You are on page 1of 13

c o r t e x 1 2 6 ( 2 0 2 0 ) 3 5 5 e3 6 7

Available online at www.sciencedirect.com

ScienceDirect
Journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/cortex

Single Case Report

Impaired spatial processing in visual perception,


imagery and art-making following parieto-occipital
infarcts

Jennifer A. Foley a,b,*, Isik Bayraktar e, Harpreet Hyare c,d and


Diana Caine a
a
Neuropsychology, National Hospital for Neurology and Neurosurgery, London, UK
b
UCL Institute of Neurology, London, UK
c
Department of Brain Repair and Rehabilitation, UCL Institute of Neurology, London, UK
d
Imaging Department, UCLH NHS Trust, London, UK
e
c/o National Hospital for Neurology & Neurosurgery, UK

article info abstract

Article history: Visual imagery, like vision as such, is widely thought to be supported by two distinct and
Received 14 June 2019 dissociable processing streams, dedicated to object representation and spatial analysis
Reviewed 30 September 2019 respectively. However, this simple dichotomy has been contested, with recent studies
Revised 27 November 2019 suggesting that impairments in perception-for-action and visuo-spatial imagery may
Accepted 22 January 2020 reflect a more general deficit in space-based attention. Although previous studies have
Published online 20 February 2020 revealed the impact of brain damage on artistic expression, few have examined the impact
on artistic expression in terms of the perceptual and spatial components of either visual
Keywords: processing or visual imagery. Here we present the case of an artist whose artistic
Visual perception expression was dramatically affected following devastating posterior brain damage. Of
Spatial processing particular interest, we demonstrate how these changes relate to impairments in inte-
Imagery grating and aligning different spatial features in both visual processing and visual imagery,
Simultanagnosia suggestive of a general simultanagnosia not previously described.
Art © 2020 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

contentious. Changes in artistic expression following brain


1. Introduction injury can be exceptionally illuminating of deficits in cognitive
functioning and their neuroanatomical underpinnings
Damage to posterior brain regions has been shown to disrupt
(Annoni, Devuyst, Carota, Bruggimann, & Bogousslavsky,
both visual processing and visual imagery, with evidence of € zner & Hennerici, 2007; Blanke & Pasqualini, 2012;
2005; Ba
dissociable deficits in perceptual and spatial abilities, zina, Botez, & Kaufman, 1985; Chatterjee,
Botez, Olivier, Ve
although the precise nature of these deficits remains

* Corresponding author. Department of Neuropsychology, Box 37, National Hospital for Neurology and Neurosurgery, Queen Square,
London, WC1N 3BG, UK.
E-mail address: Jennifer.Foley@ucl.ac.uk (J.A. Foley).
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2020.01.023
0010-9452/© 2020 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
356 c o r t e x 1 2 6 ( 2 0 2 0 ) 3 5 5 e3 6 7

2004; Cummings & Zarit, 1987; Finney & Heilman, 2007; pathways, as vision-for-perception and vision-for-action
Kaczmarek, 1991; Mell, Howard, & Miller, 2003; Petcu et al., respectively (Goodale & Milner, 1992; Milner & Goodale,
2016; Schnider, Regard, Benson, & Landis, 1993; Schott, 2012; 1995). Support for a neural basis for this apparent
Sherwood, 2012; Smith, Mindelzun, & Miller, 2003; Wapner, perception-action dissociation was amassed from psycho-
Judd, & Gardner, 1978). Here we describe recent de- physics (e.g., Bridgeman, Lewis, Heit, & Nagle, 1979), neuro-
velopments in this debate, as prelude to reporting the case of a  goire,
imaging (Culham et al., 2003; Faillenot, Toni, Decety, Gre
talented young visual artist who suffered a devastating pos- & Jeannerod, 1997; Valyear, Culham, Sharif, Westwood, &
terior stroke. Through the dual lenses of neuropsychological Goodale, 2006) and electrophysiology (Lee et al., 2002). This
investigation and artistic expression, we explore the impact of version of the dual pathway account became the dominant
bilateral parieto-occipital infarcts upon perceptual and spatial paradigm of visual processing and has wielded considerable
components of visual processing and visual imagery, and the influence over the past 25 years.
theoretical implication of these findings. Recently, however, this theoretical framework has been
challenged (Jackson et al., 2005b, 2009; Pisella et al., 2009;
1.1. Perceptual and spatial visual cognition: Rosetti et al., 2003, 2017). Several studies have elicited evi-
dissociations and interactions dence of cross-talk between the two pathways (Pisella et al.,
2009; Rossetti et al., 2017). Moreover, Jackson and colleagues
While about half of all cerebrovascular accidents result in have demonstrated that patients with optic ataxia are not
some form of visual disorder (Rowe et al., 2009), damage to only impaired on tasks involving visually-guided action, but
posterior brain regions in particular can have devastating ef- also on all tasks that require comparison of two visual and/or
fects upon visual processing (Milner et al., 1991). Damage to spatial representations (Jackson, Newport, Mort, & Husain,
the primary visual cortex can result in complete blindness in 2005a, b; 2009; 2018). This implies that the dorsal pathway
the corresponding region of the visual field (cf. Tong, 2003), has a more general role in the simultaneous coordination of
and insult to either of the two main pathways linking primary multiple spatial representations and not just the execution of
visual cortex to adjacent areas can result in distinct and visually-guided action. It is known that patients with optic
dissociable higher-order visual impairments (cf. Milner & ataxia often demonstrate simultanagnosia, the inability
Goodale, 2008). The precise nature of the processes under- visually to perceive more than one object at the same time, but
pinning these impairments remains a focus of significant recent research has shown that these patients are also
debate. impaired on tasks that require matching of two objects, irre-
Early neuropsychological studies revealed two parallel spective of whether the objects are presented visually or
fibre bundles emerging from the occipital cortex. One was a haptically (Jackson et al., 2018). Whereas simultanagnosia is
ventral pathway to inferior temporal areas associated with thought to reflect a narrowed window of visual attention
visual perception, with damage causing deficits in shape and (Dalrymple, Barton, & Kingstone, 2013; Rizzo & Robin, 1990),
colour recognition despite adequate visual acuity (Goodale the current findings hint rather at a supramodal deficit in the
et al., 1994; Humphrey, Goodale, Jakobson, & Servos, 1994; ability to hold more than one representation in the mind's eye.
Milner et al., 1991). This finding was later supported by neu-
roimaging (Grill-Spector, 2003; Haxby, Hoffman, & Gobbini, 1.2. Visual imagery: are representational and spatial
2000; Ishai, Ungerleider, Martin, Schouten, & Haxby, 1999; components dissociable?
James, Culham, Humphey, Milner, & Goodale, 2003; Malach
et al., 1995) and electrophysiological studies (Cohen, Cross, This raises important questions about the relationship be-
Tunik, Grafton, & Culham, 2009; Lee et al., 2002). tween visual processing and visual imagery. There is certainly
The second, a dorsal pathway to the posterior parietal evidence to support the Aristotelian perspective that visual
cortex, was hypothesised as being specialised for spatial images are dependent upon visual perception, as if they were
processing, with damage resulting in visuo-spatial disorien- ‘residues of the actual [sense] impression’ (De Insomniis 461b;
tation and misreaching (Ungerleider & Mishkin, 1982). Later, cf. Rhetorica 137a 28), analogous to impressions in wax.
Goodale and Milner recast the dorsal pathway as underpin- Perception and imagery paradigms are known to invoke
ning transformation of visual information for the control of similar brain regions (Klein et al., 2004; Pearson, Naselaris,
spatial action (Goodale, 2017; Goodale & Humphrey, 1998; Holmes, & Kosslyn, 2015; Slotnick, Thompson, & Kosslyn,
Goodale & Milner, 1992; James et al., 2003). Following bilat- 2005; Thirion et al., 2006), particularly the primary visual
eral lesions in occipital and temporal regions, their patient DF cortex, and the vividness of visual imagery is thought to be
proved unable to describe or demonstrate the orientation of a contingent upon this neural overlap (Diijkstra, Bosch & van
line or slot, but could reach out and post a card through the Gerven, 2017).
same slot without any difficulty. In direct contrast, they But there is also evidence to suggest that perception and
showed that patients with optic ataxia, following lesions to imagery are conceptually and phenomenologically distinct.
the parietal regions, had intact visual perception, but made Several studies have reported intact imagery in the face of
spatially inaccurate reaches and/or hand rotations when significant perceptual disorder (Bartolomeo et al., 1998;
grasping for objects (Gre a et al., 2002; Jakobson, Archibald, Behrmann, Winocur, & Moscovitch, 1992; Jankowiak,
Carey, & Goodale, 1991; Milner, Dijkerman, McIntosh, Kinsbourne, Shalev, & Bachman, 1992) and congenital blind-
Rossetti, & Pisella, 2003; Pisella et al., 2000). ness (Kerr, 1983), as well as the reverse: impaired imagery with
This apparent double dissociation led Goodale and Milner spared perception (Farah, Levine, & Calvanio, 1988; Moro,
to re-characterise the distinct outputs from ventral and dorsal Berlucchi, Lerch, Tomaiuolo, & Aglioti, 2008; Riddoch, 1990),
c o r t e x 1 2 6 ( 2 0 2 0 ) 3 5 5 e3 6 7 357

with evidence that the left temporal and parietal regions are WAIS for corroborative neuropsychology. They also proposed
critically involved in the generation of images (Levine, that JR demonstrated a general deficit in spatial imagery, cit-
Warach, & Farah, 1985; Luzzatti, Vecchi, Agazzi, Cesa- ing difficulties matching faces from different perspectives and
Bianchi, & Vergani, 1998; Papagno et al., 2002; Sack, Campro- describing familiar scenes. Without full neuropsychological
don, Pascual-Leone, & Goebel, 2005; Winlove et al., 2018). investigation, it is unclear if his deficits reflected impairment
Furthermore, there are reports of two distinct processing in the generation of images, or rather their subsequent spatial
streams for mental imagery, mirroring those for visual pro- analysis.
cessing (Cattaneo et al., 2008). Levine et al. (1985) reported Similarly, Smith et al. (2003) described an artist who
cases of impaired object imagery with intact spatial imagery developed difficulties drawing following posterior stroke. Her
(Patient 1), and impaired spatial imagery with intact object paintings became piecemeal, with difficulty centring. She had
imagery (Patient 2), with similar dissociations reported by difficulty representing a visual scene as a whole, tending to
other groups (Luzzatti et al., 1998; Papagno et al., 2002). focus on one part only which, they argued, was suggestive of
Accordingly, neuroimaging studies have found object imagery simultanagnosia or a deficit in the ability to create the image
to be associated with ventral occipito-temporal regions (Ishai, in her mind's eye. Neuropsychological assessment was again
Haxby, & Ungerleider, 2002, 2000; O'Craven & Kanwisher, limited, here to a brief comment reporting a deficit in sus-
2000), and spatial imagery with the dorsal posterior parietal tained attention and constructional apraxia, with a notable
cortex (Sack et al., 2005, 2008, 2002; Formisano et al., 2002; absence of tests of object perception, spatial processing or
Ganis, Thompson, & Kosslyn, 2004; Kawashima, Roland, & mental imagery. Thus, again it remains unclear if her deficits
O’Sullivan, 1995; Knauff, Kassubek, Mulack, & Greenlee, did indeed reflect an impairment in mental imagery, or spatial
2000; Kosslyn, Digirolamo, Thompson, & Alpert, 1998, 1997; analysis of the image in front of her.
Thompson, Slotnick, Burrage, & Kosslyn, 2009). This would Botez et al. (1985) did perform extensive neuropsychologi-
suggest that although the generation of mental images relies cal assessment of an amateur artist who complained of life-
upon the left temporal areas, spatial manipulation of them long difficulties generating mental images. Born with hypo-
invokes parietal processes (Sack & Schuhmann, 2012). plasia of the right hemisphere, he was found to demonstrate a
striking deficit in the ability to image people, places and ob-
1.3. Impact of brain injury on artistic expression jects, with a corresponding difficulty drawing objects from
memory. However, visual perception was entirely intact and
There have been several investigations of the visual cognitive his copies of the same objects were excellent. Although this
changes that impact upon artistic ability following brain suggests that his drawing performance reflects limited ca-
damage, mostly limited to unilateral brain insults (Annoni pacity for imagery, they did not investigate object perception
€ zner & Hennerici, 2007; Blanke & Pasqualini,
et al., 2005; Ba and imagery comprehensively, omitting entirely any exami-
2012; Cantagallo & Della Sala, 1997; Chatterjee, 2004; Miller & nation of spatial processing or spatial imagery. This again
Hou, 2004; Schott, 2012; Sherwood, 2012; Smith et al., 2003). precludes understanding of the precise nature of the imagery
Such reports have shown that left hemispheric damage can deficits demonstrated.
disrupt semantic knowledge necessary for drawing objects In sum, although extant reports have revealed striking
from memory (Gainotti, Silveri, Villa, & Caltagirone, 1983), as changes in creative production following brain damage,
well as the symbolic processing involved in the production of hitherto they have failed to address the impact of specific
conceptual visual art (Kaczmarek, 1991). Left hemisphere perceptual and spatial failures upon visual artistic expression.
damage may also release the functions of the right hemi- This study investigated patient IB who demonstrated signifi-
sphere, facilitating more emotional and/or creative displays cant changes in both visual processing and imagery following
€ zner & Hennerici, 2007; Mell et al., 2003;
(Annoni et al., 2005; Ba bilateral parieto-occipital infarcts. We report extensive,
Miller, Ponton, Benson, Cummings, & Mena, 1996; Miller & detailed neuropsychology and the correlated and dramatic
Hou, 2004; Schnider et al., 1993). Right hemispheric damage, changes in her visual art practice.
on the other hand, can result in attentional deficits, mani-
festing as drawing omissions, distortions and perseverations
in patients with left-sided neglect (Blanke & Pasqualini, 2012; 2. Methods
Cantagallo & Della Sala, 1997).
Accounts of changed artistic production following poste- 2.1. Case study
rior brain damage are quite rare. Moreover, the few reports
available have failed to detail the neuropsychological profile, Patient IB is a 32-year-old, right-handed Graphic Designer.
with often limited or no interrogation of the perceptual and Having attended an international school in her home country
spatial processes involved in visual processing and visual in the Middle East, she came to the UK to attend art school and
imagery. For example, Wapner et al. (1978) discussed the case following graduation had a flourishing career as a freelancer,
of patient and artist JR who suffered bilateral occipital stroke. producing intricate patterns and designs for leading brands
Impaired at drawing from memory and poor at copying ob- (see Fig. 1 for example of her work).
jects, JR would frequently lose his place, miss or misplace At the age of 29 years, a prolonged episode of viral
features, or redraw parts. This led to erroneous copies, such as myocarditis led to acute heart failure eventually requiring
a five-legged rhinoceros. The authors concluded that these urgent heart transplant. During this time, she was profoundly
errors bore witness to his profound impairment in object and acidotic and hypotensive for several hours, and ventilated
spatial processing, but referred only to his performance on the with central extra-corporeal membrane oxygenation. She
358 c o r t e x 1 2 6 ( 2 0 2 0 ) 3 5 5 e3 6 7

Fig. 1 e Examples of art work before stroke.

suffered hypoxic-ischaemic encephalopathy, resulting in worse on the left compared to the right. She also developed
watershed infarcts of the parieto-occipital lobes (see Fig. 2). seizures. She was an inpatient for three months and
When she was woken up, she complained of bilateral continued rehabilitation after discharge.
blurred vision. Ophthalmologic assessment revealed visual
acuity of 6/36 in each eye. She demonstrated bilateral altitu- 2.2. Materials and procedure
dinal and arcuate field loss, with linear demarcation on the
vertical midline. Eye movements and pupil reflexes were Following discharge from hospital, she underwent extensive
normal, suggesting intact optic nerve function. She demon- cognitive assessment. General intellectual functioning was
strated limb weakness, an ataxic gait and intention tremor assessed using tasks from the Wechsler Adult Intelligence

Fig. 2 e MRI at the time of the ischaemic insult demonstrates absence of parenchymal signal abnormality or focal atrophy
(AeC). Five months later, there is mild widening of the intra-parietal sulcus bilaterally (D), widening of the calcarine sulcus
(E) and cortical signal abnormality affecting the frontal and parietal lobes bilaterally (F), right greater than left, consistent
with ischaemic injury. Follow up imaging at two years (GeI) demonstrates progression of the parietal and occipital lobe
atrophy (E,F), which remains stable at three years (JeL).
c o r t e x 1 2 6 ( 2 0 2 0 ) 3 5 5 e3 6 7 359

Scale e Third Edition (WAIS-III; (Wechsler, 1997), pro-rated to Numbers). Reading was poor, with frequent visual errors
generate scores for both verbal (VIQ) and non-verbal intel- (CORVIST Word Reading, e.g., ‘mouse’ for ‘museum’) and ev-
lectual abilities (PIQ). Further neuropsychological examina- idence of visual crowding (CORVIST Crowding).
tion was as follows: verbal recognition and recall memory Similarly, on tests of visuo-spatial processing, she was
(Words Recognition Memory Test short form [RMT; completely unable to discriminate between different sizes
Warrington, 1984]; Adult Memory and Information Processing (CORVIST Size Discrimination), make same-different judge-
Battery Story subtest [AIMPB, Coughlan & Hollows, 1985]); ments of position (VOSP Position Discrimination), or count
language (Oldfield Naming Test [(Oldfield and Wingfield, dots on a page (CORVIST Scattered Dots, VOSP Dot Counting).
1965)]; semantic fluency); visual processing (Cortical Visual She was also unable to copy intersecting pentagons (Fig. 3).
Screening Test [CORVIST; James, Plant, & Warrington, 1991; She described her visual experience as, ‘when you lie
Visual Object and Space Perception Battery [VOSP; Warrington beneath a tree and look up to the sky, it's how you look though
& James, 1991]); other parietal functions (Oral Graded Diffi- the leaves'.
culty Spelling [Baxter & Warrington, 1994]; arithmetic tests
[Jackson & Warrington, 1986]); executive functioning (phone- 3.3. Visual cognition three years after stroke
mic fluency; Cognitive Estimates [MacPherson et al., 2014];
Hayling Sentence Completion Test [Burgess & Shallice, 1997]); Three years after her stroke, her vision and visual fields have
and speed of information processing (Counting Backwards remained broadly stable although her subjective experience is
from 30). that there has been significant improvement. She continues to
IB also underwent tests of visual imagery, as described demonstrate bilateral hemianopia, more dense in the right
below. On one of these, 11 healthy participants were also hemifield. She is unable to count fingers in right upper, right
asked to complete the test. The sample size for this control lower and left lower quadrants, but can in the left upper
group was determined through convenience and available quadrant. Automated perimetry confirms a stable pattern of
resources. In comparison to patient IB, these healthy partici- visual field loss, but with some minor improvement in that it
pants were of a similar age (mean ¼ 31.9 years) and educa- has become less dense and more patchy.
tional background (mean ¼ 18.5 years). Repeat neuropsychological assessment revealed minor
No part of the study procedures or analyses was pre- improvement in visual perception (see Table 1). Performance
registered prior to the research being conducted. had improved on two tests of object perception (CORVIST
We report how we determined our sample size for the Shape Discrimination, Shape Detection), but remained
control group, all data exclusions, all inclusion/exclusion significantly impaired when discriminating between different
criteria, whether inclusion/exclusion criteria were established colours, and identifying fragmented letters and numbers.
prior to data analysis, all manipulations, and all measures in There was also ongoing evidence of acquired dyslexia and
the study. visual crowding. Performance also remained grossly impaired
on all tests of visuo-spatial processing.
When asked to describe a visual scene (Boston Diagnostic
3. Results Aphasia Examination Cookie Theft Picture, see Box 1), she
made several visual errors (such as describing the woman at
3.1. General cognitive assessment the sink as ‘washing a cat’). She also failed to describe the
overall scene (who was in the picture, where it was set), and
Initially, five months following her stroke, cognitive assess- some of the salient features (e.g., the perilous stool). This,
ment revealed she was fully orientated to time, place and along with her poor reading and visual crowding, suggests a
person, and e remarkably - verbal memory recognition and degree of simultanagnosia.
recall were intact. Language functioning was also well pre-
served. She did demonstrate significant executive dysfunction
(Cognitive Estimates), slow speed of processing (Counting 30 Box 1
to 1) and poor arithmetic (Oral Graded Difficulty Arithmetic IB's description of the Cookie Theft scene.
Test). However, by far the most striking feature of her profile
was significant abnormalities in visual processing (see Table 1, “I do not know if this is her husband or the little girl's
with impaired scores highlighted in bold). brother… I think the brother is taking a cookie from the
cupboard from a jar and handing it to his little sister…
3.2. Visual cognition immediately following stroke while the mum is washing the dishes and the water is
spilling from the sink everywhere, but she doesn't seem
While she had little difficulty on tests of basic visual acuity to be aware or maybe she is aware. She is wearing high
(CORVIST Symbol Acuity) and visual sensory capacity (VOSP heels and curtains are open and I can see some trees
Shape Detection), performance on tests of higher-order visual outside. Is she washing dishes or is she washing a cat?
processing was impaired. She would not be washing the cat e I think she is
She failed all trials assessing the ability to discriminate washing dishes, but there is something in the sink. What
subtle colour differences (CORVIST Hue Discrimination), and is it? There is something in the sink, what is it? There is
she was poor at identifying shapes, fragmented letters and something in the sink. I think it might be because of the
numbers (CORVIST Shape Discrimination, CORVIST Shape spillage it looks like an animal.”
Detection, VOSP Incomplete Letters, CORVIST Fragmented
360 c o r t e x 1 2 6 ( 2 0 2 0 ) 3 5 5 e3 6 7

Table 1 e Performance on neuropsychological assessment five months and three years post-stroke.

Domain Test Five months post-stroke Three years post-stroke


General Intellectual Functioning WAIS VIQ 88 85
WAIS PIQ 60 63
Memory Short RMT Words 25/25 (>50th percentile) 25/25 (>50th percentile)
AMIPB Story Immediate 26/56 (10e25th percentile) 40/56 (50e75th percentile)
AMIPB Story Delayed 31/56 (25e50th percentile) 38/56 (50e75th percentile)
Naming Oldfield Naming Test 21/30 23/30
Visual CORVIST Symbol Acuity 36/36 36/36
CORVIST Shape Discrimination 3/4 4/4
CORVIST Size Discrimination 0/2 1/2
CORVIST Shape Detection 4/8 8/8
CORVIST Hue Discrimination 0/4 1/4
CORVIST Scattered Dot 1/4 1/4
CORVIST Fragmented Numbers 5/8 6/8
CORVIST Word Reading 12/16 14/16
CORVIST Face Perception 8/8 7/8
CORVIST Crowding 2/4 3/4
VOSP Shape Detection 18/20 (>5th percentile) 19/20 (>5th percentile)
VOSP Incomplete Letters 6/20 (<5th percentile) 15/20 (<5th percentile)
VOSP Dot Counting 2/10 (<5th percentile) 2/10 (<5th percentile)
VOSP Position Discrimination 7/20 (<5th percentile) 12/20 (<5th percentile)
Parietal Oral Graded Spelling 7/30 (7th percentile) 11/30 (12th percentile)
Oral Graded Arithmetic 2/24 (impaired) 5/24 (low average)
Executive Phonemic fluency ‘S’ 15 (31e34th percentile) 15 (31e34th percentile)
Category fluency - Animals 18 (25th percentile) 18 (25th percentile)
Cognitive Estimates 10 11
Hayling 13 (low average) 17 (average)
Speed Counting 30 > 1 26 s, 1 error 31 s

WAIS-III FSIQ: Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale e Third Edition Full Scale IQ (Wechsler, 1997); RMT: Recognition Memory Test (Warrington,
1984); AMIPB: Adult Memory and Information Processing Battery (Coughlan & Hollows, 1985); Oldfield Naming Test (Oldfield & Wingfield,
1965); CORVIST: Cortical Vision Screening Test (James et al., 1991); VOSP: Visual Object and Space Perception Battery (Warrington & James,
1991); Oral Graded Difficulty Spelling Test (Baxter & Warrington, 1994); Oral Graded Difficulty Arithmetic Test (Jackson & Warrington, 1986);
Cognitive Estimates (MacPherson et al., 2014); Hayling: Hayling Sentence Completion Test (Burgess & Shallice, 1997).

On other tests, arithmetic has improved, but there remain In sum, IB suffers gross deficits in visual processing, which
impairments on tests of executive function (Cognitive Esti- are confirmed upon testing. Extensive assessment revealed
mates) and speed of processing (Counting 30 to 1). impairment in visual perception and a particularly marked
deficit in visuo-spatial processing. Although she has also
sustained other cognitive deficits the impairments in visual
processing are by far the most marked and the most disabling.

3.4. Visual imagery three years after stroke

In contrast with the profound impairment in visual processing,


she denies any change in visual imagery. Three years after the
stroke, she reports that she is able to imagine and dream ob-
jects with correct form, size and colour. She was able to draw
clearly recognisable objects from memory, despite problems
with visuo-spatial aspects of the construction (see Fig. 4).
She also performed well on tests assessing object form and
size imagery (see Table 2). She was able to state correctly if a
given animal was larger or smaller than a goat, and if the ears
protruded upwards or downwards (Kosslyn, Farah, Holtzman,
& Gazzaniga, 1985). Performance was flawless on a test
assessing letter imagery: when asked if lower-case letters
contained an ascender (e.g., ‘b’, ‘h’), descender (e.g., ‘p’, ‘g’), or
neither (e.g., ‘m’, ‘e’; Weber & Castleman, 1970). Furthermore,
despite dyschromatopsia, she demonstrated intact colour
imagery. Following Riddoch and Humphreys (1987), perfor-
mance was also nearly flawless on a series of questions con-
Fig. 3 e Copy of intersecting pentagons. cerning the colour of objects, both for those with strong verbal
c o r t e x 1 2 6 ( 2 0 2 0 ) 3 5 5 e3 6 7 361

Fig. 4 e Drawing from memory.

colour associations (e.g., ‘What is the colour of snow?’) and this was comparable to the scores of 11 healthy controls (see
those without (e.g., ‘What is the colour of a frog?’). supplementary file for raw scores), there were important
However, comparison of imagined faces was less robust. qualitative differences. Similar to patient HJA (Riddoch,
As shown in Table 3, when asked to make judgements about Humphreys, Gannon, Blott, & Jones, 1999), IB seemingly
the similarity between three imagined faces (e.g., ‘Who looks drew upon piecemeal or semantic information in order to
more like Bob Dylan: Johnny Depp or Arnold Schwarzeneg- make judgements. For example, she likened David Cameron
ger?’), she made correct judgements in 6/8 trials (Young, to Boris Johnson rather than Leonardo Di Caprio because ‘he
Humphreys, Riddoch, Hellawell, & de Haan, 1994). Although had become fat’.
With regard to navigation (see Table 4), we assessed her
ability to generate familiar and novel routes (as described by
Table 2 e Performance on tests of visual imagery e form,
size and colour.

Domain Test Score Description Table 3 e Performance of IB and 11 healthy controls on a


test involving comparison between three imagined faces.
Object and Animal size 18/18 Normal
letter imagery Animal ears 17/19 Normal Domain IB Control
Letters 12/12 Normal
Mean SD Range
Colour e associations 9/10 Normal
Colour e no associations 10/10 Normal Comparison of imagined faces 6/8 6.27 1.10 5e8
362 c o r t e x 1 2 6 ( 2 0 2 0 ) 3 5 5 e3 6 7

Table 4 e Performance on tests of visual imagery e Art came to my help as a means of therapy. As my vision
navigation and mental rotation (with impaired scores was what I have been using the most and was mourning
highlighted in bold).
the lack of the most, I started with that. Drawing eyes
Domain Test Score Description was one of the first things I did. As couldn't see most of
Navigation Routes e familiar 2/2 Normal what I was drawing, not much was aligned. To begin
Routes e unfamiliar 0/2 Impaired with I could only see someone's eye and a little bit of
Mental Rotation Manikin Test e Total 15/32 Impaired their shoulder. So it could have been a world of single
Manikin Test e Upright 7/16 Impaired eyes floating in space if I hadn't known better. Some-
Manikin Test e Inverted 8/16 Impaired times I draw brains and symbols of electricity as an
attribute to the electrical malfunctioning in my brain. I
Burgess, Trinkler, King, Kennedy, & Cipolotti, 2006). She was should probably use more symbology for blood as it was
able to describe two familiar, remembered routes in great the lack of it in my brain that demolished my coordina-
detail (the bus journey she takes to the hospital in Queen tion and memory.
Square, and her walk to Queen Square from where she alights I have been told that I could recover substantially but
the bus), but was completely unable to generate new routes in they didn't know how long it would take or and they
familiar areas (such as the journey from Euston to Queen couldn't tell me the limitation of my recovery. All I could
Square, or from her house to King's Cross station). She also do was to wait the time to pass. Tried to solve puzzles, fill
performed at chance levels on a test of mental rotation in Sudoku and do crosswords unsuccessfully. I couldn't
(Manikin Test; Ratcliff, 1979). solve anything neither could use the tools that could
Thus, in contrast to her difficulties with both visuo- help me get around things and improve my neuro-
perceptual and visuo-spatial processing IB denies any connections. It was a Catch 22. So, I drew lines to sym-
change in imagery and indeed performs well on tests thought bolize the painfully slow passing of time.
to assess imagery of object form, size and colour. She did, I am starting to be happy. Depression is harmful to the
however, perform poorly on imagery tasks that require addi- brain. One cannot think clearly. Everything is distorted
tional spatial processing, generation of novel routes and and muggy. Being grateful saves oneself from falling into
spatial rotation of mental percepts and on tests involving self-pity. Neuroplasticity is something that I firmly
comparison of imagined faces. There is, therefore, a dissoci- believe in. After all, how much do we really know and
ation within the field of visual imagery, with imagery for understand the human brain? What do we truly know of
perceptual features other than faces relatively preserved, its capabilities?
while spatial imagery is impaired.

3.5. Artistic expression after stroke


In the first place, where there was relatively little repre-
sentation of the human form or features there has been since
There has been a dramatic change in her art practice and
her illness a preoccupation with face/eyes/brain, revealing
production. Prior to her stroke, she worked as a graphic artist
repetition of a different order. Where previously there was a
making digital art in a style that was linear and complex, with
focus on pattern now there is a preoccupation with the site
strong geometric organisation and often bold coloration. The
and impact of trauma, which began as an event but the effects
work was highly patterned, the pattern often repeating (see
of which recur moment by moment in her life; eyes which
Fig. 1). Since her stroke, she has continued with an undimin-
could see and through which she could imagine with a bril-
ished drive to create art, but now as a painter. The work she
liance now reduced in their capacity so that every plan or
has produced since her illness is strikingly different in several
intention, in life and in her work, is tethered by that limita-
ways (see Fig. 5 for examples and Box 2 for her description of
tion. Nevertheless, she makes art. Simpler in form, but at the
this).
same time, more communicative: images that represent her
Box 2 loss, and mourning, images of eyes that both can and cannot
IB's description of her life and artwork after stroke see what it is they are looking at. No longer made for a client,
the work is now suffused with emotion and meaning.
In the immediate aftermath of her illness there were
Upon exiting the hospital grounds, I was left with a vi-
repeated images of a face with eyes, with other features less
sual impairment; an ill-functioning brain and a poorly
distinct. At first the face appeared alone on the page, eyes
functioning right arm and pointing finger that I could not
sometimes weeping, sometimes in sombre colouring. Several
feel. That meant my career as a graphic designer was
paintings have scored out eyes, reflecting her loss of visual
over. This in itself was devastating enough. As I re-
abilities. The mood portrayed by the paintings made soon
learned how to walk, tie my shoelaces and wash
after her illness is bleak, reflecting suffering and despair. Over
myself, grew strong enough to lift a pint glass, I tried to
time colour and other elements began to appear. An occa-
live a normal life. I soon realized that I lost more than I
sional tree, a hint of sunlight as her mood changed. But also
thought. I had very little spatial awareness or sense of
tally marks, as if counting up the days ‘without vision’, like a
direction, was suffering from acute fatigue therefore
prison sentence but one that may or may not end. Sometimes
sleeping all the time and as a result, also started
eyes fill the page. The images are much simpler: the small and
suffering from major depression.
repeated patterns replaced now by fewer objects set upon
c o r t e x 1 2 6 ( 2 0 2 0 ) 3 5 5 e3 6 7 363

Fig. 5 e Examples of art work after stroke.

plainer backgrounds. A narrowed thematic range echoes the the head above the tie, for instance, has too high a mouth.
kind of repeated preoccupation often seen in artists' work. There is also separation of individual elements; the owl's legs
Here such changes may reflect the fronto-parietal brain are separate to its body, and the saw's serrated edge is sepa-
damage, observed to be important for artistic creativity (Shi, rated from its handle. This impairment in drawing from
Cao, Chen, Zhuang, & Qiu, 2017). More recently an element memory is mirrored by her difficulty in copying, as demon-
of playfulness has entered the work, with brighter colours and strated in Fig. 3.
humour. The use of colour, is, though, reduced possibly
reflecting her deficits in colour discrimination, as corrobo-
rated by her neuropsychology. 4. Discussion
As a result of her deficits, the materials have had to change.
Whereas her graphic design relied on digital tools to construct 4.1. Dissociation between representational and spatial
intricate detailed patterns, she is now much less able to use a components of visual imagery
computer both because of the difficulty successfully shifting
vision from keyboard to screen and back, and because of the IB is a young woman for whom making art has always been
difficulties following and understanding computer software. central to her life and who was on the cusp of a stellar career
Now she draws and paints using media that she encountered as a graphic artist when she sustained bilateral watershed
first during her childhood and early schooling and that were infarcts, which have had a dramatic impact on her life, her
offered to her once more during her inpatient stay: pencils, visual cognitive abilities and her art making. Formal neuro-
watercolours and A4 paper. psychological investigation demonstrated impaired visual
Most strikingly, the images created after her illness betray perception and reduced capacity for visuospatial processing.
the visual processing deficits: items, including facial features, These impairments reflect damage to both the ventral and
misaligned in space. She reports particular difficulty aligning dorsal pathways supporting higher-order visual function. In
eyes in space, in relation to one another and to the remainder contrast, we demonstrated a striking dissociation between
of the face Other facial elements are often absent if not mis- intact visual perceptual imagery for colours, shapes and let-
placed just as in her drawings of objects from memory (Fig. 4): ters and impaired performance on all tests that required
364 c o r t e x 1 2 6 ( 2 0 2 0 ) 3 5 5 e3 6 7

imagined spatial processing or manipulation, specifically, on cognitive load, such as mental rotation, leading to what ap-
tasks involving novel route generation and mental rotation. pears to be a selective deficit in space-based attention. How-
This discrepancy between object and spatial imagery supports ever, such an argument can be countered by the previous case
the claim that, like visual processing, mental visual imagery is studies reporting the reverse pattern of deficits: impaired im-
also supported by two distinct processing streams (Cattaneo agery with intact novel route generation (Levine & Calvanio,
et al., 2008). 1989; Levine et al., 1985). This apparent double dissociation
This dissociation between impaired object perception and between mental imagery and mental spatial computations
intact object imagery confirms earlier reports of distinguish- demonstrates that these are separable processes, not simply
able processes involved in the detection and generation of explained by levels of difficulty. Moreover, the notion that our
visual images (Bartolomeo et al., 1998; Behrmann, Moscovitch, patient's observed deficits are simply a consequence of task
& Winocur, 1994; Farah et al., 1988; Jankowiak et al., 1992; difficulty is a circular one. Tasks deemed more ‘difficult’ may be
Moro et al., 2008; Riddoch, 1990). Conversely, despite broadly considered so because of a greater attentional load. As we
intact face perception, performance was impaired on the task propose our patient has significant impairment in spatial
that required comparison of different imagined faces. attention, tasks drawing upon such reduced attentional re-
Although face analysis draws upon perception of basic and sources will therefore necessarily be more difficult.
largely invariant configurations (Haxby et al., 2000), compari- Similarly, mental imagery is not reducible to just attention.
son of different facial features across individuals demands a Thompson, Hsiao, and Kosslyn (2011) found that manipulating
greater degree of spatial attention and holistic representation the size of the stimuli differentially affects performance on
(Taubert & Alais, 2009). Recourse to reliance upon gross imagery and attentional tasks, confirming that these are also
perceptual features as opposed to configural information to dissociable processes. This is in line with our findings of pre-
perform this task is indicative of her deficits in the spatial served performance on simple tasks of mental imagery, but
processing of imagined facial features. impaired performance on tasks that place greater demands
upon attention, namely those that involve spatial processing.
4.2. A deficit in space-based attention?
4.3. Effect upon artistic expression
Our patient's visuo-spatial deficit affecting both the process-
ing of the visuospatial aspects of external stimuli and visuo- Unlike all previous case reports to date, we can also observe
spatial imagery is suggestive of a more general deficit in how our patient's deficits are made manifest through her
space-based attention. It has been proposed that the artistic expression. Her deficit in space-based attention is
occipito-parietal regions may be crucially involved in the notably reflected in her visual art, with a change in style from
simultaneous coordination of multiple spatial representations highly constrained and geometrically organised to much
(Jackson et al., 2005a, b; 2009; 2018). In keeping with findings simpler, more representational, and with misaligned ele-
from optic ataxia patients, the data from our patient is also ments. The combination of impaired spatial processing with
suggestive of a marked deficit in holding more than one impaired object perception likely explains her severely
spatial representation in the mind's eye. impaired copy of intersecting pentagons, whereas the execu-
Kosslyn's (1980) influential model of imagery proposed tion of drawing objects from memory is perhaps relatively
three componential processes: generation, inspection and more preserved because of her intact object imagery. How-
transformation of an image within short-term memory. Our ever, the impaired spatial processing is still apparent, partic-
patient seems to have intact generation, with satisfactory ularly in the misalignment of multiple elements and fewer
recollection of object form, size and colour from long-term identical repeating objects.
memory. However, inspection appears impaired, with poor Although previous reports have sought to illustrate the role
processing of spatial features and incoherence of the overall of impaired spatial processing in the production of visual art,
percept, precluding subsequent image transformation or these have been limited by insufficient corroborative evidence
rotation (Sack & Schuhmann, 2012). Previous research has from neuropsychology (Smith et al., 2003; Wapner et al., 1978).
suggested that whereas the left temporal and parietal cortices In our study, we have shown that our patient's impairments in
are involved in the generation of images (Levine et al., 1985; visuo-spatial processing and spatial imagery are clearly illu-
Luzzatti et al., 1998; Papagno et al., 2002; Sack et al., 2005), minated by her drawings, both when attempting to draw from
the right parietal cortex is thought to be responsible for the models and from memory.
spatial comparison of such imagined content (Sack et al.,
2005). As our patient suffered damage to both parietal lobes, 4.4. Conclusion
we propose that this bilateral insult has resulted in her
marked disorder in spatial imagery. We may conclude that our patient's deficits in integrating and
It may be that our findings simply reflect differing levels of aligning different spatial features, as witnessed by both neu-
task difficulty. Some studies have shown that vividness of ropsychological assessment and artistic expression, illustrate
mental imagery is associated with the function of the V1 the presence of impaired simultaneous coordination of multi-
(Bergmann, Genç, Kohler, Singer, & Pearson, 2016; Dijkstra, ple spatial representations. As this deficit is shared across
Bosch, & van Gerven, 2017): thus it is possible that although visuo-processing and visual imagery, it supports the recent
our patient passes our tests of object imagery, her difficulty claims by Jackson et al. (2018) that occipito-parietal regions are
generating visual images may be magnified in tasks of higher crucially involved in the ability to hold more than one spatial
c o r t e x 1 2 6 ( 2 0 2 0 ) 3 5 5 e3 6 7 365

representation in the mind's eye. Although simultanagnosia is visual agnosia. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human
a concept currently limited to the deficient perception of two Perception and Performance, 20, 636e637.
simultaneous representations, the deficits discussed here Behrmann, M., Winocur, G., & Moscovitch, M. (1992). Dissociation
between mental imagery and object recognition in a brain-
appear to reflect simultanagnosia for stimuli, either perceived
damaged patient. Nature, 359, 636e637.
or imaginal, as explained by a severely limited spatial window Bergmann, J., Genç, E., Kohler, A., Singer, W., & Pearson, J. (2016).
of attentional processing (Dalrymple et al., 2013). Smaller primary visual cortex is associated with stronger, but
less precise mental imagery. Cerebral Cortex, 26, 3838e3850.
Blanke, O., & Pasqualini, I. (2012). The riddle of style changes in
Data availability statement the visual arts after interference with the right brain. Frontiers
in Human Neuroscience, 5, 154.
Botez, M. I., Olivier, M., Vezina, J., Botez, T., & Kaufman, B. (1985).
All data concerning patient IB's behavioural performance is
Defective revisualization: Dissociation between cognitive and
contained in the manuscript. The conditions of our medical
imagistic thought case report and short review of the
ethics do not permit public archiving of patient IB's anatom- literature. Cortex, 21, 375e389.
ical MRI scan, which cannot be shared with any individual Bridgeman, B., Lewis, S., Heit, G., & Nagle, M. (1979). Relation
outside the author team under any circumstances. between cognitive and motor-oriented systems of visual
Due to legal copyright restrictions, the test materials used position perception. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human
in this study are not publicly archived and can be obtained Perception and Performance, 5, 692e700.
Burgess, P. W., & Shallice, T. (1997). The hayling and brixton tests.
from the copyright holders in the cited references.
Bury St Edmunds, UK: Thames Valley Test Company.
Burgess, N., Trinkler, I., King, J., Kennedy, A., & Cipolotti, L. (2006).
Impaired allocentric spatial memory underlying topographical
Credit author statement disorientation. Reviews in the Neurosciences, 17, 239e251.
Cantagallo, A., & Della Sala, S. (1997). Preserved insight in an
Jennifer A. Foley: Conceptualization; Data curation; Formal artist with extrapersonal spatial neglect. Cortex, 34,
analysis; Investigation; Methodology; Project administration; 163e189.
Cattaneo, Z., Vecchi, T., Cornoldi, C., Mammarella, I., Bonino, D.,
Resources; Roles/Writing - original draft; Writing - review &
Ricciardi, E., et al. (2008). Imagery and spatial processes in
editing.
blindness and visual impairment. Neuroscience and
Isik Bayraktar: Data curation; Resources; Writing - review Biobehavioral Reviews, 32, 1346e1360.
& editing. Chatterjee, A. (2004). The neuropsychology of visual artistic
Harpreet Hyare: Data curation; Formal analysis; Writing - production. Neuropsychologia, 42, 1568e1583.
review & editing. Cohen, N. R., Cross, E. S., Tunik, E., Grafton, S. T., & Culham, J. C.
Diana Caine: Conceptualization; Data curation; Investiga- (2009). Ventral and dorsal stream contributions to the online
control of immediate and delayed grasping: A TMS approach.
tion; Methodology; Project administration; Supervision; Visu-
Neuropsychologia, 47, 1553e1562.
alization; Roles/Writing - original draft; Writing - review &
Coughlan, A. K., & Hollows, S. E. (1985). The adult memory and
editing. information processing battery. Leeds, UK: St James’ University
Hospital.
Culham, J. C., Danckert, S. L., DeSouza, J. F. X., Gati, J. S.,
Supplementary data Menon, R. S., & Goodale, M. A. (2003). Visually-guided grasping
produces activation in dorsal but not ventral stream brain
areas. Experimental Brain Research, 153, 180e189.
Supplementary data to this article can be found online at
Cummings, J. L., & Zarit, J. M. (1987). Probable Alzheimer's disease
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2020.01.023.
in an artist. Journal of the American Medical Association, 258,
2731e2734.
Dalrymple, K. A., Barton, J. J. S., & Kingstone, A. (2013). A world
references unglued: Simultanagnosia as a spatial restriction of attention.
Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 145.
Dijkstra, N., Bosch, S. E., & van Gerven, M. A. J. (2017). Vividness of
Annoni, J. M., Devuyst, G., Carota, A., Bruggimann, L., & visual imagery depends of the neural overlap with perception
Bogousslavsky, J. (2005). Changes in artistic style after minor in visual areas. Journal of Neuroscience, 37, 1367e1373.
posterior stroke. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry, Faillenot, I., Toni, I., Decety, J., Gregoire, M. C., & Jeannerod, M.
76, 797e803. (1997). Visual pathways for object-oriented action and object
Bartolomeo, P., Bachoud-Le vi, A., De Gelder, B., Denes, G., Dalla recognition: Functional anatomy with PET. Cerebral Cortex, 7,
Barba, G., Brugieres, P., et al. (1998). Multiple-domain 77e85.
dissociation between impaired visual perception and Farah, M. J., Levine, D. N., & Calvanio, R. (1988). A case study of
preserved mental imagery in a patient with bilateral mental imagery deficit. Brain and Cognition, 8, 147e164.
extrastriate lesions. Neuropsychologia, 36, 239e249. Finney, G. R., & Heilman, K. M. (2007). Artwork before and after
Baxter, D. M., & Warrington, E. K. (1994). Measuring dysgraphia: A onset of progressive nonfluent aphasia. Cognitive and
graded-difficulty spelling test. Behavioural Neurology, 7, Behavioural Neurology, 20, 7e10.
107e116. Formisano, E., Linden, D. J., Di Salle, F., Trojano, L., Esposito, F.,
€ zner, H., & Hennerici, M. G. (2007). Painting after right-
Ba Sack, A. T., et al. (2002). Tracking the mind's image in the
hemisphere stroke e case studies of professional artists. brain: W. Time-resolved fMRI during visuospatial imagery.
Frontiers in Neurology and Neuroscience, 22, 1e13. Neuron, 35, 185e194.
Behrmann, M., Moscovitch, M., & Winocur, G. (1994). Intact visual Gainotti, G., Silveri, M. C., Villa, G., & Caltagirone, C. (1983).
imagery and impaired visual perception in a patient with Drawing objects from memory in aphasia. Brain, 106, 613e622.
366 c o r t e x 1 2 6 ( 2 0 2 0 ) 3 5 5 e3 6 7

Ganis, G., Thompson, W. L., & Kosslyn, S. M. (2004). Brain areas of associative visual agnosia. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 4,
underlying visual mental imagery and visual perception: An 119e131.
fMRI study. Cognitive Brain Research, 20, 226e241. Kaczmarek, B. L. J. (1991). Aphasia in an artist: A disorder of
Goodale, M. A. (2017). Duplex vision: Separate cortical pathways symbolic processing. Aphasiology, 5, 361e371.
for conscious perception and the action of control. In Kawashima, R., Roland, P. E., & O'Sullivan, B. T. (1995). Visuo-
S. Schneider, & M. Velmans (Eds.), The blackwell companion to motor learning: A positron emission tomography study.
consciousness. Malden, MA: Blackwell. Cerebral Cortex, 5, 111e122.
Goodale, M. A., & Humphrey, G. K. (1998). The objects of action Kerr, N. H. (1983). The role of vision in ‘visual imagery’
and perception. Cognition, 67, 181e207. experiments: Evidence from the congentially blind. Journal of
Goodale, M. A., Meenan, J. P., Bülthoff, H. H., Nicolle, D. A., Experimental Psychology: General, 112, 265e277.
Murphy, K. J., & Racicot, C. I. (1994). Separate neural pathways Klein, I., Dubois, J., Mangin, J., Kherif, F., Flandin, G., Poline, J.,
for the visual analysis of object shape in perception and et al. (2004). Retinotopic organization of visual mental images
prehension. Current Biology, 4, 604e610. as revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging.
Goodale, M. A., & Milner, A. D. (1992). Separate visual pathways Cognitive Brain Research, 22, 26e31.
for perception and action. Trends in Neuroscience, 15, 20e25. Knauff, M., Kassubek, J., Mulack, T., & Greenlee, M. W. (2000).
Grea, H., Pisella, L., Rossetti, Y., Desmurget, M., Tilikete, C., Cortical activation evoked by visual mental imagery as
Grafton, S., et al. (2002). A lesion of the posterior parietal measured by fMRI. Neuroreport, 11, 3957e3962.
cortex disrupts on-line adjustments during aiming Kosslyn, S. M. (1980). Image and mind. Cambridge, MA: Harvard
movements. Neuropsychologia, 40, 2471e2480. University Press.
Grill-Spector, K. (2003). The neural basis of object perception. Kosslyn, S. M., Digirolamo, G. J., Thompson, W. L., & Alpert, N. M.
Current Opinions in Neurobiology, 13, 159e166. (1998). Mental rotation of objects versus hands: Neural
Haxby, J. V., Hoffman, E. A., & Gobbini, M. I. (2000). The distributed mechanisms revealed by positron emission tomography.
human neural system for face perception. Trends in Cognitive Psychophysiology, 35, 151e161.
Sciences, 4, 223e233. Kosslyn, S. M., Farah, M. J., Holtzman, J. D., & Gazzaniga, M. S.
Humphrey, G. K., Goodale, M. A., Jakobson, L. S., & Servos, P. (1985). A computational analysis of mental image generation:
(1994). The role of surface information in object recognition: Evidence from functional dissociations in split-brain patients.
Studies of a visual form agnosic and normal subjects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 114, 311e341.
Perception, 23, 1457e1481. Kosslyn, S. M., Thompson, W. L., & Alpert, N. M. (1997). Neural
Ishai, A., Haxby, J. V., & Ungerleider, L. (2002). Visual imagery of systems shared by visual imagery and visual perception: A
famous faces: Effects of memory and attention revealed by positron emission tomography study. Neuroimage, 6, 320e334.
fMRI. Neuroimage, 17, 1729e1741. Lee, J., & van Donkelaar, P. (2002). Dorsal and ventral visual
Ishai, A., Ungerleider, L., & Haxby, J. V. (2000). Distributed neural stream contributions to perception-action interactions during
systems for the generation of visual images. Neuron, 28, pointing. Experimental Brain Research, 143, 440e446.
979e990. Levine, D. N., & Calvanio, R. (1989). Prosopagnosia: A defect in
Ishai, A., Ungerleider, L. G., Martin, A., Schouten, J. L., & visual configural processing. Brain and Cognition, 10, 149e170.
Haxby, J. V. (1999). Distributed representation of objects in the Levine, D. N., Warach, J., & Farah, M. (1985). Two visual systems in
human ventral visual pathway. Proceedings of the National mental imagery: Dissociation of “what” and “where” in
Academy of Sciences, 96, 9379e9384. imagery disorders due to bilateral posterior cerebral lesions.
Jackson, S. R., Condon, L. A., Newport, R. W., Pears, S., Husain, M., Neurology, 35, 1010e1018.
Bajaj, N., et al. (2018). Optic ataxia and the dorsal visual stream Luzzatti, C., Vecchi, T., Agazzi, D., Cesa-Bianchi, M., & Vergani, C.
re-visited: Impairment in bimanual haptic matching (1998). A neurological dissociation between preserved visual
performed without vision. Cortex, 98, 60e72. and impaired spatial processing in mental imagery. Cortex, 34,
Jackson, S. R., Newport, R., Husain, M., Fowlie, J. E., 461e469.
O'Donoghue, M., & Bajaj, N. (2009). There may be more to MacPherson, S. E., Wagner, G. P., Murphy, P., Bozzali, M.,
reaching than meets the eye: Re-thinking optic ataxia. Cipolotti, L., & Shallice, T. (2014). Bringing the cognitive
Neuropsychologia, 47, 1397e1408. estimation task into the 21st century: Normative data on two
Jackson, S. R., Newport, R., Mort, D., & Husain, M. (2005a). Where new parallel forms. Plos One, 9, e92554.
the eye looks, the hand follows: Limb-dependent magnetic Malach, R., Reppas, J. B., Benson, R. R., Kwong, K. K., Jiang, H.,
misreaching in optic ataxia. Current Biology, 15, 42e46. Knnedy, W. A., et al. (1995). Object-related activity revealed by
Jackson, S. R., Newport, R., Mort, D., Husain, M., Jackson, G. M., functional magnetic resonance imaging in human occipital
Swainson, R., et al. (2005b). Action binding and parietal lobes: cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 92,
Some new perspectives on optic ataxia. In G. W. Humphreys, 8135e8139.
& M. J. Riddoch (Eds.), Attention in action: Advances from cognitive Mell, J. C., Howard, S. M., & Miller, B. L. (2003). Art and the brain:
neuroscience. Hove, UK: Psychology Press. The influence of frontotemporal dementia on an
Jackson, M., & Warrington, E. K. (1986). Arithmetic skills in accomplished artist. Neurology, 60, 1707e1710.
patients with unilateral cerebral lesions. Cortex, 22, 611e620. Miller, B. L., & Hou, C. E. (2004). Portraits of artists: Emergence of
Jakobson, L. S., Archibald, Y. M., Carey, D. P., & Goodale, M. A. visual creativity in dementia. Archives of Neurology, 61,
(1991). A kinematic analysis of reaching and grasping 842e844.
movements in a patient recovering from optic ataxia. Miller, B. L., Ponton, M., Benson, D. F., Cummings, J., & Mena, I.
Neuropsychologia, 29, 803e809. (1996). Enhanced artistic creativity with temporal lobe
James, T. W., Culham, J., Humphey, K., Milner, A. D., & degeneration. Lancet, 348, 1744e1745.
Goodale, M. A. (2003). Ventral occipital lesions impair object Milner, A. D., Dijkerman, H. C., McIntosh, R. D., Rossetti, Y., &
recognition but not object-directed grasping: An fMRI study. Pisella, L. (2003). Delayed reaching and grasping in patients
Brain, 126, 2463e2475. with optic ataxia. Progress in Brain Research, 142, 225e242.
James, M., Plant, G. T., & Warrington, E. K. (1991). Cortical vision Milner, A. D., & Goodale, M. A. (1995). The visual brain in action.
screening test. Oxford, UK: Thames Valley Test Company. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
Jankowiak, J., Kinsbourne, M., Shalev, R. S., & Bachman, D. L. Milner, A. D., & Goodale, M. A. (2008). Two visual systems re-
(1992). Preserved visual imagery and categorization in a case viewed. Neuropsychologia, 46, 774e785.
c o r t e x 1 2 6 ( 2 0 2 0 ) 3 5 5 e3 6 7 367

Milner, A. D., Perrett, D. I., Johnston, R. S., Benson, P. J., Sack, A. T., Sperling, J. M., Prvulovic, D., Formisano, E., Goebel, R.,
Jordan, T. R., Heeley, D. W., et al. (1991). Perception and action Di Salle, F., et al. (2002). Tracking the mind's image in the
in ‘visual form agnosia’. Brain, 114, 405e428. brain: II. Transcranial magnetic stimulation reveals parietal
Moro, V., Berlucchi, G., Lerch, J., Tomaiuolo, F., & Aglioti, S. M. (2008). asymmetry in visuospatial imagery. Neuron, 35, 195e204.
Selective deficit of mental visual imagery with intact primary Schnider, A., Regard, M., Benson, D. F., & Landis, T. (1993). Effects
visual cortex and visual perception. Cortex, 44, 109e118. of a right-hemisphere stroke on an artist's performance.
Oldfield, R. C., & Wingfield, A. (1965). A series of pictures for use in Neuropsychiatry, Neuropsychology, and Behavioral Neurology, 6,
object-naming. MRC Psycholinguistic Research Unit Special 249e255.
Report No. PLU/65/19. Schott, G. D. (2012). Pictures as a neurological tool: Lessons from
O'Craven, K. M., & Kanwisher, N. (2000). Mental imagery of faces enhanced and emergent artistry in brain disease. Brain, 135,
and places activates corresponding stimulus-specific brain 1947e1963.
regions. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12, 1013e1023. Sherwood, K. (2012). How a cerebral haemorrhage altered my art.
Papagno, C. (2002). Progressive impairment of constructional Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6, 55.
abilities: A visuospatial sketchpad deficit? Neuropsychologia, 40, Shi, B., Cao, X., Chen, Q., Zhuang, K., & Qiu, J. (2017). Different
1858e1867. brain structures associated with artistic and scientific
Pearson, J., Naselaris, T., Holmes, E. A., & Kosslyn, S. M. (2015). creativity: A voxel-based morphometry study. Scientific reports,
Mental imagery: Functional mechanisms and clinical 7, 42911.
applications. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 19, 590e602. Slotnick, S. D., Thompson, W. L., & Kosslyn, S. M. (2005). Visual
Petcu, E. B., Sherwood, K., Popa-Wagner, A., Buga, A. M., Aceti, L., mental imagery induces retinotopically organized activation
& Miroiu, R. I. (2016). Artistic skills recovery and compensation of early visual areas. Cerebral Cortex, 15, 1570e1583.
in visual artists after stroke. Frontiers in Neurology, 7, 76. Smith, W. S., Mindelzun, R. E., & Miller, B. (2003). Simultanagnosia
a, H., Tilikete, C., Vighetto, A., Desmurget, M.,
Pisella, L., Gre through the eyes of an artist. Neurology, 60, 1832e1834.
Rode, G., et al. (2000). An automatic pilot for the hand in the Taubert, J., & Alais, D. (2009). The composite illusion requires
human posterior parietal cortex toward a reinterpretation of composite face stimuli to be biologically plausible. Vision
optic ataxia. Nature Neuroscience, 3, 729e736. Research, 49, 1877e1885.
Pisella, L., Sergio, L., Blangero, A., Torchin, H., Vighetto, A., & Thirion, B., Duchesnay, E., Hubbard, E., Dubois, J., Poline, J. B.,
Rossetti, Y. (2009). Optic ataxia and the function of the dorsal Lebihan, D., et al. (2006). Inverse retinotopy: Inferring the
stream: Contributions to perception and action. visual content of images from brain activation patterns.
Neuropsychologia, 47, 3033e3044. Neuroimage, 33, 1104e1116.
Ratcliff, G. (1979). Spatial though, mental rotation and the right Thompson, W. L., Hsiao, Y., & Kosslyn, S. M. (2011). Dissociation
cerebral hemisphere. Neuropsychologia, 17, 49e54. between visual attention and visual mental imagery. Journal of
Riddoch, M. J. (1990). Loss of visual imagery: A generation deficit. Cognitive Psychology, 23, 256e263.
Cognitive Neuropsychology, 7, 249e273. Thompson, W. L., Slotnick, S. D., Burrage, M. S., & Kosslyn, S. M.
Riddoch, M. J., & Humphreys, G. W. (1987). A case of integrative (2009). Two forms of spatial imagery: Neuroimaging evidence.
visual agnosia. Brain, 110, 1431e1462. Psychological Science, 20, 1245e1253.
Riddoch, M. J., Humphreys, G. W., Gannon, T., Blott, W., & Tong, F. (2003). Primary visual cortex and visual awareness.
Jones, V. (1999). Memories are made of this: The effect of time Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 4, 219e229.
on stored visual knowledge in a case of visual agnosia. Brain, Ungerleider, L. G., & Mishkin, M. (1982). Two cortical visual
122, 537e559. systems. In D. J. Ingle, M. A. Goodale, & R. J. W. Mansfield
Rizzo, M., & Robin, D. A. (1990). Simultanagnosia: A defect of (Eds.), Analysis of visual behaviour. Cambridge, Massachusetts:
sustained attention yields insights on visual information The MIT Press.
processing. Neurology, 40, 447. Valyear, K. F., Culham, J. C., Sharif, N., Westwood, D., &
Rosetti, Y., Pisella, L., & McIntosh, R. D. (2017). Rise and fall of two Goodale, M. A. (2006). A double dissociation between
visual systems theory. Annals of Physical and Rehabilitation sensitivity to changes in object identity and object orientation
Medicine, 60, 130e140. in the ventral and dorsal visual streams: A human fMRI study.
Rossetti, Y., Vighetto, A., & Pisella, L. (2003). Optic ataxia revisited: Neuropsychologia, 44, 218e228.
Immediate motor control versus visually guided action. Wapner, W., Judd, T., & Gardner, H. (1978). Visual agnosia in an
Experimental Brain Research, 153, 171e179. artist. Cortex, 14, 343e364.
Rowe, F. J., Hepworth, L. R., Howard, C., Hanna, K. L., Warrington, E. K. (1984). Recognition memory test. Windsor, UK:
Cheyne, C. P., & Currie, J. (2009). High incidence and NEFR-Nelson.
prevalence of visual problems after acute stroke: An Warrington, E. K., & James, M. (1991). The visual object and space
epidemiology study with implications for service delivery. Plos perception battery. Bury St Edmunds, UK: Thames Valley Test
One, 14, e0213035. Company.
Sack, A. T., Camprodon, J. A., Pascual-Leone, A., & Goebel, R. Weber, R. J., & Castleman, J. (1970). The time it takes to imagine.
(2005). The dynamics of interhemispheric compensatory Perception and Psychophysics, 8, 165e168.
processes in mental imagery. Science, 308, 702e704. Wechsler, D. (1997). In WAIS-III. San Antonio, TX, USA:
Sack, A. T., Jacobs, C., De Martino, F., Staeren, N., Goebel, R., & Psychological Corporation.
Formisano, E. (2008). Dynamic premotor-to-parietal Winlove, C. I. P., Milton, F., Ranson, J., Fulford, J., MacKisack, M.,
interactions during spatial imagery. Journal of Neuroscience, 28, Macpherson, F., et al. (2018). The neural correlates of visual
8417e8429. imagery: A co-ordinate-based meta-analysis. Cortex, 105, 4e25.
Sack, A. T., & Schuhmann, T. (2012). Hemispheric differences Young, A. W., Humphreys, G. W., Riddoch, M. J., Hellawell, D. J., &
within the fronto-parietal network dynamics underlying de Haan, E. H. F. (1994). Recognition impairments and face
spatial imagery. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 214. imagery. Neuropsychologia, 32, 693e702.

You might also like