strategy was related to showed children a deck of black or red cards,
216 ZELAZO, CARTER, REZNICK, AND FRYE

and children were required to predict the color respond to novel stimuli (e.g., Overman, 1990;
of each card. On each trial, they received see also Diamond, Towle, & Boyer, 1994).
feedback. Children were then shifted to a new An important study by Gladstone (1969)
deck of cards, in which the ratio of black to red revealed that preschoolers' tendency to persever-
cards differed. Despite the explicit error informa- ate despite error information can be revealed
tion that they received, 3-year-olds, but not even in extremely simple situations that afford
4-year-olds, continued to make predictions only a single type of response. Gladstone
based on the ratio in the first deck of cards that examined rate of extinction on an operant task in
they saw. Furthermore, 3-year-olds were more which to-be-retrieved rewards were visible.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

likely than 4-year-olds to repeat an incorrect After a certain amount of responding, it could be
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

response on consecutive trials (again, their seen that the reward supply had been exhausted.
behavior could be described as win-or-lose- Nonetheless, children below 3.5 years kept
stay). repeating their responses. Older children pro-
In a follow-up study, Jeffrey and Cohen duced fewer responses during this extinction
(1965) considered whether age-related changes phase, with the oldest children (around 4.5
years) immediately ceasing to respond when it
in probability learning might reflect a general
became apparent that the reward dispenser was
shift from perseveration at 3 years of age to
empty. Gladstone argued that the younger
spontaneous alternation at 4 years. These
children recognized the illogic of their extinc-
authors used a two-choice task with indiscrim- tion behavior, and were able to detect their
inable stimuli and with reinforcements pre- errors, but had difficulty using this information
sented on either 100%, 50%, or 33% of trials to control their behavior.
regardless of children's response. Results from
Summary. Even young preschoolers appear
the 100% condition were striking: 3-year-olds
to be proficient at evaluating the products of
almost always perseverated on a particular their own (and others') goal-directed activities.
position, whereas all but one 4-year-old alter- However, age differences have been observed in
nated. (Similar results were found using a very the termination of behavior when a goal is
different paradigm by Douglas, Packouz, & achieved (e.g., Luria, 1961) and in error
Douglas, 1972.) correction (e.g., DeLoache et al., 1985). Chil-
Based on an earlier study by Cross and dren's tendencies to repeat a previously correct
Vaughter (1966), Berman (1973) presented 40 response without regard to its appropriateness
children between the ages of 31 and 60 months for a new situation, especially in motorically
with 24 two-trial object-discrimination prob- complex tasks and other situations requiring
lems. On the first trial in each pair, children were effort, suggests a general difficulty in using an
shown an object that was either associated with abstract or arbitrary goal to govern one's
a reward or not. On the second trial, children activity. In broader terms, this might be
were shown that same object plus a new one. described as a difference between exhibiting
Under different conditions, children were re- will and possessing the recursive evaluative
warded on the second trials according to one of processes that are crucial for intentional action
the following rules: win-stay, win-shift, lose- (Zelazo & Jacques, 1997). The evidence previ-
stay, or lose-shift. Age-related improvements ously cited suggests that it is particularly this
were found across conditions. However, in latter aspect of volitional behavior that seems to
contrast to the work we described that shows undergo a change between 3 and 4 years of age
tendencies in preschoolers toward perseveration (e.g., Kessen & Kessen, 1961; Russell et al.,
(versus alternation), Berman found that shift 1991).
problems were easier than stay problems
(perhaps because they capitalize on a preference Theoretical Approaches to Age-Related
for novelty, or inhibition of return). More recent Changes in Executive Function
work on the relative difficulty of delayed
matching-to-sample compared to delayed non- A review of research on executive function in
matching-to-sample problems has also indicated early childhood indicates clearly that there are
that young children have difficulty learning age-related changes in all four aspects of
rules that require them to inhibit a tendency to executive function identified in the problem-
EXECUTIVE FUNCTION 217

solving framework. Preschool age children cases, it seems equally possible to treat inhibi-
exhibit inflexibility at the level of problem tion as a behavior to be explained rather than as
representation, planning, execution, and evalua- a separate explanatory construct (cf. Hayes,
tion. Behaviorally, this inflexibility is expressed Gifford, & Rucktuhl, 1996; Zelazo & Frye, in
as perseveration. The problem-solving frame- press). Observed failures of inhibition (i.e., in
work serves to organize instances of persevera- behavior) may turn out eventually to be
tion according to where in the process of explained by an inadequate inhibition mecha-
problem solving they occur (cf. Goldberg, 1986; nism, but in the absence of an independent
Goldberg & Bilder, 1987; Sandson & Albert, justification for invoking such a mechanism,
1984). Thus, the framework permits us to these explanations fail to add to our understand-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

recognize the commonalities among these in- ing of the behaviors themselves.
stances, but it also allows us to appreciate their One limitation of inhibition explanations is
differences. A coherent description of executive their inability to support predictions regarding
function at different ages is the first step toward the specific situations that will pose problems
an explanation of developmental changes in for children at different ages. Perseveration (i.e.,
executive function. Given that there are age- a persistent failure to inhibit responding) is
related changes in perseveration at different ubiquitous in the developmental literature, but it
problem solving phases, how might these is highly unlikely that it always reflects that
changes be explained? same underlying cognitive difficulties. Superfi-
Early efforts to explain age-related changes in cially, infants' perseveration in the A-not-B
executive function emphasized the role of search task (Piaget, 1952) resembles 3-year-
inhibition (e.g., Luria, 1959b; White, 1965). olds' perseveration on the dimensional change
Luria (e.g., 1973) provided an insightful and card sort (Zelazo et al., 1996), which, in turn,
comprehensive account of the ubiquitous role of resembles prefrontal patients' perseveration on
inhibition in attention, memory, reasoning, and the WCST (Milner, 1963). However, these are
problem solving. He noted that prefrontal very different tasks, and they are sensitive to
cortical damage produced disinhibition in all of perseveration in different populations; attribut-
these areas (although Luria is often portrayed as ing perseveration to lack of inhibition in each
having specifically emphasized the inhibition of case obscures potentially important differences
motor responses; cf. Shimamura, 1995). In light among them. One possible remedy is to suggest
of the possibility that maturation of prefrontal that children at different ages are more or less
cortex may lead to increased inhibitory control, susceptible to different 'formal' categories of
a number of recent theorists have attributed interference (i.e., motoric, perceptual, linguistic;
age-related changes in aspects of executive Dempster, 1993). This suggestion seems plau-
function to the growth of an inhibition mecha- sible and deserves to be investigated further, but
nism (e.g., Dagenbach & Carr, 1994; Dempster, it is undermined by the fact that perseveration at
1992; Diamond & Gilbert, 1989; Harnishfeger a given age can often be elicited in a number of
& Bjorklund, 1993; Tipper, 1992; see the special different tasks and in a number of different
issue of Brain and Cognition, 30, 1996). An response modalities (e.g., motoric and verbal;
inhibition mechanism provides a straightfor- Zelazo & Jacques, 1997). At the least, inefficient
ward explanation for numerous findings in the or ineffective inhibition must interact with memory
literature, including negative priming (e.g., demand (e.g., Cohen & Servan-Schreiber, 1992)
Tipper, 1992) and retrieval-induced forgetting or with task complexity (e.g., Halford, 1993;
(Anderson & Spellman, 1995). Moreover, re- Robin & Holyoak, 1992; Zelazo & Frye, in
search with monkeys has demonstrated that press) to produce perseveration at a particular
orbitofrontal lesions lead to impairments in the age and in a particular situation.
extinction of conditioned responses (Butter, Memory accounts of executive function
Mishkin, & Rosvold, 1963), which the authors typically invoke the construct of working
attributed to "abnormal difficulty suppressing memory (e.g., Gathercole, 1994; Goldman-
strong, habitual modes of response" (p. 74). Rakic, 1987; Pennington, 1994; Roberts, Hager,
Difficulty in such a simple situation seems to & Heron, 1994; Smith & Jonides, 1995), which
demand an explanation in terms of a basic is similar to, but different from, short-term
process such as inhibition. However, in most memory (Baddeley, 1986; Case, 1985). Whereas
218 ZELAZO, CARTER, REZNICK, AND FRYE

short-term memory involves the passive storage & Lau, 1996). In all of these cases (e.g., the
of information, as in digit span (Dempster, dimensional change card sort described above),
1981), working memory is believed to be more it is logically necessary to reflect on the possible
active and prospective. In a number of neo- rules in order to make a deliberate decision
Piagetian accounts of cognitive development about which rules to use on a particular trial.
(Case, 1985; Pascual-Leone, 1970), changes in However, there is other, independent evidence
executive control are limited by changes in the of increases in reflection during the preschool
actual or functional space of working memory years. For example, recent research has revealed
(or M-power). Consistent with this approach, improvements at around age 4 years in source
work using delayed-response tasks (e.g., Hunter, monitoring (Lindsay, Johnson, & Kwon, 1991),
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

1917) and span tasks (e.g., the counting span episodic memory (Perner & Ruffman, 1995),
task; Case, 1985) has shown that there are and the recollection of one's own false beliefs
regular age-related increases in working memory. (Gopnik & Astington, 1988). Brain-imaging
These increases could affect children's execu- data for adults (Fletcher et al., 1995) have linked
tive function at any of the problem-solving social reasoning to specific areas of prefrontal
phases, but they seem to be especially important cortex that have previously been linked to
for planning and execution (especially the conditional reasoning in human and nonhuman
intending phase of execution). Future work primates (e.g., Petrides, 1990). These findings
might profitably be directed at determining how are consistent with the view that conditional
working memory interacts with other aspects of reasoning is a prerequisite for the reflection that
executive function in specific instances of permits understanding mental states (Frye et al.,
problem solving (see Pennington, 1994, for 1995).
some preliminary suggestions). Summary. According to the problem-solv-
A third approach to the development of ing framework for understanding executive
executive function has been to illustrate the way function, disinhibition, as evidenced by perse-
in which age-related changes in reflection can veration, can occur in any of the four phases of
permit children to exercise increased control problem solving. The problem-solving frame-
over their thought and action. Reflection is work reveals the similarities among various
defined as a recursive process whereby the instances of perseveration, but it also permits an
contents of consciousness become an object of appreciation of their differences. Ultimately,
consciousness so that they can be operated on instances of disinhibition must to be explained
and modified (Zelazo & Jacques, 1997). Accord- by an appeal to other psychological processes,
ing to the cognitive complexity and control such as inhibition, working memory (WM), or
theory (the CCC theory; Frye, Zelazo, & Palfai, reflection. Currently, theories that invoke WM
1992; Zelazo & Frye, in press), higher order or reflection are better able to provide a
rules mediate reflective awareness of lower developmental account of executive function. In
order rules and make possible the deliberate order for inhibition to be a meaningful explana-
selection of lower order rules for use. In the tory construct, it must be combined with a
absence of a higher order rule, children will specification of the way in which it operates at
perseverate on whichever lower order rules are different ages and in different situations. Other-
most compelling. Furthermore, dissociations wise, it adds little to the current understanding
between knowledge and the ability to use that of the behavior to be explained.
knowledge occur until knowledge is subordi-
nated to a higher order rule and integrated Conclusion
within a single system of rules that applies to a
particular situation (Zelazo et al., 1996; Zelazo Despite its appeal, executive function has
&Reznick, 1991). been plagued by definitional difficulties
The CCC theory is designed to capture (Borkowski & Burke, 1996, p. 244). Occasion-
age-related changes in rule use that have been ally, definitions of executive function have
found in card sorting (e.g., Zelazo et al., 1996; seemed so broad as to span the whole range of
Zelazo & Reznick, 1991), social understanding human cognition. On the other hand, more
(e.g., Frye et al., 1995), physical causality (e.g., specific definitions have typically overempha-
Frye et al., 1996), and morality (Zelazo, Helwig, sized one aspect of executive function (e.g.,
EXECUTIVE FUNCTION 219

resistance to interference) to the exclusion of on global assessments of executive function


many others. These definitions fail to capture the (e.g., the WCST), but for different reasons.
intuition (based on neuropsychological studies) Unified theories that reduce executive function
that processes ranging from flexible problem to a single underlying construct such as
representation to self-awareness belong to a inhibition would lack the theoretical tools
coherent (if complex) family of processes. In required to differentiate between different kinds
this article, we have presented a functional of executive function impairment. Although
account of executive function according to there have been efforts to use tasks that target
which the function of executive function is to specific aspects of executive function in clinical
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

solve problems. Diverse aspects of executive populations (e.g., Ozonoff, Strayer, McMahon,
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

function are organized around four temporally & Filloux, 1994; Zelazo, Jacques, Burack, &
and functionally distinct phases of problem Frye, 1996), more work is needed before the
solving. Executive function involves represent- promise of the problem-solving framework can
ing a problem flexibly, planning organized be fulfilled. Ideally, it will be possible to identify
sequences of thought or action, executing those specific psychopathologies by their problem-
sequences, and evaluating the results of one's solving profiles (cf. Pennington & Ozonoff,
rule use. The problem-solving framework makes 1996).
it possible to consider together behaviors that Clinical research based on the problem-
are normally studied in isolation from one solving framework may help to characterize
another. One important conclusion to emerge aspects of executive function in various disor-
from this review is that between 2 and 5 years of ders and to distinguish between those aspects of
age, there are dramatic changes in all four development that do, and those that do not,
aspects of executive function as children gradu- continue to be related to one another even in
ally come to control their thought and behavior cases of markedly atypical development. Clini-
when solving problems in a wide variety of cal and age-related dissociations between differ-
content domains. The problem-solving approach ent phases of problem solving support theoreti-
to executive function not only reveals this cal efforts to analyze problem solving and
convergence of findings, but also provides a executive function into meaningful components
useful framework for distinguishing between (cf. Mundy, 1995; Sigman & Ungerer, 1984). As
inflexibility at each of the four phases of our literature review reveals, examination of
problem solving. children's errors and the circumstances in which
In addition to organizing findings from the they occur can contribute to an understanding of
developmental literature, the problem-solving these executive processes and of problem
framework provides a perspective for under- solving in general. Interference conditions are
standing the cognitive and executive difficulties especially sensitive to developmental variation
associated with a variety of clinical disorders. and reveal age-related changes in inflexibility.
Children affected with several different child- However, a comparison of both interference
hood-onset psychiatric conditions (e.g., autism, and facilitation conditions is required for an
Gilles de laTourette syndrome, attention-deficit/ accurate assessment of executive function ver-
hyperactivity disorder, obsessive-compulsive sus more automatic processes (Jacoby, 1991;
disorder) have been described as exhibiting Maier, 1937).
deficits in executive function, but it is unlikely The problem-solving framework presents
that the specific pattern of executive function executive function as a macroconstruct that
deficits in each of these groups is the same. For captures the way in which a variety of
example, the impulsivity observed in many psychological processes work together to allow
children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity dis- an organism to solve problems in a wide range
order (e.g., Barkley, 1997) appears to be quite of content domains. The wide applicability of
different from the repetitive stereotypic behav- executive function is in keeping with the spirit
iors seen in individuals with autism (e.g., of Luna's (1980) emphasis on integrative
Pennington & Ozonoff, 1996). Different disor- functional systems as opposed to narrowly
ders may involve difficulties during different localized, modular mechanisms. Part of the
phases of problem solving. If so, these disorders appeal of modular mechanisms consists in the
might produce superficially similar impairments simplicity that attends their specificity. This
220 ZELAZO, CARTER, REZNICK, AND FRYE

simplicity is theoretically satisfying insofar as it tions in mental development: A study in social


yields easily stated theories (e.g., in which psychology. New York: Macmillan.
Behavior X is explained by the operation of Barkley, R. A. (1996). Linkages between attention
Module X, which has evolved to do X). and executive functions. In G. R. Lyon & N. A.
However, in absence of elaboration, these Krasnegor (Eds.), Attention, memory, and execu-
tive function (pp. 307-325). Baltimore: Paul H.
theories run the risk of simply substituting an
Brooks.
unobservable mechanism for the behavior to be
Barkley, R. A. (1997). Behavioral inhibition, sus-
explained. Moreover, an assumption of specific-
tained attention, and executive functions: Construct-
ity invites psychologists to fractionate the field ing a unifying theory of ADHD. Psychological
and misconstrue cognitive constructs by consid-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Bulletin, 121, 65-94.


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

ering them outside of the broader context of Berman, P. W. (1973). Response perseveration and
reasoning and action (Jacques & Zelazo, 1994). response shift in young children following reward
This, in turn, may lead to a proliferation of local and nonreward. Child Development, 44, 367-371.
explanations that would be manifestly untenable Bernstein, N. A. (1967). The coordination and
in light of a framework that reveals psychologi- regulation of movements. Oxford England: Per-
cal similarities across a range of situations. gamon.
In the case of executive function, what is Bidell, T. R., & Fischer, K. W. (1994). Developmental
needed is a macroconstruct that captures the transitions in children's early on-line planning. In
way in which subfunctions work together to M. Haith, J. Benson, R. Roberts, & B. Pennington
accomplish a single higher-order function. (Eds.), The development of future-oriented pro-
cesses (pp. 141-176). Chicago: University of
Conceptualizing executive function as a macro-
Chicago Press.
construct acknowledges the inherent complexity
Blades, M., & Spencer, C. (1987). The use of maps by
of higher-order cognitive processes and permits
4-6-year-old children in a large-scale maze. British
us to proceed with the difficult task of mapping Journal of Developmental Psychology, 5, 19-24.
out the interactions among subprocesses. Just as Borkowski, J. G., & Burke, J. E. (1996). Theories,
the human brain consists of interactive func- models, and measurements of executive function-
tional systems that involve the integration of ing: An information processing perspective. In
subsystems (Luria, 1980), higher-order con- G. R. Lyon & N. A. Krasnegor (Eds.), Attention,
trolled processes such as executive function are memory, and executive function (pp. 235—261).
likely to involve complex systems. Subsystems Baltimore: Paul H. Brooks.
have specific roles to play, but it is counterpro- Bransford, J. D., & Stein, B. S. (1993). The ideal
ductive to consider them outside of the larger problem solver (2nd ed.). New York: Freeman.
systems of which they are a part. Brockman, L. (1977). Potential of mazes for assess-
ment of 18- to 36-month-old children. Canadian
Journal of Behavioural Science, 9(2), 197-200.
References Briars, D., & Siegler, R. S. (1984). A featural analysis
Alp, E. (1994). Measuring the size of working of preschoolers' counting knowledge. Developmen-
memory in very young children: The imitation tal Psychology, 20, 607-618.
sorting task. International Journal of Behavioral Brooks, P. J., Frye, D., & Samuels, M. C. (1996). The
Development, 17(1), 125-141. comprehension and production of deception for
Anderson, M. C , & Spellman, B. A. (1995). On the self and other. Manuscript under review.
status of inhibitory mechanisms in cognition: Brown, J. W. (1987). The microstructure of action. In
Memory retrieval as a model case. Psychological J. Perecman (Ed.), The frontal lobes revisited (pp.
Review, 102, 68-100. 251-271). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Asso-
Antonietti, A., & Nava, B. (1995). Solution of an ciates.
insight problem at different age levels. Perceptual Bugelski, B. R., & Alampay, D. A. (1961). The role of
and Motor Skills, 80, 1215-1218. frequency in developing perceptual set. Canadian
Astington, J., Harris, P., & Olson, D. (1988). Journal of Psychology, 15, 205-211.
Developing theories of mind. Oxford: Oxford Bullock, M., & Lutkenhaus, P. (1988). The develop-
University Press. ment of volitional behavior in the toddler years.
Baddeley, A. D. (1986). Working memory. Oxford, Child Development, 59, 664-674.
England: Clarendon Press. Butter, C. M., Mishkin, M., & Rosvold, H. E. (1963).
Baldwin, J. M. (1894). Imitation: A chapter in the Conditioning and extinction of a food-rewarded
natural history of consciousness. Mind, 3, 26-55. response after selective ablation of frontal cortex in
Baldwin, J. M. (1897). Social and ethical interpreta- rhesus monkeys. Experimental Neurology, 7, 65-75.
EXECUTIVE FUNCTION 221

Byrnes, M. M., & Spitz, H. H. (1977). Developmental inhibitory mechanism: Toward a unified theory of
progression of performance on the Tower of Hanoi cognitive development and aging. Developmental
problem. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 14, Review, 12, 45-75.
379-381. Dempster, F. N. (1993). Resistance to interference:
Case, R. (1985). Intellectual Development: Birth to Developmental changes in a basic processing
adulthood. Orlando, FL: Academic Press Inc. mechanism. In M. Howe & R. Pasnak (Eds.),
Ceci, S. J., & Bronfenbrenner, U. (1985). "Don't Emerging themes in cognitive development: Vol. 1,
forget to take the cupcakes out of the oven": Foundations (pp. 3-27). New York: Springer-
Prospective memory, strategic time-monitoring, Verlag.
and context. Child Development, 56, 152-164. Dempster, F. N. (1995). Interference and inhibition in
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Chandler, M., Fritz, A. S., & Hala, S. M. (1989). cognition: An historical perspective. Tn F. N.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Small scale deceit: Deception as a marker of two-, Dempster & C. J. Brainerd (Eds.), Interference and
three-, and four-year-olds' early theories of mind. inhibition in cognition (pp. 3-26). San Diego, CA:
Child Development, 60, 1263-1277. Academic Press.
Cohen, J. D., & Servan-Schreiber, D. (1992). Dewey, J. (1985). How we think: A restatement of the
Context, cortex, and dopamine: A connectionist relation of reflective thinking to the educative
approach to behavior and biology in schizophrenia. process. In J. A. Boydston (Ed.) & B. A. Walsh
Psychological Review, 99, 45-77. (Textual Ed.), John Dewey: The later works,
Comalli, P. E., Jr., Wapner, S., & Werner, H. (1962). 1925-1953 (Vol. 8 1933; pp. 105-352). Carbon-
Interference effects of Stroop Color-Word Test in dale, IL: Southern Illinois University Press. (Origi-
childhood, adulthood, and aging. The Journal of nal work published in 1933)
Genetic Psychology, 100, 47-53. Diamond, A., & Gilbert, J. (1989). Development as
Crisafi, M. A., & Brown, A. L. (1986). Analogical progressive inhibitory control of action: Retrieval
transfer in very young children: Combining two of a contiguous object. Cognitive Development, 4,
separately learned solutions to reach a goal. Child 223-249.
Development, 57, 953-968. Diamond, A., & Taylor, C. (1996). Development of an
Cross, H. A., & Vaughter, R. M. (1966). The aspect of executive control: Development of the
Moss-Harlow effect in preschool children as a abilities to remember what I said and to "Do as I
function of age. Journal of Experimental Child say, not as I do." Developmental Psychobiology,
Psychology, 4, 280-285. 29(4), 315-334.
Dagenbach, D., & Carr, T. (1994). Inhibitory Diamond, A., & Towle, C , & Boyer, K. (1994).
processes in attention, memory, and language. Young children's performance on a task sensitive
New York: Academic Press. to the memory functions of the medial temporal
Delis, D. C , Squire, L. R., Bihrle, A., & Massman, P. lobe in adults—the delayed nonmatching to sample
(1992). Componential analysis of problem-solving task—reveals problems that are due to non-memory-
ability: Performance of patients with frontal lobe related task demands. Behavioral Neuroscience,
damage and amnesic patients on a new sorting test. 108, 659-680.
Neuropsychologia, 30(8), 683-697. Douglas, R. J., Packouz, K., & Douglas, D. (1972).
DeLoache, J. S. (1987). Rapid change and symbolic Development of inhibition in man, Proceedings of
functioning of very young children. Science, 238, the American Psychological Association, 7, 121-
1556-1557. 122.
DeLoache, J. S., & Brown, A. (1984). Where do I go Duncker, K. (1945). On problem solving. Psychologi-
next? Intelligent searching by very young children. cal Monographs, 58(5, Whole No. 270).
Developmental Psychology, 20, 37-44. Enns, J. T., & Cameron, S. (1987). Selective attention
DeLoache, J. S., & Burns, N. M. (1993). Symbolic in young children: The relations between visual
development in young children: Understanding search, filtering, and priming. Journal ofExperimen-
models and pictures. In C. Pratt, & A. F. Garton tal Child Psychology, 44, 38-63.
(Eds.), Systems of representation in children: Fabricius, W. (1988). The development of forward
Development and use. London: John Wiley & search planning in preschoolers. Child Develop-
Sons, Ltd. ment, 59, 1473-1488.
DeLoache, J. S., Sugarman, S., & Brown, A. L. Flavell, J. H., Freidrichs, A. G., & Hoyt, J. D. (1970).
(1985). The development of error correction Developmental changes in memorization pro-
strategies in young children's manipulative play. cesses. Cognitive Psychology, 1, 324-340.
Child Development, 56, 928-939. Fletcher, P. C , Happe, F., Frith, U., Baker, S. C ,
Dempster, F. N. (1981). Memory span: Sources of Dolan, R. J., Frackowiak, R. S. J., & Frith, C.
individual and developmental differences. Psycho- (1995). Other minds in the brain: A functional
logical Bulletin, 89, 63-100. imaging study of "theory of mind" in story
Dempster, F. N. (1992). The rise and fall of the comprehension. Cognition, 57, 109-128.
222 ZELAZO, CARTER, REZNICK, AND FRYE

Folk, C. L., Remington, R. W., & Johnston, J. C. cortical circuitry and cognitive function. Child
(1992). Involuntary covert orienting is contingent Development, 58, 601-622.
on attentional control settings. Journal of Experi- Goldstein, K., & Scheerer, M. (1941). Abstract and
mental Psychology: Human Perception and Perfor- concrete behavior: An experimental study with
mance, J8, 1030-1044. special tests. Psychological Monographs, 53,
Friedman, S., Scholnick, E. K., & Cocking, R. (1987). 1-151.
Reflections on reflections: What planning is and Goodman, J. (1981). The Lock Box: A measure of
how it develops. In S. Friedman, E. K. Scholnick, psyebomotor competence and organized behavior
& R. Cocking (Eds.), Blueprints for thinking: The in retarded and normal preschoolers. Journal of
role of planning in cognitive development (pp. Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 49, 369-378.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

428-465). Cambridge, London: Cambridge Univer- Gopnik, A., & Astington, J. W. (1988). Children's
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

sity Press. understanding of representational change and its


Frye, D., Braisby, N., Lowe, J., Maroudas, C , & relation to the understanding of false belief and the
Nicholls, J. (1989). Young children's knowledge of appearance-reality distinction. Child Development,
counting and cardinality. Child Development, 60, 59, 26-37.
1158-1171. Gottschalk, J., Bryden, M. P., & Rabinovitch, M. S.
Frye, D., & Moore, C. (1991). Children's theories of (1964). Spatial organization of children's responses
mind: Mental states and social understanding. to a pictorial display. Child Development, 35,
Hillsdale, New Jersey: Erlbaum. 811-815.
Frye, D., Zelazo, P. D., Brooks, P. J., & Samuels, Halford, G. S. (1993). Children's understanding: The
M. C. (1996). Inference and action in early causal development of mental models. Hillsdale, NJ:
reasoning. Developmental Psychology, 32, 120— Erlbaum.
131. Halliday, M. S. (1977). Behavioral inference in young
Frye, D., Zelazo, P. D., & Palfai, T. (1992). The children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychol-
cognitive basis of theory of mind. Unpublished ogy, 23, 378-390.
manuscript, New York University. Hamishfeger, K. K., & Bjorklund, D. F. (1993). The
Frye, D., Zelazo, P. D., & Palfai, T. (1995). Theory of ontogeny of inhibition mechanisms: A renewed
mind and rule-based reasoning. Cognitive Develop- approach to cognitive development. In R. Pasnak &
ment, JO, 483-527. M. Howe (Eds.), Emerging Themes in Cognitive
Gathercole, S. E. (1994). Neuropsychology and Development (pp. 28-49). Lincoln, NB: University
working memory: A review. Neuropsychology, 8, of Nebraska Press.
494-505. Harris, P. L. (1993). Pretending and planning. In S.
Gelman, R. & Meek, D. (1983). Preschoolers' Baron-Cohen, H. Tager-Flusberg, & D. Cohen
counting: Principles before skill. Cognition, 13, (Eds.), Understanding other Minds: Perspectives
343-359. from autism (pp. 228-246). Oxford, England:
Gerstadt, C. L., Hong, Y. J., & Diamond, A. (1994). Oxford University Press.
The relationship between cognition and action: Hayes, J. (1989). The complete problem solver (2nd
Performance of children 3'/2- 7 years old on a ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Stroop-like day-night test. Cognition, 53, 129-153. Hayes, S. C, Gifford, E. V., & Ruckstuhl, L. E., Jr.
Gladstone, R. (1969). Age, cognitive control and (1996). Relational frame theory and executive
extinction. Journal of Experimental Child Psychol- function; A behavioral approach. In G. R. Lyon, &
ogy, 7, 31-35. N. A. Krasnegor (Eds.), Attention, memory, and
Goldberg, E. (1986). Varieties of perseveration: A executive function (pp. 279-305). Baltimore: Paul
comparison of two taxonomies. Journal of Clinical H. Brookes.
and Experimental Neuropsychology, 5(6), 710- Heisel, B. E., & Ritter, K. (1981). Young children's
726. storage behavior in a memory-for-location task.
Goldberg, E., & Bilder, Jr., R. M. (1987). The frontal Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 31,
lobes and hierarchical organization of cognitive 350-364.
control. In E. Perecman (Ed), The frontal lobes Hewson, S. (1978). Inferential problem solving in
revisited (pp. 159-187). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence young children. Developmental Psychology, 14,
ErJbaum Associates. 93-98.
Goldberg, G. (1987). From intent to action: Evolution Holyoak, K. (1990). Problem solving. In D. Osherson
and function of the premotor systems of the frontal & E. Smith (Eds.), Thinking: An invitation to
lobe. In E. Perecman (Ed.), The frontal lobes cognitive science (Vol. Ill, pp. 117-146). Cam-
revisited (pp. 273-305). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence bridge, MA: MIT Press.
Erlbaum Associates. Hudson, J. A., Shapiro, L. R., & Sosa, B. B. (1995).
Goldman-Rakic, P. S. (1987). Development of Planning in the real world: Preschool children's
EXECUTIVE FUNCTION 223

scripts and plans for familiar events. Child (Ed.), Children's thinking: What develops ?
Development, 66, 984-998. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Hughes, C , & Russell, J. (1993). Autistic children's Klahr, D. (1985). Solving problems with ambiguous
difficulty with mental disengagement from an subgoal ordering: Preschoolers' performance. Child
object: Its implications for theories of autism. Development, 56, 940-952.
Developmental Psychology, 29, 498-510. Klahr, D., & Robinson, M. (1981). Formal assess-
Hull, C. L. (1935). The mechanism of assembly of ment of problem solving and planning processes in
behavior segments in novel combinations suitable preschool children. Cognitive Psychology, 13,
for problem solutions. Psychological Review, 42, 113-148.
219-245. Kohler, W. (1959). The mentality of apes (2nd ed.; E.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Humphrey, M. M. (1982). Children's avoidance of Winter, trans.). New York: Vintage. (Original work
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

environmental simple task internal, and complex published in 1917)


task internal distracters. Child Development, 53, Konow, A., & Pribram, K. H. (1970). Error recogni-
736-745. tion and utilization produced by injury to the
Hunter, W. S. (1917). Delayed reaction in a child. frontal cortex in man. Neuropsychologia, 8,
Psychological Review, 24, 74-87. 489^91.
Jacoby, L. L. (1991). A process-dissociation frame- Kopp, C. B. (1982). Antecedents of self-regulation: A
work: Separating automatic from intentional uses developmental perspective. Developmental Psychol-
of memory. Journal of Memory and Language, 30, ogy, 18, 199-214.
513-531. Levin, H. S., Culhane, K., Hartmann, J., Evankovich,
Jacques, S., & Zelazo, P. D. (1994). Several strictures K., Mattson, A. J., Harward, H., Ringholz, G.,
on specificity. Current Psychology of Cognition/ Ewing-Cobbs, L., & Fletcher, J. M. (1991).
Cahiers de Psychologie Cognitive, 13, 623-629. Developmental changes in performance on tests of
Jeffrey, W. E., & Cohen, L. (1965). Response purported frontal lobe functioning. Developmental
tendencies of children in a two-choice situation. Neuropsychology, 7, 377-395.
Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2, Levine, B., Stuss, D. T., & Milberg, W. P. (1995).
248-254. Concept generati on: Validation of a test of
Kagan, J. (1966). Reflection-impulsivity: The general- executive function in a normal aging population.
ity and dynamics of conceptual tempo. Journal of Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsy-
Abnormal Psychology, 71(\), 17-24. chology, 17(5), 740-758.
Kagan, J. (1981). The second year, Cambridge, MA: Lewis, C , & Mitchell, P. (1994). Children's early
Harvard University Press. understanding of mind: Origins and development.
Kendler, H. HM & Kendler, T. S. (1956). Inferential Hove, England: Erlbaum.
behavior in preschool children. Journal of Experi- Lindsay, D. S., & Jacoby, L. (1994). Stroop process
mental Psychology, 51, 311-314. dissociations: The relationship between facilitation
Kendler, H. H., & Kendler, T. S. (1962). Vertical and and interference. Journal of Experimental Psychol-
horizontal processes in problem solving. Psycho- ogy: Human Perception and Performance, 20,
logical Review, 69, 1-16. 219-234.
Kendler, T. S., & Kendler, H. H. (1967). Experimental Lindsay, D. S., Johnson, M. K., & Kwon, P. (1991).
analysis of inferential behavior in children. In L. P. Developmental changes in memory source monitor-
Lipsitt, & C. C. Spiker (Eds.), Advances in child ing. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 52,
development and behavior (Vol. 3; pp. 157-190). 297-318.
New York: Academic Press. Luchins, A. S. (1942). Mechanization in problem
Keren, G. (1984). On the importance of identifying solving: The effect of Einstellung. Psychological
the correct 'problem space.' Cognition, 16, 121- Monographs, 54 (6, Whole No. 248).
128. Luria, A. R. (1959a). The directive function of speech
Kessen, W., & Kessen, M. (1961). Behavior of young in development and dissolution. Part I. Develop-
children in a two-choice guessing problem. Child ment of the directive function of speech in early
Development, 32, 779-788. childhood. Word, 15, 341-352.
Kimberg, D. Y., & Farah, M. J. (1993). A unified Luria, A. R. (1959b). The directive function of speech
account of cognitive impairments following frontal in development and dissolution. Part n. Dissolu-
lobe damage: The role of working memory in tion of the regulative function of speech in
complex, organized behavior. Journal of Experi- pathological states of the brain. Word, 15, 453-464.
mental Psychology: General, 122, 411-428. Luria, A. R., (1961). The role of speech in the
Klahr, D. (1978). Goal formation, planning, and regulation of normal and abnormal behaviour (J.
learning by pre-school problem solvers or: ""My Tizard, Ed.) New York: Pergamon.
socks are in the dryer" (pp. 181-212). In R. Siegler Luria, A. R. (1973). The working brain: An
224 ZELAZO, CARTER, REZNICK, AND FRYE

introduction to neuropsychology (B. Haigh, trans.). Neuropsychological development of behavior attrib-


New York: Basic Books Inc. uted to frontal lobe functioning in children.
Luria, A. R. (1980). Higher cortical functions in man Developmental Neuropsychology, 1, 349-370.
(2nd Ed.). New York: Basic Books. (Original work Pennington, B. F. (1994). The working memory
published in 1962) function of the prefrontal cortices: Implications for
Lyon, G. R., & Krasnegor, N. A. (1996). Attention, developmental and individual differences in cogni-
memory, and executive function. Baltimore: Paul tion. In M. Haith, J. B. Benson, R. J. Roberts, Jr., &
H. Brooks. B. F. Pennington (Eds.), Future-oriented processes
MacLeod, C. M. (1991). Half a century of research on in development. Chicago: University of Chicago
the Stroop effect: An integrative review. Psychologi- Press.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

cal Bulletin, 109, 163-203. Pennington, B. F., & Ozonoff, S. (1996). Executive
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Maier, N. R. F. (1932). The effect of cerebral functions and developmental psychopathology.


destruction on reasoning and learning in rats. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 37( 1),
Journal of Comparative Neurology, 54, 45-75. 51-87.
Maier, N. R. F. (1936). Reasoning in children. Perecman, J. (1987). The frontal lobes revisited.
Journal of Comparative Psychology, 21, 357-366. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Maier, N. R. F. (1937). Reasoning in rats and human Perret, E. (1974). The left frontal lobe of man and the
beings. Psychological Review, 44, 365-378. suppression of habitual responses in verbal categori-
Miller, G., Galanter, E., & Pribram, K. H. (1960). cal behaviour. Neuropsychologia, 12, 323-330.
Plans and the structure of behavior. New York: Perner, J., & Ruffman, T. (1995). Episodic memory
Henry Holt. and autonoetic consciousness: Developmental evi-
Milner, B. (1963). Effects of different brain lesions on dence and a theory of childhood amnesia. Journal
card sorting. Archives of Neurology, 9, 90-100. of Experimental Child Psychology, 59, 516-548.
Milner, B. (1964). Some effects of frontal lobectomy Petrides, M. (1990). Nonspatial conditional learning
in man. In J. M. Warren & K. Akert (Eds.), The impaired in patients with unilateral frontal but not
frontal granular cortex and behaviour (pp. 313- unilateral temporal lobe excisions. Neuropsycholo-
334). New York: McGraw-Hill. gia, 28, 137-149.
Milner, B., Petrides, M., & Smith, M. L. (1985). Piaget, J. (1952). The origins of intelligence in the
Frontal lobes and the temporal organization of child. New York: Norton.
memory. Human Neurobiology, 4, 137-142. Piaget, J. (1976). The grasp of consciousness.
Mundy, P. (1995). Joint attention and social- Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
emotional approach behavior in children with Pintner, R. (1915). The standardization of Knox's
autism. Development and Psychopathology, 7, cube test. Psychological Review, 22, 377-401.
63-82. Polya, G. (1945). How to solve it. Princeton, NJ:
Newell, A., Shaw, J. C , & Simon, H. (1958). Princeton University Press.
Elements of a theory of human problem solving. Porteus, S. D. (1950). The Porteus Maze Test and
Psychological Review, 65, 151-166. intelligence. Palo Alto, CA: Pacific Books.
Newell, A., & Simon, A. (1972). Human problem Porteus, S. D. (1959). The maze test and clinical
solving. Inglewood Cliff, NJ: Prentice-Hall. psychology. Palo Alto, CA: Pacific Books.
Ohlsson, S. (1996). Learning from performance Potter, M. C , & Levy, E. I. (1968). Spatial
errors. Psychological Review, 103(2), 241-262. enumeration without counting. Child Develop-
Overman, W. (1990). Performance on traditional ment, 39, 265-272.
matching to sample, non-matching to sample, and Powers, W. T. (1973). Feedback: Beyond behavior-
object discrimination tasks by 12- to 32-month-old ism. Science, 179, 351-356.
children: A developmental progression. In A. Pressley, M., Reynolds, W. M., Stark, K. D., &
Diamond (Ed.), Annals of the New York Academy Gettinger, M. (1983). Cognitive strategy training
of Sciences, Vol 608. The development and neural and children's self control. In M. Pressley & J. R.
bases of higher cognitive processes. New York: Levin (Eds.), Cognitive strategy research (pp.
New York Academy of Sciences. 267-300). New York: Springer-Verlag.
Ozonoff, S., Strayer, D. L., McMahon, W. M., & Rand, G., Wapner, S., Werner, H., & McFarland, J. H.
Filloux, F. (1994). Executive function abilities in (1963). Age differences in performance on the
autism and Tourette syndrome: An information Stroop Color-Word test. Journal of Personality, 31,
processing approach. Journal of Child Psychology 534-558.
and Psychiatry, 35', 1015-1032. Reed, M. A., Pien, D. L., & Rothbart, M. K. (1984).
Pascual-Leone, J. (1970). A mathematical model for Inhibitory self-control in preschool children. Mer-
the transition rule in Piaget's developmental stages. rill-Palmer Quarterly, 30, 131-147.
Acta Psychologica, 32, 301-345. Reese, H. (1963a). "Perceptual set" in young
Passler, M. A., Isaac, W., & Hynd, G. (1985). children. Child Development, 34, 151-159.
EXECUTIVE FUNCTION 225

Reese, H. (1963b). "Perceptual set" in young structure. Cambridge, England: Cambridge Univer-
children: 1L Child Development, 34, 451^54. sity Press.
Reiser, J. J., Doxsey, P. A., McCairell, N. S., & Shimamura, A. P. (1995). Memory and frontal lobe
Brooks, P. H. (1982). Wayfinding and toddlers' use function. In M. Gazzaniga (Ed.), The cognitive
of information from an aerial view of a maze. neurosciences (pp. 803-813). Cambridge, MA:
Developmental Psychology, 18, 714-720. MIT Press.
Roberts, R. J., Hager, L. D., & Heron, C. (1994). Shimamura, A. P., Janowsky, J. S. & Squire, L. R.
Prefrontal cognitive processes: Working memory (1991). What is the role of frontal lobe damage in
and inhibition in the antisaccade task. Journal of memory disorders? In H. S. Levin, H. M.
Experimental Psychology: General, 123, 374—393. Eisenberg, & A. R. Benton (Eds.), Frontal lobe
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Robin, N., & Holyoak, K. J. (1992). Relational function and dysfunction (pp. 173-195). New York:
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

complexity and the functions of prefrontal cortex. Oxford University Press.


In M. Gazzaniga (Ed.), The cognitive neurosci- Shute, G. E., & Huertas, V. (1990). Developmental
ences (pp. 987-997). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. variability in frontal lobe function. Developmental
Rock, I., Gopnik, A., & Hall, S. (1994). Do young Neuropsychology, 6, 1-11.
children reverse ambiguous figures? Perception, Sigman, M., & Ungerer, J. A. (1984). Cognitive and
23, 635-644. language skills in autistic, mentally retarded, and
Rogoff, B., Newcombe, N., & Kagan, J. (1974). normal children. Developmental Psychology, 20,
Planfulness and recognition memory. Child Devel- 293-302.
opment, 45, 972-977. Smith, E. E., & Jonides, J. (1995). Working memory
Rose, D. (1980). Some functional correlates of the in humans: neuropsychological evidence. In M.
maturation of neural systems. In D. Caplan (Ed.), Gazzaniga (Ed.), The cognitive neurosciences (pp.
Biological studies of mental processes (pp. 27-43). 1009-1020). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Sodian, B., Taylor, C , Harris, P. L., & Perner, J.
(1991). Early deception and the child's theory of
Rosvold, H., Mirsky, A., Sarason, I., Brasome, E., &
mind: False trials and genuine markers. Child
Beck, A. (1956). A continuous performance test of
Development, 62, 753-766.
brain damage. Journal of Consulting Psychology,
Sophian, C , & Somerville, S. C. (1988). Early
20, 343-352.
developments in logical reasoning: Considering
Russell, J., Jarrold, C , & Potel, D. ,(1994). What alternative possibilities. Cognitive Development, 3,
makes strategic deception difficult for children— 183-222.
the deception or the strategy. British Journal of
Spreen, O., Risser, A. H., & Edgell, D. (1995).
Developmental Psychology, 12, 301-314.
Developmental Neuropsychology. New York: Ox-
Russell, J., Mauthner, N., Sharpe, S., & Tidswell, T. ford University Press.
(1991). The 'windows task' as a measure of Stevenson, H. W., & Weir, M. W. (1961). Developmen-
strategic deception in preschoolers and autistic tal changes in the effects of reinforcement and
subjects. British Journal ofDevelopmental Psychol- non-reinforcement of a single response. Child
ogy. 9, 331-349. Development, 32, 1-5.
Samuels, M. C , Brooks, P. J., & Frye, D. (1996). Stevenson, H. W., & Zigler, E. F. (1958). Probability
Strategic game playing in children through the learning in children. Journal of Experimental
windows task. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 56, 185-192.
Psychology, 14, 159-172. Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial
Sandson, J., & Albert, M. L. (1984). Varieties of verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychol-
perseveration. Neuropsychologia, 22, 715-132. ogy, 18, 643-661.
Schneider-Rosen, K., & Wenz-Gross, M. (1990). Stuss, D. T., & Benson, D. S. (1984). Neuropsycho-
Patterns of compliance from eighteen to thirty logical studies of the frontal lobes. Psychological
months of age. Child Development, 61, 104—112. Bulletin, 95, 3-28.
Scholnick, E. K., & Friedman, S. (1987). The Stuss, D. X, & Benson, D. S. (1986). The frontal
planning construct in the psychological literature. lobes. New York: Raven Press.
In S. Friedman, E. K. Scholnick & R. Cocking Stuss, D. T., Eskes, G. A., & Foster, J. K. (1994).
(Eds.), Blueprints for thinking: The role of Experimental neuropsychological studies of frontal
planning in cognitive development (pp. 3—38). lobe functions. In F. Boiler, & J. Grafman (Eds.),
Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press. Handbook of Neuropsychology (Vol. 9; pp. 149-
Shallice, T. (1982). Specific impairments of planning. 185). Amsterdam: Elsevier.
In D. E. Broadbent & L. Weiskrantz (Eds.), The Teuber, H. L. (1964). The riddle of frontal lobe
neuropsychology of cognitive function (pp. 199- function in man. In J. M. Warren and K. Akert
209). London: The Royal Society. (Eds.), The frontal granular cortex and behaviour
Shallice, T. (1988). From neuropsychology to mental (pp. 410-440). New York: McGraw-Hill.
226 ZELAZO, CARTER, REZNICK, AND FRYE

Tikhomirov, O. K. (1978). The formation of voluntary class-concept mediation of perceptual sets in


movements in children of preschool age. In M. preschool children. Child Development, 43, 1242-
Cole (Ed.), The selected writings of A. R. Luria (pp. 1248.
229-269). New York: M. E. Sharpe. White, S. H. (1965). Evidence for a hierarchical
Tipper, S. (1992). Selection for actions: The role of arrangement of learning processes. In L. P. Lipsitt
inhibitory mechanisms. Current Directions in & C. C. Spiker, Advances in Child Development
Psychological Science, 1, 105-112. and Behavior (Vol. 2, pp. 187-220). New York:
Tumblin, A., & Gholson, B. (1981). Hypothesis Academic Press.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

theory and the development of conceptual learning. Zelazo, P. D., & Frye, D. (in press). Cognitive
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Psychological Bulletin, 90(1), 102-124. complexity and control: A theory of the develop-
Vaughn, B. R, Kopp, C. B., & Krakow, J. B. (1984). ment of deliberate reasoning and intentional action.
The emergence and consolidation of self-control In M. Stamenov (Ed.), Language Structure, Discourse,
from eighteen to thirty months of age: Normative and the Access to Consciousness. Amsterdam &
trends and individual differences. Child Develop- Philadelphia: John Benjamins.
ment, 55, 990-1004. Zelazo, P. D., Frye, D., & Rapus, T. (1996). An
Vygotsky, L. S. (1962). Thought and language. (E. age-related dissociation between knowing rules
Hanfmann & G. Vakar, Trans.). Cambridge, MA: and using them. Cognitive Development, 11, 37-63.
MIT Press. (Original published in 1934) Zelazo, P. D., Helwig, C. C , & Lau, A. (1996).
Weiner, N. (1948). Cybernetics. Cambridge, MA: Intention, act, and outcome in behavioral predic-
MIT Press. tion and moral judgement. Child Development, 67,
Weir, M. (1964). Developmental changes in problem- 2478-2492.
solving strategies. Psychological Review, 71, 473- Zelazo, P. D., & Jacques, S. (1997). Children's rule
490. use: Representation, reflection, and cognitive control.
Weissberg, R., Ruff, H. A., & Lawson, K. R. (1990). In R. Vasta (Ed.), Annals of Child Development
The usefulness of reaction time tasks in studying (Vol. 12, pp. 119-176). London: Jessica Kingsley
attention and organization of behavior in young Press.
children. Journal of Developmental and Behav- Zelazo, P. D., Jacques, S., Burack, J., & Frye, D.
ioral Pediatrics, 11, 59-64. (1996, August). The relation between theory of
Wellman, H. M., Fabricius, W. V., & Sophian, C. mind and rule use in autism. Paper presented at the
(1985). The early development of planning. In H. XlVth biennial meeting of the International Soci-
M. Wellman (Ed.), Children's searching (pp. ety for the Study of Behavioural Development,
123-149). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Quebec City, Quebec, Canada.
Wellman, H. M., Somerville, S. C , Revelle, G., Zelazo, P. D., & Reznick, J. S. (1991). Age-related
Haake, R. J., & Sophian, C. (1984). The develop- asynchrony of knowledge and action. Child Develop-
ment of comprehensive search skills. Child Devel- ment, 62,719-735.
opment, 55, 472^81. Zelazo, P. D., Reznick, J. S., & Pinon, D. E. (1995).
Welsh, M. (1991). Rule-guided behavior and self- Response control and the execution of verbal rules.
monitoring on the Tower of Hanoi Disk-Transfer Developmental Psychology, 31, 508-517.
Task. Cognitive Development, 6, 59-76.
Wertlieb, D., & Rose, D. (1979). Maturation of maze
behavior in preschool children. Developmental Received August 26, 1996
Psychology, 75(4), 478-479. Revision received January 27, 1997
West, H., & Abravanel, E. (1972). Evidence for Accepted January 28, 1997 •

You might also like