You are on page 1of 9

RESEARCH REPORT

A TEMPORAL LOBE FACTOR IN VERB FLUENCY


Per Östberg*, Raffaella M. Crinelli*, Rimma Danielsson, Lars-Olof Wahlund, Nenad Bogdanovic
and Sven-Erik Fernaeus
(Section for Clinical Geriatrics, Neurotec Department, Karolinska Institutet, Karolinska University Hospital, Huddinge,
Stockholm, Sweden; *Equal contribution)

ABSTRACT

Verb fluency requires self-sustained verb retrieval. The brain correlates of this task are virtually unknown. We
investigated the relations between verb and noun (semantic) fluency and regional brain perfusion in subjects with varying
degrees of cognitive decline, ranging from very mild subjective impairment to Alzheimer’s disease (AD). Data consisted of
single-photon emission computed tomography (SPECT) data and temporally resolved verb and noun fluency scores from
93 participants. Impaired verb fluency was predicted by a temporal lobe hypoperfusion factor and low education, whereas
high age and low perfusion in the parietotemporal-occipital region predicted impaired noun fluency. Analysis of perfusion
within the temporal region indicated primary involvement of the temporal pole and medial temporal lobe in AD. This
might reflect pathology of the anterior parahippocampal region, which appears early in neurodegenerative disease. Although
temporal lobe structures have not usually been implicated in verb processing, early temporal pathology thus appears to
contribute to impaired verb fluency in cognitive decline.

Key words: dementia, language, mild cognitive impairment, nouns, verbs

INTRODUCTION lexical category: things, or more specifically,


physical objects for nouns; and (human) activities
Verb and noun deficits figure prominently among for verbs. Nouns certainly express all physical
neurocognitive dissociations. In fact, the double object concepts but can also express virtually any
dissociation of impaired verb/noun production was other conceptual category, including state or
already documented in the mid-18th century (Denes activity concepts normally associated with verbs.
and Dalla Barba, 1998; Östberg, 2003). The PET studies with conceptually matched noun and
linguistic basis for such observations goes back to verb stimuli for lexical decision have yielded
antiquity, however, with the division of words into largely overlapping or identical brain activation
eight parts-of-speech by Dionysius Thrax (Robins, patterns (Perani et al., 1999; Tyler et al., 2001). It
1989). In Dionysius’ classification, a verb or rhēma would appear that the complex interrelationships
lacked case inflection but was inflected for tense, between lexical category, conceptual structure, and
person, and number. Semantically, verbs were phonological structure in lexical items preclude a
defined as signifying an activity or a process straightforward neural localization of noun and
performed or undergone. A noun or ónoma, in verb processing (Black and Chiat, 2003). It has
contrast, was characterized by case inflection and by been proposed, however, that verb processing
signifying a concrete or abstract entity. requires more executive resources than noun
Today, verbs and nouns are considered processing, implying that disproportionate problems
universally distinct lexical categories, although a with verbs are to be expected in various brain
few languages appear to lack the distinction pathologies. Action naming was indeed more
between nouns and verbs (see Baker, 2003, for impaired than object naming in frontotemporal
discussion). The brain correlates of verb and noun dementia, and correlated with executive
processing remain unclear. In particular, it remains dysfunction rather than linguistic dysfunction as in
obscure whether verbs and nouns as lexical Alzheimer’s disease (AD) (Silveri et al., 2003; see
categories depend on differential brain systems or also Cappa et al., 1998).
regions. Verb deficits are generally held to be Recent studies have employed the verb fluency
associated with frontal lobe lesions, whereas noun task as a measure of verb processing (Piatt et al.,
deficits are associated with posterior lesions, in 1999a, 1999b, 2004; Östberg et al., 2005). Verb
particular of the left temporal lobe. This general fluency requires self-sustained enumeration of
conclusion, however, is weakened by the tacit activity verbs during one minute. It may be
assumption of a one-to-one relationship between construed as a “pure” measure of verb retrieval in
lexical category and conceptual category. contrast to action picture naming, which involves
Experimental stimuli have typically been drawn visual perception and access to semantics from the
from conceptual categories associated with each visual system, and in contrast to verb generation in

Cortex, (2007) 43, 607-615


608 Per Östberg and Others

which verbs are produced in response to stimulus superordinate. Thirdly, the “lead-in” concepts for
nouns. In contrast to lexical decision, verb fluency verb and noun fluency differ in clarity: compare
also engages the speech production system. It is “things that people do” with “animals”.
disproportionately impaired, as compared to noun PD and AD are neurodegenerative diseases that
and letter-based word fluency, in Parkinson’s may have relatively long preclinical and
disease (PD) with dementia (Piatt et al., 1999a). oligosymptomatic phases. Patients do not meet
Piatt et al. (1999a) suggested that the task is clinical diagnostic criteria until the
sensitive to the striatal loop pathology associated neuropathological process has spread into several
with PD. A comparable verb fluency deficit was brain regions and caused marked devastation in
observed in persons with HIV-1 disease, which is structures that are affected early (Braak and Braak,
also associated with striatal loop pathology (Woods 1991; Braak et al., 2003). Up to now, however, the
et al., 2005). Verb fluency has been studied in brain correlates of verb and noun fluency deficits
healthy elderly subjects (Piatt et al., 1999b, 2004). have not been studied in subjects with milder
It was found to be in modest to moderate relation grades of cognitive decline, i.e., with less severe
to tests of executive function, but unrelated to brain involvement. The neuropathological lesions
episodic memory and picture (i.e. noun) naming. of Alzheimer’s and Parkinson’s diseases appear
This suggests that verb fluency taps aspects of very early in the anterior parahippocampal region
executive function not measured by standard tests. including the perirhinal and entorhinal cortex.
Further statistical evidence for distinctness of the Severe neuronal loss in this region can precede
verb fluency test comes from factor analyses of dementia with several years, and in PD
verb, letter-based, and noun (category) fluency, in parahippocampal atrophy also appears in the same
which verb fluency loads on a separate factor stage of the disease process as the well-known
(Östberg et al., 2005). nigrostriatal pathology (Braak et al., 2003). Since
Verb fluency contrasts with letter-based (or lesions in the parahippocampal region not only
phonemic) fluency, that requires the enumeration of produce deficits in recognition memory but also in
words that begin with a given letter, and with noun executive function (Davachi and Goldman-Rakic,
fluency (or category fluency or semantic fluency), 2001; Chavoix et al., 2002), it is possible that verb
that requires subjects to enumerate nouns from a fluency is affected not only by striatal loop
taxonomy such as animals or a semantic field such pathology but also by early temporal lobe
as supermarket items. Noun fluency is particularly neurodegenerative changes that isolate the higher
impaired in AD (Monsch et al., 1997), semantic centers of the limbic loop (Östberg et al., 2005).
dementia (Hodges et al., 1992), schizophrenia The present study was aimed at investigating a
(Kremen et al., 2003), depression (Fossati et al, possible relation between impaired performance in
2003; c.f., Ravdin et al., 2003), and – as compared verb and noun fluency tasks and perfusion deficits
to letter-based fluency – PD (Henry and Crawford, in specific brain areas in subjects with different
2004). levels of cognitive impairment. In particular, we
In contrast to verb fluency, noun fluency and hypothesized that signs of temporal lobe pathology
letter-based fluency are widely used in the are associated with decreased verb fluency. Toward
assessment of dementia and other cognitive that end, we used regional hypoperfusion factors
disorders. Although both verb and noun fluency are derived from single-photon emission computed
semantically oriented fluency tasks, they differ in tomography (SPECT) and temporally resolved verb
important regards. First, nouns are typically used and noun fluency scores from participants assessed
for referring, whereas verbs are typically used to at a memory disorders clinic, ranging from very
predicate. The task of enumerating single verbs mild subjective cognitive decline to mild (objective)
may therefore be considered less “natural” for the cognitive impairment and AD. Mild cognitive
speaker than enumerating single nouns from a impairment (MCI) is associated with a strongly
well-defined category. Secondly, a nominal increased risk for dementia of the Alzheimer’s type;
category such as animals represents a taxonomy. indeed up to 84% of MCI patients show Alzheimer-
This means that a particular hierarchical sense type changes on neuropathological examination
relation, taxonymy (Cruse, 1986), holds between (Morris et al., 2001).
the target words. The targeted word set is therefore
characterized by a relatively “tight” configuration.
The word set targeted by verb fluency, in contrast, MATERIAL AND METHOD
is less tightly defined (‘human activities’), with
limited taxonomical structure. Some taxonomical Participants
structure or troponymy does exist among verbs but
is more shallow and “bushy” than taxonymy The participants (n = 93) were examined at the
among nouns (Fellbaum, 1998). Verb taxonomies memory clinic, Department of Geriatrics,
thus tend to have fewer hierarchical levels than Karolinska University Hospital, Huddinge, because
noun taxonomies, with a relatively large number of of cognitive complaints. This is a secondary/tertiary
subordinate verbs (troponyms) for each referral center with referrals from e.g. occupational
Temporal factor in verb fluency 609

TABLE I Braak and Braak, 1991; high limbic stage,


Means and standard deviations in age, years of education and Mesulam, 2000). Magnetic resonance imaging
MMSE scores for the three diagnostic groups in this study
disclosed two small cerebral infarctions (right
Age Education MMSE parietal and left temporal) in one MCI case; this
AD 64.8; 9.4 11.6; 4.0 23.4; 3.4 was regarded a case of vascular cognitive
MCI 60.5; 8.7 12.5; 4.0 27.6; 2.1 impairment. The other participants with MCI did
SCI 56.9; 7.4 12.9; 2.7 29.3; 0.8 not show evidence of cerebrovascular pathology.

Alzheimer’s Disease (AD)


physicians, general practitioners, and neurologists.
Patients are relatively young and in general do not AD was diagnosed according to the International
have cerebrovascular disease (see below). The Classification of Diseases, tenth edition (ICD-10).
examination included physical and psychiatric AD is usually diagnosed when the Alzheimer-type
evaluations, computed tomography or magnetic changes have caused widespread destruction of
resonance imaging scans, electroencephalography, neocortical areas, i.e. in advanced stages of
cerebral single-photon emission computed pathological changes (neocortical stages V and VI,
tomography, lumbar puncture, blood analyses, and Braak and Braak, 1991; low and high neocortical
neuropsychological assessment. All participants stages of Mesulam, 2000). The overall cognitive
spoke Swedish. Participants were drawn from three impairment in this group with a clinical diagnosis of
diagnostic categories reflecting three levels of AD was mild (mean MMSE score 23.4).
cognitive functioning: Subjective Cognitive It should be noted that there were significant
Impairment (n = 30), Mild Cognitive Impairment (n differences between the three diagnoses in age, F (2,
= 30), and Alzheimer’s disease (n = 33). 90) = 6.58, p < .01, and in MMSE scores, F (2, 89) =
Descriptive statistics for the participants’ age, 50.72, p < .001, but not in years of education. Fisher
education, and Mini-Mental State Examination least significant difference (LSD) post-hoc analysis
(MMSE) scores are given in Table I. showed significant differences in age between AD
and SCI. Moreover, there were significant
Subjective Cognitive Impairment (SCI) differences in MMSE scores between AD and MCI,
AD and SCI, and between MCI and SCI.
SCI was diagnosed in cases of memory
complaint without objective signs of cognitive Cerebral Single-Photon Emission Computed
decline. These patients had been referred for Tomography (SPECT)
cognitive complaints and had been thoroughly
assessed without any signs of cognitive impairment Acquisition
being found. SCI corresponds to level 2 of the
Global Deterioration Scale, i.e. very mild cognitive All participants underwent a SPECT scan at the
decline or Age-Associated Memory Impairment, Department of Radiology, Karolinska University
although objective impairment was ruled out by Hospital, Huddinge. The scan was performed as
neuropsychological assessment in our patients. SCI part of a routine work-up for dementia. Data were
is likely to contain many normal individuals but acquired as follows. Each participant was injected
might also possibly reflect very early neuronal loss with 1000 Mbq Tc-99m hexamethylpropylenamine
or Alzheimer-type changes in the anterior medial oxime (HMPAO; Ceretec, Amersham Ltd). Data
temporal lobe (transentorhinal stages I or II, Braak acquisition started 30 minutes after the injection. A
and Braak, 1991; low limbic stage, Mesulam, 2000). single-headed rotating gamma camera (Siemens
Diacam) was used for collecting data in 64
Mild Cognitive Impairment (MCI) projections, evenly spread through 360°. Total
acquisition time was 32 minutes. Tomographic
MCI was diagnosed according to the following slices were reconstructed using an iterative
criteria (Wahlund et al., 2003): (a) subjective algorithm (Hosem, Nuclear Diagnostics AB,
memory complaint; (b) objective signs (1.5 Sweden) with Chang attenuation correction. The
standard deviation units below age-matched attenuation coefficient was 0.12 cm– 1. The data
controls) of decline in any cognitive domain; (c) were formatted as a 3D data set with 64 × 64 × 64
intact activities of daily living; (d) not fulfilling the cubic voxels with 3.5 mm sides. The resolution in
DSM-IV or ICD-10 criteria for dementia. This a tomographic slice was 10.2 mm full width at half
definition of MCI might also cover preclinical maximum (FWHM). The data sets were post-
stages of non-Alzheimer neurodegenerations; filtered with a Butterworth filter, cutoff 1.0 cm– 1.
patients that fulfilled criteria for frontotemporal
lobar degeneration or PD were not diagnosed as BRASS Analysis
MCI, however. Pathoanatomically, MCI may reflect
more severe Alzheimer-type changes in the The BRASS software was used for image
temporal lobe than in SCI (limbic stages III and IV, registration and quantification (Radau et al., 2001).
610 Per Östberg and Others

The data sets were iteratively registered using 9 TABLE II

parameter linear registration to a normal template, SPECT variables and abbreviations


using normalized mutual information as similarity Variable Abbreviation
function. The relative cerebral blood flow in the
Left lentiform nucleus* LNL
selected regions was calculated as cerebellar ratios Right lentiform nucleus* RNL
(mean count per voxel of region/mean count per Left caudate nucleus LCN
voxel of bilateral cerebellar cortex). The Right caudate nucleus RCN
Left thalamus LT
information obtained from the SPECT scans Right thalamus RT
consisted of measurements of relative blood flow Left sensorimotor cortex LSCTX
(cerebellar ratios) in 42 brain areas normalized to Right sensorimotor cortex RSCTX
four regions of the cerebellum (see Table II with Left occipital cortex LOCTX
Right occipital cortex ROCTX
abbreviations). The use of the cerebellum as a Left superior parietal lobule LSPL
reference region is a better choice when Right superior parietal lobule RSPL
investigating AD subjects than global cerebral blood Left anterior dorsal frontal cortex LADFCTX
flow. This is because large parts of the brain can be Right anterior dorsal frontal cortex RADFCTX
Left posterior dorsal frontal cortex LPDFCTX
affected in AD, thus lowering the global ratio (Syed Right posterior dorsal frontal cortex RPDFCTX
et al., 1992). Four of the 42 regions did not show Left anterior orbital frontal cortex LAOFCTX
hypoperfusion in any of the subjects: left and right Right anterior orbital frontal cortex RAOFCTX
lentiform nuclei and left and right anterior cingulate Left posterior orbital frontal cortex LPOFCTX
Right anterior orbital frontal cortex RPOFCTX
gyri. These were excluded from further analysis.
Left parietotemporal cortex LPTCTX
Right parietotemporal cortex RPTCTX
Word Fluency Tasks Left medial temporal lobe LMTL
Right medial temporal lobe RMTL
Participants performed two fluency tasks: noun Left lateral temporal lobe LLTL
Right lateral temporal lobe RLTL
fluency and verb fluency. Scores were divided into Left posterior temporal lobe LPTL
six 10-seconds intervals as described in Fernaeus and Right posterior temporal lobe RPTL
Almkvist (1998), according to which the number of Left temporal pole LTP
appropriate words was recorded at each interval. Right temporal pole RTP
Left insular cortex LICTX
Noun fluency was measured using the animal Right insular cortex RICTX
naming task. Here, subjects were requested to name Left anterior cingulate gyrus* LACG
as many sorts of animals as possible during one Right anterior cingulate gyrus* RACG
minute. Verb fluency was measured by the verb or Left posterior cingulate gyrus LPCG
Right posterior cingulate gyrus RPCG
action fluency task (Piatt et al., 1999a, 1999b).
Subjects were instructed to tell by single words as Pons and midbrain P&M
Left anterior subcortical LAS
many things as possible that a person can do, Right anterior subcortical RAS
exemplified by “swim” and “smell”. Activity verbs Left posterior subcortical LPS
Right posterior subcortical RPS
such as “cut” but also state verbs such as “love” were Other subcortical OS
scored as appropriate responses. Nouns derived from
*Not analyzed further.
verbs and phrases using the same verb were not
accepted (for example, ‘drive a car’, ‘drive a train’).
analyses were applied, with age, education and
Statistical Analysis mean hypoperfusion factor scores as predictor
variables and verb fluency and noun fluency mean
Statistical analyses were conducted using the scores per interval as dependent variables. This was
Statistica package (4.1) for PC. Principal factor done in order to find out whether specific
analysis with Varimax rotation was undertaken (i) hypoperfusion factors as well as age and education
on noun and verb fluency interval scores (12 would predict noun or verb fluency performance.
variables in sum) to determine the number of
factors involved in verb and noun fluency Ethical Approval
performance in the sample and (ii) on SPECT
regional perfusion quotes to uncover patterns of The Ethical Committee at Karolinska Institutet,
decreased perfusion spanning larger anatomical Karolinska University Hospital, Huddinge,
regions. Multivariate analysis of variance approved this study (registration number 342/03).
(MANOVA) was applied in order to assess (i)
differences in hypoperfusion factor scores between
the three diagnostic groups; (ii) effects of RESULTS
diagnostic group and type of fluency test (noun vs.
verb); (iii) effects of diagnostic group and fluency Factor Analysis of Perfusion Patterns
score interval (noun 1-6, verb 1-6). Fisher LSD
post-hoc analysis was used to assess whether Principal factor analysis of the SPECT data
differences were significant. Multiple regression revealed seven factors with eigenvalues above one.
Temporal factor in verb fluency 611

TABLE III

Varimax rotated factor loadings based on SPECT data

Hypoperfusion factors
SBC DF-C OF TL PTO BS SP-C
Variable
LCN – .74 – .13 .22 – .22 – .02 – .17 – .21
RCN – .81 – .09 .18 – .14 – .07 – .22 – .16
LT – .78 – .22 .10 – .30 – .05 – .07 – .18
RT – .76 – .12 .05 – .29 – .07 – .15 – .20
LSCTX .04 – .73 – .12 – .11 – .17 .05 .55
RSCTX – .02 – .76 – .06 – .09 – .16 .02 .56
LOCTX – .04 – .18 – .21 – .24 – .75 .07 .30
ROCTX – .09 – .24 – .19 – .14 – .77 .21 .25
LSPL .24 – .44 – .14 – .03 – .23 – .03 .75
RSPL .18 – .45 – .07 – .01 – .28 .02 .75
LADFCTX – .10 – .88 – .23 .06 – .08 – .00 – .04
RADFCTX – .07 – .87 – .19 .01 – .17 – .05 – .06
LPDFCTX – .17 – .90 – .10 – .03 – .13 – .01 .19
RPDFCTX – .18 – .90 – .07 – .11 – .09 – .03 .19
LAOFCTX .11 – .20 – .89 .01 – .15 .09 – .02
RAOFCTX .11 – .18 – .89 .03 – .25 .02 .04
LPOCTX – .25 – .26 – .72 – .37 .07 .10 .10
RPOCTX – .27 – .23 – .75 – .36 .08 .02 .20
LPTCTX – .44 – .37 – .01 – .35 – .59 – .07 .11
RPTCTX – .35 – .46 .05 – .32 – .62 .01 – .00
LMTL – .25 .22 – .44 – .66 – .12 .04 .10
RMTL – .26 .12 – .42 – .61 – .06 .10 .09
LLTL – .34 – .21 – .17 – .75 – .29 .10 – .01
RLTL – .31 – .13 – .06 – .78 – .32 .04 – .02
LPTL – .52 – .16 – .00 – .58 – .41 .14 – .07
RPTL – .18 – .14 .07 – .58 – .36 .08 – .22
LTP – .22 – .00 – .02 – .77 .00 .14 .02
RTP – .26 .06 – .01 – .79 – .05 – .02 .14
LICTX – .60 – .31 – .19 – .50 .02 .19 .05
RICTX – .62 – .26 – .20 – .47 .12 .06 .01
LPCG – .58 – .08 – .16 – .06 – .35 .11 .32
RPCG – .75 .07 – .16 – .11 – .25 .24 .16
P&M – .21 .02 – .20 – .15 – .08 .85 .08
LAS – .86 – .14 – .17 – .23 .11 .12 .06
RAS – .87 – .06 – .18 – .21 .11 .05 .05
LPS – .87 .0 – .06 – .23 – .27 .05 – .05
RPS – .85 .14 – .07 – .21 – .28 .07 – .07
OS .18 .03 .02 – .16 – .07 .90 – .06
Variance .22 .15 .10 .14 .08 .05 .05
Note. Loadings ≥ .55 are boldface. For abbreviations, see Table II.

These factors were Varimax rotated and labeled by – The Brainstem (BS) Factor included: Pons
their highest loadings from the anatomical regions; and medulla and “other subcortical”.
they are listed below (see Table III for factor – Finally, the Superoparietal-Central (SP-C)
loadings). Factor included: Left and right sensorimotor cortex
– The Subcortical (SBC) Factor included: Left and left and right superior parietal lobule.
and right caudate nucleus, left and right thalamus, As can be seen in Table III there was decreased
left and right insular cortex, left and right posterior perfusion in both hemispheres for all seven factors.
cingulate gyrus, left and right anterior subcortical,
and left and right posterior subcortical. Diagnostic Differences in Large-Scale
– The Dorsofrontal-Central (DF-C) Factor Hypoperfusion Patterns
included: Left and right sensorimotor cortex, left
and right anterior dorsal frontal cortex, and left and Hypoperfusion factors scores for each subject
right posterior dorsal frontal cortex. were derived on the basis of the seven-factor
– The Orbitofrontal (OF) Factor included: Left solution. Further analysis of the hypoperfusion
and right anterior orbital frontal cortex and left and factor scores using MANOVA revealed significant
right posterior orbital frontal cortex. differences between AD on one hand and SCI and
– The Temporal Lobe (TL) Factor included: MCI on the other in SPECT factor scores overall,
Left and right medial temporal lobe, left and right F (2, 89) = 11.92, p < .001. Post-hoc analysis
lateral temporal lobe, left and right posterior showed significant differences between SCI on one
temporal lobe, and left and right temporal pole. hand and MCI and AD on the other in the SBC
– The Parietotemporal-Occipital (PTO) Factor factor (see Figures 1 and 2; AD vs. SCI: LSD, p <
included: Left and right occipital cortex and left .001; MCI vs. SCI: LSD, p < .02) and significant
and right parietotemporal cortex. differences between AD and SCI in the TL factor
612 Per Östberg and Others

TABLE IV
Factor loadings derived from Principal Factors analysis of noun
fluency (ANIM1-6) and verb fluency (VERB1-6) interval scores

Variable NF factor VF factor


ANIM1 .58 .40
ANIM2 .72 .35
ANIM3 .81 .19
ANIM4 .76 .21
ANIM5 .67 .18
ANIM6 .71 .29
VERB1 .39 .64
VERB2 .35 .62
VERB3 .32 .68
VERB4 .24 .68
Fig. 1 – Mean factor scores in the diagnostic groups AD, VERB5 .28 .66
MCI and SCI across the seven hypoperfusion factors. VERB6 .04 .78
1: Subcortical; 2: Dorsofrontal-Central; 3: Orbitofrontal; 4:
Temporal Lobe; 5: Parietotemporal-Occipital; 6: Brainstem; 7: Variance 30 27
Superoparietal-Central. Note. Loadings > .55 are boldface.

lobe, and the posterior temporal lobe. A significant


interaction was found, Rao R (6, 176) = 2.47, p <
.05. This indicated significant differences in the
temporal pole and medial temporal lobe between
SCI and MCI on one hand and AD on the other,
with decreased perfusion in AD. There was also a
significant difference between MCI and SCI in the
temporal pole, with decreased perfusion in MCI
(LSD, p < .01).

Factor Analysis of Noun and Verb Fluency


Fig. 2 – Mean scores in hypoperfusion for the TL (Temporal
Lobe) factor in the three diagnostic groups. Fluency data were also factor-analyzed using
the six interval scores from noun fluency and verb
fluency, i.e. 12 variables in all as input to the
Principal Factors analysis. Two factors, the noun
fluency factor and the verb fluency factor, emerged
after Varimax rotation. Table IV shows that the two
factors correspond to the two types of test (noun
vs. verb); the factors explain 30 and 27 percent
each of the total variance.

Effects of Diagnostic Group, Fluency Type, and


Fluency Interval

There was a significant main effect of diagnostic


Fig. 3 – Mean scores in noun and verb fluency. group in fluency performance, F (2, 89) = 46.7, p <
.001. There was also a significant effect of type of
(LSD, p < .05). There were no significant test, F (1, 90) = 63.56, p < .001, indicating higher
differences between MCI and the other diagnoses performance in noun fluency than in verb fluency.
in the TL factor, see Figures 1 and 3. In the PTO Moreover, there was a significant interaction
factor there was a significant difference between between type of fluency and diagnostic group, F (2,
SCI and MCI (LSD, p < .05) and between AD and 90) = 7.22, p < .01, indicating a larger difference
SCI on the other, see Figure 1 (LSD, p < .001). between SCI and MCI on one hand and AD on the
other in noun fluency, see Figure 3. There was also
Effects of Diagnostic Group on Regional Temporal a significant effect of interval, F (5, 450) = 119.85,
Lobe Perfusion p < .001, and a significant interaction between type
of test and interval, F (5, 450) = 2.40, p < .05,
The eight temporal SPECT variables loading on indicating higher scores in the initial intervals in
the TL factor were included in a MANOVA with noun fluency. Fisher LSD post-hoc analysis showed
diagnostic group (AD, MCI, SCI) as independent a significant difference in noun fluency tasks
variable. The dependent variables were the SPECT between SCI and MCI on one hand and AD on the
scores (for each hemisphere) from the temporal other (p < .01) and a significant difference in verb
pole, the medial temporal lobe, the lateral temporal fluency between AD and SCI (p < .001).
Temporal factor in verb fluency 613

TABLE V
Prediction of fluency factors based on multiple regression analysis

a. Prediction of noun fluency


Variable BETA St. Err. of BETA B St. Err. of B t(92) p-level
Age – .44 .09 – .05 .01 – 4.75 .001
PTO – .21 .09 – .21 .09 – 2.38 .020

b. Prediction of verb fluency


Variable BETA St. Err. of BETA B St. Err. of B t(92) p-level
Education .42 .09 .12 .03 4.6 .001
TL – .24 .09 – .24 .09 – 2.7 .010

Prediction of Verb and Noun Fluency Using explanation for the positive influence of
Hypoperfusion Factors, Age, and Education educational level on verb fluency. Within the
temporal region, the temporal poles and the medial
Multiple regression analyses were applied temporal lobes were primarily affected in MCI and
including the noun fluency and verb fluency factors AD, with a significant decrease in perfusion in the
as predicted variables and age, education and the temporal poles already in MCI. This temporal
hypoperfusion factors as predictor variables. With a subregion contains the anterior parahippocampal
strict criterion of including variables only with F- region, including the perirhinal and entorhinal
values for beta weights > 4, two variables cortices that are affected early and severely by the
predicted noun fluency, R = .47, p < .001, namely neuropathological changes seen in Alzheimer’s and
age and the PTO factor (see Table Va).With the PD (Braak and Braak, 1991; Mesulam, 2000;
application of the same criterion, two variables Braak et al., 2003). These areas are not discernible
predicted verb fluency, R = 49, p < . 001, namely on SPECT scans, but their involvement can be
education and the TL factor (see Table Vb). inferred by hypoperfusion observed in adjacent
areas. The anterior parahippocampal region has not
been implicated in verb processing previously.
DISCUSSION Rather, verbs are held to depend on frontal
networks, and verb deficits are typically observed
This study used factor analysis as a method of in subjects with frontal lesions (Damasio and
distinguishing large-scale patterns of decreased Tranel, 1993).
brain perfusion and two types of word fluency. One explanation for the temporal lobe factor in
Temporally resolved verb and noun fluency scores verb fluency may be inherent task differences
loaded on different factors, indicating that these between verb and noun fluency. Such differences
tasks are indeed distinct as proposed by Piatt et al. exist between the word relations specific to each
(1999a, 1999b). Verb and noun fluency were also task. Thus, noun fluency – as represented by the
associated with partly different anatomical regions. animal naming task – targets a taxonomy, a word
Our data did not point toward a lateralized neuronal set that is familiar, hierarchically structured, and
substrates of verb and noun fluency. This is not to easily defined. The semantic field targeted by verb
be expected, however, as semantic neuronal sets are fluency is less coherent, requiring actively
thought to be bihemispherically represented structured retrieval. The perirhinal cortex is
(Pulvermüller, 2003) and the statistical techniques important in this regard through its connections to
used here detect correlations rather than differences. the frontal motor cortex (Kyuhou and Gemba,
Decreased noun fluency was predicted by 2002) and subcortical projections to the basal
higher age (see Kozora and Cullum, 1995) and ganglia, basal forebrain, and amygdala (for review
decreased perfusion in the parietotemporal-occipital see Burwell, 2000). It is particularly noteworthy
regions. This makes sense, since these regions that accumulation of the pathological form of α-
support object knowledge and thus concrete noun synuclein (a pathognomonic marker for PD) takes
processing (Pulvermüller, 2003; Fuster, 2003) and place in the deep neurons of the perirhinal cortex
degenerate heavily in PD (Braak and Braak, 1991). in PD (Braak et al., 2003). These neurons are the
Verb fluency, in contrast, was predicted by main source of subcortical and cortical projections.
education (see Piatt et al., 2004) and a temporal What looks like frontal-subcortical deficits in
hypoperfusion factor. Awareness of verbs as a cognitive functioning may thus derive to some
grammatical category is obviously not necessary extent from lesions in specific areas of the
for producing verbs in response to the instruction temporal lobe, resulting in disconnection; this
“things that people do”. Well-educated subjects appears plausible also for the verb processing
may however be better prepared for the task by deficits observed in PD. The fact that limbic loop
using a grammatical frame such as the sign of the and striatal loop deficits may coexist in certain
infinitive for self-prompting; this might be one disorders like PD (c.f., HIV-1 infection; Woods et
614 Per Östberg and Others

al., 2005) clearly complicates inferences about tasks: Functional mapping with 2-DG. Journal of
Neurophysiology, 85: 2590-2601, 2001.
lexical processing based on a temporolimbic- DENES G and DALLA BARBA G. G.B. Vico, precursor of cognitive
frontostriatal dichotomy. It is possible, however, neuropsychology? The first reported case of noun-verb
that more detailed analysis of word production dissociation following brain damage. Brain and Language,
62: 29-33, 1998.
might reveal more fine-grained clinico-pathological FELLBAUM C. A semantic network of English verbs. In Fellbaum C
correlations. For instance, the syntactic valence of (Ed), Wordnet: An Electronic Lexical Database. Cambridge,
verbs (i.e., how many syntactic arguments a verb MA: MIT Press, 1998.
FERNAEUS SE and ALMKVIST O. Word production: Dissociation of
takes) does not appear to influence the verb deficit two retrieval modes of semantic memory across time. Journal
in PD (Kim and Thompson, 2004). This is in of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 20: 137-143,
contrast with agrammatic subjects, who typically 1998.
FOSSATI P, LE BASTARD G, ERGIS AM and ALLILAIRE JF.
have frontal lobe lesions. As verb fluency does not Qualitative analysis of verbal fluency in depression.
involve syntactic processing, at least not overtly, it Psychiatry Research, 117: 17-24, 2003.
FUSTER JM. Cortex and Mind: Unifying Cognition. Oxford:
does not seem very likely that problems with the Oxford University Press, 2003.
lexical-syntactic structure of verbs would influence HENRY JD and CRAWFORD JR. Verbal fluency deficits in
performance on this task. One way to assess this Parkinson’s disease: A meta-analysis. Journal of the
International Neuropsychological Society, 10: 608-610, 2004.
would be to investigate what types of verbs are HODGES JR, PATTERSON K, OXBURY S and FUNNELL E. Semantic
actually produced by different diagnostic dementia: Progressive fluent aphasia with temporal lobe
categories. For instance, assuming that syntactic atrophy. Brain, 115: 1783-1806, 1992.
KIM M and THOMPSON CK. Verb deficits in Alzheimer’s disease
processing depends largely on frontal networks, and agrammatism: Implications for lexical organization. Brain
producing verbs with high syntactic valence might and Language, 88: 1-20, 2004.
be associated with an increased “cost” for patients KOZORA E and CULLUM CM. Generative naming in normal aging:
Total output and qualitative changes using phonemic and
with frontal network deficits, causing them to semantic constraints. Clinical Neuropsychologist, 9: 313-325,
“avoid” them in verb fluency although the task 1995.
KREMEN WS, SEIDMAN LJ, FARAONE SV and TSUANG MT. Is there
does not require grammatical encoding. disproportionate impairment in semantic or phonemic fluency
To conclude, factor analysis disclosed large- in schizophrenia? Journal of the International
scale patterns of decreased perfusion in subjects Neuropsychological Society, 9: 79-88, 2003.
KYUHOU S and GEMBA H. Projection from the perirhinal cortex to
with different levels of cognitive decline. Noun and the frontal motor cortex in the rat. Brain Research, 929: 101-
verb fluency impairments were distinguished by 104, 2002.
decreased perfusion in partly different brain MESULAM MM. Aging, Alzheimer’s disease, and dementia. In
Mesulam MM (Ed), Principles of Behavioral and Cognitive
regions, parietotemporal-occipital versus temporal, Neurology. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 2000.
respectively. Decreased verb fluency in cognitive MONSCH AU, SEIFRITZ E, TAYLOR KI, ERMINI-FÜNFSCHILLING D,
decline may thus in part be associated with STÄHELIN R and SPIEGEL R. Category fluency is also
predominantly affected in Swiss Alzheimer’s disease patients.
temporal lobe dysfunction. Acta Neurologica Scandinavica, 95: 81-84, 1997.
MORRIS JC, STORANDT M, MILLER JP, MCKEEL DW, PRICE JL,
Acknowledgments. This study was supported by a grant RUBIN EH and BERG L. Mild cognitive impairment represents
from Alzheimerfonden to Professor Lars-Olof Wahlund. early-stage Alzheimer disease. Archives of Neurology, 58:
397-405, 2001.
ÖSTBERG P. 18th century cases of noun-verb dissociation: The
contribution of Carl Linnaeus. Brain and Language, 84: 448-
REFERENCES 450, 2003.
ÖSTBERG P, FERNAEUS SE, HELLSTRÖM Å, BOGDANOVIC N and
BAKER MC. Lexical Categories: Verbs, Nouns, and Adjectives. WAHLUND L-O. Impaired verb fluency: A sign of mild cognitive
Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, 2003. impairment. Brain and Language, 95: 273-279, 2005.
BLACK M and CHIAT S. Noun-verb dissociations: A multi-faceted PERANI D, CAPPA SF, SCHNUR T, TETTAMANTI M, COLLINA S, ROSA
phenomenon. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 16: 231-250, 2003. MM and FAZIO F. The neural correlates of verb and noun
BRAAK H and BRAAK E. Neuropathological stageing of Alzheimer- processing: A PET study. Brain, 122: 2337-2344, 1999.
related changes. Acta Neuropathologica, 82: 239-259, 1991. PIATT AL, FIELDS JA, PAOLO AM, KOLLER WC and TRÖSTER AI.
BRAAK H, DEL TREDICI K, RÜB U, DE VOS RA, JANSEN STEUR EN Lexical, semantic, and action verbal fluency in Parkinson’s
and BRAAK E. Staging of brain pathology related to sporadic disease with and without dementia. Journal of Clinical and
Parkinson’s disease. Neurobiology of Aging, 24: 197-211, Experimental Neuropsychology, 21: 435-443, 1999a.
2003. PIATT AL, FIELDS JA, PAOLO AM and TRÖSTER AI. Action (verb
BURWELL RD. The parahippocampal region: Corticocortical naming) fluency as an executive function measure:
connectivity. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, Convergent and divergent evidence of validity.
911: 25-42, 2000. Neuropsychologia, 37: 1499-1503, 1999b.
CAPPA SF, BINETTI G, PEZZINI A, PADOVANI A, ROZZINI L and PIATT AL, FIELDS JA, PAOLO AM and TRÖSTER AI. Action verbal
TRABUCCHI M. Object and action naming in Alzheimer’s fluency normative data for the elderly. Brain and Language,
disease and frontotemporal dementia. Neurology, 50: 351-355, 89: 580-583, 2004.
1998. PULVERMÜLLER F. The Neuroscience of Language. Cambridge,
CHAVOIX C, BLAIZOT X, MEGURO K, LANDEAU B and BARON J. UK: Cambridge University Press, 2003.
Excitotoxic lesions of the rhinal cortex in the baboon RADAU PE, SLOMKA PJ, JULIN P, SVENSSON L and WAHLUND L-O.
differentially affect visual recognition memory, habit memory Evaluation of linear registration algorithms for brain SPECT
and spatial executive functions. European Journal of and the error due to hypoperfusion lesions. Journal of Medical
Neuroscience, 15: 1225-1236, 2002. Physics, 28: 1660-1668, 2001.
CRUSE DA. Lexical Semantics. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge RAVDIN LD, KATZEN HL, AGRAWAL P and RELKIN NR. Letter and
University Press, 1986. semantic fluency in older adults: effects of mild depressive
DAMASIO A and TRANEL D. Nouns and verbs are retrieved with symptoms and age-stratified normative data. Clinical
differently distributed neural systems. Proceedings of the Neuropsychologist, 17: 195-202, 2003.
National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 90: 4957-4960, ROBINS RH. A Short History of Linguistics (third ed.). London:
1993. Longman, 1989.
DAVACHI L and GOLDMAN-RAKIC PS. Primate rhinal cortex SILVERI MC, SALVIGNI BL, CAPPA A, DELLA VEDOVA C and
participates in both visual recognition and working memory PUOPOLO M. Impairment of verb processing in frontal variant-
Temporal factor in verb fluency 615

frontotemporal dementia: A dysexecutive symptom. Dementia WAHLUND L-O, PIHLSTRAND E and ERIKSDOTTER JÖNHAGEN M.
and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders, 16: 296-300, 2003. Mild cognitive impairment: Experience from a memory clinic.
SYED GM, EAGGER S, TOONE BK, LEVY R and BARRET Acta Neurologica Scandinavica, 179: S21-S24, 2003.
JJ. Quantification of regional cerebral blood flow (rCBF) WOODS SP, CAREY CL, TRÖSTER AI and GRANT I. Action (verb)
using 99mTC-HMPAO and SPECT: Choice of the reference generation in HIV-1 infection. Neuropsychologia, 43: 1144-
region. Nuclear Medicine Communications, 13: 811-816, 1151.
1992.
TYLER LK, RUSSELL R, FADILI J and MOSS HE. The neural Nenad Bogdanovic, Section for Clinical Geriatrics, Neurotec Department, Karolinska
representation of nouns and verbs: PET studies. Brain, 124: Institutet, Novum, Floor 5, Karolinska University Hospital, Huddinge, SE-141 86
1619-1634, 2001. Stockholm, Sweden. e-mail: nenad.bogdanovic@ki.se

(Received 12 October 2004; reviewed 14 December 2004; revised 18 October 2005; accepted 15 February 2006; Action
Editor Stefano Cappa)

You might also like