You are on page 1of 14

Vision Research 41 (2001) 801– 814

www.elsevier.com/locate/visres

Modulation of spatial attention with unidirectional field motion:


an implication for the shift of the OKN beating field
Katsumi Watanabe *
Computation and Neural Systems, California Institute of Technology, Mail Code 139 -74, Pasadena, CA 91125, USA

Received 15 November 1999; received in revised form 10 August 2000

Abstract

During optokinetic nystagmus (OKN) the mean eye position of gaze (the beating field) shifts in the direction of the fast phases.
The function of this shift may be to re-orient the eyes in the direction of self-motion which optic flow implies (in-coming field).
This idea leads to the hypothesis that visual attention may be directed toward the In-coming field. In Experiment 1, subjects
detected a visual flash presented against unidirectional field motion. The OKN beating field was shifted toward the In-coming
field, and manual reaction times were shorter when the target appeared in the In-coming field. Experiment 2 revealed that this
In-coming field advantage occurred even when OKN (and thus the mean eye-position shift) was suppressed. Subsequent
experiments showed that the In-coming field advantage is not due to a local motion interaction (Experiment 3), survives subject’s
voluntary allocation of attention (Experiment 4), and develops over less than 320 ms after the onset of the motion field
(Experiment 5). These results suggest that unidirectional field motion tends to automatically shift visual attention toward the
In-coming field. © 2001 Elsevier Science Ltd. All rights reserved.

Keywords: Optokinetic nystagmus; Beating field; Attention; Reaction time; Motion field

1. Introduction 1981), in cats (Schweigart & Hoffmann, 1988; Schwei-


gart, 1995), and in rats (Meier & Dieringer, 1993;
A large moving visual field evokes a series of rhyth- Bähring, Meier, & Dieringer, 1994). A similar shift of
mic reflexive eye movements called optokinetic nystag- the ocular beating field is also observed with vestibular
mus (OKN) (Cohen, Matsuo, & Raphan, 1977; van Die nystagmus. The mean eye position of vestibular nystag-
& Collewijn, 1982; Carpenter, 1988; Collewijn, 1991; mus shifts in the direction of the head rotation (Melvill-
Leigh & Zee, 1991; Howard, 1993). Slow phases of Jones, 1964; Chun & Robinson, 1978; Roucoux,
OKN smoothly track the movement of the visual field, Crommelinck, Guerit, & Meulders, 1981; Crommelinck,
and fast phases (saccades) in the opposite direction Roucoux, & Veraart, 1982; Vidal, Berthoz, & Milan-
interrupt the slow phases. The purpose of OKN is to voye, 1982; Siegler, Israel, & Berthoz, 1998).
stabilize the retinal image to maintain the high visual Based on these observations of optokinetic and
acuity. vestibular nystagmus, some researchers interpreted the
Curiously, the average eye position of gaze during shift of the beating field of nystagmus as a goal-directed
OKN (the beating field) shifts in the direction of the involuntary response, or a reflexive orienting response
fast phases, namely, in the direction opposite to the toward a ‘center of interest’ (Melvill-Jones, 1964; Chun
visual field motion. This is true in man (Jung & Mitter- & Robinson, 1978; Crommelinck et al., 1982; Vidal et
maier, 1939; Miyoshi, Shirato, & Hiwatashi, 1978; al., 1982; Meier & Dieringer, 1993; Bähring et al., 1994;
Dubois & Collewijn, 1979; Abadi, Howard, & Ohmi, Schweigart, 1995; Siegler et al., 1998). The basic idea is
1999), in monkeys (Kubo, Igarashi, Jensen, & Hormick, that the fast phases of involuntary nystagmus may
strategically re-orient the eyes in the direction of self
motion so that the visual system can detect a target
* Tel.: +1-626-395-2362; fax: + 1-626-844-4514. more efficiently in the visual field toward which the
E-mail address: kw@caltech.edu (K. Watanabe). head and/or body move (in-coming field).

0042-6989/01/$ - see front matter © 2001 Elsevier Science Ltd. All rights reserved.
PII: S 0 0 4 2 - 6 9 8 9 ( 0 0 ) 0 0 3 0 1 - 1
802 K. Watanabe / Vision Research 41 (2001) 801–814

It has been suggested that OKN is mediated in rather short period after the onset of the background
part by cortical mechanisms (Miles, Kawano, & Opti- motion (Experiment 5). These results are interpreted
can, 1986; Howard & Simpson, 1989; Boussaoud, in terms of the automatic effect of unidirectional field
Ungerleider, & Desimone, 1992; Busettini, Masson, & motion on the distribution of spatial attention and its
Miles, 1996; Mestre & Masson, 1997; Watanabe, relation to the shift of the OKN beating field.
1998). Critically for the present study, the shift of the
beating field is under the control of the frontal eye
fields (FEF) in primates (Kubo et al., 1981; Ouchi, 2. Experiment 1: manual reaction time with the OKN
Igarashi, & Kubo, 1981; Fuchs & Mustari, 1993) and beating-field shift
by the FEF homologous structure in rats (dorsome-
dial shoulder of the prefrontal cortex; Meier and Experiment 1 was conducted to examine the effect
Dieringer (1993); Bähring et al. (1994)). Since the of the shift of the OKN beating field on manual
functional role of the FEF involves selective attention reaction time. Spatial selective attention at the loca-
(Robinson & Fuchs, 1968; Ouchi et al. 1981; Welch tion of a target reduces manual reaction time for the
& Stuteville, 1985; Liechnetz & Goldberg, 1988), the target (Posner, 1978, 1980; Posner & Cohen, 1984).
shift of the beating field may be understood as a shift Therefore, if the shift of the OKN beating field is
of spatial attention (Meier & Dieringer; Bähring et accompanied by a shift of spatial attention, the reac-
al.). tion time would be shorter when a visual target was
The purpose of the present study is to directly ex- in the In-coming field (e.g. a right target with left-
amine the attention hypothesis about the shift of the ward field motion).
OKN beating field in humans and to explore the na-
ture of such attentional modulation. In Experiment 1, 2.1. Method
it was observed that with the shift of the OKN beat-
ing field, manual reaction times for detecting a small 2.1.1. Subjects
visual target were shorter when the target occurred in Three male subjects (S1 the author, S2, and S3; age
the In-coming field. Then, Experiment 2 revealed that range from 24 to 28 yr) voluntarily participated in
this In-coming field advantage happened even when a the experiment. All subjects had normal or corrected-
stationary fixation stimulus was provided to suppress to-normal vision, reported no oculomotor and
OKN, and hence there was no shift of eye position. vestibular dysfunction, and had previously partici-
Further experiments showed that the In-coming field pated in other OKN experiments. Except for the au-
advantage is not due to a local motion interaction thor, the subjects were naı̈ve with regard to the
(Experiment 3), survives subject’s voluntary allocation purpose of the present study. Informed consent was
of attention (Experiment 4), and develops over a obtained from each subject before the experiment.

2.1.2. Stimuli
Stimuli were displayed on a CRT color monitor
(frame rate 75 Hz), controlled by a Silicon Graphics
Iris workstation, in an otherwise dark environment
(B0.01 cd/m2). The stimuli were composed of a
background and a target (Fig. 1). The background
was a random-dot pattern moving either leftward or
rightward. The speed of the background moving pat-
terns was 12.9 deg/s. The size of the patterns was
30× 30 deg2 in visual angle (consisting of 800 dots;
dot density 0.89 dot/deg2; individual dot size 0.03
deg). Luminance of the dots was 30 cd/m2 and back-
ground luminance was 0.02 cd/m2. As imaginary
targets cannot suppress OKN (Howard, Giaschi, &
Murasugi, 1989), the display employed in Experiment
1 reliably elicited OKN in all subjects. The target was
Fig. 1. Schematic of stimulus configuration: While the random-dot a small white square (0.5 deg; 30 cd/m2). It was pre-
background moved leftward or rightward, a bar (Experiments 2 – 5) sented for one frame (about 13.3 ms) at either 4 deg
or a small square (Experiment 1, not shown in the figure above) was
to the left or 4 deg to the right of the center of the
presented for about 13 ms at either the left or the right of the fixation
stimulus (or, of the display center in Experiment 1). In Experiments display. The delay of the target presentation from the
2–5, color and shape of the target were varied randomly. Note that onset of the background motion was randomized
the fixation stimulus was not presented in Experiment 1. from 3 to 5 s.
K. Watanabe / Vision Research 41 (2001) 801–814 803

2.1.3. Procedure
The subject observed the stimulus display binocularly
from a distance of 80 cm, with the head stabilized with
a chin and chest support. While trying to keep the eyes
stationary relative to the head and to the stimulus
display, and not to track any particular detail of the
background pattern (so-called ‘stare’ OKN; Honrubia
et al. (1968)), the subject detected the target as soon as
possible by pressing a mouse button. After the subject’s
responses, the background motion field was turned off.
For each combination of the background motion direc-
tion and the target location, 36 trials were repeated
randomly [background motion direction (2)× target lo-
cation (2)× repeat (36)= 144 trials]. The stimulus con-
ditions were classified into two categories: In-coming or
Out-going conditions. If the target was presented in the
visual field from which the background motion came, it
was called the In-coming condition. Otherwise, it was
defined as the Out-going condition. Each condition
contained 72 trials. A full experiment was conducted in
one session. For each subject, reaction times faster or
slower than the mean reaction time by two standard
deviations were excluded.

2.1.4. Eye mo6ement recording


Fig. 2. (a) Representative trace of eye position (S3) in Experiment 1. Horizontal positions of the subject’s right eye were
(b) Histograms of eye position for the leftward background motion recorded with an infrared eye-tracker (Permobil,
(top panel) and for the rightward background motion (bottom panel).
Ober2 ). Eye position data were sampled at a 100 Hz
The horizontal axis indicates eye position in visual angle. Negative
values mean that the eyes were deviated toward left from the primary rate. Data from the onset of the background motion to
position. The vertical axis shows the relative frequency of eye posi- the onset of the target for each trial were stored for
tion. The eye position (the OKN beating field) was clearly shifted in off-line analysis. At the beginning of a session the
the direction opposite to motion, namely, in the In-coming field. eye-tracker was calibrated with 10-deg horizontal
saccades.

2.2. Results

Fig. 2 shows a representative trace of eye position


(S3) and histograms of eye positions for all subjects.
The trace of eye position indicates the clear occurrence
of OKN. The histograms confirm that the mean posi-
tions of gaze tended to be shifted in the direction
opposite to the background motion (i.e. toward the
In-coming field). The reaction time results of Experi-
ment 1 are shown in Fig. 3. All three subjects re-
sponded more quickly when the visual target appeared
in the In-coming field than when it appeared in the
Out-going field (two-tailed t-test within each subject,
PB 0.05).

2.3. Discussion

The OKN beating field was shifted toward the In-


Fig. 3. The effect of the background motion on manual reaction time
coming field (Fig. 2), which is consistent with other
for detecting the target in Experiment 1 (in-coming condition versus
Out-going condition). The vertical axis shows the mean reaction time. OKN studies (Jung & Mittermaier, 1939; Miyoshi et
Each symbol represents data from an individual subject. T bars al., 1978; Dubois & Collewijn, 1979; Abadi et al., 1999).
indicate 1 SE. The new finding in Experiment 1 was that the manual
804 K. Watanabe / Vision Research 41 (2001) 801–814

reaction times were shorter when the visual target ap- stimulus, the results would be similar to those without
peared in the In-coming field (In-coming field advan- fixation. Additionally, Experiment 2 involved a manip-
tage). This result partially supports the attention ulation of task requirement. Four different tasks (de-
hypothesis for the shift of the OKN beating field be- tection, localization, color discrimination, and shape
cause it implies a link between spatial visual attention discrimination) were employed.
and the mean eye-position during OKN. However, the 3.1. Methods
results of Experiment 1 are equivocal with respect to
whether the shift of spatial attention is the cause or 3.1.1. Subjects
result of the beating-field shift. Experiment 2 was The same three subjects as in Experiment 1 took
aimed to tease apart these two alternatives. part in the experiment.

3.1.2. Stimuli
3. Experiment 2: In-coming field advantage without the The stimuli were identical to those used in Experi-
OKN beating-field shift ment 1 except for the following. A yellow fixation
cross (1.5 deg; 18.7 cd/m2) and a black disc (3.0 deg;
Experiment 1 demonstrated a positive correlation 0.02 cd/m2) were presented at the center of the display
between the shortening of reaction time and the shift throughout a session, both of which occluded the
of the OKN beating field. The strong version of the background motion (Fig. 1). A target was either a
attention hypothesis proposes that the optokinetic horizontal or vertical bar, the color of which was
stimulation alters the spatial distribution of attention either red or green, and the location of which was at
so that attention is directed toward the In-coming either the left or right of the fixation stimulus. The
field. This shift of attention may then result in the luminance of the target was 19 cd/m2 for both colors.
shift of the mean eye position of OKN in the direction The length and the width of the target were 1.2 and
opposite to the optokinetic stimulus motion. If this is 0.5 deg, respectively.
the case, even when OKN is suppressed with a fixation
3.1.3. Procedure
Each subject was instructed to firmly stare at the
fixation cross throughout each trial. The subject’s task
was to report either (1) the appearance of a target by
pressing a mouse button as quickly as possible (detec-
tion), (2) the target location (left or right) by pressing
one of the two mouse buttons as quickly and accu-
rately as possible (localization), (3) the target color
(red or green) by button pressing (color discrimina-
tion), or (4) the target shape (horizontal or vertical) by
button pressing (shape discrimination). For each com-
bination of the background motion direction and the
target type, nine trials were repeated randomly [back-
ground motion direction (2)× target location (2)×
target color (2)× target shape (2)× repeat (9)=144
trials]. The subject performed different tasks in sepa-
rate sessions. Trials were classified as in Experiment 1
(In-coming versus Out-going), resulting in 72 trials for
each condition. Reaction times faster or slower than
the mean reaction time by two standard deviations,
and trials with erroneous responses, were excluded
from analysis. The other procedures were the same as
those of Experiment 1.

3.2. Results

Fig. 4. (a) Representative trace of eye position (S3) in Experiment 2. The direction of the background motion did not
The large spike-like amplitudes were eye-blink artifacts, which were
bias the eye position of gaze (Fig. 4), confirming that
excluded for the eye-movement analysis. (b) Histograms of eye posi-
tion. There was no significant shift of eye position, showing that the the fixation stimulus and instruction successfully sup-
fixation stimulus and instruction in Experiment 2 were effective to pressed OKN in Experiment 2 (OKN suppression:
suppress OKN. Murphy, Kowler, and Steinman (1975), Barnes and
K. Watanabe / Vision Research 41 (2001) 801–814 805

Fig. 5. The effect of the background motion on manual reaction time for detecting (left top), localizing (right top), discriminating color (left
bottom), and discriminating shape of (right bottom) the target in Experiment 2. The vertical axis shows the mean reaction time. Each symbol
represents data from an individual subject. T bars indicate 1 SE.

Crombie (1985), Murasugi, Howard, and Ohmi (1986), of the attentional effect on manual reaction times
Pola and Wyatt (1993)). Nonetheless, the In-coming (Jonides, 1981). Hence, the shift of the OKN beating
field advantage was still observed with the detection field is not necessary for the In-coming field advantage
and the localization tasks (Fig. 5; two-tailed t-test to occur. This result strengthens the attention hypothe-
within each subject, P B 0.05). However, the reaction sis that visual attention is modulated by unidirectional
times for the color discrimination and the shape dis- field motion.
crimination did not show a significant In-coming field Interestingly, in Experiment 2, the In-coming field
advantage (Fig. 5; two-tailed t-test within each subject, advantage was found only with the orienting tasks
P \0.05). (detection and localization), but not with the identifica-
tion tasks (color- and shape-discrimination). If subjects
3.3. Discussion deliberately shifted visual attention, all the tasks should
have exhibited the In-coming field advantage. Since this
When the subject’s task was the detection or the was not the case, it is inferred that the attentional shift
localization of the target, the unidirectional motion toward the In-coming field may be caused mainly by an
field affected the spatial distribution of attention (as automatic mechanism for orienting responses.
measured with manual reaction time), suggesting that Researchers have suggested that the characteristics of
attention was directed toward the visual field from attention may differ for different task requirements,
which the motion came (In-coming field advantage). especially between orienting tasks (detection and local-
The effect size in the detection task of Experiment 2 ization) and identification tasks (e.g., color discrimina-
was almost the same as that in Experiment 1, which is tion and shape discrimination) (Nakayama &
also similar to the effect size observed in many studies Mackeben, 1989; Tanaka & Shimojo, 1996). Crudely
806 K. Watanabe / Vision Research 41 (2001) 801–814

speaking, identification may depend heavily on en- foveal region, whereas the pattern in the right visual
dogenous (voluntary, top-down) components of atten- field moves from foveal to peripheral region. In other
tion, whereas orientation may depend more on words, the results of Experiment 2 could be inter-
exogenous (automatic, bottom-up) components of at- preted as indicating that the reaction time to detect
tention. Much evidence now suggests that cortical or localize the visual target might be faster in the
mechanisms for localization (or orientation) and iden- peripheral-fovea background motion than in the
tification of ob-jects are distinct, and so are atten- fovea-peripheral background motion.
tional mechanisms. Starting from occipital cortex, the
ventral pathway to temporal cortex mediates identifi-
cation (‘what an object is’: Haxby et al. (1991), 4. Experiment 3: rejecting an artifact due to local
Grady et al. (1992)), whereas the dorsal pathway to motion interaction
the parietal region processes orientation and spatial
attention (‘where an object is’ and ‘where to look’: It is known that visual latencies for detecting a
Haxby et al. (1991), Grady et al. (1992), Corbetta, small moving target are shorter for motion towards
Shulman, Miezin, and Peterses (1995)). The results of the fovea than for motion away (Mateeff & Hohns-
Experiment 2 may be related to the exogenous-dorsal- bein, 1988; Mateeff et al., 1991). If the briefly pre-
orientation versus endogenous-ventral-identification sented target was perceived to move in the same
distinction. direction as the background motion (motion capture;
Overall, the results of Experiment 2 are consistent Ramachandran and Inada (1985), Ramachandran and
with the attention hypothesis about the shift of the Cavanagh (1987), Yo and Wilson (1992), Murakami
OKN beating field. But, there is a potential artifact and Shimojo (1993)), the results of Experiment 2
due to local motion interaction. With unidirectional might be simply due to some interactions of local
background motion, there is the inherent asymmetry motion processes. Experiment 3 was designed to ex-
of the direction of the background motion. For ex- amine whether motion interactions could be responsi-
ample, with rightward background motion, the pat- ble for the difference in manual reaction time. For
tern in the left visual field moves from peripheral to this purpose, the stimulus display was configured to
produce the same level of motion signal and interac-
tion as the display of Experiment 2, but not to pro-
duce any consistent OKN signal.

4.1. Method

4.1.1. Subjects
The same three subjects as in the previous experi-
ments took part in Experiment 3.

4.1.2. Stimuli
There were two conditions of the background mo-
tion in Experiment 3 (Fig. 6). In half of the trials, the
random-dot pattern in the left visual field moved
rightward and that in the right visual field moved
leftward (Inward condition). In the other half of the
trials, the random-dot pattern in the left visual field
moved leftward and that in the right visual field
moved rightward (Outward condition). Note that the
local stimulus relationship between the target and the
background was identical between the Inward condi-
tion and the In-coming condition in Experiment 2,
and between the outward condition and the Out-go-
ing condition in Experiment 2. The other stimulus
parameters were the same as those of Experiment 2.

Fig. 6. Schematic of stimulus configuration of Experiment 3. The


4.1.3. Procedure
random-dot background moved inward or outward. The other stimu- The procedures were identical to those of Experi-
lus configurations were identical to those of Experiment 2. ment 2.
K. Watanabe / Vision Research 41 (2001) 801–814 807

modulates spatial attention, as manifested in the differ-


ence in manual reaction time in the detection and
localization tasks (i.e. in orienting tasks).

5. Experiment 4: automaticity of the In-coming field


advantage

So far, the automatic nature of the In-coming field


advantage has been speculative from the fact that only
the orienting tasks led to the significant effect. Experi-
ment 4 was conducted to obtain a direct evidence for
the involuntary shift of visual attention in the In-com-
ing field advantage. With the combination of the In-
coming versus Out-going field paradigm (Experiment
2), a directional cue was provided in Experiment 4 so
that subjects would have knowledge regarding the
probable location of the target and could allocate atten-
tion endogenously (Posner, 1980; Jonides, 1981; Posner
& Cohen, 1984). If the In-coming field advantage is
caused by an automatic mechanism, it should be ob-
served even with an endogenous shift of attention.

5.1. Method

Fig. 7. (a) Representative trace of eye position (S3) in Experiment 3. 5.1.1. Subjects
The large spike-like amplitudes were eye-blink artifacts, which were The same three subjects took part in the experiment.
excluded for the eye-movement analysis. (b) Histograms of eye posi-
tion. With both the inward and the outward background motions, no
significant deviation of eye position from the primary position was
5.1.2. Stimuli
observed. The stimuli were almost the same as those used in
Experiment 2. However, a directional cue was added
for manipulating the subject’s endogenous attention.
4.2. Results One side of the horizontal bar of the fixation cross was
elongated by 0.75 deg for 1 s. The onset of the cue was
The eye movement recording did not show any randomized from 1.5 to 2.5 s before the target presenta-
systematic deviation of gaze while the subjects tion. In 80% of trials, the elongated side of the fixation
performed the task (Fig. 7). The results of Experiment cross and the target location were in the same side
3 are shown in Fig. 8. For all the tasks, the reaction (valid cue). In the remaining 20% of trials, the cue was
times did not differ significantly between the Inward in the opposite side of the target (invalid cue).
and the Outward conditions (two-tailed t-test within
each subject, P\ 0.1). 5.1.3. Procedure
The target location, color, and shape were deter-
4.3. Discussion mined randomly. There were four types of conditions:
(1) In-coming condition with a valid cue, (2) In-coming
The local motion fields in which the target was condition with an invalid cue, (3) Out-going condition
presented were identical between the In-coming condi- with a valid cue, and (4) Out-going condition with an
tion in Experiment 2 and the Inward condition in invalid cue. Each condition with a valid cue consisted
Experiment 3, and that between the Out-going condi- of 192 trials, while each condition with an invalid cue
tion in Experiment 2 and the Outward condition in consisted of 48 trials. Therefore, the total number of
Experiment 3. Yet, Experiment 3 did not produce con- trials was 480 trials (divided into two sessions). Since
sistent differences in the manual reaction time. These the In-coming field advantage was expected only for the
results clearly eliminate the possibility that the In-com- detection and the localization tasks, the color discrimi-
ing field advantage observed in Experiment 2 was due nation and the shape discrimination tasks were not
to a local interaction between the flashed target and the involved in Experiment 4. The detection task and the
background motion. Together, the results of Experi- localization task were performed in separate sessions.
ment 2 and 3 suggest that the translation motion field The subject was informed that 80% of the directional
808 K. Watanabe / Vision Research 41 (2001) 801–814

Fig. 8. The effect of the background motion on manual reaction time for detecting (left top), localizing (right top), discriminating color (left
bottom), and discriminating shape of (right bottom) the target in Experiment 3. The vertical axis shows the mean reaction time. Each symbol
represents data from an individual subject. T bars indicate 1 SE.

cues were valid. The other experimental procedures (Posner, 1980; Jonides, 1981; Posner & Cohen, 1984;
were identical to those of Experiment 2. Müller & Rabbit, 1989; Nakayama & Mackeben, 1989;
Remington, Johnston, & Yantis, 1992; Shimojo,
5.2. Results Tanaka, & Watanabe, 1996; Yantis, 1996). This is in
accordance with the claim that exogenous and endoge-
Fig. 9 shows the results of Experiment 4. There was nous attention show several different characteristics
a clear effect of cueing: the valid cue shortened manual and may be mediated by separate neural mechanisms
reaction times in all subjects under most of the condi- (Butter, 1987; Nakayama & Mackeben; Klein, King-
tions (one-tailed t-test, P B0.05; except for S3’s local- stone, & Pontefract, 1992; Klein, 1994; Robinson &
ization performance under the Out-going condition, Kertzman, 1995; Riggio & Kirsner, 1997; Briand, 1998;
where P = 0.21). The In-coming field advantage sur- Coull, Frith, Büchel, & Nobre, 2000).
vived even with the cueing effect (one-tailed t-test,
P B 0.05, except for S2’s detection and localization
performance with invalid cues, where P = 0.42 and 6. Experiment 5: time-course of the In-coming field
P = 0.21, respectively). advantage

5.3. Discussion In the previous experiments, subjects were exposed to


the background motion for 3–5 s prior to the presenta-
The results of Experiment 4 suggest that the In-com- tion of the target. The results have suggested that the
ing field advantage involves processes separate from translation motion field automatically shifts visual at-
endogenous (top-down) attention, and may be due to a tention toward the In-coming field for the detection and
process classified as exogenous (bottom-up) attention the localization tasks. Obviously, when the target is
K. Watanabe / Vision Research 41 (2001) 801–814 809

presented at the moment the background motion timings after the onset of the background motion (0, 40,
appears, there will be no difference in manual reaction 80, 160, 320 and 1280 ms after the onset).
time between the In-coming and the Out-going
conditions. This raised an interesting question: How 6.1.3. Procedure
does the In-coming field advantage develop over time? The target location, color and shape were determined
Experiment 5 addressed this question in order to at random. The subject’s task was to localize the target
further characterize the In-coming field advantage and as quickly and accurately as possible. For each combi-
to have an idea about possible underlying mechanisms. nation of the background motion condition (In-coming
or Out-going) and the target presentation timing, 40
6.1. Method trials were presented in random order [background
motion condition (2)× target timing (6)× repeat (40)=
6.1.1. Subjects 480 trials]. A full experiment was divided into four
The author (S1) and one naı̈ve subject (S4) partici- sessions. As a control experiment, the identical proce-
pated. Both had normal vision, and reported no oculo- dure was repeated by using the inward –outward back-
motor and vestibular dysfunction. ground motion display used in Experiment 3.

6.2. Results
6.1.2. Stimuli
The stimuli were almost identical to those used in The results of Experiment 5 are presented in Fig. 10.
Experiment 2, except that the target appeared at various When the unidirectional motion field was used, the
In-coming field advantage was observed in both sub-
jects. However, the reaction time difference between the
In-coming condition and the Out-going condition did
not show significance until some time after the onset of
the background motion (one-tailed t-test; PB0.01, af-
ter 160 ms for S1; after 320 ms for S4). There was no
significant difference between the Inward versus the
Outward conditions, irrespective of the target timing
(one-tailed t-test; P\ 0.1). In general, the reaction time
became shorter as the duration of the background
increased (one-way ANOVA, F\ 3, PB 0.001 for both
subjects and for all conditions), probably reflecting a
standard foreperiod effect (Mowrer, 1940; Bertelson,
1967).
6.3. Discussion

The In-coming field advantage did develop over time.


It took about 160 –320 ms for the effect to reach the
significant level. These rather short latencies suggest
that visually-induced self-motion perception (vection)
may not be responsible for the In-coming field
advantage.
Although the magnitude of vection was not mea-
sured, it was quite unlikely that vection occurred under
the stimulus conditions used in all the present experi-
ments. The stimulus visual field was restricted to 30-deg
of the central visual field in all experiments. The dura-
tion of the background motion was 5 s at most (Exper-
iment 1–4). The significant In-coming field advantage
was achieved with the background motion duration less
Fig. 9. The effect of the background motion on manual reaction time than 320 ms (Experiment 5). These values are known to
for the detection task (left column) and for the localization task (right be inefficient to induce a strong sensation of self-motion
column) in Experiment 4. The vertical axis shows the mean reaction (Brandt, Dichgans, & Koening, 1973; Berthoz, Pavard,
time. Each symbol represents data from an individual subject. T bars
indicate 1 SE. Even with the presence of the cueing effect (endoge-
& Young, 1975; Johansson, 1977; Dichgans & Brandt,
nous shift of attention), the In-coming field advantage [out-going 1978; Wong & Frost, 1987; Telford & Frost, 1993;
(white square) − In-coming (black square)] tended to be preserved. Kennedy, Hettinger, Harm, Ordy, & Dunlap, 1996).
810 K. Watanabe / Vision Research 41 (2001) 801–814

Fig. 10. The effect of the duration of the background motion on the In-coming field advantage (Experiment 5). The top panels show the results
with the incoming– outgoing unidirectional motion. The bottom panels show the results with the inward– outward motion. The vertical axis shows
the mean reaction time. T bars indicate 1 SE. The In-coming field advantage is conspicuous with the unidirectional motion field and develops over
time.

Moreover, when asked after the experiments, no subject 7. General discussion


reported a compelling sensation of self-motion. There-
fore, the attentional shift underlying the In-coming field The initial motivation of the present study was to
advantage may occur automatically before conscious examine the relationship between the shift of the OKN
experience of self-motion. This parallels the suggestions beating field and spatial visual attention. The results
that the threshold for the perception of self-motion may were consistent with the idea that the subject’s attention
be higher than the threshold for automatic orienting is shifted along with the shift of the OKN beating field
response such as postural reflex (body sway) induced by (Experiment 1). Moreover, the present study directly
a visual field motion (Stoffregen, 1985), and that the demonstrated the effect of unidirectional field motion
latency for self-motion experience may be also larger on the distribution of spatial attention. Spatial atten-
than the latency for postural reflex (Previc & Mullin, tion, measured as manual reaction times, tends to be
1991). The reflexive postural adjustment and the atten- shifted toward the In-coming field (i.e. toward the
tional shift observed in the present study may share the direction from which the motion originates) without the
same mechanism for motion analysis1. large shift of the mean position of gaze (Experiment 2
and Experiment 3).
Optic flow contains rich information about the move-
1
Thilo, Guerraz, Bronstein, and Gresty (2000) have recently shown ment of the observer and the three-dimensional envi-
that self-motion perception due to a prolonged observation of an ronmental structure (Gibson, 1950, 1954; Gibson,
optokinetic motion field enhances the shift of the OKN beating field.
Their effect may be explained by another component besides the
Olum, & Rosenblatt, 1955). Unidirectional field mo-
automatic shift of visual attention, presumably an additional compo- tion, as one type of optic flow, implies that the observ-
nent of endogenous attention. er’s head and/or body translates in the direction
K. Watanabe / Vision Research 41 (2001) 801–814 811

opposite to the field motion. The system of spatial 1988; Goldreich, Krauzlis, & Lisberger, 1992; Schwartz
attention may utilize such information to enhance ori- & Lisberger, 1994). For example, some cells in the
enting responses for stimuli that appear suddenly in rostral superior colliculus in monkeys become active
the visual field toward which the head and/or body both during smooth pursuit and fixation (Munoz &
move. Wultz, 1993a,b; Krauzlis, Basso, & Wultz, 1997).
The attention hypothesis might be related to other The other related phenomenon is the residual
previous findings which concerned other types of optic smooth eye movement under OKN suppression (Tam-
flow. During simulated forward motion with a radial minga & Collewijn, 1981; Wyatt & Pola, 1984;
optic flow stimulus, the distribution of gaze tends to be Collewijn & Tamminga, 1986; Waespe & Schwarz,
clustered near the focus of expansion (Lappe, Pekel, & 1986, 1987; van den Berg & Collewijn, 1987; Pola,
Hoffmann, 1998; Niemann, Lappe, Büscher, & Hoff- Wyatt, & Lustgarten, 1995; Wyatt, Pola, Lustgarten, &
mann, 1999). Since the heading judgment becomes Aksionoff, 1995). When the optokinetic motion field
poor with increasing angle between gaze and the focus changes in a sinewave fashion, small residual smooth
of radial flow (Warren & Kurtz, 1992), this gaze strat- eye movements, which are roughly counter-phase to
egy may also reflect an involuntary attentional shift, the field motion (i.e., toward the in-coming field), can
which would reduce the error in the heading judgment. be observed (Wyatt & PolaPola, Wyatt, & Lustgarten,
Similarly, during driving, driver’s gaze tends to be 1992). These residual eye movements are enhanced
directed at informative places in scene (e.g. ‘tangent with a foveally stabilized target and background mo-
point’ on the inside of a road curve; Land (1992), tion (open-loop condition) and depends critically on
Land & Lee (1994)). These strategic shifts of eye stimulus predictability (Wyatt et al.). Although the size
movements seem to occur automatically and might of eye movements are quite small, the residual eye
reflect the utilization of global motion signal by the movements could be detectable even with unidirec-
attention system. Thus the modulation of attention by tional field motion (Waespe & Schwarz, 1987)2. Based
a motion field may not be restricted to translation on extensive explorations of this phenomenon, Pola et
motion, but could be generalized to various other al. suggested that the OKN suppression with a station-
types of optic flow patterns. However, further investi- ary fixation stimulus may be mediated by automatic
gation is required for the issue of generality. mechanisms that process relative motion between the
At this point, it is unavoidable that any explanation fixation target and the motion field and depend on the
about the underlying mechanism of the In-coming field subject’s attention. This tendency for the eyes to devi-
advantage is speculative. However, there are two phe- ate into the In-coming field under the OKN suppres-
nomena, which seem to be related to the In-coming sion may be closely related to the shift of the OKN
field advantage observed in the present experiments. beating field.
One is the directional asymmetry of visual attention The suggested automaticity of the residual smooth
during smooth pursuit eye movements (Tanaka, eye movements (Pola et al., 1995) parallels the results
Yoshida, & Fukushima, 1998; van Donkelaar, 1999). of Experiment 4 which showed that the In-coming field
During smooth pursuit eye movements, saccades in the advantage survives the subject’s endogenous allocation
same direction as the preceding pursuit have shorter of attention. Also, the dependency of the residual eye
latencies than those in the opposite direction (Tanaka movements on stimulus predictability (Wyatt et al.,
et al., 1998). Also, manual reaction times are shorter 1995) parallels that of smooth pursuit (Yasui &
for stimuli flashed ahead of the pursuit target than for Young, 1984) and, presumably, the results of Experi-
those presented behind (van Donkelaar, 1999). Thus, ment 5. It took, at least, 150 ms for the In-coming
the distribution of visual attention is asymmetric along field advantage to be significant, suggesting that this
the line of the eye movement. If one considers the duration of predictable motion stimulation is neces-
retinal event during smooth pursuit eye movements sary. The shared characteristics among smooth pursuit,
against a structured field, it is identical to the retinal active fixation, the residual eye movement during
event during active fixation against an optokinetic mo- OKN, the shift of the OKN beating field, and the
tion field. Plus, the attentional shift during smooth In-coming field advantage are suggestive of the exis-
pursuit and that during active fixation (in the present tence of a mechanism which processes relative motion
experiment) are in the same direction, namely, toward signals in the retinal coordinate and quickly regulates
the In-coming field. These similarities imply a common various orienting responses.
mechanism for active fixation and smooth pursuit. In
fact, recent studies have shown that the smooth-pur-
2
suit system and the fixation system may share common In fact, a close examination of Fig. 4 would suggest that there
might be a slight tendency of deviation toward the in-coming field.
mechanisms to some extent (Munoz & Wultz, 1993a,b; This could be due to the residual slow eye movements. However, it is
Tam & Ono, 1994; Krauzlis & Miles, 1996), although quite unlikely that such small deviations of gaze were the main cause
these systems are not identical (Luebke & Robinson, of the in-coming field advantage in Experiment 2.
812 K. Watanabe / Vision Research 41 (2001) 801–814

Finally, the In-coming field advantage was found Berthoz, A., Pavard, B., & Young, L. R. (1975). Perception of linear
horizontal self-motion induced by peripheral vision (linear-vec-
only for the detection task and the localization task (i.e.
tion): basic characteristics and visual-vestibular interactions. Ex-
orienting tasks), not for the color discrimination task perimental Brain Research, 23, 471– 489.
and the shape discrimination task (i.e. identification Boussaoud, D., Ungerleider, L., & Desimone, R. (1992). Subcortical
tasks). Although a clear conclusion warrants more in- connections of visual areas MST and FST in macaques. Visual
vestigation, it seems to be consistent with the idea that Neuroscience, 9, 291– 302.
Brandt, T., Dichgans, J., & Koening, E. (1973). Differential effects of
identification depends mainly on endogenous compo-
central versus peripheral vision on egocentric and exocentric
nents of attention whereas orientation depends mainly motion perception. Experimental Brain Research, 16, 476–491.
on exogenous components of attention (Nakayama & Briand, K. A. (1998). Feature integration and spatial attention: more
Mackeben, 1989). However, if the In-coming field ad- evidence of a dissociation between endogenous and exogenous
vantage is a form of exogenous attention, the sustained orienting. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception
and Performance, 24, 1243– 1256.
nature of it (Experiment 5) is puzzling. This is because, Busettini, C., Masson, G. S., & Miles, F. A. (1996). A role for
it has been suggested that endogenous attention is slow stereoscopic depth cues in the rapid visual stabilization of the
and sustained, and exogenous attention is fast and eyes. Nature, 380, 342– 345.
transient. (Nakayama & Mackeben, 1989). Therefore, Butter, C. M. (1987). Varieties of attention and disturbances of
attention: a neurophysiological analysis. In M. Jeannerod, Neuro-
further investigation should be conducted to examine
physiological and neuropsychological aspects of spatial neglect (pp.
the relationship between other forms of exogenous at- 1 – 23). Amsterdam: Elsevier.
tention and the In-coming field advantage. Carpenter, R. H. S. (1988). Mo6ements of the eyes. London: Pion.
Cohen, B., Matsuo, V., & Raphan, T. (1977). Quantitative analysis of
the velocity characteristics of optokinetic nystagmus and optoki-
netic afternystagmus. Journal of Physiology, 270, 321– 344.
8. Conclusion Collewijn, H. (1991). The optokinetic contribution. In R. H. S.
Carpenter, Vision and 6isual dysfunction. Eye mo6ements, vol. 8
The present study demonstrated that unidirectional (pp. 45 – 70). London: Macmillan.
Collewijn, H., & Tamminga, E. P. (1986). Human fixation and
field motion tends to shift visual attention in the visual pursuit in normal and open-loop conditions: effects of central and
field from which the motion field originates (In-coming peripheral retinal targets. Journal of Physiology, 379, 109–129.
field advantage). The In-coming field advantage seems Chun, K. S., & Robinson, D. A. (1978). A model of quick phase
to be based on a relatively quick automatic mechanism, generation in the vestibular-ocular reflex. Biological Cybernetics,
28, 209– 221.
which analyzes motion field and modulates spatial at-
Corbetta, M., Shulman, G., Miezin, F., & Peterses, S. (1995). Supe-
tention accordingly. Such a mechanism might allow a rior parietal cortex activation during spatial attention shifts and
faster orienting response for the target appeared in the visual feature conjunction. Science, 270, 802– 805.
direction of self-motion and thus provide behavioral Coull, J. T., Frith, C. D., Büchel, C., & Nobre, A. C. (2000).
benefits. Orienting attention in time: behavioural and neuroanatomical
distinction between exogenous and endogenous shifts. Neuropsy-
chologia, 38, 808– 819.
Crommelinck, M., Roucoux, A., & Veraart, C. (1982). The relation
of neck muscles activity to horizontal eye position in the alert cat.
Acknowledgements
II. Head free. In A. Roucoux, & M. Crommelinck, Physiological
and pathological aspects of eye mo6ements (pp. 379– 398). Hague:
Thanks are due to Romi Nijhawan and Rory Sayres Junk.
for comments on an earlier version of the manuscript Dichgans, J., & Brandt, T. (1978). Visual-vestibular interaction and
and two anonymous reviewers for valuable comments. motion perception and postural control. In R. Held, H. W.
Leibowitz, & H.-L. Teuber, Handbook of sensory physiology.
This work was supported by the Sloan Foundation Perception, vol. 8 (pp. 755– 804). Berlin: Springer.
Fellowship for Theoretical Neurobiology. Dubois, M. F. W., & Collewijn, H. (1979). Optokinetic reactions in
man elicited by localised retinal motion stimuli. Vision Research,
19, 1105– 1115.
Fuchs, A. F., & Mustari, M. J. (1993). The optokinetic response in
References primates and its possible neuronal substrate. In F. A. Miles, & J.
Wallman, Visual motion and its role in the stabilization of gaze (pp.
Abadi, R. V., Howard, I. P., & Ohmi, M. (1999). Gaze orientation 343– 369). Amsterdam: Elsevier.
during full-field and peripheral field passive optokinesis. Oph- Gibson, J. J. (1950). Perception of the 6isual world. Boston: Houghton
thalmic and Physiological Optics, 19, 261–265. Mifflin.
Bähring, R., Meier, R. K., & Dieringer, N. (1994). Unilateral abla- Gibson, J. J. (1954). The visual perception of objective motion and
tion of the frontal eye field of the rat affects the beating field of subjective movement. Psychological Re6iew, 61, 304– 314.
ocular nystagmus. Experimental Brain Research, 98, 391– 400. Gibson, J. J., Olum, P., & Rosenblatt, F. (1955). Parallax and
Barnes, G. R., & Crombie, J. W. (1985). The interaction of conflict- perspective during aircraft landings. American Journal of Psychol-
ing retinal motion stimuli in oculomotor control. Experimental ogy, 68, 372– 385.
Brain Research, 59, 548–558. Goldreich, D., Krauzlis, R. J., & Lisberger, S. G. (1992). Effect of
Bertelson, P. (1967). The time course of preparation. Quarterly changing feedback delay on spontaneous oscillation in smooth
Journal of Experimental Psychology, 19, 272–279. pursuit eye movements of monkeys. Journal of Neurophysiology,
67, 625– 638.
K. Watanabe / Vision Research 41 (2001) 801–814 813

Grady, C. L., Haxby, J. V., Horwitz, B., Schapiro, M. B., Rapoport, Philadelphia: Davis.
S. I., Ungerleider, L. G., Mishkin, M., Carson, R. E., & Herscov- Liechnetz, G. R., & Goldberg, M. E. (1988). Higher centers con-
itch, O. (1992). Dissociation of object and spatial vision in human cerned with eye movement and visual attention: cerebral cortex
extrastriate cortex: age-related changes in activation of regional and thalamus. In J. A. Büttner-Ennever, Neuroanatomy of the
cerebral blood flow measured with [15O] water and positron oculomotor system (pp. 365– 429). Amsterdam: Elsevier.
emission tomography. Journal of Cogniti6e Neuroscience, 4, 23 – Mateeff, S., & Hohnsbein, J. (1988). Perceptual latencies are shorter
34. for motion towards the fovea than for motion away. Vision
Haxby, J. V., Grady, C. L., Horwitz, B., Ungerleider, L. G., Mishkin, Research, 28, 711– 719.
M., Carson, R. E., Herscovitch, O., Schapiro, M. B., & Rapoport, Mateeff, S., Yakimoff, N., Hohnsbein, J., Ehrenstein, W. H., Bohda-
S. I. (1991). Dissociation of object and spatial visual processing necky, Z., & Radil, T. (1991). Selective directional sensitivity in
pathways in human extrastriate cortex. Proceedings of the Na- visual motion perception. Vision Research, 31, 131– 138.
tional Academy of Science USA, 88, 1621–1625. Meier, R. K., & Dieringer, N. (1993). The role of compensatory eye
Honrubia, V., Downey, W. L., Mitchell, D. P., & Ward, P. H. (1968). and head movements in the rat for image stabilization and gaze
Experimental studies on optokinetic nystagmus. II. Normal hu- orientation. Experimental Brain Research, 96, 54 – 64.
mans. Acta Otolaryngologica, 65, 441–448. Melvill-Jones, G. (1964). Predominance of anti-compensatory oculo-
Howard, I. P. (1993). The optokinetic system. In J. A. Sharpe, & H. motor response during rapid head rotation. A6iation, Space and
O. Barber, The 6estibular-ocular reflex and 6ertigo (pp. 163– 184). En6ironmental Medicine, 35, 956– 968.
New York: Raven Press. Mestre, D. R., & Masson, G. S. (1997). Ocular responses to motion
Howard, I. P., Giaschi, D., & Murasugi, C. M. (1989). Suppression of parallax stimuli: the role of perceptual and attentional factors.
OKN and VOR by afterimages and imaginary objects. Experi- Vision Research, 37, 1627– 1641.
mental Brain Research, 75, 139–145. Miles, F. A., Kawano, K., & Optican, L. (1986). Short-latency ocular
Howard, I. P., & Simpson, W. A. (1989). Human optokinetic nystag- following responses of monkey. I. Dependence on temporalspatial
mus is linked to the stereoscopic system. Experimental Brain properties of visual input. Journal of Neurophysiology, 56, 1321–
Research, 78, 309– 314. 1354.
Johansson, G. H. (1977). Studies on visual perception of locomotion. Miyoshi, T., Shirato, M., & Hiwatashi, S. (1978). Foveal and periph-
Perception, 6, 365– 376. eral vision in optokinetic nystagmus. In J. D. Hood, Vestibular
Jonides, J. (1981). Voluntary versus automatic control over the mechanisms in health and disease (pp. 294– 301). London: Aca-
mind’s eye. In J. B. Long, & A. D. Baddeley, Attention and demic Press.
performance IX (pp. 187–207). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Mowrer, O. H. (1940). Preparatory set (expectancy): some methods of
Jung, G., & Mittermaier, R. (1939). Zur objektiven Registrierung und measurement. Psychological Re6iew Monographs, 52 (Whole no.
Analyse verschiedener Nystagmusformen: Vestibulärer, op- 233).
tokinetischer und spontaner Nystagmus in ihren Wechsel- Müller, H. J., & Rabbit, P. M. A. (1989). Reflexive and voluntary
beiziehungen. Archi6 für Ohren-Nasen-Kehlkopf-Heilkunde, 146, orienting of visual attention: time course of activation and resis-
410– 439. tance to interruption. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human
Perception and Performance, 15, 315– 330.
Kennedy, R. S., Hettinger, L. J., Harm, D. L., Ordy, J. M., &
Munoz, D. P., & Wultz, R. H. (1993a). Fixation cells in monkey
Dunlap, W. P. (1996). Psychophysical scaling of circular vection
superior colliculus. I. Characteristics of cell discharge. Journal of
(CV) produced by optokinetic (OKN) motion: individual differ-
Neurophysiology, 70, 559– 575.
ences and effects of practice. Journal of Vestibular Research, 6,
Munoz, D. P., & Wultz, R. H. (1993b). Fixation cells in monkey
331– 341.
superior colliculus. II. Reversible activation and deactivation.
Klein, R. M. (1994). Perceptual-motor expectancies interact with
Journal of Neurophysiology, 70, 576– 589.
covert visual orienting under conditions of endogenous but not
Murakami, I., & Shimojo, S. (1993). Motion capture changes to
exogenous control. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology,
induced motion at higher luminance contrasts, smaller eccentric-
48, 167– 181.
ities, and larger inducer sizes. Vision Research, 33, 2091–2107.
Klein, R. M., Kingstone, A., & Pontefract, A. (1992). Orienting of
Murasugi, C. M., Howard, I. P., & Ohmi, M. (1986). Optokinetic
visual attention. In K. Rayner, Eye mo6ements and 6isual cogni-
nystagmus: the effects of stationary edges, alone and in combina-
tion: scene perception and reading (pp. 46–65). New York:
tion with central occlusion. Vision Research, 26, 1155– 1162.
Springer.
Murphy, B. J., Kowler, E., & Steinman, R. M. (1975). Slow oculomo-
Krauzlis, R. J., Basso, M. A., & Wultz, R. H. (1997). Shared motor
tor control in the presence of moving backgrounds. Vision Re-
error for multiple eye movements. Science, 276, 1693–1695.
search, 15, 1236– 1268.
Krauzlis, R. J., & Miles, F. A. (1996). Initiation of saccades during
Nakayama, K., & Mackeben, M. (1989). Sustained and transient
fixation or pursuit: evidence in humans for a single mechanism. components of focal visual attention. Vision Research, 29, 1631–
Journal of Neurophysiology, 76, 4175–4179. 1647.
Kubo, T., Igarashi, M., Jensen, D. W., & Hormick, J. L. (1981). Niemann, T., Lappe, M., Büscher, A., & Hoffmann, K.-P. (1999).
Eye-head coordination during optokinetic stimulation in squirrel Ocular responses to radial optic flow and single accelerated
monkeys. Annals of Otology, Rhinology and Laryngology, 90, targets in humans. Vision Research, 39, 1359– 1371.
85– 88. Ouchi, T., Igarashi, M., & Kubo, T. (1981). Effect of frontal-eye-field
Land, M. F. (1992). Predictable eye-head coordination during driv- lesion on eye-hand coordination in squirrel monkeys. Annals of
ing. Nature, 24, 318–320. the New York Academy of Sciences, 374, 656– 673.
Land, M. F., & Lee, D. N. (1994). Where we look when we steer. Pola, J., & Wyatt, H. J. (1993). The role of attention and cognitive
Nature, 30, 742– 744. processes. In F. A. Miles, & J. Wallman, Visual motion and its role
Lappe, M., Pekel, M., & Hoffmann, K.-P. (1998). Optokinetic eye in the stabilization of gaze (pp. 371– 392). Amsterdam: Elsevier.
movements elicited by radial optic flow in the macaque monkey. Pola, J., Wyatt, H. J., & Lustgarten, M. (1992). Suppression of
Journal of Neurophysiology, 79, 1461–1480. optokinesis by a stabilized target: effects of instruction and stimu-
Luebke, A. E., & Robinson, D. A. (1988). Transition dynamics lus frequency. Perception and Psychophysics, 52, 186– 200.
between pursuit and fixation suggest different systems. Vision Pola, J., Wyatt, H. J., & Lustgarten, M. (1995). Visual fixation of a
Research, 28, 941– 946. target and suppression of optokinetic nystagmus: effects of vary-
Leigh, R. J., & Zee, D. S. (1991). The neurology of eye mo6ements. ing target feedback. Vision Research, 35, 1079– 1087.
814 K. Watanabe / Vision Research 41 (2001) 801–814

Posner, M. I. (1978). Chronometric explorations of mind. Hillsdale, reveals dissociation between two visual functions. Vision Re-
NJ: Erlbaum. search, 36, 2125– 2140.
Posner, M. I. (1980). Orienting of attention. Quarterly Journal of Tanaka, M., Yoshida, T., & Fukushima, K. (1998). Latency of
Experimental Psychology, 32, 3–25. saccades during smooth-pursuit eye movement in man – direc-
Posner, M. I., & Cohen, Y. (1984). Components of visual orienting. tional asymmetries. Experimental Brain Research, 121, 92–98.
In H. Bouma, & D. G. Bouwhuis, Attention and performance X Telford, L., & Frost, B. J. (1993). Factors affecting the onset and
(pp. 531– 556). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. magnitude of linear vection. Perception and Psychophysics, 53,
Previc, F. H., & Mullin, T. J. (1991). A comparison of the latencies 682– 692.
of visually induced postural change and self-motion perception. Thilo, K. V., Guerraz, M., Bronstein, A. M., & Gresty, M. A. (2000).
Journal of Vestibular Research, 1, 317–323. Changes in horizontal oculomotor behaviour coincide with a shift
Ramachandran, V. S., & Cavanagh, P. (1987). Motion capture an- in visual motion perception. Neuroreport, 11, 1987– 1990.
isotropy. Vision Research, 27, 97–106. van Die, G., & Collewijn, H. (1982). Optokinetic nystagmus in man.
Ramachandran, V. S., & Inada, V. (1985). Spatial phase and fre- Human Neurobiology, 1, 111– 119.
quency in motion capture of random-dot patterns. Spatial Vision, van den Berg, A. V., & Collewijn, H. (1987). Voluntary smooth eye
1, 57 – 67. movements with foveally stabilized targets. Experimental Brain
Remington, R. W., Johnston, J. C., & Yantis, S. (1992). Involuntary Research, 68, 195– 204.
attentional capture by abrupt onset. Perception and Psycho- van Donkelaar, P. (1999). Spatiotemporal modulation of attention
physics, 51, 279– 290. during smooth pursuit eye movements. Neuroreport, 10, 2523–
Riggio, L., & Kirsner, K. (1997). The relationship between central 2526.
cues and peripheral cues in covert visual orientation. Perception Vidal, P. P., Berthoz, A., & Milanvoye, M. (1982). Difference be-
and Psychophysics, 59, 885–899. tween eye closure and visual stabilization in the control of the
Robinson, D. A., & Fuchs, A. F. (1968). Eye movements evoked by posture in man. A6iation, Space and En6ironmental Medicine, 53,
stimulation of frontal eye field. Journal of Neurophysiology, 32,
1966– 1967.
637– 648.
Waespe, W., & Schwarz, U. (1986). Characteristics of eye velocity
Robinson, D. L., & Kertzman, C. (1995). Covert orienting of atten-
storage during periods of suppression and reversal of eye velocity
tion in macaques. III. Contributions of the superior colliculus.
in monkeys. Experimental Brain Research, 65, 49 – 58.
Journal of Neurophysiology, 74, 713–721.
Waespe, W., & Schwarz, U. (1987). Slow eye movements induced by
Roucoux, A., Crommelinck, M., Guerit, J. M., & Meulders, M.
apparent target motion in monkey. Experimental Brain Research,
(1981). Two models of eye-head coordination and the role of the
67, 433– 435.
vestibulo-ocular-reflex in these two strategies. In A. F. Fuchs, &
Warren, W. H., & Kurtz, K. J. (1992). The role of central and
W. Becker, Progress in oculomotor research (pp. 309–315). New
peripheral vision in perceiving the direction of self-motion. Per-
York: Elsevier.
ception and Psychophysics, 51, 443– 454.
Schwartz, J. D., & Lisberger, S. G. (1994). Initial tracking conditions
Watanabe, K. (1998). Optokinetic nystagmus with spontaneous rever-
modulate the gain of visuo-motor transmission for smooth pursuit
eye movements in monkeys. Visual Neuroscience, 11, 411– 424. sal of transparent motion perception. Experimental Brain Re-
Schweigart, G. (1995). Gaze shift during optokinetic stimulation in search, 129, 156– 160.
head free cats. Neuroscience Letters, 183, 124–126. Welch, K., & Stuteville, P. (1985). Experimental production of unilat-
Schweigart, G., & Hoffmann, K.-P. (1988). Optokinetic eye and head eral neglect in monkeys. Brain, 81, 341– 347.
movements in the unrestrained cat. Beha6ioral Brain Research, 31, Wong, S. C. P., & Frost, B. F. (1987). Subjective motion and
121– 129. acceleration induced by the movement of the observer’s entire
Shimojo, S., Tanaka, Y., & Watanabe, K. (1996). Stimulus-driven visual field. Perception and Psychophysics, 24, 115– 120.
facilitation and inhibition of visual information processing in Wyatt, H. J., & Pola, J. (1984). A mechanism for suppression of
environmental and retinotopic representations of space. Cogniti6e optokinesis. Vision Research, 24, 1931– 1945.
Brain Research, 5, 11–21. Wyatt, H. J., Pola, J., Lustgarten, M., & Aksionoff, E. (1995).
Siegler, I., Israel, I., & Berthoz, A. (1998). Shift of the beating field of Optokinetic nystagmus (OKN) suppression by fixation of a stabi-
vestibular nystagmus: an orientation strategy? Neuroscience Let- lized target: the effect of OKN-stimulus predictability. Vision
ters, 254, 94 – 96. Research, 35, 2903– 2910.
Stoffregen, T. A. (1985). Flow structure versus retinal location in the Yantis, S. (1996). Attentional capture in vision. In A. F. Kramer, M.
optical control of stance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: G. H. Coles, & G. D. Logan, Con6erging operations in the study of
Human Perception and Performance, 11, 554–565. 6isual selecti6e attention (pp. 45 – 76). Washington, DC: American
Tam, W. A. J., & Ono, H. (1994). Fixation disengagement and Psychological Association.
eye-movement latency. Perception and Psychophysics, 56, 251– Yasui, S., & Young, L. R. (1984). On the predictive control of foveal
260. eye tracking and slow phases of optokinetic and vestibular nystag-
Tamminga, G. A., & Collewijn, H. (1981). The effect of a structured mus. Journal of Physiology, 347, 17 – 33.
background on human oculomotor pursuit of visual targets. Yo, C., & Wilson, H. R. (1992). Moving two-dimensional patterns
Documenta Ophthalmologica Proceedings Series, 30, 668–683. can capture the perceived directions of lower or higher spatial
Tanaka, Y., & Shimojo, S. (1996). Location vs feature: reaction time frequency gratings. Vision Research, 32, 1263– 1269.

You might also like