You are on page 1of 16

Hearing Research 373 (2019) 32e47

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Hearing Research
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/heares

Research Paper

The power of language: Functional brain network topology of deaf and


hearing in relation to sign language experience
Michel R.T. Sinke a, *, Jan W. Buitenhuis a, Frank van der Maas c, d, Job Nwiboko d,
Rick M. Dijkhuizen a, Eric van Diessen b, 1, Willem M. Otte a, b, 1
a
Biomedical MR Imaging and Spectroscopy Group, Center for Image Sciences, University Medical Center Utrecht and Utrecht University, Utrecht, the
Netherlands
b
Department of Pediatric Neurology, Brain Center Rudolf Magnus, University Medical Center Utrecht, Utrecht, the Netherlands
c ~o Baseadana Comunidade (RBC) Effata, Bissora
Reabilitaça ~, Oio, Guinea-Bissau
d
CBR Effata, Omorodu Iseke Ebonyi LGA, Ebonyi State, Nigeria

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Prolonged auditory sensory deprivation leads to brain reorganization. This is indicated by functional
Received 31 May 2018 enhancement in remaining sensory systems and known as cross-modal plasticity. In this study we
Received in revised form investigated differences in functional brain network topology between deaf and hearing individuals. We
8 December 2018
also studied altered functional network responses between deaf and hearing individuals with a recording
Accepted 12 December 2018
Available online 15 December 2018
paradigm containing an eyes-closed and eyes-open condition.
Electroencephalography activity was recorded in a group of sign language-trained deaf (N ¼ 71) and
hearing people (N ¼ 122) living in rural Africa. Functional brain networks were constructed from the
Keywords:
Cross-modal plasticity
functional connectivity between fourteen electrodes distributed over the scalp. Functional connectivity
Deafness was quantified with the phase lag index based on bandpass filtered epochs of brain signal. We studied
ASL the functional connectivity between the auditory, somatosensory and visual cortex and performed
EEG whole-brain minimum spanning tree analysis to capture network backbone characteristics.
Functional networks Functional connectivity between different regions involved in sensory information processing tended
Minimum spanning tree to be stronger in deaf people during the eyes-closed condition in both the alpha and beta frequency
band. Furthermore, we found differences in functional backbone topology between deaf and hearing
individuals. The backbone topology altered during transition from the eyes-closed to eyes-open condi-
tion irrespective of deafness, but was more pronounced in deaf individuals. The transition of backbone
strength was different between individuals with congenital, pre-lingual or post-lingual deafness. Func-
tional backbone characteristics correlated with the experience of sign language. Overall, our study
revealed more insights in functional network reorganization caused by auditory deprivation and cross-
modal plasticity. It further supports the idea of a brain plasticity potential in deaf and hearing people. The
association between network organization and acquired sign language experience reflects the ability of
ongoing brain adaptation in people with hearing disabilities.
© 2018 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction

Impairment or loss of hearing interferes with many activities in


daily life, specifically limiting communication with others. This
Abbreviations: ASL, American Sign Language; CBR, Community-Based Rehabil- could easily lead to social isolation. The prevalence of this serious
itation; EEG, Electroencephalography; MRI, Magnetic Resonance Imaging; MST, disability is greatest in middle- and low-income countries (Durkin,
Minimum Spanning Tree 2002; Stevens et al., 2013; WHO, 2014). While in the United States
* Corresponding author. Biomedical MR Imaging and Spectroscopy Group, Center
for Image Sciences, University Medical Center Utrecht, Yalelaan 2, 3584 CM,
about two out of every 1000 children are born with disabling
Utrecht, the Netherlands. hearing loss (Vohr, 2003), this number is considerably higher in
E-mail address: m.r.t.sinke@umcutrecht.nl (M.R.T. Sinke). Sub-Saharan Africa where about two percent of the children is born
1
Shared last author.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.heares.2018.12.006
0378-5955/© 2018 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
M.R.T. Sinke et al. / Hearing Research 373 (2019) 32e47 33

with disabling hearing loss (WHO, 2012). Many of these children topological properties of these networks (Bullmore and Sporns,
have profound hearing loss resulting in absolute deafness. Infec- 2009; Heuvel et al., 2012; Rubinov and Sporns, 2010). The healthy
tious diseases are a major cause of deafness in these regions human brain has been characterized as a complex network that
(Mulwafu et al., 2016). effectively combines global and efficient integration with segrega-
Prolonged periods of sensory deprivation often leads to exten- tion of functionally specialized brain regions (Bullmore and Sporns,
sive reorganization in the brain. This reorganization is caused by 2012). This unique topology with high integration and segregation
compensatory and cross-modal plasticity (Bavelier and Neville, is defined as a small-world network organization (Bullmore and
2002; Merabet and Pascual-Leone, 2010; Ptito et al., 2001). Brain Sporns, 2009; Watts and Strogatz, 1998). Deviation from a small-
reorganization after auditory deprivation has been mapped by world organization has been related to many neurological and
different functional neuroimaging modalities, such as positron psychiatric disorders (Bassett and Bullmore, 2009; Stam, 2014).
emission tomography, functional near-infrared spectroscopy, Surprisingly few studies have used network analysis to examine
functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) and electroen- reorganization of brain networks in deaf individuals. Pre-lingual
cephalography (EEG) (Buckley and Tobey, 2011; Dewey and Hartley, deaf adults showed increased network clustering and nodal effi-
2015; Doucet et al., 2006; Finney et al., 2001; Yoshida et al., 2011). ciency compared to controls, whereas brain networks from post-
Consequently, deaf are better than hearing people at detecting vi- lingual deaf adults did not differ from controls (Kim et al., 2014).
sual stimuli (e.g. Almeida et al., 2015; Bavelier et al., 2006, 2000; This indicates that auditory experience might affect the
Bosworth and Dobkins, 2002; Brozinsky and Bavelier, 2004; Dye morphology of brain networks in deaf adults. In another study
et al., 2007; Finney and Dobkins, 2001; Hauser et al., 2007; increased functional connectivity was found between regions
Neville and Lawson, 1987a, 1987b) and show increased tactile within the limbic system, a system involved in sensory information
sensitivity as well (Auer et al., 2010; Lev€anen and Hamdorf, 2001; processing (Li et al., 2016). Functional network hubs shifted in the
Meredith and Lomber, 2011). Accordingly, the auditory cortex of deaf subjects. The small-worldness did not differ between pre-
deaf is found to be responsive to non-auditory stimuli (e.g. Almeida lingual deaf as compared to hearing controls (Li et al., 2016). Yet
et al., 2015; Auer et al., 2010; Buckley and Tobey, 2011; Doucet et al., another study found increased functional connectivity in brain
2006; Finney et al., 2001; Karns et al., 2012; Meredith and Lomber, networks in deaf during rest, which was also shown to be related to
2011; Neville and Lawson, 1987b; Scott et al., 2014). For example sign language experience (Malaia et al., 2014).
fMRI and positron emission tomography studies showed that the Resting-state functional connectivity measurements can be
cortical auditory and association areas of deaf people are respon- performed in an eyes-open or eyes-closed condition. Opening and
sive to visual motion stimuli. These regions include the planum closing the eyes are very basic attention-directing behaviors. Eyes-
temporale (Petitto, 2000; Sadato et al., 2005; Shiell et al., 2016) and open is related to ‘exteroceptive’ awareness, characterized by more
primary auditory cortices, like posterior superior temporal gyrus specialized overt attention and oculomotor activity, whereas eyes-
(Almeida et al., 2015; Ding et al., 2015; Karns et al., 2012; Li et al., closed is related to ‘interoceptive’ awareness, characterized by
2015) and Heschl's gyrus (Karns et al., 2012; Meyer et al., 2007; more integrative multisensory activity and imagination (Xu et al.,
Scott et al., 2014; Smith et al., 2011). Cortical reorganization 2014). Eyes-open and eyes-closed conditions relate to different
(Campbell and Sharma, 2014, 2013) and responsive auditory cortex brain states (Marx et al., 2004, 2003; Zhang et al., 2015) and to-
to visual stimuli in deaf have also been described with functional pological organizations of functional networks (Go mez-Ramírez
brain data recorded with EEG (e.g. Buckley and Tobey, 2011; Doucet et al., 2017; Tan et al., 2013; Xu et al., 2014). This is even so in
et al., 2006; Neville and Lawson, 1987b, 1987a). While this reor- darkness (Hüfner et al., 2009, 2008). In EEG-studies functional
ganization occurs inevitably as a result of profound deafness, cross- networks showed increased global efficiency and decreased clus-
modal plasticity also strongly relates to the acquisition and use of tering during the eyes-open state, specifically in the alpha band,
sign language (Meyer et al., 2007; Pe nicaud et al., 2013). Further- which might be due to alpha desynchronization, i.e. a reduction in
more, the extent of cross-modal plasticity is dependent on the age the number of functional connections in the eyes-open state
of onset and the duration of deafness (Brotherton et al., 2016; Li (Go mez-Ramírez et al., 2017; Miraglia et al., 2016; Tan et al., 2013).
et al., 2013). Although stimuli- and task-based approaches have Despite the usefulness of classical network analysis in capturing
provided valuable insights in cross-modal plasticity, they do not brain network reorganization, it has some intrinsic limitations. The
capture the mutual dependency of different functional brain re- classical network analysis is particularly limited in comparing inter-
gions as well as the integrative nature of the human brain (Hackett, subject networks with different network densities (van Wijk et al.,
2012; Stam and van Straaten, 2012). 2010). The network density is defined as the number of connections
The human brain forms a complex integrative network, which relative to the potential number of connections. Commonly used
consists of spatially distributed, but functionally connected regions network metrics, such as the clustering coefficient e used to
that continuously interact with each other (Bassett et al., 2018; measure network segregation e and average path length e used to
Bassett and Sporns, 2017; Bullmore et al., 2009; Bullmore and measure network integration e are highly affected by the number
Sporns, 2009; van den Heuvel and Hulshoff Pol, 2010). As such, of connections within a network (Stam et al., 2014; van Wijk et al.,
functional brain connectivity and reorganization can be better 2010). Therefore, comparing healthy and affected (or reorganized)
understood when these processes are not studied in isolation. Brain brain networks, in a situation of cross-modal plasticity or alpha
network analysis explicitly takes the interdependencies between desynchronization, might yield biased results (Tewarie et al., 2015;
functionally connected regions into account as it shifts emphasis van Wijk et al., 2010; Zalesky et al., 2010). Solutions have been
from specific locational changes to global topological alternations. provided. A promising alternative network characterization
The functional network topology, and potential reorganization approach not limited by the network density is the assessment of
therein, can be effectively mapped with several network metrics the network backbone. Network backbones are robustly and effi-
(Bassett et al., 2018; Bassett and Sporns, 2017; Bullmore and Sporns, ciently operationalized by the minimum spanning tree (MST) (Stam
2012). Classical graph analysis describes the human brain as a et al., 2014; Tewarie et al., 2015). An increasing number of studies
collection of nodes (i.e. functional brain regions such as the audi- have shown the usefulness of MSTs in capturing subtle network
tory or visual cortex) and edges (i.e. the functional connections changes in brain development and ageing (Boersma et al., 2013;
between regions), and provides quantitative information on the Otte et al., 2015; Smit et al., 2016; Vourkas et al., 2014). MSTs have
34 M.R.T. Sinke et al. / Hearing Research 373 (2019) 32e47

also been useful in characterizing multiple sclerosis, Alzheimer's part of a Community-Based Rehabilitation (CBR) program, which
disease and epilepsy (Engels et al., 2015; Tewarie et al., 2014; van implies that all students has to live in surrounding villages and
Diessen et al., 2016, 2014; Sinke et al., 2018). communities, aiming for a full integration within the community.
The present study therefore investigated the effects of prolonged Inclusive education means that both students with and without
periods of deafness on the functional brain network backbone to- disabilities are allowed to participate in regular classes, and are
pology. To that aim we acquired resting-state EEGs in deaf and hearing supported to learn, contribute and participate in all aspects of the
individuals. Data were recorded within a unique homogeneous pop- educational program. This inclusive educational approach is a po-
ulation living in a representative rural region in sub-Saharan Africa. In tential strategy to reduce the individual as well as shared burden of
this region deafness is a common disability and cochlear implants are disability (Eleweke and Rodda, 2002; Pfo €rtner, 2014). The number
not available. We hypothesized stronger functional connectivity (i.e. of students with and without disabilities enrolled in the schools is
more integration) between auditory cortex and other sensory (i.e. approximately equal. Since the integration of deaf people forms one
visual and somatosensory) cortices in deaf people, due to cross-modal of the main focuses of this CBR program, the majority of students
plasticity. Hence, we also expected differences in functional network with disability are deaf. Standard ASL forms an integral part of the
backbone topology between controls and deaf. Given the expected educational program for more than twenty years. This sign lan-
cross-modal plasticity as well as auditory sensitivity to visual stimuli, guage has to be learned by all teachers and students, both the
we further expected larger shifts in functional network topology be- hearing and the deaf. Besides, deaf students also receive speech
tween eyes-open (i.e. ‘exteroceptive’ awareness) and eyes-closed (i.e. therapy. All lessons are taught in English and if a teacher does not
‘interoceptive’ awareness) in deaf. We also explored whether (shifts yet sufficiently master sign language there will be an interpreter
in) functional backbone topologies were different between congen- who translates spoken language into sign language for deaf stu-
ital, pre-lingual and post-lingual deaf. Lastly, we anticipated a rela- dents and vice versa. The CBR program leads to an increasing
tionship between functional backbone characteristics and years of amount of people within the community that speak sign language.
American Sign Language (ASL) experience. This implies a more regular use, development and integration of
sign language by deaf students in their daily lives.
2. Methods Our study was approved by the organizational boards (RBC/CBR
Effata), the local health ministry (Izzi, Local Government Area) and
2.1. Study setting and ethics the federal government (Ebonyi State House of Assembly, Abakaliki
[7-11-2016]) in Nigeria. The study protocol was clearly explained to
The study pipeline as described below is schematically visual- all students in class before they were asked to participate in the EEG
ized in Fig. 1. Our study was conducted at two inclusive primary recordings. Written informed consent was obtained from adult
schools and one inclusive secondary school, located in two separate participants and caretakers of students below eighteen years. In
rural villages in Ebonyi State, southeast Nigeria. The schools are addition, we also obtained assent from the students below eighteen

Fig. 1. Schematic overview of the study pipeline. Resting-state electroencephalography (EEG) was acquired for 5 min with wireless headsets in deaf and controls. The sensor
locations corresponding to the fourteen channels are shown in orange; the reference sensors are shown in blue. The first minute of data acquisition, required for acclimatization,
was discarded, yielding two blocks of recordings e 2 min with eyes-closed and 2 min with eyes-open. The order of the eyes condition (‘open’ and ‘closed’) was alternated. Sub-
sequently, functional networks were constructed from ten-second epochs from distinct frequency bands (i.e. delta, theta, alpha, beta and gamma), and functional network backbone
metrics were determined for all bands. These network backbone metrics were related to years of sign language experience. Main effects between groups (i.e. deaf and control) and
conditions (i.e. eyes-open and eyes-closed) as well as interaction effects between group and condition were assessed using model selection within a Bayesian framework.
M.R.T. Sinke et al. / Hearing Research 373 (2019) 32e47 35

years. signal quality score was below four for any of the channels, and iii)
if the absolute deviation of the gyroscope signals relative to the
2.2. Participants gyroscope signal median exceeded five times the standard devia-
tion. This threshold was based on visual data inspection (See
Table 1 shows the demographic information of all participants. example in Suppl. Figure 3). Subsequently, the cleaned and filtered
We included 193 participants between ten and 43 years old (mean time-series were cut into ten-second epochs. Functional connec-
age of 18.5 (standard deviation 6.0); gender: 103 male, 90 female), tivity measurements as well as multiple network backbone metrics
both students and teachers. Sign language experience of partici- has been shown to stabilize within recordings if the epoch length is
pants varied between zero and seventeen years at the time of 6 s or longer (Fraschini et al., 2016). In addition, multiple epochs per
recording. We selected both hearing (n ¼ 122) and deaf (n ¼ 71) subject further increase the stability of network backbone metrics
participants. The pre-lingual deaf participants were all capable of (van Diessen et al., 2015). Therefore we used multiple epochs
lip reading. combined with this conservative ten-second length.

2.3. Data acquisition 2.5. Functional connectivity

We used a sixteen sensor/fourteen channel EEG monitor For each epoch a functional network was constructed, which can
configured to sample at 128 Hz with a 16-bit resolution (EMOTIV either be visualized as a connectivity matrix or a network graph
Inc, San Francisco, USA), which has been validated and successfully (Fig. 3). Recorded time-series within each epoch were used to
applied in several studies (Aspinall et al., 2013; Badcock et al., 2015, determine functional connectivity (i.e. forming the ‘edges’ in a
2013; McMahan et al., 2015; Prause et al., 2016; Yu and Sim, 2016). network) between different electrodes capturing neuronal signals
This wireless headset can be connected to a computer via Bluetooth from underlying brain areas (i.e. forming the ‘nodes’ in a network).
and is an invaluable tool to collect EEG signals from participants in Functional connectivity was computed and quantified with the
rural or resource-limited areas, where access to a standard EEG phase lag index. This is a measure of the asymmetry of the distri-
system is often impossible or burdensome. Two sensors were bution of instantaneous phase differences between two time series
preserved for reference and grounding: the ‘common mode sense’ and scales between zero and one (Pillai and Sperling, 2006). It is
(CMS; located at P3) sensor was used as the active reference for relative resistant to the influence of common sources, including
absolute referencing. The ‘driven right leg’ (DRL; located at P4) volume conduction and active reference electrodes. An index of
sensor was used for feedback noise cancelation. The electrodes zero indicates no phase coupling (i.e. no functional connectivity)
were located at anterofrontal (AF3, AF4, F3, F4, F7, F8), frontocentral between time series, or coupling with a phase difference centered
(FC5, FC6), occipital (O1, O2), parietal (P7, P8) and temporal sites on zero ± p radians. A non-zero index indicates the presence of
(T7, T8), according to the International 10e20 system. Signal quality phase coupling (i.e. functional connectivity), where higher values
scores are recorded for each electrode with a range from one to five indicate stronger functional connections. A more mathematical
(no units), with five as best quality score. description of computing the phase lag index can be found else-
Participants were seated in a comfortable chair in a sound- where (Stam et al., 2007).
attenuated room where the Emotiv headset was placed. Partici-
pants were instructed to keep their eyes closed for the first 3 min and 2.6. Functional connectivity strength between auditory, visual and
open in the next 2 min, or vice versa. The order of the condition somatosensory cortices
sequence was assigned alternatingly, so that half of the participants
started in the eyes-open condition whereas the other half of the Since we expected remodeling of the auditory cortex in deaf
participants started in the eyes-closed condition. The researcher people, reflected as enhanced functional connectivity between the
kept a log on deviations from the protocol, or unusual events in the auditory and sensory cortices, we characterized in both groups the
environment, that may affect the experiment. Example recordings average phase lag index between sets of electrodes. These sets were
are shown in Fig. 2. As an EEG contains a multitude of overlapping the temporal electrodes T7/T8, covering the auditory cortex, the
signal waves with distinct amplitudes and frequencies we separated parietal electrodes P7/P8, covering the somatosensory cortex, and
the signal into the five most common frequency ranges. The EEG the occipital electrodes O1/O2, covering the visual cortex, in eyes-
signals were band-pass filtered into the delta (0.5e4 Hz), theta open and eyes-closed conditions.
(4e8 Hz), alpha (8e16 Hz), beta (16e32 Hz) and gamma (32e64 Hz)
frequency bands. Examples are shown in Suppl. Figure 1 and 2. 2.7. Minimum spanning tree analysis

2.4. Data cleaning and window selection For each functional network a minimum spanning tree was
calculated from the connectivity graph G by applying Kruskal's al-
Time segments were removed from the recordings if i) the gorithm (Kruskal, 1956) (Fig. 3B). This tree captures the network's
research log indicated a deviation from the protocol, ii) the EEG backbone and is defined as a subset of the network nodes (forming

Table 1
Participant demographics.

Deaf Hearing controls

congenital pre-lingual post-lingual sign language experience no sign language experience

N total 42 22 7 65 57
Sex 13 males/29 females 16 males/6 females 7 males 31 males/34 females 36 males/21 females
Age (years) 18 ± 5 (range 10e30) 19.6 ± 4.4 (range 10e26) 16.6 ± 4.1 (range 10e22) 17.3 ± 5.6 (range 12e39) 20 ± 7.6 (range 12e43)
First eyes condition 25 open/17 closed 11 open/11 closed 2 open/5 closed 30 open/35 closed 28 open/29 closed
Sign language experience (years) 8.9 ± 3.5 (range 2e16) 8.9 ± 3.4 (range 1e16) 8.1 ± 3.7 (range 3e15) 2.3 ± 2.6 (range 1e17) e
36 M.R.T. Sinke et al. / Hearing Research 373 (2019) 32e47

Fig. 2. Raw EEG time-series of one participant. The visualized time-series were acquired from a deaf male (22 years old). Vertical grey lines indicate the initial acclimatization
period, the first 2 min (i.e. eyes-closed in this subject) and the second 2 min (i.e. eyes-open). The y-scaling is arbitrary. Labels of the fourteen channels are shown on top of the time-
series. Abbreviations of these channel labels are explained in the main text.

the original weighted graph G) that connects all the nodes and does weights of the functional network before determining the MSTs.
not contain cycles or loops (Jackson and Read, 2010). Mathemati- Subsequently, several MST metrics were calculated at the nodal or
cally, a minimum spanning tree T minimizes the sum of the costs of network level. Although some metrics are determined at the nodal
its edges, over the set of all possible spanning trees on G (Hidalgo level (e.g. degree or betweenness centrality), they can still be used
et al., 2007). Since we are interested in the strongest functional to summarize e or indicate e specific properties for the backbone
connections (i.e. the network backbone), we first inverted the edge as a whole. For example with the ‘maximum degree’ or the ‘average
M.R.T. Sinke et al. / Hearing Research 373 (2019) 32e47 37

Fig. 3. Schematic overview of functional network and minimum spanning tree (MST) construction. (A) EEG epochs were measured from 14 electrodes, represented as network
nodes. The electrodes were placed across the left (L, yellow) and right (R, orange) hemisphere (top), two electrodes served as references (blue). The recorded time-series (bottom)
were used to determine functional connectivity between brain regions. (B) The determined functional connections can either be depicted as a weighted functional network graph
(top) or an adjacency matrix (bottom) where columns and rows represent nodes and colored squares indicate functional connectivity between nodes. The color indicate functional
connectivity strength (i.e. darker ¼ stronger). (C) The weighted functional network can be used to determine the minimal spanning tree (MST), which only includes the strongest
connections, forming a functional network backbone. This determined MST can also be depicted as a network graph (top) or an adjacency matrix (bottom). Subsequently, MST
metrics, such as leaf fraction and diameter, can be determined. Abbreviations of channel labels are explained in the main text.

strength’, where higher values may indicate higher overall con- 1


Pn gjk ðiÞ
bci ¼ ðn1Þðn2Þ jsk;ksi;jsi gjk where gjk is the shortest path
nectivity. The following MST metrics were calculated at nodal or
between two nodes and gjk(i) is the number of node paths
network level:
that actually pass through i. We summarized the tree by
taking the maximum betweenness centrality.
i) Maximum node degree (nodal): every tree was summarized
viii) Closeness centrality (nodal): the inverse of the sum of all
by taking the maximum node degree: Smax, the node with
distances to other nodes (Sabidussi, 1966).
the maximum number of connections.
ii) Leaf number (Nleaf) (network): the number of nodes of the tree
with exactly one connection to any other node (with
maximum degree ¼ 1). A higher leaf number is related to 2.8. Statistical analyses
increased global efficiency and integration (Stam et al., 2014;
Tewarie et al., 2015). All statistical analyses were performed within a Bayesian
iii) Diameter (d) (network): the largest distance between any two framework. Differences between groups, eye conditions and in-
nodes in a tree, which has a lower bound of two and an upper teractions between groups and eye conditions were evaluated with
bound of m ¼ N e 1. The largest possible diameter will Bayes factors. Bayes factors were extracted from Bayesian model
decrease with increasing leaf number (Boersma et al., 2013; comparisons. This was done for all frequency bands separately. We
Stam et al., 2014; Tewarie et al., 2015). determined the model likelihood of a null model without an
iv) Eccentricity (network): the shortest path length between a interaction effect of group and condition as well as the likelihood of
tree node I and any other node from the tree. Eccentricity an alternative model with an interaction effect of group and con-
decreases when nodes become more central in the tree. dition for functional connectivity strength. This was done for all
v) Radius (nodal): the smallest node eccentricity in the tree. The MST metrics and for their potential relationship with sign language
lower the eccentricity, the more central a node in a tree. experience. Bayes factors give the ratio of model likelihoods, indi-
vi) Strength (nodal): the tree node strength is a summation of all cating which model is supported by the data. For example, if e
nodal connection weights (Hagmann et al., 2010; Rubinov given the data e a null model (M0) without an effect of condition
and Sporns, 2010). (i.e. eyes-open versus eyes-closed) on functional connectivity
vii) Maximum betweenness centrality (BCmax): a network hub strength has a very low probability, whereas an alternative model
metric which relies on the identification of the number of (M1) with an effect of condition on functional connectivity strength
shortest paths that pass through a node (Rubinov and Sporns, has a high probability, this would yield a high Bayes factor (e.g. 50).
2010). The more the passages, the higher the betweenness- This may be interpreted as very strong evidence for M1 as M1 is fifty
centrality (i.e. hubness), which is defined by times more likely than M0 in explaining the data. Table 2 gives an
overview of Bayes factors and their interpretation (Raftery, 1995).
38 M.R.T. Sinke et al. / Hearing Research 373 (2019) 32e47

Table 2
Bayes factors and their interpretations, based on (Raftery, 1995). M0: the baseline model without interaction term, M1: the alternative model with interaction term. In the
subsequent tables the Bayes factors for M1 are reported only.

Bayes factor Interpretation

> 100 Extreme evidence for M1


30 e 100 Very strong evidence for M1
10 e 30 Strong evidence for M1
3 e 10 Moderate evidence for M1
1 e 3 Anecdotal evidence for M1
1 No evidence
1/3 e 1 Anecdotal evidence for M0
1/10 e 1/3 Moderate evidence for M0
1/30 e 1/10 Strong evidence for M0
1/100 e 1/30 Very strong evidence for M0
< 1/100 Extreme evidence for M0

Since sex (Boersma et al., 2011) and age (Smit et al., 2012) influence functional connectivity strength were most pronounced in the
functional network topologies, we tested whether models with alpha frequency band with a significant reduction of 54.9% (95%
strong evidence were affected by sex- and age. confidence interval (CI): -68.2% to 41.6%) in controls and an even
All network analyses, statistical modeling and visualizations larger reduction of 88.0% (95% CI: -112% to 63.6% in deaf (Fig. 4B).
were performed in R (http://www.r-project.org/) using the pack- A decrease in functional connectivity strength from eyes-closed to
ages igraph, BayesFactor, reshape2, dplyr and ggplot2. Epoch data eyes-open was also present in the beta frequency band, with a
and scripts are freely available at the Open Science Framework in reduction of 27.7% (95% CI: -37.3 to 18.0) in controls and 36.1%
anonymized form (Otte et al., 2018b) and the GitHub repository (95% CI: -49.2 to 23.0) in deaf. These reductions in functional
(Otte et al., 2018a). connectivity strength in the alpha and beta band were supported
by Bayes factors of respectively >100 (i.e. labeled as ‘extreme evi-
3. Results dence’) and 2.6 (‘moderate evidence’) (Table 3).
Similar trends were seen for functional connectivity strength be-
3.1. Functional connectivity strength between auditory, visual and tween the occipital cortex and temporal cortex (Suppl. Figure 4 and
somatosensory cortices Suppl. Figure 7) as well as between the parietal cortex and tempo-
ral cortex (Suppl. Figure 5 and Suppl. Figure 8). However, for those
Fig. 4 and Suppl. Figure 6 show the functional connectivity connections no significant differences were found between controls
strength between the occipital cortex and the parietal cortex for the and deaf in functional connectivity reduction from eyes-open to eyes-
different frequency bands. Occipital-parietal functional connectiv- closed, as indicated by Bayes factors of <1 (Suppl. Table 1 and
ity strength was lower in the eyes-open condition compared to the Suppl. Table 2). The parietal-temporal delta-band connectivity
eyes-closed condition in the theta, alpha and beta frequency bands. strength was significantly more reduced from eyes-closed to eyes-
After transition from eyes-closed to eyes-open, differences in open in deaf, while no reduction was found in controls

Fig. 4. Functional connectivity strength between the occipital cortex and the parietal cortex. (A) The average functional connectivity, quantified with the phase lag index,
between O1/O2 and P7/P8 (y-axis) is shown for eyes-open and eyes-closed conditions (x-axis) and all frequency bands (top) in controls and deaf people. (B) The delta functional
connectivity between eyes-open en eyes-closed is plotted as percentage change relative to the eyes-open functional connectivity values for each frequency band, based on the data
shown in the left panel: D in % ¼ 100  (closed e open)/open.
M.R.T. Sinke et al. / Hearing Research 373 (2019) 32e47 39

Table 3 Furthermore, for the alpha band there was moderate to strong
Bayes factors of the interaction effect between condition (i.e. evidence for a larger increase in both betweenness and closeness
eyes-closed and eyes-open) and group (i.e. deaf and controls) in
relation to occipital-parietal functional connectivity strength
centrality in deaf. Anecdotal evidence was found for larger shifts in
(per frequency band). leaf number and diameter e in the alpha band e as well as in
closeness centrality e in the beta band e in deaf (Table 4). All ev-
Frequency Bayes factor
idence was still present if age and sex were added to the models.
Delta (0.5e4 Hz) 0.04 We found that the decrease in average connectivity strength
Theta (4e8 Hz) 0.07
(Suppl. Figure 9) and increase in closeness centrality
Alpha (8e16 Hz) >100 V
Beta (16e32 Hz) 2.39 I (Suppl. Figure 10) from eyes-closed to eyes-open was different
Gamma (32e64 Hz) 0.04 across the congenital, pre-lingual and post-lingual deafness types.
I
Anecdotal evidence, V
Extreme evidence for interaction.
This is supported by the Bayes factors in Suppl. Table 3.

3.4. Relation between backbone characteristics and American Sign


(Suppl. Figure 5), as indicated by the high Bayes factor (Suppl. Table 2). Language
All evidence was still present if age and sex were added to the models.
We investigated the relationship between sign language expe-
3.2. Functional backbone differences between eyes-open and eyes- rience and functional backbone characteristics. Initially, with no
closed conditions distinction made between deaf and controls. Sign language expe-
rience was related to altered backbone characteristics in the delta,
Transition from eyes-closed to eyes-open initiated visible theta and alpha band. The most pronounced effects were found in
changes in functional network topology, as indicated by changes in the theta band (Table 5), which are visualized in Figs. 6 and 7. More
backbone metrics. The most notable effects were found in the alpha specifically, for the theta band an increase in ASL experience was
and beta bands (Table 4), which are shown in Fig. 5. Overall the related to a higher average backbone connectivity strength and a
backbone leaf number, the average and maximum strength as well lower closeness centrality for the eyes-closed condition (Fig. 6).
as the kappa were lower in the eyes-open condition as compared to Furthermore, strong evidence was found for a positive relationship
the eyes-closed condition. In contrast, the diameter, eccentricity, between sign language experience and higher backbone connec-
radius as well as the median and maximum closeness centrality tivity strength for the delta band (Table 5). Fig. 7 shows the rela-
were higher in the eyes-open condition compared to the eyes- tionship between sign language experience and functional
closed condition. Interestingly, in contrast to deaf, controls did backbone characteristics for deaf subjects only. Again most pro-
not show a transition effect on some network metrics in the alpha nounced effects were found for the theta band (Table 6) with a
band (i.e. leaf number, eccentricity, radius and diameter). positive relation between sign language experience and higher
average backbone connectivity strength. All associations were still
3.3. Larger functional backbone modifications in deaf present if age and sex were added to the models.

Some functional backbone characteristics were different be- 4. Discussion


tween deaf and hearing controls (Fig. 5). In both the alpha and beta
band, functional backbone strength was stronger in the eyes-closed The present study used resting-state EEG to map functional
condition and weaker in the eyes-open condition in deaf as network backbone differences between deaf and hearing people.
compared to hearing controls. In the alpha band the leaf number We showed that transition from eyes-closed to eyes-open was
was lower in deaf, whereas the diameter and radius were larger in associated with changes in functional connectivity strength be-
the eyes-open condition. tween occipital and parietal lobes as well as changes in functional
Several functional backbone characteristics showed larger shifts backbone topology. In both deaf and hearing, these changes
in deaf than controls when comparing eyes-closed to eyes-open occurred especially in the alpha and beta frequency bands. More-
(Fig. 5). For both the alpha and beta band, evidence was found for over, the difference in functional connectivity strength as well as in
a larger decrease in the average and maximum backbone connec- functional backbone characteristics between eyes-closed to eyes-
tivity strength in deaf (as indicated by the Bayes factors in Table 4). open tended to be larger in deaf as compared to hearing controls.

Table 4
Bayes factors of the interaction effect between condition (i.e. eyes-closed and eyes-open) and group (i.e. deaf and controls) in relation to the network backbone metrics (shown
per frequency band).

MST-metric Delta band (0.5-4 Hz) Theta band (4e8 Hz) Alpha band (8e16 Hz) Beta band (16e32 Hz) Gamma band (32e64 Hz)

Strength (max) 0.09 0.05 >100 V 1.37 I 0.06


Strength (mean) 0.03 0.19 >100 V 57.82 IV
0.07
Degree (max) 0.04 0.06 0.27 0.06 0.09
BC (max) 0.02 0.06 0.08 0.06 0.06
BC (median) 0.06 0.06 4.63 II 0.06 0.14
CC (max) 0.04 0.27 26.64 III 1.59 I 0.09
CC (median) 0.04 0.26 93.37 IV 2.61 I 0.10
Leaf 0.07 0.06 2.31 I 0.08 0.09
Diameter 0.08 0.06 2.42 I 0.08 0.10
Eccentricity 0.07 0.06 0.39 0.07 0.08
Radius 0.08 0.05 0.16 0.06 0.06
Tree-hierarchy 0.06 0.05 0.73 0.14 0.11
Kappa 0.10 0.10 0.62 0.06 0.12
I II III IV V
BC ¼ betweenness centrality, CC ¼ closeness centrality, Anecdotal evidence, Moderate evidence, Strong evidence, Very strong evidence Extreme evidence for
interaction effect between group and condition.
40 M.R.T. Sinke et al. / Hearing Research 373 (2019) 32e47

Fig. 5. Network backbone comparisons between eyes-open and eyes-closed conditions for both deaf and controls. Functional network backbone characteristics in the alpha
band (8e16Hz) and beta band (16e32 Hz) (top), are shown for deaf (yellow) and controls (blue) for both the eyes-open and eyes-closed condition (x-axis) and indicated by the
following minimum spanning tree metrics (y-axis), (from top-left to bottom-right): diameter, maximum closeness centrality, median closeness centrality, leaf number, maximum
strength, mean strength, eccentricity, radius and kappa. Error bars represent the 95% confidence intervals.

4.1. Functional cortical remapping connectivity appears stronger in deaf, specifically in the alpha and
beta bands. This might indicate increased integration between the
We examined functional connectivity strength between sensory different sensory cortical regions in deaf. Our results are in agree-
regions involved in auditory, visual and somatosensory information ment with previous fMRI studies that also found increased audio-
processing. With some evidence we can state that functional visual connectivity in deaf (Bola et al., 2017; Li et al., 2016, 2013;
M.R.T. Sinke et al. / Hearing Research 373 (2019) 32e47 41

Table 5
Bayes factors of the relationship between the network backbone metrics and sign language experience in all participants.

MST-metric Delta band (0.5-4 Hz) Theta band (4e8 Hz) Alpha band (8e16 Hz) Beta band (16e32 Hz) Gamma band (32e64 Hz)

Strength (max) 20.92 III 0.73 0.07 0.04 0.05


Strength (mean) >100 V >100 V 66.84 IV
0.09 0.09
Degree (max) 0.22 0.06 0.37 0.08 0.04
BC (max) 3.94 II 0.05 0.06 0.06 0.05
BC (median) 0.07 0.05 0.09 0.04 0.05
CC (max) 0.91 2.37 I 62.60 IV
0.05 0.18
CC (median) 11.80 III 52.06 IV 46.38 IV
0.04 0.25
Leaf 0.04 0.04 0.21, 0.05 0.05
Diameter 0.04 0.04 0.21 0.04 0.05
Eccentricity 0.84 0.06 0.13 0.05 0.05
Radius 2.40 I 0.05 0.09 0.05 0.05
Tree-hierarchy 0.04 0.04 0.11 0.04 0.04
Kappa 0.17 0.05 0.86 0.08 0.04

BC ¼ betweenness centrality, CC ¼ closeness centrality, I Anecdotal evidence, II Moderate evidence, III Strong evidence, IV Very strong evidence, V Extreme evidence for relation
of MST metric with sign language experience.

Fig. 6. The relation between American Sign Language (ASL) and functional backbone characteristics. The sign language experience in years (x-axis) is related to functional
backbone characteristics (y-axis) in the theta band (4e8 Hz) for both the eyes-open (yellow) and eyes-closed (blue) condition, as indicated by the following minimum spanning tree
metrics (from top-left to bottom-right): mean strength, maximum degree, median betweenness centrality (BC), median closeness centrality (CC), leaf number, diameter, radius and
kappa. Shaded areas: 95% confidence intervals.

Shiell et al., 2014). With the shifts from an eyes-closed to an eyes- specifically by the enhanced sensitivity of the auditory cortex to
open condition we found a reduction in functional connectivity non-auditory stimuli (e.g. Almeida et al., 2015; Auer et al., 2010;
strength. This is expected given the shift from more ‘interoceptive’ Brozinsky and Bavelier, 2004; Dye et al., 2007; Finney and Dobkins,
awareness, characterized by integrative multisensory activity, to- 2001; Leva€nen and Hamdorf, 2001; Neville and Lawson, 1987a,
wards more ‘exteroceptive’ awareness focused on attention and 1987b) and increased audiovisual connectivity in deaf (Bola et al.,
oculomotor activity (Marx et al., 2004, 2003; Xu et al., 2014). Spe- 2017; Li et al., 2016, 2013; Shiell et al., 2014) together with the
cifically for the alpha band, this change may be related to ‘alpha reduced integrative multi-sensory activity in the eyes-open state
desynchronization’ during the eyes-open state (Barry et al., 2009;  mez-Ramírez et al., 2017; Xu et al., 2014).
(Go
Go mez-Ramírez et al., 2017; Xu et al., 2014). Moreover, we found
this reduction in functional connectivity strength from eyes-closed 4.2. Functional backbone differences between eyes-open and eyes-
to eyes-open to be larger in deaf than hearing controls. This may be closed conditions
explained by long-term auditory deprivation and related cross-
modal plasticity mechanisms (Bavelier and Neville, 2002; We found functional backbone differences between the eyes-
Merabet and Pascual-Leone, 2010). It may be explained more open and eyes-closed conditions in both deaf and hearing
42 M.R.T. Sinke et al. / Hearing Research 373 (2019) 32e47

Fig. 7. The relation between American Sign Language (ASL) and functional backbone characteristics in deaf subject only. The sign language experience in years (x-axis) related
to functional backbone characteristics (y-axis) in the theta band (4e8 Hz) for both the eyes-open (yellow) and eyes-closed (blue) condition, as indicated by the following minimum
spanning tree metrics (from top-left to bottom-right): mean strength, maximum degree, median betweenness centrality (BC), median closeness centrality (CC), leaf number,
diameter, radius and kappa. Shaded areas: 95% confidence intervals.

Table 6
Bayes factors of the relationship between the network backbone metric and ASL-experience in deaf people only.

MST-metric Delta band (0.5-4 Hz) Theta band (4e8 Hz) Alpha band (8e16 Hz) Beta band (16e32 Hz) Gamma band (32e64 Hz)
I
Strength (max) 1.56 3.37 II 0.05 0.12 0.10
Strength (mean) 0.18 43.35 IV 0.71 0.11 0.09
II
Degree (max) 8.14 0.22 0.51 0.10 0.16
I
BC (max) 1.97 0.06 0.07 0.06 0.07
BC (median) 0.61 0.45 0.09 0.06 0.07
I
CC (max) 0.04 0.22 2.08 0.06 0.07
I
CC (median) 0.07 0.99 1.75 0.07 0.07
Leaf 0.36 0.46 0.15 0.10 0.08
Diameter 0.38 0.46 0.16 0.10 0.08
Eccentricity 0.69 0.10 0.10 0.06 0.05
I
Radius 2.44 0.10 0.08 0.05 0.07
Tree-hierarchy 0.04 0.38 0.11 0.09 0.06
II
Kappa 5.00 0.44 0.50 0.21 0.09

BC ¼ betweenness centrality, CC ¼ closeness centrality, I Anecdotal evidence, II


Moderate evidence, IV
Very strong evidence for relation of MST metric with sign language
experience.

subjects, mainly in the alpha and beta bands. During the eyes-open 2007; Chen et al., 2008; Go mez-Ramírez et al., 2017; Marx et al.,
condition the functional network backbone showed an increased 2004; Xu et al., 2014). Other studies reported increased global ef-
diameter and eccentricity combined with a decreased leaf number ficiency and decreased clustering of functional networks in the
and connectivity strength, although hearing controls did not show alpha band (Miraglia et al., 2016; Tan et al., 2013) and beta-band
differences in diameter and leaf number in the alpha band. These (Gaal et al., 2010; Knyazev et al., 2015) in the eyes-open condition
network metrics indicate that the functional backbone topology in as compared to the eyes-closed condition, which seem to partly
the eyes-open condition was more chainlike (i.e. less integrated and contradict our findings. This could potentially be explained by the
with reduced global efficiency), whereas during the eyes-closed distinct age groups. One study showed similar effects as our study
condition the topology was more star-like (i.e. more functional in participants between 18 and 35 years, but the opposite effects in
integration and increased global efficiency) (Stam et al., 2014). participants between 51 and 80 years (Knyazev et al., 2015). This
These results also nicely fit with the reduced connectivity and pattern is in line with (Miraglia et al., 2016). However, two other
integration in the ‘exteroceptive’ state, as well as the alpha studies also showed contrasting results in younger participants
desynchronization, during the eyes-open condition (Barry et al., (Gaal et al., 2010; Tan et al., 2013). These discrepancies might be due
M.R.T. Sinke et al. / Hearing Research 373 (2019) 32e47 43

to methodological differences as two studies (i.e. Miraglia et al., comprehension requires higher cognitive functions. Increased use
2016; Tan et al., 2013) used a large number of EEG electrodes or and experience in sign language may therefore arguably enhance
regions of interest (i.e. respectively 128 and 84) compared to our functional integration between different brain regions as well as
study. Increased network sizes, with potential higher densities, cross-modal plasticity (Nishimura et al., 1999; Petitto, 2000; Sadato
may arguably affect alteration of topological network characteris- et al., 2005). Accordingly, we found a relation between years of sign
tics if analyzed with classical network metrics (van Wijk et al., language experience and functional backbone characteristics. This
2010). Network size also affects backbone metrics (Tewarie et al., was found most prominently in the theta band, but also in the delta
2014), which may explain the discrepancy between our study and and alpha bands. In both deaf and hearing, increasing experience in
Gaal et al. (2010). sign language was related to a higher average backbone strength,
suggesting increased global efficiency and integration. The
decreased closeness centrality suggests more segregation and local
4.3. Larger functional backbone modifications in deaf
clustering. Our reported relations with sign language experience
are in line with previous examinations of functional network dif-
We found some backbone differences between deaf and hearing
ferences between hearing signers and non-signers. That study
subjects. Functional backbones in deaf showed higher strength in
found sign-language comprehension to be related to increased
the eyes-closed, while lower strength during the eyes-open con-
local efficiency, small-worldness and modularity (i.e. segregation)
dition, in both the alpha and beta band. Also in the eyes-open
(Liu et al., 2017). To our knowledge no network studies were per-
condition, the leaf number was lower, while the radius and diam-
formed on sign-language comprehension in deaf. However, other
eter were larger, which means that the functional network topol-
connectivity studies in deaf showed enhanced functional connec-
ogy in this state is different between deaf and hearing. More
tivity between brain regions which are specifically recruited for
specifically, the functional backbone of deaf shows less integration
higher cognitive functions, such as comprehension of sign language
(i.e. reduced global efficiency) and increased clustering. Previous
(Li et al., 2016; Malaia et al., 2014). Further research is needed to
network studies in deaf also showed increased clustering and local
elucidate the mechanisms of reorganization in functional brain
efficiency, although the small-worldness was preserved (Kim et al.,
networks in relation to sign language acquisition and experience.
2014; Li et al., 2016).
Furthermore, we found larger functional backbone shifts in
these functional backbone characteristics, from the eyes-open to 4.5. Differences between frequency bands
the eyes-closed condition in deaf people. The patterns in functional
backbone strength correspond with changes we found in functional Our study shows that the effects of deafness, eyes-condition or
connectivity strength between the different sensory cortical re- sign-language experience are limited to specific frequency bands,
gions. They additionally indicate large-scale functional connectivity which suggests that auditory deprivation does not alter functional
changes, i.e. beyond the sensory cortices, since the functional networks as much in all frequency bands. Different brain networks
backbone topology is more altered in deaf when going from eyes- and behavioral functionalities are related to distinct frequency
closed to eyes-open. Accordingly, these findings may also be bands (Başar et al., 2000; Buschman and Miller, 2007; Klimesch,
explained by transition from ‘exteroceptive’ awareness to ‘intero- 1999; Wro bel, 2000; Wrobel et al., 2007). Cognitive tasks that
ceptive’ awareness (Marx et al., 2004, 2003; Xu et al., 2014) involve working memory, such as sign language, are mostly related
together with alterations in inter-regional connectivity e partly to alpha and theta activity (Klimesch, 1999; Stam and van Straaten,
due to alpha desynchronization (Barry et al., 2009; Go  mez-Ramírez 2012). This may explain the relationship we found between sign
et al., 2017; Xu et al., 2014) e combined with auditory deprivation language experience and functional backbone topology in those
and cross-modal plasticity (Bavelier and Neville, 2002; Merabet frequency bands. Furthermore, both the alpha and beta band are
and Pascual-Leone, 2010). related to attention, including visual attention (Stam and van
We also found that the functional backbone connectivity Straaten, 2012; Wro  bel, 2000; Wrobel et al., 2007). These bands
strength differed between the different forms of deafness. Func- alter by transition from eyes-closed (i.e. ‘interoceptive’ awareness)
tional backbone connectivity might depend on whether people are to eyes-open (i.e. ‘exteroceptive’ awareness) (Barry et al., 2009,
born deaf or acquired deafness later in life. Both congenital and pre- 2007; Go  mez-Ramírez et al., 2017). Overall, it seems therefore that
lingual deafness showed similar patterns as hearing controls, reorganization in topology of functional brain networks in deaf is
whereas post-lingual deaf showed the largest deviation, which is in strongly associated with cognitive functioning as well as with
agreement with previous findings. It has been shown that cross- attentional state. Further research is nonetheless needed to inves-
modal plasticity is dependent on the age of onset and duration of tigate this into more detail.
deafness (Brotherton et al., 2016; Li et al., 2016, 2013; Sadato et al.,
2004). However, given the limited number of post-lingual deaf in
4.6. Advantages of study design and tools
our study as well as the complex relationship of cross-modal
plasticity with other factors, such as age of onset, duration of
Our study used the unbiased MST approach (Tewarie et al., 2015)
deafness and sign-language experience, it is impossible to be
for network analyses. The methodology illustrates that acquired
conclusive.
backbone metrics are highly suitable in exploring the topology and
connectivity of brain networks and cross modal neuroplasticity
4.4. Relation between backbone characteristics and sign language (Engels et al., 2015; Tewarie et al., 2014; van Diessen et al., 2016).
experience Our study yielded a unique dataset of subjects with intermediate to
long periods of auditory deprivation combined with different levels
Sign language comprehension have shown to be related to of sign language experience. Such a dataset would be difficult to
activation of brain regions which are normally considered to be acquire in Western countries with well-established health-care
involved in unimodal (e.g. speech or sound) auditory processing, systems and many people with hearing disabilities equipped with a
such as frontal and temporal regions (Li et al., 2016; Liu et al., 2017; cochlear implant. Cochlear implants have shown to be related to
Malaia et al., 2014; Meyer et al., 2007; Neville et al., 1998; functional cortical reorganization (Strelnikov et al., 2010) and
Nishimura et al., 1999; Petitto, 2000; Sadato et al., 2005). Language almost normal developing auditory language processing (Hammes
44 M.R.T. Sinke et al. / Hearing Research 373 (2019) 32e47

et al., 2002). In our study population, none of the participants had a Contributors
cochlear implant. Lastly, our study shows the usefulness of a
portable EEG device. These mobile devices are invaluable tools for MRTS, JWB, EvD and WMO were involved in the study design.
use in rural or resource-limited settings. It enabled us to acquire MRTS, JWB, FvdM, JN and WMO were involved in the data acqui-
recordings from participants in a country where auditory depri- sition. MRTS, JWB, and WMO performed the data analysis. MRTS,
vation e and neurological disorders in general e are more preva- JWB, FvdM, JN, RMD, EvD and WMO interpreted the data and wrote
lent than in Western countries, but where neuroimaging research is the manuscript.
often impossible or burdensome due to geographic conditions,
poor health-care infrastructure and high levels of poverty. Funding

This work was supported by the Netherlands Organisation for


4.7. Study limitations and future directions
Scientific Research (NWO-VENI 016.168.038), and the Dutch Brain
Foundation [F2014(1)-06].
Our study has limitations. The neural brain activity was
measured at a limited amount of scalp locations. EEG also lacks
Disclosure/conflict of interest
information from deeper brain structures. The EEG signals are
linear combinations of the neural generators they project to the
None of the authors has any conflict of interest to disclose in
scalp location of the electrodes. The precision of functional network
relation to this work.
mapping is consequently reduced. Other neuroimaging techniques,
such as fMRI, might therefore be more capable to capture different
activation patterns of the whole brain at both cortical and sub- Appendix A. Supplementary data
cortical level. The use of wireless headsets for EEG recording may
have increased the noise in the EEG signal, and affected the back- Supplementary data to this article can be found online at
bone computations. However, recent studies have shown similar https://doi.org/10.1016/j.heares.2018.12.006.
performance of wireless headsets compared to standard EEG
hardware (Badcock et al., 2013; David Hairston et al., 2014; Schiatti References
et al., 2016). 
Almeida, J., He, D., Chen, Q., Mahon, B.Z., Zhang, F., Gonçalves, O.F., Fang, F., Bi, Y.,
History taking in our study might be affected by a recall bias. 2015. Decoding visual location from neural patterns in the auditory cortex of
Patient records (i.e. dates of births) in Nigeria are not stored like the congenitally deaf. Psychol. Sci. 26, 1771e1782. https://doi.org/10.1177/
0956797615598970.
they are in modern Western countries. Many people living in rural
Aspinall, P., Mavros, P., Coyne, R., Roe, J., 2013. The urban brain: analysing outdoor
Nigeria do not exactly know their birthday. This bias may have physical activity with mobile EEG. Br. J. Sports Med. 1e6. https://doi.org/
increased the noise-level in the regression analysis. 10.1136/bjsports-2012-091877.
Apart from backbone analysis future studies may try alternative Auer, E., Bernstein, L., Sungkarat, W., Singh, M., 2010. Vibrotactile activation of the
auditory cortices in deaf versus hearing adults. Changes 38, 319e335. https://
promising network analysis techniques such as dynamic functional doi.org/10.1146/annurev.neuro.31.060407.125627.Brain.
connectivity modeling (Avena-Koenigsberger et al., 2017; Avena-Koenigsberger, A., Misic, B., Sporns, O., 2017. Communication dynamics in
Breakspear, 2017), Bayesian exponential random graph models complex brain networks. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 19, 17e33. https://doi.org/10.1038/
nrn.2017.149.
(Caimo and Friel, 2011; Sinke et al., 2016), mixed-effect models Badcock, N.A., Mousikou, P., Mahajan, Y., de Lissa, P., Thie, J., McArthur, G., 2013.
(Simpson and Laurienti, 2015) and Gibbs distribution models (La Validation of the Emotiv EPOC ® EEG gaming system for measuring research
Rosa et al., 2016). These techniques also enable unbiased compar- quality auditory ERPs. PeerJ 1, e38. https://doi.org/10.7717/peerj.38.
Badcock, N.A., Preece, K.A., de Wit, B., Glenn, K., Fieder, N., Thie, J., McArthur, G.,
ison of networks differing in size and density but may capture more 2015. Validation of the Emotiv EPOC EEG system for research quality auditory
subtle differences between groups. Combining techniques might event-related potentials in children. PeerJ 3. https://doi.org/10.7717/peerj.907
further elucidate the role of specific brain areas in functional e907.
Barry, R.J., Clarke, A.R., Johnstone, S.J., Brown, C.R., 2009. EEG differences in children
network alterations in normal and sensory lacking conditions. This
between eyes-closed and eyes-open resting conditions. Clin. Neurophysiol. 120,
may ultimately improve our understanding of neuroplasticity 1806e1811. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.clinph.2009.08.006.
occurring after auditory and other types of sensory deprivation. Barry, R.J., Clarke, A.R., Johnstone, S.J., Magee, C.A., Rushby, J.A., 2007. EEG differ-
ences between eyes-closed and eyes-open resting conditions. Clin. Neuro-
physiol. 118, 2765e2773. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.clinph.2007.07.028.
5. Conclusion Başar, E., Başar-Eroglu, C., Karakaş, S., Schürmann, M., 2000. Gamma, alpha, delta,
and theta oscillations govern cognitive processes. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 39,
241e248. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0167-8760(00)00145-8.
In conclusion, we were able to detect functional backbone dif- Bassett, D.S., Bullmore, E.T., 2009. Human brain networks in health and disease.
ferences between eyes-closed and eyes-open conditions as well as Curr. Opin. Neurol. 22, 340e347. https://doi.org/10.1097/
WCO.0b013e32832d93dd.Human.
larger shifts in functional backbone characteristics in deaf as Bassett, D.S., Sporns, O., 2017. Network neuroscience. Nat. Neurosci. 20, 353e364.
compared to controls. Effects that are presumably a consequence of https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.4502.Network.
auditory deprivation and cross-modal plasticity. Subtle differences Bassett, D.S., Zurn, P., Gold, J.I., 2018. On the nature and use of models in network
neuroscience. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 19, 566e578. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41583-
were seen between different forms of deafness. Our study 018-0038-8.
demonstrated functional network backbone characteristics to be Bavelier, D., Dye, M.W.G., Hauser, P.C., 2006. Do deaf individuals see better? Trends
related with increasing experience of sign language. Our study Cognit. Sci. 10, 512e518. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2006.09.006.
Bavelier, D., Neville, H.J., 2002. Cross-modal plasticity: where and how? Nat. Rev.
provide original insights into the organization and reorganization Neurosci. 3, 443e452. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn848.
of functional brain networks derived from EEG data, both in deaf Bavelier, D., Tomann, A., Hutton, C., Mitchell, T., Corina, D., Liu, G., Neville, H., 2000.
and healthy people. Our results further underpin the notion of Visual attention to the periphery is enhanced in congenitally deaf individuals.
J. Neurosci. 20. RC93-RC93.
brain-wide neuroplasticity mechanisms and global network reor-
Boersma, M., Smit, D.J.A., Boomsma, D.I., De Geus, E.J.C., Delemarre-van de
ganization in the cortex of deaf, which emphasize the importance Waal, H.A., Stam, C.J., 2013. Growing trees in child brains: graph theoretical
to study the brain e as well as cross-modal plasticity e from a analysis of electroencephalography-derived minimum spanning tree in 5- and
network perspective. The link between the functional network 7-year-old children reflects brain maturation. Brain Connect. 3, 50e60. https://
doi.org/10.1089/brain.2012.0106.
backbone characteristics and acquired sign language experience Boersma, M., Smit, D.J.A., De Bie, H.M.A., Van Baal, G.C.M., Boomsma, D.I., De
reflects ongoing brain adaptation in both hearing and deaf people. Geus, E.J.C., Delemarre-Van De Waal, H.A., Stam, C.J., 2011. Network analysis of
M.R.T. Sinke et al. / Hearing Research 373 (2019) 32e47 45

resting state EEG in the developing young brain: structure comes with matu- pp. 1e26. https://doi.org/10.1101/118174.
ration. Hum. Brain Mapp. 32, 413e425. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.21030. Hackett, T.A., 2012. Information flow in the auditory cortical network. Hear. Res.
Bola, Ł., Zimmermann, M., Mostowski, P., Jednoro  g, K., Marchewka, A., Rutkowski, P., 271, 133e146. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.heares.2010.01.011.Information.
Szwed, M., 2017. Task-specific reorganization of the auditory cortex in deaf Hagmann, P., Sporns, O., Madan, N., Cammoun, L., Pienaar, R., Wedeen, V.J., Meuli, R.,
humans. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 114, E600eE609. https://doi.org/10.1073/ Thiran, J.-P., Grant, P.E., 2010. White matter maturation reshapes structural
pnas.1609000114. connectivity in the late developing human brain. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A
Bosworth, R.G., Dobkins, K.R., 2002. The effects of spatial attention on motion 107, 19067e19072. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1009073107.
processing in deaf signers, hearing signers, and hearing nonsigners. Brain Cogn. Hammes, D.M., Novak, M.A., Rotz, L.A., Willis, M., Edmondson, D.M., Thomas, J.F.,
49, 152e169. https://doi.org/10.1006/brcg.2001.1497. 2002. Early identification and cochlear implantation: critical factors for spoken
Breakspear, M., 2017. Dynamic models of large-scale brain activity. Nat. Neurosci. language development. Ann. Otol. Rhinol. Laryngol. Suppl. 189, 74e78.
20, 340e352. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.4497. Hauser, P.C., Dye, M.W.G., Boutla, M., Green, C.S., Bavelier, D., 2007. Deafness and
Brotherton, H., Plack, C.J., Schaette, R., Munro, K.J., 2016. Time course and frequency visual enumeration: not all aspects of attention are modified by deafness. Brain
specificity of sub-cortical plasticity in adults following acute unilateral depri- Res. 1153, 178e187. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2007.03.065.
vation. Hear. Res. 341, 210e219. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.heares.2016.09.003. Heuvel, M.P. Van Den, Kahn, R.S., Gon ~ i, J., Sporns, O., 2012. Brain communication.
Brozinsky, C.J., Bavelier, D., 2004. Motion velocity thresholds in deaf signers: Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A 109, 11372e11377. https://doi.org/10.1073/
changes in lateralization but not in overall sensitivity. Cognit. Brain Res. 21, pnas.1203593109/-/DCSupplemental. www.pnas.org/cgi/doi/10.1073/pnas.
1e10. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2004.05.002. 1203593109.
Buckley, K.A., Tobey, E.A., 2011. Cross-modal plasticity and speech perception in pre- Hidalgo, C.A., Klinger, B., Barabasi, A.-L., Hausmann, R., 2007. The product space
and postlingually deaf cochlear implant users. Ear Hear. 32, 2e15. https:// conditions the development of nations. Science (80-. ) 317, 482e487. https://
doi.org/10.1097/AUD.0b013e3181e8534c. doi.org/10.1126/science.1144581.
Bullmore, E., Barnes, A., Bassett, D.S., Fornito, A., Kitzbichler, M., Meunier, D., Hüfner, K., Stephan, T., Flanagin, V.L., Deutschla €nder, A., Stein, A., Kalla, R., Dera, T.,
Suckling, J., 2009. Generic aspects of complexity in brain imaging data and Fesl, G., Jahn, K., Strupp, M., Brandt, T., 2009. Differential effects of eyes open or
other biological systems. Neuroimage 47, 1125e1134. https://doi.org/10.1016/ closed in darkness on brain activation patterns in blind subjects. Neurosci. Lett.
j.neuroimage.2009.05.032. 466, 30e34. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2009.09.010.
Bullmore, E., Sporns, O., 2012. The economy of brain network organization. Nat. Rev. Hüfner, K., Stephan, T., Glasauer, S., Kalla, R., Riedel, E., Deutschla €nder, A., Dera, T.,
Neurosci. 13, 336e349. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn3214. Wiesmann, M., Strupp, M., Brandt, T., 2008. Differences in saccade-evoked brain
Bullmore, E., Sporns, O., 2009. Complex brain networks: graph theoretical analysis activation patterns with eyes open or eyes closed in complete darkness. Exp.
of structural and functional systems. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 10, 186e198. https:// Brain Res. 186, 419e430. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-007-1247-y.
doi.org/10.1038/nrn2575. Jackson, T.S., Read, N., 2010. Theory of minimum spanning trees. I. Mean-field
Buschman, T.J., Miller, E.K., 2007. Top-down versus bottom-up control of attention theory and strongly disordered spin-glass model. Phys. Rev. E - Stat.
in the prefrontal and posterior parietal cortices. Science (80- 315, 1860e1862. Nonlinear Soft Matter Phys. 81, 1e18.
https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1138071. Karns, C.M., Dow, M.W., Neville, H.J., 2012. Altered cross-modal processing in the
Caimo, A., Friel, N., 2011. Bayesian inference for exponential random graph models. primary auditory cortex of congenitally deaf adults: a visual-somatosensory
Soc. Network. 33, 41e55. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socnet.2010.09.004. fMRI study with a double-flash illusion. J. Neurosci. 32, 9626e9638. https://
Campbell, J., Sharma, A., 2014. Cross-modal re-organization in adults with early doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6488-11.2012.
stage hearing loss. PLoS One 9. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0090594. Kim, E., Kang, H., Lee, H., Lee, H.-J., Suh, M.-W., Song, J.-J., Oh, S.-H., Lee, D.S., 2014.
Campbell, J., Sharma, A., 2013. Compensatory changes in cortical resource allocation Morphological brain network assessed using graph theory and network filtra-
in adults with hearing loss. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 7, 1e9. https://doi.org/ tion in deaf adults. Hear. Res. 315, 88e98. https://doi.org/10.1016/
10.3389/fnsys.2013.00071. j.heares.2014.06.007.
Chen, A.C.N., Feng, W., Zhao, H., Yin, Y., Wang, P., 2008. EEG default mode network Klimesch, W., 1999. EEG alpha and theta oscillations reflect cognitive and memory
in the human brain: spectral regional field powers. Neuroimage 41, 561e574. performance: a review and analysis. Brain Res. Rev. 29, 169e195. https://
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.12.064. doi.org/10.1016/S0165-0173(98)00056-3.
David Hairston, W., Whitaker, K.W., Ries, A.J., Vettel, J.M., Cortney Bradford, J., Knyazev, G.G., Volf, N.V., Belousova, L.V., 2015. Age-related differences in electro-
Kerick, S.E., McDowell, K., 2014. Usability of four commercially-oriented EEG encephalogram connectivity and network topology. Neurobiol. Aging 36,
systems. J. Neural. Eng. 11, 046018. https://doi.org/10.1088/1741-2560/11/4/ 1849e1859. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2015.02.007.
046018. Kruskal, J.B., 1956. On the shortest spanning subtree of a graph and the traveling
Dewey, R.S., Hartley, D.E.H., 2015. Cortical cross-modal plasticity following deafness salesman problem. Proc. Am. Math. Soc. 7, 48e50. https://doi.org/10.2307/
measured using functional near-infrared spectroscopy. Hear. Res. 325, 55e63. 2033241.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.heares.2015.03.007. La Rosa, P.S., Brooks, T.L., Deych, E., Shands, B., Prior, F., Larson-Prior, L.J.,
Ding, H., Qin, W., Liang, M., Ming, D., Wan, B., Li, Q., Yu, C., 2015. Cross-modal Shannon, W.D., 2016. Gibbs distribution for statistical analysis of graphical data
activation of auditory regions during visuo-spatial working memory in early with a sample application to fcMRI brain images. Stat. Med. 35, 566e580.
deafness. Brain 138, 2750e2765. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awv165. https://doi.org/10.1002/sim.6757.
Doucet, M.E., Bergeron, F., Lassonde, M., Ferron, P., Lepore, F., 2006. Cross-modal Leva€nen, S., Hamdorf, D., 2001. Feeling vibrations: enhanced tactile sensitivity in
reorganization and speech perception in cochlear implant users. Brain 129, congenitally deaf humans. Neurosci. Lett. 301, 75e77. https://doi.org/10.1016/
3376e3383. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awl264. S0304-3940(01)01597-X.
Durkin, M., 2002. The epidemiology of developmental disabilities in low-income Li, W., Li, J., Wang, J., Zhou, P., Wang, Z., Xian, J., He, H., 2016. Functional re-
countries. Ment. Retard. Dev. Disabil. Res. Rev. 8, 206e211. https://doi.org/ organizations of brain network in prelingually deaf adolescents. Neural Plast.
10.1002/mrdd.10039. 2016, 1e10. https://doi.org/10.1155/2016/9849087.
Dye, M.W.G., Baril, D.E., Bavelier, D., 2007. Which aspects of visual attention are Li, W., Li, J., Wang, Z., Li, Y., Liu, Z., Yan, F., Xian, J., He, H., 2015. Grey matter con-
changed by deafness? The case of the Attentional Network Test. Neuro- nectivity within and between auditory, language and visual systems in pre-
psychologia 45, 1801e1811. https://doi.org/10.1016/ lingually deaf adolescents. Restor. Neurol. Neurosci. 33, 279e290. https://
j.neuropsychologia.2006.12.019. doi.org/10.3233/RNN-140437.
Eleweke, C.J., Rodda, M., 2002. The challenge of enhancing inclusive education in Li, Y., Booth, J.R., Peng, D., Zang, Y., Li, J., Yan, C., Ding, G., 2013. Altered intra- and
developing countries. Int. J. Incl. Educ. 6, 113e126. https://doi.org/10.1080/ inter-regional synchronization of superior temporal cortex in deaf people.
13603110110067190. Cerebr. Cortex 23, 1988e1996. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhs185.
Engels, M.M.A., Stam, C.J., van der Flier, W.M., Scheltens, P., de Waal, H., van Liu, L., Yan, X., Liu, J., Xia, M., Lu, C., Emmorey, K., Chu, M., Ding, G., 2017. Graph
Straaten, E.C.W., 2015. Declining functional connectivity and changing hub lo- theoretical analysis of functional network for comprehension of sign language.
cations in Alzheimer's disease: an EEG study. BMC Neurol. 15, 145. https:// Brain Res. 1671, 55e66. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2017.06.031.
doi.org/10.1186/s12883-015-0400-7. Malaia, E., Talavage, T.M., Wilbur, R.B., 2014. Functional connectivity in task-
Finney, E.M., Dobkins, K.R., 2001. Visual contrast sensitivity in deaf versus hearing negative network of the Deaf: effects of sign language experience. PeerJ 2,
populations: exploring the perceptual consequences of auditory deprivation e446. https://doi.org/10.7717/peerj.446.
and experience with a visual language. Cognit. Brain Res. 11, 171e183. https:// Marx, E., Deutschl€ ander, A., Stephan, T., Dieterich, M., Wiesmann, M., Brandt, T.,
doi.org/10.1016/S0926-6410(00)00082-3. 2004. Eyes open and eyes closed as rest conditions: impact on brain activation
Finney, E.M., Fine, I., Dobkins, K.R., 2001. Visual stimuli activate auditory cortex in patterns. Neuroimage 21, 1818e1824. https://doi.org/10.1016/
the deaf. Nat. Neurosci. 4, 1171e1173. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn763. j.neuroimage.2003.12.026.
Fraschini, M., Demuru, M., Crobe, A., Marrosu, F., Stam, C.J., Hillebrand, A., 2016. The Marx, E., Stephan, T., Nolte, A., Deutschl€ ander, A., Seelos, K.C., Dieterich, M.,
effect of epoch length on estimated EEG functional connectivity and brain Brandt, T., 2003. Eye closure in darkness animates sensory systems. Neuroimage
network organisation. J. Neural. Eng. 13, 036015. https://doi.org/10.1088/1741- 19, 924e934. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1053-8119(03)00150-2.
2560/13/3/036015. McMahan, T., Parberry, I., Parsons, T.D., 2015. Modality specific assessment of video
Gaal, Z.A., Boha, R., Stam, C.J., Molnar, M., 2010. Age-dependent features of EEG- game player's experience using the Emotiv. Entertain. Comput. 7, 1e6. https://
reactivity-Spectral, complexity, and network characteristics. Neurosci. Lett. doi.org/10.1016/j.entcom.2015.03.001.
479, 79e84. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2010.05.037. Merabet, L.B., Pascual-Leone, A., 2010. Neural reorganization following sensory loss:
Go mez-Ramírez, J., Freedman, S., Mateos, D., Pe rez-Vela zquez, J.L., Valiante, T., 2017. the opportunity of change. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 11, 44e52. https://doi.org/
Eyes Closed or Eyes Open? Exploring the Alpha Desynchronization Hypothesis 10.1038/nrn2758.
in Resting State Functional Connectivity Networks with Intracranial EEG, Meredith, M.A., Lomber, S.G., 2011. Somatosensory and visual crossmodal plasticity
46 M.R.T. Sinke et al. / Hearing Research 373 (2019) 32e47

in the anterior auditory field of early-deaf cats. Hear. Res. 280, 38e47. https:// j.neuroimage.2016.04.066.
doi.org/10.1016/j.heares.2011.02.004. Sinke, Michel RT., Otte, Willem M., van Meer, Maurits PA., van der Toorn, Annette,
Meyer, M., Toepel, U., Keller, J., Nussbaumer, D., Zysset, S., Friederici, A.D., 2007. Dijkhuizen, Rick M., 2018. Modified structural network backbone in the con-
Neuroplasticity of sign language: implications from structural and functional tralesional hemisphere chronically after stroke in rat brain. J. Cereb. Blood Flow
brain imaging. Restor. Neurol. Neurosci. 25, 335e351. Metab. Suppl. 38 (9), 1642e1653.
Miraglia, F., Vecchio, F., Bramanti, P., Rossini, P.M., 2016. EEG characteristics in “eyes- Smit, D.J.A., Boersma, M., Schnack, H.G., Micheloyannis, S., Boomsma, D.I., Hulshoff
open” versus “eyes-closed” conditions: small-world network architecture in Pol, H.E., Stam, C.J., de Geus, E.J., 2012. The brain matures with stronger func-
healthy aging and age-related brain degeneration. Clin. Neurophysiol. 127, tional connectivity and decreased randomness of its network. PLoS One 7.
1261e1268. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.clinph.2015.07.040. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0036896 e36896.
Mulwafu, W., Kuper, H., Ensink, R.J.H., 2016. Prevalence and causes of hearing Smit, D.J.A., Geus, E.J.C. de, Boersma, M., Boomsma, D.I., Stam, C.J., 2016. Life-span
impairment in Africa. Trop. Med. Int. Health 21, 158e165. https://doi.org/ development of brain network integration assessed with Phase Lag Index
10.1111/tmi.12640. connectivity and minimum spanning tree graphs. Brain Connect. 8, 1e38.
Neville, H.J., Bavelier, D., Corina, D., Rauschecker, J., Karni, A., Lalwani, A., Braun, A., https://doi.org/10.1089/brain.2015.0359.
Clark, V., Jezzard, P., Turner, R., 1998. Cerebral organization for language in deaf Smith, K.M., Mecoli, M.D., Altaye, M., Komlos, M., Maitra, R., Eaton, K.P., Egelhoff, J.C.,
and hearing subjects: biological constraints and effects of experience. Proc. Holland, S.K., 2011. Morphometric differences in the heschl's gyrus of hearing
Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A 95, 922e929. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.95.3.922. impaired and normal hearing infants. Cerebr. Cortex 21, 991e998. https://
Neville, H.J., Lawson, D., 1987a. Attention to central and peripheral visual space in a doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhq164.
movement detection task: II. Brain Res. 405, 268e283. Stam, C.J., 2014. Modern network science of neurological disorders. Nat. Rev. Neu-
Neville, H.J., Lawson, D., 1987b. Attention to central and peripheral visual space in a rosci. 15, 683e695. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn3801.
movement detection task: III. Brain Res. 405, 284e294. Stam, C.J., Nolte, G., Daffertshofer, A., 2007. Phase lag index: assessment of func-
Nishimura, H., Hashikawa, K., Doi, K., Iwaki, T., Watanabe, Y., Kusuoka, H., tional connectivity from multi channel EEG and MEG with diminished bias from
Nishimura, T., Kubo, T., 1999. Sign language “heard” in the auditory cortex. common sources. Hum. Brain Mapp. 28, 1178e1193. https://doi.org/10.1002/
Nature 397, 116. https://doi.org/10.1038/16376. hbm.20346.
Otte, W.M., Sinke, M.R.T., Van Diessen, E., 2018a. Functional Brain Network Analysis Stam, C.J., Tewarie, P., Van Dellen, E., van Straaten, E.C.W., Hillebrand, a, Van
with Minimum Spanning Trees. https://zenodo.org/record/2066281#. Mieghem, P., 2014. The trees and the forest: characterization of complex brain
XAudsRNKhDU. networks with minimum spanning trees. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 92, 129e138.
Otte, W.M., Sinke, M.R.T., van Diessen, E., Dijkhuizen, R.M., 2018b. Functional con- https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2014.04.001.
nectivity and brain networks in deaf and hearing. Open Sci. Framew. Stam, C.J., van Straaten, E.C.W., 2012. The organization of physiological brain net-
Otte, W.M., van Diessen, E., Paul, S., Ramaswamy, R., Subramanyam Rallabandi, V.P., works. Clin. Neurophysiol. 123, 1067e1087. https://doi.org/10.1016/
Stam, C.J., Roy, P.K., 2015. Aging alterations in whole-brain networks during j.clinph.2012.01.011.
adulthood mapped with the minimum spanning tree indices: the interplay of Stevens, G., Flaxman, S., Brunskill, E., Mascarenhas, M., Mathers, C.D., Finucane, M.,
density, connectivity cost and life-time trajectory. Neuroimage 109, 171e189. 2013. Global and regional hearing impairment prevalence: an analysis of 42
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.01.011. studies in 29 countries. Eur. J. Public Health 23, 146e152. https://doi.org/
nicaud, S., Klein, D., Zatorre, R.J., Chen, J.K., Witcher, P., Hyde, K., Mayberry, R.I.,
Pe 10.1093/eurpub/ckr176.
2013. Structural brain changes linked to delayed first language acquisition in Strelnikov, K., Rouger, J., Demonet, J.F., Lagleyre, S., Fraysse, B., Deguine, O.,
congenitally deaf individuals. Neuroimage 66, 42e49. https://doi.org/10.1016/ Barone, P., 2010. Does brain activity at rest reflect adaptive strategies? evidence
j.neuroimage.2012.09.076. from speech processing after cochlear implantation. Cerebr. Cortex 20,
Petitto, L.A., 2000. Speech-like cerebral activity in profoundly deaf people pro- 1217e1222. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhp183.
cessing signed languages: implications for the neural basis of human language. Tan, B., Kong, X., Yang, P., Jin, Z., Li, L., 2013. The difference of brain functional
Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 97, 13961e13966. connectivity between eyes-closed and eyes-open using graph theoretical
€rtner, K., 2014. Community-based inclusive education: best practices from
Pfo analysis. Comput. Math. Meth. Med. 1e15. https://doi.org/10.1155/2013/976365.
Nicaragua, El Salvador, Guatemala and Honduras. Disabil. CBR Incl. Dev. 25, Tewarie, P., Hillebrand, A., Schoonheim, M.M., van Dijk, B.W., Geurts, J.J.G.,
72e81. https://doi.org/10.5463/DCID.v25i1.321. Barkhof, F., Polman, C.H., Stam, C.J., 2014. Functional brain network analysis
Pillai, J., Sperling, M.R., 2006. Interictal EEG and the diagnosis of epilepsy. Epilepsia using minimum spanning trees in Multiple Sclerosis: an MEG source-space
47, 14e22. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1528-1167.2006.00654.x. study. Neuroimage 88, 308e318. https://doi.org/10.1016/
Prause, N., Siegle, G.J., Deblieck, C., Wu, A., Iacoboni, M., 2016. EEG to primary re- j.neuroimage.2013.10.022.
wards: predictive utility and malleability by brain stimulation. PLoS One 11. Tewarie, P., van Dellen, E., Hillebrand, A., Stam, C.J., 2015. The minimum spanning
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0165646. tree: an unbiased method for brain network analysis. Neuroimage 104,
Ptito, M., Giguere, J.F., Boire, D., Frost, D.O., Casanova, C., 2001. When the auditory 177e188. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.10.015.
cortex turns visual. Prog. Brain Res. 134, 447e458. van den Heuvel, M.P., Hulshoff Pol, H.E., 2010. Exploring the brain network: a re-
Raftery, A.E., 1995. Bayesian model selection in social research. Socio. Methodol. view on resting-state fMRI functional connectivity. Eur. Neuro-
https://doi.org/10.2307/271063. psychopharmacol. 20, 519e534. https://doi.org/10.1016/
Rubinov, M., Sporns, O., 2010. Complex network measures of brain connectivity: j.euroneuro.2010.03.008.
uses and interpretations. Neuroimage 52, 1059e1069. https://doi.org/10.1016/ van Diessen, E., Numan, T., van Dellen, E., van der Kooi, A.W., Boersma, M.,
j.neuroimage.2009.10.003. Hofman, D., van Lutterveld, R., van Dijk, B.W., van Straaten, E.C.W.,
Sabidussi, G., 1966. The centrality index of a graph. Psychometrika 31, 581e603. Hillebrand, A., Stam, C.J., 2015. Opportunities and methodological challenges in
Sadato, N., Okada, T., Honda, M., Matsuki, K.-I., Yoshida, M., Kashikura, K.-I., EEG and MEG resting state functional brain network research. Clin. Neuro-
Takei, W., Sato, T., Kochiyama, T., Yonekura, Y., 2005. Cross-modal integration physiol. 126, 1468e1481. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.clinph.2014.11.018.
and plastic changes revealed by lip movement, random-dot motion and sign van Diessen, E., Otte, W.M., Braun, K.P.J., Stam, C.J., Jansen, F.E., 2014. Does sleep
languages in the hearing and deaf. Cerebr. Cortex 15, 1113e1122. https://doi.org/ deprivation alter functional EEG networks in children with focal epilepsy?
10.1093/cercor/bhh210. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 8, 67. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnsys.2014.00067.
Sadato, N., Yamada, H., Okada, T., Yoshida, M., Hasegawa, T., Matsuki, K.-I., van Diessen, E., Otte, W.M., Stam, C.J., Braun, K.P.J., Jansen, F.E., 2016. Electroen-
Yonekura, Y., Itoh, H., 2004. Age-dependent plasticity in the superior temporal cephalography based functional networks in newly diagnosed childhood epi-
sulcus in deaf humans: a functional MRI study. BMC Neurosci. 5, 1e6. https:// lepsies. Clin. Neurophysiol. 127, 2325e2332. https://doi.org/10.1016/
doi.org/10.1186/1471-2202-5-56. j.clinph.2016.03.015.
Schiatti, L., Faes, L., Tessadori, J., Barresi, G., Mattos, L., 2016. Mutual information- van Wijk, B.C.M., Stam, C.J., Daffertshofer, A., 2010. Comparing brain networks of
based feature selection for low-cost BCIs based on motor imagery. In: Proc. different size and connectivity density using graph theory. PLoS One 5. https://
Annu. Int. Conf. IEEE Eng. Med. Biol. Soc. EMBS 2016eOctob, pp. 2772e2775. doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0013701 e13701.
https://doi.org/10.1109/EMBC.2016.7591305. Vohr, B., 2003. Infants and children with hearing loss - part 1. Ment. Retard. Dev.
Scott, G.D., Karns, C.M., Dow, M.W., Stevens, C., Neville, H.J., 2014. Enhanced pe- Disabil. Res. Rev. 9, 218e219. https://doi.org/10.1002/mrdd.10082.
ripheral visual processing in congenitally deaf humans is supported by multiple Vourkas, M., Karakonstantaki, E., Simos, P.G., Tsirka, V., Antonakakis, M.,
brain regions, including primary auditory cortex. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8, 1e9. Vamvoukas, M., Stam, C.J., Dimitriadis, S., Micheloyannis, S., 2014. Simple and
https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00177. difficult mathematics in children: a minimum spanning tree EEG network
Shiell, M.M., Champoux, F., Zatorre, R.J., 2016. The right hemisphere planum tem- analysis. Neurosci. Lett. 576, 28e33. https://doi.org/10.1016/
porale supports enhanced visual motion detection ability in deaf people: evi- j.neulet.2014.05.048.
dence from cortical thickness. Neural Plast. 1e10. https://doi.org/10.1155/2016/ Watts, D.J., Strogatz, S.H., 1998. Collective dynamics of `small-world’ networks.
7217630. Nature 393, 440e442.
Shiell, M.M., Champoux, F., Zatorre, R.J., 2014. Reorganization of auditory cortex in WHO, 2014. WHO j Deafness and Hearing Loss. World Heal. Organ. https://doi.org//
early-deaf people: functional connectivity and relationship to hearing aid use. entity/mediacentre/factsheets/fs300/en/index.html.
J. Cognit. Neurosci. 27, 150e163. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn. WHO, 2012. WHO global estimates on prevalence of hearing loss. World Heal. Or-
Simpson, S.L., Laurienti, P.J., 2015. A two-part mixed-effects modeling framework gan. 1e15. https://doi.org/10.1002/2014GB005021.
for analyzing whole- brain network data. Neuroimage 113, 310e319. Wrobel, A., 2000. Beta activity: a Carrier for visual attention Andrzej. Acta Neuro-
Sinke, M.R.T., Dijkhuizen, R.M., Caimo, A., Stam, C.J., Otte, W.M., 2016. Bayesian biol. Exp. 60, 247e260. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.procs.2015.07.351.
exponential random graph modeling of whole-brain structural networks across Wrobel, A., Ghazaryan, A., Bekisz, M., Bogdan, W., Kaminski, J., 2007. Two streams
lifespan. Neuroimage 135, 79e91. https://doi.org/10.1016/ ofAttention-dependent B activity in the striate recipient zone of cat's lateral
M.R.T. Sinke et al. / Hearing Research 373 (2019) 32e47 47

posteriorepulvinar complex. J. Neurosci. 27, 2230e2240. https://doi.org/ Yu, J.-H., Sim, K.-B., 2016. Classification of color imagination using Emotiv EPOC and
10.1080/00268976.2013.805848. event-related potential in electroencephalogram. Opt. - Int. J. Light Electron
Xu, P., Huang, R., Wang, J., Van Dam, N.T., Xie, T., Dong, Z., Chen, C., Gu, R., Zang, Y.F., Opt. 127, 9711e9718. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijleo.2016.07.074.
He, Y., Fan, J., Luo, Y. jia, 2014. Different topological organization of human brain Zalesky, A., Fornito, A., Bullmore, E.T., 2010. Network-based statistic: identifying
functional networks with eyes open versus eyes closed. Neuroimage 90, differences in brain networks. Neuroimage 53, 1197e1207. https://doi.org/
246e255. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.12.060. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.06.041.
Yoshida, H., Kanda, Y., Takahashi, H., Miyamoto, I., Chiba, K., 2011. Observation of Zhang, D., Liang, B., Wu, X., Wang, Z., Xu, P., Chang, S., Liu, B., Liu, M., Huang, R., 2015.
cortical activity during speech stimulation in prelingually deafened adults with Directionality of large-scale resting-state brain networks during eyes open and
cochlear implantation by positron emission tomography-computed tomogra- eyes closed conditions. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 9, 1e10. https://doi.org/10.3389/
phy. Ann. Otol. Rhinol. Laryngol. 120, 499e504. https://doi.org/10.1177/ fnhum.2015.00081.
000348941112000802.

You might also like