You are on page 1of 12

Disorders of Peripheral and Central Auditory Processing

Handbook of Clinical Neurophysiology, Vol. 10


G.G. Celesia (Vol. Ed.)
# 2013 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved 449

CHAPTER 23

Auditory agnosias
Ayse P. Saygin* and Luke E. Miller
Department of Cognitive Science, MC 0515, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla,
CA 92093-0515, USA

23.1. Introduction Rauschecker and Scott, 2009). Human neuroimaging


studies have shown differential (though not necessarily
The goal of this chapter is to review the research on audi- selective) response patterns in temporal cortex to differ-
tory agnosias, the impaired capacity to recognize sounds, ent types of sound such as speech sounds, voices,
despite adequate hearing. Throughout the review, we or environmental sounds (Price et al., 1996, 2005; Belin
aim to link neuropsychological findings to theories of et al., 2000; Binder et al., 2000; Scott et al., 2000; Wise
auditory processing in humans and functional neuroanat- et al., 2001; Zatorre and Belin, 2001; Davis and
omy of cortical auditory areas in the brain. Johnsrude, 2003; Dick et al., 2004; Lewis et al., 2004;
Auditory agnosia can affect the processing of ver- Vigneau et al., 2006; Whalen et al., 2006; Campbell,
bal (speech) or nonverbal (environmental) sounds. 2008; Leech and Saygin, 2011). However, whether acti-
Cortical disorders affecting sound processing are not vation differentials arise due to sound category effects,
limited to agnosias and include amusia and “auditory perceptual differences in the stimuli, and in case of lin-
neglect” or sound localization impairments (Clarke guistic sounds, phoneme processing, lexical access, or
and Thiran, 2004; Pavani et al., 2004; Peretz et al., semantic retrieval is not always clear. It is likely that
2009); but these aspects of auditory processing are a combination of these factors is at play and such effects
covered in other chapters in this volume. can be difficult to distinguish with neuroimaging.
The concepts of functional specialization, localiza- The idea that the brain has specialized regions for
tion, and modularity have long been a part of discourse processing specific kinds of information has been
in neurology and neuropsychology (Broca, 1863; more prominent in vision research (e.g., brain areas
Lashley, 1950; Fodor, 1983; Elman et al., 1996). Mul- that specialize in motion or color processing have been
tiple lines of evidence (neuroanatomy, connectivity, reported (Zeki and Bartels, 1998). The extent to which
imaging, and lesion studies) indicate that the superior cortical specialization for sound processing is localized,
temporal cortex is crucial for higher-level auditory modular, or domain specific (e.g., verbal vs. nonverbal)
processing in humans (see other chapters in this volume is under debate (Saygin et al., 2003, 2004; Romanski,
for earlier stages of processing). There are several audi- 2004; Bates et al., 2005; Dick et al., 2005; Justus and
tory areas in the primate brain outside of A1 in the supe- Hutsler, 2005; Thierry and Price, 2006; Pernet et al.,
rior temporal region. Combining new neuroimaging 2007; Chartrand et al., 2008; Staeren et al., 2009; Cum-
studies with comparative neuroanatomy, the functional mings and Ceponiene, 2010; Goll et al., 2010b; Leech
neuroanatomy of cortical auditory areas in the human and Saygin, 2011). The auditory cortex of the primate
brain is becoming better understood (Galaburda and brain is much smaller than the visual cortex and has
Pandya, 1983; Rauschecker, 1998; Kaas et al., 1999; fewer distinct, well-characterized areas (Van Essen
Dick et al., 2004; Lomber and Malhotra, 2008; and Gallant, 1994). These factors make it difficult to
use lesion sites of neuropsychological patients to draw
*
conclusions about functional localization in the audi-
Correspondence to: Dr. A.P. Saygin, Department of Cogni- tory domain. More generally, lesions in humans are
tive Science, MC 0515, University of California, San Diego,
rarely as small and focal as in experimental animals.
9500 Gilman Drive, La Jolla, CA 92093-0515, USA.
Lesions also have indirect effects on the functioning
E-mail: saygin@cogsci.ucsd.edu;
Web: http://www.sayginlab.org of other areas. Often, little is known about the patients’
450 A.P. SAYGIN AND L.E. MILLER

functional neuroanatomy before the lesion and their 23.2. Auditory agnosias
variability in post-lesion functional reorganization
and recovery. These factors combined make it difficult Auditory agnosia is a rare neurological disorder char-
to use neuropsychological data to reach conclusions acterized by a relatively isolated deficit in auditory
about localization claims. Recent computational comprehension despite normal hearing. Much of the
approaches to lesion mapping with large groups of literature on auditory agnosia consists of case studies
patients is one way to acquire more precise information (Spreen et al., 1965; Albert et al., 1972; Miceli, 1982;
about localization of functions (Bates et al., 2003b). Rosati et al., 1982; Vignolo, 1982; Lechevalier et al.,
However, it is also possible to use data from patient 1984; Motomura et al., 1986; Lambert et al., 1989;
research to shed light on functional associations and Eustache et al., 1990; Kazui et al., 1990; Pasquier
dissociations. Thus, even though fine localization of et al., 1991; Engelien et al., 1995; Griffiths et al.,
functions is difficult with neuropsychological studies 1999; Taniwaki et al., 2000), including a few on children
of the auditory system, patient studies are still invalu- (Hattiangadi et al., 2005). The associated lesions are not
able in evaluating claims of domain specificity and particularly consistent and have included unilateral right
modularity (Dick et al., 2004, 2005). (Spreen et al., 1965; Haguenauer et al., 1979; Vignolo,
In this review we discuss aspects of auditory agno- 1982; Eustache et al., 1990; Fujii et al., 1990), unilateral
sias in relation to neuropsychological theories of left (Vignolo, 1982; Haguenauer et al., 1979; Eustache
functional specialization, domain specificity, and mod- et al., 1990; Pasquier et al., 1991; Clarke et al., 2000)
ularity in higher-level auditory processing. For both and bilateral cortical lesions (Albert et al., 1972;
verbal and nonverbal auditory agnosia, we consider Haguenauer et al., 1979; Rosati et al., 1982; Vignolo,
the selectivity of patients’ impairments, and associa- 1982; Lechevalier et al., 1984; Motomura et al., 1986;
tions and dissociations in the literature. In particular, Mendez and Geehan, 1988; Engelien et al., 1995; Kaga
since the early days of neurology, there have been et al., 2000). Subcortical lesions have also been reported
speculation and empirical evidence supporting an inti- (Kazui et al., 1990). Experimental studies with larger
mate relationship between language processing and groups of patients have been more informative regarding
other domains. Finkelnburg (1870) proposed an under- lesion sites associated with auditory deficits (Varney and
lying factor, sometimes called asymbolia, may be com- Damasio, 1986; Schnider et al., 1994; Saygin et al.,
mon to both the language impairments that patients 2003) and will be discussed in more detail below.
with aphasia exhibit, and deficits in nonverbal domains Auditory agnosia is a rare condition, certainly not
(Finkelnburg, 1870; Duffy and Liles, 1979). Hughlings as widely reported or investigated as visual agnosia.
Jackson (1878) had also observed a high incidence of There are several reasons for the relatively low prev-
nonverbal impairments in aphasic patients and posited alence of auditory agnosia. Auditory pathways tend
that aphasics may be “lame in thinking”. Despite such a not to be affected often by stroke due to redundancies
long history of evidence to the contrary, aphasia was at in blood supply. The auditory system also features
times presented as a deficit restricted to the linguistic redundancies at the cortical level, with strong bilateral
domain. In modern cognitive neuroscience and representation (Hausler and Levine, 2000). This is not
neuropsychology, there is ever-increasing evidence that to say auditory processing is not affected following
language may best be viewed as an embodied system, stroke, but that the literature on auditory agnosia
interwoven with sensory, motor and nonlinguistic cogni- focuses on a small number of patients with pro-
tive processes (Barsalou, 1999; Dick et al., 2005; nounced deficits. In fact, subtler but significant defi-
Meteyard et al., 2010; Saygin et al., 2010b). This cits in sound processing are not infrequent following
view is consistent with a link between aphasia and stroke if detailed psychoacoustic or electrophysiolog-
impairments in non-linguistic tasks that share neuro- ical testing is carried out (Schnider et al., 1994; Saygin
computational resources. Although such effects were et al., 2003, 2010a).
most commonly discussed in the realm of gesture, action When auditory agnosia affects only verbal material
and pantomime processing (Duffy and Duffy, 1981; it is often called word deafness (WD); when the deficit
Varney, 1982; Wang and Goodglass, 1992; Saygin is in recognizing nonverbal sounds such as environ-
et al., 2004; Saygin, 2007), there are also relevant find- mental sounds (Ballas, 1993), it is often termed non-
ings in the auditory modality that we will present in this verbal auditory agnosia. We will follow this
review. Thus we situate auditory agnosias in the broader nomenclature, although one main point we make is
discussion on localization, modularity and domain spec- that dissociations between verbal and nonverbal
ificity in human perception and cognition. domains are the exception rather than the norm.
CENTRAL DISORDERS OF AUDITORY PROCESSING 451

23.2.1. Nonverbal auditory agnosia environmental sounds to examine both verbal and
nonverbal comprehension deficits in the same aphasic
23.2.1.1. Clinical features and lesion sites patients and found that impairments in environmental
Auditory agnosia restricted to nonverbal material is a sound recognition were seen only in subjects with
very rare phenomenon usually associated with bilat- impaired verbal comprehension, and that aphasic
eral (Spreen et al., 1965; Albert et al., 1972; Kazui patients with intact verbal comprehension also per-
et al., 1990) and rarely with unilateral left (Saygin formed well on sound recognition. There were some
et al., 2010a) or right hemisphere (Fujii et al., 1990) patients who were impaired in verbal comprehension,
lesions. Studies with groups of patients and lesion- but not in sound recognition. More recently, Schnider
symptom mapping have shown that temporal cortex et al. (1994) observed that both LHD and RHD
lesions are associated with nonverbal sound patients performed significantly worse than a group
processing deficits (Schnider et al., 1994; Saygin of normal controls on an environmental sound recog-
et al., 2003). nition test. They found no significant differences in the
Two forms of auditory agnosia have been pro- performance of the two patient groups; however, the
posed: perceptual-discriminative, with patients failing pattern of errors appeared to differ over groups:
to identify whether two consecutive sounds are LHD patients made more semantically based errors,
identical, and associative-semantic, with patients while RHD patients and control subjects made almost
being impaired at audio-visual matching or naming. exclusively acoustic errors. For all patients, accuracy
Bilateral lesions appear to be implicated in severe in recognizing environmental sounds correlated with
discriminative disorders (Albert et al., 1972; Rosati language comprehension as measured by the Western
et al., 1982; Vignolo, 1982; Lechevalier et al., 1984; Aphasia Battery (Kertesz, 1979). Lesion–behavior
Motomura et al., 1986; Mendez and Geehan, 1988; correlations showed that LHD patients with impaired
Kazui et al., 1990; Taniwaki et al., 2000). Unilateral environmental sound recognition tended to have dam-
right hemisphere lesions can lead to normal associa- age to the posterior superior temporal gyrus (pSTG)
tion with impaired discrimination (Vignolo, 1982; and the inferior parietal lobe.
Eustache et al., 1990), deficient association with nor- Although these studies supported a link between
mal discrimination (Spreen et al., 1965), or deficient aphasia and nonverbal auditory agnosia, none had
association and deficient discrimination (Fujii et al., attempted a precise comparison between verbal and
1990). Unilateral left hemisphere lesions have been nonverbal auditory processing in the same patients.
linked to deficient association (Saygin et al., 2003) Those that did compare performance between
and normal discrimination (Vignolo, 1982), though domains had used different tasks or tests in the two
in most studies with this sample, discrimination was domains and did not control for factors such as stim-
not tested on left hemisphere injured patients due to ulus frequency and identifiability, or the relationship
a focus on language processing (Varney, 1980; Varney between the auditory and visual stimuli. In addition,
and Damasio, 1986; Saygin et al., 2003). Some of the previous studies had used 4 or 5 picture displays in
inconsistencies in these findings may be due in part to sound to picture matching tasks, entailing a lengthy
the heterogeneity of the tests used in the literature (for visual processing component to the task and thus were
review see Vignolo, 1982; Griffiths et al., 1999; limited to analyzing accuracy data only, losing
Clarke et al., 2000; Griffiths, 2002; Saygin et al., potentially important information about the time
2003, 2010a; Goll et al., 2010b). course of processing. In a large neuropsychological
and lesion mapping study, our group addressed these
23.2.1.2. Specificity, associations, and dissociations gaps in knowledge and assessed the relationship
In the 1960s, Vignolo, Spinnler and Faglioni reported between verbal and nonverbal comprehension of
disturbances of environmental sound recognition fol- complex, meaningful information in the auditory
lowing unilateral hemispheric damage in a group of modality by examining aphasic patients’ abilities to
patients (Spinnler and Vignolo, 1966; Faglioni et al., match environmental sounds and corresponding
1969). They noted that right hemisphere-damaged linguistic phrases to associated pictures (Saygin
(RHD) patients tended to perform significantly worse et al., 2003, 2005). Task demands, stimulus character-
than controls on perceptual tests involving environ- istics, and semantic features were all carefully
mental sounds, while left hemisphere-damaged controlled to reduce confounds, and to focus on
(LHD) patients performed significantly worse on asso- the relationship between verbal and nonverbal
ciative or semantic tests. In 1980, Varney used domains (Fig. 1).
452 A.P. SAYGIN AND L.E. MILLER

A Verbal stimuli and related distracter

“cow mooing”

[Speech Sound]

B Nonverbal stimuli and related distracter

Moooo o
o o. . .
[Environmental Sound]

C Verbal stimuli and unrelated distracter

“cow mooing”

[Speech Sound]

D Nonverbal stimuli and unrelated distracter

Moooo o
o o. . .
[Environmental Sound]

Fig. 1. Summary of experimental design (Saygin et al., 2003, 2005). Domain (verbal/nonverbal) and distracter type (related to target/unrelated
to target) were within-subject factors. As an example, the target “cow” appeared four times, twice with verbal sound stimuli (the phrase “cow
mooing”), twice with nonverbal stimuli (the sound of a cow mooing), twice with “sheep” as the distracter (related condition), and twice with
“violin” as the distracter (unrelated condition). 45 pictures and sounds were used as targets and related and unrelated distracters, giving rise to
45 triplets such as “cow–sheep–violin”.

Results from 30 LHD and 5 RHD patients (along the two domains utilize some shared perceptual and
with 21 neurologically intact age-matched control neural mechanisms (Fig. 2).
subjects) showed that RHD patients were only mildly We then used both lesion overlays (Saygin et al.,
impaired in the task, performing similarly to mild 2003) and a novel lesion-symptom mapping method
(anomic) aphasic LHD patients, but significantly called Voxel-Based Lesion-Symptom Mapping or
worse than controls. Aphasic patients’ deficits were VLSM (Bates et al., 2003b) and found that damage
not restricted to the verbal domain; rather, patients to posterior regions in the left middle and superior
were impaired to the same extent in comprehending temporal gyri and to the inferior parietal lobe was a
speech and environmental sounds, as measured by predictor of deficits for both speech and environmen-
both accuracy and reaction time measures. Impair- tal sounds (Fig. 3). The posterior portion of
ments in both domains were significantly correlated Brodmann’s area 22 (as roughly identified in stereo-
with language impairments as measured by the West- taxic space) corresponds to the original Wernicke’s
ern Aphasia Battery. Performance within the task area. In our data, this area and the surrounding middle
between the two domains went hand in hand, with a temporal and inferior parietal regions were found
strong correlation between accuracy and reaction time implicated also in environmental sound processing
for speech and environmental sounds suggesting that (Fig. 3). In fact, Wernicke’s area itself, in the posterior
CENTRAL DISORDERS OF AUDITORY PROCESSING 453

A B
Accuracy Reaction Time
Patient M
100 * 3000

Verbal

Verbal
*
Patient M

80 1000
80 100 500 3500
Nonverbal Nonverbal
Fig. 2. Correlation of performance in the verbal and nonverbal domains within the aphasic group for (A) accuracy and (B) reaction time
(Saygin et al., 2003). Linear fits and density ellipses using a confidence interval of 95% are shown. Correlations are significant
( p < 0.0001 for both, r ¼ 0.75 and 0.95, respectively) indicating that the aphasic patients were impaired to the same extent in comprehending
language and environmental sounds. Marked with a * is patient M’s accuracy and reaction time (Saygin et al., 2010a), the only quantifiable
dissociation in the sample (Bates et al., 2003a, 2005).

VERBAL
3.3

-3.5
Middle Temporal Superior Temporal Inferior Parietal

NONVERBAL

Middle Temporal Superior Temporal Inferior Parietal


Fig. 3. Axial slices through middle temporal, superior temporal and inferior parietal areas of VLSM maps, showing the extent to which
damage to each voxel was associated with task deficits in speech (verbal) and environmental (nonverbal) sound comprehension (Saygin
et al., 2003). The values displayed at each voxel are t statistics comparing the accuracy of patients lesioned at that voxel to the patients intact
at that voxel (Bates et al., 2003b; Dronkers et al., 2004; Saygin, 2007). High t scores top the scale in red, indicating areas where damage led to
significant deficits in task performance. Voxels denoted in blue reflect negative t scores, which arise when patients with lesions to those voxels
performed better than those who did not (but had lesions elsewhere).Voxels which are not color coded were not well represented in the lesions
in our sample.

superior temporal gyrus, was more strongly associated with healthy controls, we found that the more consis-
with performance in the nonverbal domain than the tent lesion-deficit mapping in a given region, the more
verbal domain (p < 0.01; Saygin et al., 2003). that same region tended to be activated in the fMRI
We followed up on our neuropsychological studies experiments in the healthy brain (Dick et al., 2007).
with neuroimaging studies in healthy controls. In a Recent studies with primary progressive aphasia have
functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) study also revealed concomitant deficits in verbal and
454 A.P. SAYGIN AND L.E. MILLER

nonverbal sound processing in this patient population (Bates et al., 2003a, 2005). Doing so, we found three
(Goll et al., 2010a). Our stimuli and/or tasks have also outliers, of which only one, patient M, showed a dis-
been used in a variety of developmental and clinical sociation between the two domains (Fig. 2). M, a right-
populations (Cummings et al., 2006, 2008, 2009; handed individual who suffered a left hemisphere
Cummings and Ceponiene, 2010). Overall, although stroke, had a persistent difficulty with environmental
there are no doubt differences in the processing of ver- sound comprehension, but had recovered language
bal and nonverbal sounds, there appear to be shared function to within normal levels. We carried out exten-
perceptual, neural and cognitive resources utilized sive audiological and behavioral testing in order to
for both types of sounds. characterize the patient’s unusual neuropsychological
Although an association is the norm between speech profile (Saygin et al., 2010a). We also examined the
and nonverbal sounds, dissociations in neuropsychol- patient’s and controls’ neural responses to verbal
ogy can also be informative, even when rare (Bates and nonverbal auditory stimuli using fMRI.
et al., 2003a, 2005). Varney (1980) had reported deficits We verified that the patient exhibited persistent and
in nonverbal comprehension only in patients who also severe auditory agnosia for nonverbal sounds in the
exhibited deficits in verbal comprehension, but did not absence of verbal comprehension deficits or periph-
find dissociations in the opposite direction. Clarke et al. eral hearing problems. Acoustical analyses suggested
(1996) reported a patient who was deficient in the non- that his residual processing of a minority of environ-
verbal auditory domain but had no diagnosed verbal mental sounds might rely on his speech processing
comprehension deficits (although formal testing was abilities. In the patient’s brain, contralateral (right)
not reported). When both the tasks and stimuli were temporal cortex as well as perilesional (left) anterior
closely matched across domains as we have done, def- temporal cortex were strongly responsive to verbal,
icits in the two domains largely went hand in hand. Pre- but not to nonverbal sounds (Fig. 4), a pattern that
vious studies reporting dissociations between verbal stood in marked contrast to the controls’ data
and nonverbal auditory processing (Varney, 1980; (Fig. 5). This substantial reorganization of auditory
Clarke et al., 1996) had classified impaired perfor- processing may have supported the recovery of M’s
mance as performing below the level of the worst con- speech processing.
trol subject. According to these criteria, we had 9 In summary, performance in verbal and nonverbal
patients who performed worse than the poorest per- domains is highly correlated following brain injury.
forming control subject in the verbal domain, and It is possible to identify not only patients who perform
8 patients in the nonverbal domain: 3 patients were worse in the verbal domain (i.e., the expected result
more impaired in verbal processing compared to non- based on an aphasic sample and Varney (1980)), but
verbal, and 2 patients were more deficient in the non- we can also reliably identify patients who perform
verbal domain than the verbal. worse in the nonverbal domain, an unexpected and
Instead of using such cutoffs, we advocate rarely reported outcome (Saygin et al., 2010a). It is
assessing dissociations quantitatively and statistically possible that these dissociations are due to variation

A Patient M: LH B Patient M: RH

Peak MNI = –48, –9, –18 Peak MNI = 48, –27, –6


Fig. 4. Sagittal sections through patient M’s brain showing fMRI data as the patient listened in the scanner to speech and environmental sounds
(Saygin et al., 2010a). There were two clusters in which activation for speech sounds was significantly greater than activation for environ-
mental sounds: a perilesional cluster in the left anterior temporal cortex (A, LH), and a large contralateral cluster in the right temporal cortex
extending along the superior temporal sulcus (B, RH).
CENTRAL DISORDERS OF AUDITORY PROCESSING 455

A B
Patient M: LH Patient M: RH

Parameter Estimate
2 2
p<0.0001 p<0.0001

1 1

0 0

Env Sounds Speech Env Sounds Speech


Peak MNI = –48, –9, –18 Peak MNI = 48, –27, –6

C Controls: LH D Age Matched Controls: LH


[N=8] [N=2]
2
n.s. n.s. n.s.

E F
Controls: RH Age Matched Controls: RH
[N=8] [N=2]
2
n.s. n.s. n.s.

Fig. 5. Evidence for post-stroke functional reorganization in patient M’s brain (Saygin et al., 2010a). Effect sizes from the areas shown in
Fig. 4 are shown for patient M (A, B), a group of younger adult controls (N ¼ 9; C, E), and for two age-matched, older adult controls (D,
F) listening to the same sounds. Error bars are S.E.M. As clearly follows from the way these peaks were defined, there was significantly more
activation for speech sounds compared with environmental sounds in M’s data (A, B, p < 0.0001). However, no such difference was found in
the controls’ data (C–F, for all comparisons p > 0.5, not significant (n.s.)). Thus, regions that are preferentially active for speech sounds in M’s
brain (shown in Fig. 4) show no evidence of being speech-sensitive areas in the healthy brain, suggesting M may have recovered his speech
functions via post-stroke functional reorganization in both hemispheres.

between individuals’ premorbid brain organization for functions such as reading, writing, and speaking.
these functions, as well as nonuniform post-stroke Thus, WD is arguably not a result of deficits in
recovery patterns across patients and across domains. “central” language processing (although it often
progresses to or from Wernicke’s aphasia). Patients
23.2.2. Word deafness with WD should also be able to comprehend other
complex sounds such as music and environmental
23.2.2.1. Clinical features and lesion sites sounds (though this is not always assessed, see
Word deafness (WD), sometimes also called pure below). Patients with WD may describe speech as
word deafness (PWD), was first described in the sounding like “noise” or “jabbering” (Buchman
19th century and is a rare neurological condition char- et al., 1986). Some patients report speech sounds
acterized by a dramatic deficit in speech comprehen- “like a foreign language” or that “words come too
sion (Buchman et al., 1986; Polster and Rose, 1998; quickly”, indicating an ability to recognize but not
Poeppel, 2001; Stefanatos et al., 2005). Whereas early perceive speech (Albert and Bear, 1974). Patients
auditory processing is normal in patients with PWD, may be able to compensate for their deficits
they present with profound deficits in the compre- through lip-reading (Auerbach et al., 1982; Miceli,
hension and repetition of spoken language. Patients 1982; Shindo et al., 1991) and the use of contextual
also have preserved performance in other language cues (Saffran et al., 1976).
456 A.P. SAYGIN AND L.E. MILLER

The exact neuroanatomical locus of lesions leading classified as a “pure” case of WD (Mesulam, 1982) also
to WD is not clear. Most cases have had bilateral tem- had deficits in environmental sound and music
poral lobe (Miceli, 1982; Tanaka et al., 1987; Yaqub processing.
et al., 1988; Buchtel and Stewart, 1989; Praamstra The exact nature of the deficit in WD is also a mat-
et al., 1991; Shindo et al., 1991; Wirkowski et al., ter of debate. Several studies have used psycholinguis-
2006), unilateral left temporal lobe (Saffran et al., tic measures to characterize the deficit in WD (Saffran
1976; Caramazza et al., 1983; Metz-Lutz and Dahl, et al., 1976; Auerbach et al., 1982; Miceli, 1982;
1984; Wang et al., 2000; Stefanatos et al., 2005; Slevc Caramazza et al., 1983; Praamstra et al., 1991; Jacobs
et al., 2011) or subcortical lesions (Hayashi and and Schneider, 2003). Patients often exhibit impaired
Hayashi, 2007). A review of the literature reported processing of consonants with relatively spared while
the majority of WD cases had bilateral lesions of the the processing of vowels (Saffran et al., 1976; Auerbach
superior temporal cortex (42 out of 59 reviewed et al., 1982; Yaqub et al., 1988; Pinard et al., 2002;
cases); unilateral lesions of the left temporal cortex Stefanatos et al., 2005) – though there are exceptions
(16 out of the 59 reviewed cases) tended to extend (Tanaka et al., 1987; Praamstra et al., 1991). Whereas
deep into subcortical transcallosal projections vowels are characterized acoustically by slow formant
(Poeppel, 2001; Takahashi et al., 1992). Cases of frequency transitions (100–150 ms), the formant
WD due to unilateral damage could therefore be due frequency transitions of consonants are very rapid
to a “disconnection syndrome”. (20–50 ms), leading to the view that WD may be
linked to deficits in temporal processing (for review,
23.2.2.2. Specificity, associations, and dissociations see Phillips and Farmer, 1990; Stefanatos et al.,
WD has often been considered evidence for domain 2005). In support of this view, it has been reported
specificity for processing speech sounds. According that patients’ accuracy in speech processing will
to these models, specific types of auditory information often improve when speech is slowed down (Albert
are segregated into different functional streams and Bear, 1974). Deficits in processing dynamic non-
(Polster and Rose, 1998). While the characterization verbal stimuli have also been reported in WD. For
of PWD as a disorder of speech comprehension is rel- example, using a discrimination task between pairs
atively straightforward, the existence of truly pure cases of dynamically changing sine wave stimuli, Wang
of the disorder is a matter of debate. Although by def- et al. (2000) found that their WD patient scored at
inition, patients with PWD should not have deficits in chance, demonstrating a profound inability to fully
auditory processing of other kinds of sounds or other process rapidly changing auditory stimuli. Other stud-
aspects of language, a closer look reveals that patients ies have found problems of temporal resolution at the
often do present with other conditions including non- prephonemic level of processing in WD. Whereas
verbal auditory agnosia (Miceli, 1982; Tanaka et al., controls required around 1–3 ms between two clicks
1987) and amusia (Auerbach et al., 1982; Tanaka to perceive them as distinct, patients with WD
et al., 1987). Due to high comorbidity with these other required between 15 and 300 ms (Albert and Bear,
disorders, dropping “pure” from the diagnosis and 1974; Auerbach et al., 1982; Tanaka et al., 1987;
renaming the syndrome “word deafness” was consid- Buchtel and Stewart, 1989; Hayashi and Hayashi,
ered more appropriate (Buchman et al., 1986). 2007). Patients with WD were also unable to count
Although cases that can be classified as “pure” do clicks presented above 2 Hz, well below the level of
exist in the literature (Metz-Lutz and Dahl, 1984; controls (9–11 Hz) (Albert and Bear, 1974; Auerbach
Yaqub et al., 1988; Stefanatos et al., 2005; Hayashi et al., 1982; Yaqub et al., 1988). While most studies
and Hayashi, 2007; Slevc et al., 2011), as in the case have restricted their stimuli to the auditory domain,
of nonverbal auditory agnosia discussed above, many temporal processing deficits of the patients may
studies did not assess performance in other domains also extend to the somatosensory and visual domains,
as rigorously. For instance, Yaqub et al. (1988) reported raising the possibility of a supramodal deficit in WD
that their WD patient could “recognize and appreciate (Tanaka et al., 1987).
music”, but no formal testing was carried out. It is pos- The relationship between nonverbal deficits in tem-
sible that the patient had subtler deficits in music poral processing and the speech processing deficits in
perception that escaped the experimenters. In that vein, WD remains unclear. Whereas in one patient, the res-
using more controlled laboratory tests, Pinard et al. olution of WD led to the normalization of click fusion
(2002) found that a patient who was previously time (Godefroy et al., 1995), others have failed to find
CENTRAL DISORDERS OF AUDITORY PROCESSING 457

deficits in click fusion time despite evidence for a def- agnosia and their neural bases. Further research is
icit in temporal processing (Stefanatos et al., 2005). needed to improve our understanding of both auditory
Training on rapid auditory events improved a patient’s agnosias and functional specialization in human audi-
ability to perceive rapid temporal differences for non- tory perception and cognition more broadly.
speech, but not for speech sounds (Slevc et al., 2011).
Whereas prephonemic temporal processing deficits References
may contribute to WD, the theory would need to be
extended to account for these findings. Albert, ML and Bear, D (1974) Time to understand. A case
Auerbach et al. (1982) proposed dividing WD into study of word deafness with reference to the role of time
two sub-types: a prephonemic temporal processing dis- in auditory comprehension. Brain, 97: 373–384.
order that results from bilateral temporal lobe lesions, Albert, ML, Sparks, R, Stockert, TV and Sax, D (1972)
A case study of auditory agnosia: linguistic and non-
and a phonemic processing disorder that results from
linguistic processing. Cortex, 8: 427–443.
unilateral damage to the left temporal lobe (leading Auerbach, SH, Allard, T, Naeser, M, Alexander, MP and
to disconnection). Temporal processing should there- Albert, ML (1982) Pure word deafness. Analysis of a
fore be spared following unilateral left temporal dam- case with bilateral lesions and a defect at the
age. This model was further refined by Poeppel prephonemic level. Brain, 105: 271–300.
(2001) to include different temporal integration win- Ballas, JA (1993) Common factors in the identification of an
dows for acoustic processing in the left (20–50 ms) assortment of brief everyday sounds J. Exp. Psychol.:
and right (150–250 ms) hemispheres (see also Hickok Hum. Percept. Perform., 19: 250–267.
and Poeppel, 2007). According to this model, damage Barsalou, LW (1999) Perceptual symbol systems. Behav.
to the right hemisphere should be required for the dis- Brain Sci., 2: 577–609.
ruption of prephonemic processing. This claim has Bates, E, Appelbaum, M, Salcedo, J, Saygin, AP and
Pizzamiglio, L (2003a) Quantifying dissociations in neu-
recently been tested in a patient with WD due to a uni-
ropsychological research. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol.,
lateral left hemisphere lesion (Slevc et al., 2011). Dif- 25: 1128–1153.
fusion tensor imaging (DTI) revealed that transcallosal Bates, E, Wilson, SM, Saygin, AP, Dick, F, Sereno, MI,
white matter projects from the right auditory areas to Knight, RT and Dronkers, NF (2003b) Voxel-based
the left temporal lobe. While this provides some evi- lesion-symptom mapping. Nature Neurosci., 6: 448–450.
dence against the claim that all cases of unilateral Bates, E, Saygin, AP, Moineau, S, Marangolo, P and
PWD are the result of disconnection, the evidence is Pizzamiglio, L (2005) Analyzing aphasia data in a mul-
not conclusive since DTI is unable to determine tidimensional symptom space. Brain Lang., 92: 106–116.
whether the tracts are in fact functionally intact. Belin, P, Zatorre, RJ, Lafaille, P, Ahad, P and Pike, B (2000)
Voice-selective areas in human auditory cortex. Nature
(Lond.), 403: 309–312.
23.3. Summary and conclusion Binder, JR, Frost, JA, Hammeke, TA, Bellgowan, PS,
Springer, JA, Kaufman, JN and Possing, ET (2000)
In this review, we briefly presented the literature on Human temporal lobe activation by speech and non-
auditory agnosias. We discussed both nonverbal and speech sounds. Cereb. Cortex, 10: 512–528.
verbal types of auditory agnosia, the latter also known Broca, P (1863) Localisation des fonctions cérébrales: siège
as (pure) word deafness. While neuropsychological du langage articulé. Bull. Soc. Anthropol. (Paris), 4:
cases exhibiting dissociations between domains or 200–204.
modalities are important in shaping theories of human Buchman, AS, Garron, DC, Trost-Cardamone, JE,
perception and cognition, these are the exception rather Wichter, MD and Schwartz, M (1986) Word deafness:
than the norm in the domain of auditory agnosias. For one hundred years later. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychia-
example, patients with speech comprehension deficits try, 49: 489–499.
also have nonverbal sound comprehension deficits that Buchtel, HA and Stewart, JD (1989) Auditory agnosia –
apperceptive or associative disorder. Brain Lang., 37:
go hand in hand. Despite the label “pure” word deaf-
12–25.
ness, cases of verbal auditory agnosia with unequivo- Campbell, R (2008) The processing of audio-visual speech:
cally spared nonverbal sound comprehension are rare. empirical and neural bases. Phil. Trans. Roy. Soc. Lond.
Patients with auditory agnosias tend to present with B: Biol. Sci., 363: 1001–1010.
concomitant deficits in other domains, and sometimes Caramazza, A, Berndt, RS and Basili, AG (1983) The selec-
also other modalities. We also noted other open ques- tive impairment of phonological processing: a case study.
tions concerning the nature of the deficits in auditory Brain Lang., 18: 128–174.
458 A.P. SAYGIN AND L.E. MILLER

Chartrand, JP, Peretz, I and Belin, P (2008) Auditory Elman, JL, Bates, EA, Johnson, MH, Karmiloff-Smith, A,
recognition expertise and domain specificity. Brain Parisi, D and Plunkett, K (1996) Rethinking Innateness:
Res., 1220: 191–198. a Connectionist Perspective on Development. MIT Press,
Clarke, S and Thiran, AB (2004) Auditory neglect: what and Cambridge, MA467.
where in auditory space. Cortex, 40: 291–300. Engelien, A, Silbersweig, D, Stern, E, Huber, W, Doring, W,
Clarke, S, Bellmann, A, De Ribaupierre, F and Assal, G Frith, C and Frackowiak, RSJ (1995) The functional anat-
(1996) Non-verbal auditory recognition in normal sub- omy of recovery from auditory agnosia: a PET study of
jects and brain-damaged patients: evidence for parallel sound categorization in a neurological patient and normal
processing. Neuropsychologia, 34: 587–603. controls. Brain, 118: 1395–1409.
Clarke, S, Bellmann, A, Meuli, RA, Assal, G and Steck, AJ Eustache, F, Lechevalier, B, Viader, F and Lambert, J (1990)
(2000) Auditory agnosia and auditory spatial deficits fol- Identification and discrimination disorders in auditory
lowing left hemispheric lesions: evidence for distinct perception: a report on 2 cases. Neuropsychologia, 28:
processing pathways. Neuropsychologia, 38: 797–807. 257–270.
Cummings, A and Ceponiene, R (2010) Verbal and nonver- Faglioni, P, Spinnler, H and Vignolo, LA (1969) Contrasting
bal semantic processing in children with developmental behavior of right and left hemisphere-damaged patients
language impairment. Neuropsychologia, 48: 77–85. on a discriminative and a semantic task of auditory rec-
Cummings, A, Ceponiene, R, Koyama, A, Saygin, AP, ognition. Cortex, 5: 366–389.
Townsend, J and Dick, F (2006) Auditory semantic net- Finkelnburg, FC (1870) Sitzung der Niederrheinischen
works for words and natural sounds. Brain Res., 1115: Gesellschaft in Bonn. Klin. Wschr. (Berl.), 7:
92–107. 449–450460–462.
Cummings, A, Ceponiene, R, Dick, F, Saygin, AP and Fodor, JA (1983) The Modularity of Mind: an Essay on
Townsend, J (2008) A developmental ERP study of ver- Faculty Psychology. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, 145 pp.
bal and non-verbal semantic processing. Brain Res., Fujii, T, Fukatsu, R, Watabe, S, Ohnuma, A, Teramura, K,
1208: 137–149. Kimura, I, Saso, S and Kogure, K (1990) Auditory sound
Cummings, A, Saygin, AP, Bates, E and Dick, F (2009) agnosia without aphasia following a right temporal lobe
Infants’ recognition of meaningful verbal and nonverbal lesion. Cortex, 26: 263–268.
sounds. Lang. Learn. Dev., 5: 172–190. Galaburda, AM and Pandya, DN (1983) The intrinsic archi-
Davis, MH and Johnsrude, IS (2003) Hierarchical processing tectonic and connectional organization of the superior
in spoken language comprehension. J. Neurosci., 23: temporal region of the rhesus monkey. J. Comp. Neurol.,
3423–3431. 221: 169–184.
Dick, F, Saygin, AP, Moineau, S, Aydelott, J and Bates, E Godefroy, O, Leys, D, Furby, A, Dereuck, J, Daems, C,
(2004) Language in an embodied brain: the role of animal Rondepierre, P, Debachy, B, Deleume, JF and
models. Cortex, 40: 226–227. Desaulty, A (1995) Psychoacoustical deficits related to
Dick, F, Dronkers, N, Pizzamiglio, L, Saygin, AP, bilateral subcortical hemorrhages – a case with appercep-
Small, SL and Wilson, SM (2005) Language and the tive auditory agnosia. Cortex, 31: 149–159.
brain. In: M Tomasello and DI Slobin (Eds.), Beyond Goll, JC, Crutch, SJ, Loo, JHY, Rohrer, JD, Frost, C,
Nature– Nurture. Lawrence Erlbaum, Mahwah, NJ, Bamiou, D-E and Warren, JD (2010a) Non-verbal sound
pp. 237–260. processing in the primary progressive aphasias. Brain,
Dick, F, Saygin, AP, Galati, G, Pitzalis, S, Bentrovato, S, 133: 272–285.
D’Amico, S, Wilson, S, Bates, E and Pizzamiglio, L Goll, JC, Crutch, SJ and Warren, JD (2010b) Central audi-
(2007) What is involved and what is necessary for com- tory disorders: toward a neuropsychology of auditory
plex linguistic and nonlinguistic auditory processing: objects. Curr. Opin. Neurol., 23: 617–627.
evidence from functional magnetic resonance imaging Griffiths, T (2002) Central auditory processing disorders.
and lesion data. J. Cogn. Neurosci., 19: 799–816. Curr. Opin. Neurol., 15: 31–33.
Dronkers, NF, Wilkins, DP, Van Valin, RD, Jr., Redfern, BB Griffiths, TD, Rees, A and Green, GGR (1999) Disorders of
and Jaeger, JJ (2004) Lesion analysis of the brain areas human complex sound processing. Neurocase, 5: 365–378.
involved in language comprehension. Cognition, 92: Haguenauer, JP, Schott, B, Michel, F, Dubreuil, C and
145–177. Romanet, P (1979) Three case histories of cortical and
Duffy, RJ and Duffy, JR (1981) Three studies of deficits in sub-cortical auditory lesions – audiological and
pantomimic expression and pantomimic recognition in tomodensimetrical confrontations. Ann. Oto-Laryngol.
aphasia. J. Speech Hear. Res., 24: 70–84. Chir. Cervico-Fac., 96: 185–196.
Duffy, RJ and Liles, BZ (1979) A translation of Hattiangadi, N, Pillion, JP, Slomine, B, Christensen, J,
Finkelnburg’s 1870 lecture on aphasia as “asymbolia” Trovato, MK and Speedie, LJ (2005) Characteristics of
with commentary. J. Speech Hear. Disord., 44(2): auditory agnosia in a child with severe traumatic brain
156–168. injury: a case report. Brain Lang., 92: 12–25.
CENTRAL DISORDERS OF AUDITORY PROCESSING 459

Hausler, R and Levine, RA (2000) Auditory dysfunction in Meteyard, L, Cuadrado, SR, Bahrami, B and Vigliocco, G
stroke. Acta Otolarngol., 120: 689–703. (2010) Coming of age: a review of embodiment and
Hayashi, R and Hayashi, K (2007) Pure word deafness due to the neuroscience of semantics. Cortex, 48(7): 788–804.
left subcortical lesion: neurophysiological studies of two Metz-Lutz, MN and Dahl, E (1984) Analysis of word com-
patients. Clin. Neurophysiol., 118: 863–868. prehension in a case of pure word deafness. Brain Lang.,
Hickok, G and Poeppel, D (2007) The cortical 23: 13–25.
organization of speech processing. Nature Rev. Neurosci., Miceli, G (1982) The processing of speech sounds in a patient
8: 393–402. with cortical auditory disorder. Neuropsychologia, 20: 5–20.
Hughlings Jackson, J (1878) On affections of speech from Motomura, N, Yamadori, A, Mori, E and Tamaru, F (1986)
disease of the brain. Brain, 1: 304–330. Auditory agnosia: analysis of a case with bilateral sub-
Jacobs, B and Schneider, S (2003) Analysis of lexical- cortical lesions. Brain, 109: 379–391.
semantic processing and extensive neurological, electro- Pasquier, F, Leys, D, Steinling, M, Guieu, JD, Petit, H and
physiological, speech perception, and language evalua- Cambier, J (1991) Right unilateral auditory agnosia asso-
tion following a unilateral left hemisphere lesion: pure ciated with a left lenticular hemorrhage. Rev. Neurol.,
word deafness? Aphasiology, 17: 123–141. 147: 129–137.
Justus, T and Hutsler, JJ (2005) Fundamental issues in the Pavani, F, Husain, M, Ladavas, E and Driver, J (2004) Audi-
evolutionary psychology of music: assessing innateness tory deficits in visuospatial neglect patients. Cortex, 40:
and domain specificity. Mus. Percept., 23: 1–27. 347–365.
Kaas, JH, Hackett, TA and Tramo, MJ (1999) Auditory Peretz, I, Brattico, E, Järvenpää, M and Tervaniemi, M
processing in primate cerebral cortex. Curr. Opin. (2009) The amusic brain: in tune, out of key, and
Neurobiol., 9: 164–170. unaware. Brain, 132: 1277–1286.
Kaga, K, Shindo, M, Tanaka, Y and Haebara, H (2000) Neu- Pernet, C, Schyns, PG anad Demonet, J-F (2007) Specific,
ropathology of auditory agnosia following bilateral tem- selective or preferential: comments on category specific-
poral lobe lesions: a case study. Acta Otolaryngol., 120: ity in neuroimaging. Neuroimage, 35: 991–997.
259–262. Phillips, DP and Farmer, ME (1990) Acquired word deafness,
Kazui, S, Naritomi, H, Sawada, T, Inoue, N and Okuda, J and the temporal grain of sound representation in the pri-
(1990) Subcortical auditory agnosia. Brain Lang., 38: mary auditory cortex. Behav. Brain Res., 40: 85–94.
476–487. Pinard, M, Chertkow, H, Black, S and Peretz, I (2002) A case
Kertesz, A (1979) Aphasia and Associated Disorders: Tax- study of pure word deafness: modularity in auditory
onomy, Localization, and Recovery, Grune and Stratton, processing? Neurocase, 8: 40–55.
New York. Poeppel, D (2001) Pure word deafness and the bilateral
Lambert, J, Eustache, F, Lechevalier, B, Rossa, Y and processing of the speech code. Cogn. Sci., 25: 679–693.
Viader, F (1989) Auditory agnosia with relative sparing Polster, MR and Rose, SB (1998) Disorders of auditory
of speech perception. Cortex, 25: 71–82. processing: evidence for modularity in audition. Cortex,
Lashley, KS (1950) In search of the engram. Symp. Soc. Exp. 34: 47–65.
Biol. (NY), 4: 454–482. Praamstra, P, Hagoort, P, Maassen, B and Crul, T (1991)
Lechevalier, B, Rossa, Y, Eustache, F, Schupp, C, Boner, L Word deafness and auditory cortical function A case his-
and Bazin, C (1984) Cortical deafness with partial tory and hypothesis. Brain, 114: 1197–1225.
sparing of music: a case report. Rev. Neurol., 140: Price, CJ, Wise, RJ, Warburton, EA, Moore, CJ, Howard, D,
190–201. Patterson, K, Frackowiak, RS and Friston, KJ (1996)
Leech, R and Saygin, AP (2011) Distributed processing and Hearing and saying. The functional neuro-anatomy of
cortical specialization for speech and environmental auditory word processing. Brain, 119: 919–931.
sounds in human temporal cortex. Brain Lang., 116: 83–90. Price, C, Thierry, G and Griffiths, T (2005) Speech-specific
Lewis, JW, Wightman, FL, Brefczynski, JA, Phinney, RE, auditory processing: where is it? Trends Cogn. Sci., 9:
Binder, JR and De Yoe, EA (2004) Human brain regions 271–276.
involved in recognizing environmental sounds. Cereb. Rauschecker, JP (1998) Cortical processing of complex
Cortex, 14: 1008–1021. sounds. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol., 8: 516–521.
Lomber, SG and Malhotra, S (2008) Double dissociation of Rauschecker, JP and Scott, SK (2009) Maps and streams in
‘what’ and ‘where’ processing in auditory cortex. Nature the auditory cortex: nonhuman primates illuminate human
Neurosci., 11: 609–616. speech processing. Nature Neurosci., 12: 718–724.
Mendez, MF and Geehan, GR, Jr (1988) Cortical auditory Romanski, LM (2004) Domain specificity in the primate pre-
disorders: clinical and psychoacoustic features. J. Neu- frontal cortex. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci., 4: 421–429.
rol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry, 51: 1–9. Rosati, G, Debastiani, P, Paolino, E, Prosser, S, Arslan, E and
Mesulam, MM (1982) Slowly progressive aphasia without Artioli, M (1982) Clinical and audiological findings in a
generalized dementia. Ann. Neurol., 11: 592–598. case of auditory agnosia. J. Neurol., 227: 21–27.
460 A.P. SAYGIN AND L.E. MILLER

Saffran, EM, Marin, OS and Yeni-Komshian, GH (1976) An Tanaka, Y, Yamadori, A and Mori, E (1987) Pure word deaf-
analysis of speech perception in word deafness. Brain ness following bilateral lesions: a psychophysical analy-
Lang., 3: 209–228. sis. Brain, 110: 381–403.
Saygin, AP (2007) Superior temporal and premotor brain Taniwaki, T, Tagawa, K, Sato, F and Iino, K (2000) Auditory
areas necessary for biological motion perception. Brain, agnosia restricted to environmental sounds following
130: 2452–2461. cortical deafness and generalized auditory agnosia. Clin.
Saygin, AP, Dick, F, Wilson, SM, Dronkers, NF and Bates, E Neurol. Neurosurg., 102: 156–162.
(2003) Neural resources for processing language and Thierry, G and Price, CJ (2006) Dissociating verbal and
environmental sounds: evidence from aphasia. Brain, nonverbal conceptual processing in the human brain.
126: 928–945. J. Cogn. Neurosci., 18: 1018–1028.
Saygin, AP, Wilson, SM, Dronkers, NF and Bates, E (2004) Van Essen, DC and Gallant, JL (1994) Neural mechanisms
Action comprehension in aphasia: linguistic and non- of form and motion processing in the primate visual sys-
linguistic deficits and their lesion correlates. tem. Neuron, 13: 1–10.
Neuropsychologia, 42: 1788–1804. Varney, N (1980) Sound recognition in relation to aural lan-
Saygin, AP, Dick, F and Bates, E (2005) An online task for guage comprehension in aphasic patients. J. Neurol. Neu-
contrasting auditory processing in the verbal and nonver- rosurg. Psychiatry, 43: 71–75.
bal domains and norms for younger and older adults. Varney, NR (1982) Pantomime recognition defect in apha-
Behav. Res. Meth., 37: 99–110. sia: implications for the concept of asymbolia. Brain
Saygin, AP, Leech, R and Dick, F (2010a) Nonverbal audi- Lang., 15: 32–39.
tory agnosia with lesion to Wernicke’s area. Varney, NR and Damasio, H (1986) CT scan correlates of
Neuropsychologia, 48: 107–113. sound recognition defect in aphasia. Cortex, 22: 483–486.
Saygin, AP, McCullough, S, Alac, M and Emmorey, K Vigneau, M, Beaucousin, V, Hervé, PY, Duffau, H,
(2010b) Modulation of BOLD response in Crivello, F, Houde, O, Mazoyer, B and Tzourio-
motion-sensitive lateral temporal cortex by real and fic- Mazoyer, N (2006) Meta-analyzing left hemisphere lan-
tive motion sentences. J. Cogn. Neurosci., 22: guage areas: phonology, semantics, and sentence
2480–2490. processing. Neuroimage, 30: 1414–1432.
Schnider, A, Benson, F, Alexander, DN and Schnider-Klaus, A Vignolo, LA (1982) Auditory agnosia. Phil. Trans. Roy. Soc.
(1994) Nonverbal environmental sound recognition after Lond. B Biol. Sci., 298: 49–57.
unilateral hemispheric stroke. Brain, 117: 281–287. Wang, E, Peach, RK, Xu, Y, Schneck, M and Manry, C, 2nd
Scott, SK, Blank, CC, Rosen, S and Wise, RJ (2000) Iden- (2000) Perception of dynamic acoustic patterns by an
tification of a pathway for intelligible speech in the left individual with unilateral verbal auditory agnosia. Brain
temporal lobe. Brain, 123: 2400–2406. Lang., 73: 442–455.
Shindo, M, Kaga, K and Tanaka, Y (1991) Wang, LZ and Goodglass, H (1992) Pantomime, praxis and
Speech-discrimination and lip reading in patients with aphasia. Brain Lang., 42: 402–418.
word deafness or auditory agnosia. Brain Lang., 40: Whalen, DH, Benson, RR, Richardson, M, Swainson, B,
153–161. Clark, VP, Lai, S, Mencl, WE, Fulbright, RK,
Slevc, LR, Martin, RC, Hamilton, AC and Joanisse, MF Constable, RT and Liberman, AM (2006) Differentiation
(2011) Speech perception, rapid temporal processing, of speech and nonspeech processing within primary audi-
and the left hemisphere: a case study of unilateral pure tory cortex. J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 119: 575–581.
word deafness. Neuropsychologia, 49: 216–230. Wirkowski, E, Echausse, N, Overby, C, Ortiz, O and
Spinnler, H and Vignolo, LA (1966). Impaired recognition Radler, L (2006) I can hear you yet cannot
of meaningful sounds in aphasia. Cortex, 2: 337–348. comprehend: a case of pure word deafness. J. Emerg.
Spreen, O, Benton, AL and Fincham, RW (1965) Med., 30: 53–55.
Auditory agnosia without aphasia. Arch. Neurol., 13: Wise, RJ, Scott, SK, Blank, SC, Mummery, CJ, Murphy, K
84–92. and Warburton, EA (2001) Separate neural subsystems
Staeren, N, Renvall, H, De Martino, F, Goebel, R and within ’Wernicke’s area’. Brain, 124: 83–95.
Formisano, E (2009) Sound categories are represented Yaqub, BA, Gascon, GG, Al-Nosha, M and Whitaker, H
as distributed patterns in the human auditory cortex. (1988) Pure word deafness (acquired verbal auditory agno-
Curr. Biol., 19: 498–502. sia) in an Arabic speaking patient. Brain, 111: 457–466.
Stefanatos, GA, Gershkoff, A and Madigan, S (2005) On Zatorre, RJ and Belin, P (2001) Spectral and temporal
pure word deafness, temporal processing, and the left processing in human auditory cortex. Cereb. Cortex,
hemisphere. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc., 11: 456–470. 11: 946–953.
Takahashi, N, Kawamura, M, Shinotou, H, Hirayama, K, Zeki, S and Bartels, A (1998) The autonomy of the visual
Kaga, K and Shindo, M (1992) Pure word deafness due systems and the modularity of conscious vision. Phil.
to left hemisphere damage. Cortex, 28: 295–303. Trans. Roy. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci., 353: 1911–1914.

You might also like