You are on page 1of 9

Biological Conservation 115 (2003) 319–327

www.elsevier.com/locate/biocon

Can small, isolated primate populations be effectively reinforced


through the release of individuals from a captive population?
Adam Britta,*, Charles Welchb, Andrea Katzb,c
a
Madagascar Fauna Group, 1 Zoo Road, San Francisco, CA 94132, USA
b
Madagascar Fauna Group, BP 442, Toamasina 501, Madagascar
c
Duke University Primate Center, 3705 N. Erwin Road, Durham, NC 27705, USA

Received 4 October 2002; received in revised form 4 March 2003; accepted 18 March 2003

Abstract
Since November 1997 the Madagascar Fauna Group (MFG) has released 13 captive-bred black and white ruffed lemurs (Varecia
variegata variegata) into the Betampona reserve in eastern Madagascar. Five of the thirteen are currently surviving, including one
male who has integrated into a wild group. Five of the released lemurs fell victim to predation by fossa (Cryptoprocta ferox), one
died of malnutrition, one disappeared from the reserve and one was withdrawn from the release programme. Ten of the released
lemurs survived for more than one year post release, although all required varying levels of supplementary feeding. One pair bred
twice—the first time the infants did not survive, but on the second occasion triplets were successfully raised beyond weaning. One of
the triplets (male) is still surviving. One male has integrated into a wild group and sired a single infant in 2002 and a female released
in 2001 gave birth to twins sired by a wild male also in 2002. The release programme has thus achieved a major objective of rein-
forcing the existing wild population. To date there is no significant difference in survival rates between animals born and raised in
free-ranging environments and those raised in cages, between animals greater than or less than 5 years old at time of release, or
between males and females.
# 2003 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
Keywords: Re-introduction; restocking; Varecia; Conservation; Cryptoprocta; Captive-breeding

1. Introduction case in Brazil’s Atlantic coastal forest in the late 1970s


(Coimbra-Filho and Mittermeier, 1977). However, one
The idea of breeding animals in captivity for eventual small primate species was to become a symbol of hope
release into the wild is a major aim of modern zoologi- for the conservation of this habitat, and for the role of
cal collections (Durrell and Mallinson, 1987). The basic captive-breeding in conservation. The golden lion
philosophy of captive breeding for conservation is that tamarin (Leontopithecus rosalia) became the first example
the captive population forms part of a metapopulation, of a ‘flagship species’, a species that promoted conserva-
i.e. the total population in captivity and in the wild, and tion of the Atlantic coastal forest ecosystem as a whole
both are managed interactively to increase the persis- (Mittermeier, 1986, 1988). In the late 1970s the total wild
tence time of the species (Seal et al., 1994). population of golden lion tamarins was estimated at
With the human population ever increasing, more and 100–200 individuals (Coimbra-Filho and Mittermeier,
more of the natural environment is being destroyed, 1977). The first re-introductions of captive-bred animals
especially in the tropics. At best only small, isolated took place in the mid-80s (Kleiman et al., 1986). By
fragments of more or less intact habitat remain: islands 1989, 71 animals had been released, 27 survived (38%)
amongst an ocean of human devastation. Such was the and 26 more were born to the released animals (Beck et
al., 1991). By 1994 it was estimated that a wild popula-
tion of 450 existed with an additional 550 in captivity
* Corresponding author. Present address: Herbarium, Royal Bota-
nic Gardens, Kew, Richmond, Surrey TW9 3AB, UK. Tel.: +44-20
(Dietz et al., 1994).
8332-5265; fax: +44-20-8332-5278. Just as the golden lion tamarin served as a flagship
E-mail address: a.britt@rbgkew.org.uk (A. Britt). species for the conservation of an entire ecosystem in
0006-3207/03/$ - see front matter # 2003 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/S0006-3207(03)00150-2
320 A. Britt et al. / Biological Conservation 115 (2003) 319–327

Brazil, it was hoped that the black and white ruffed behavioural (Mayr, 1963). This is as a consequence of
lemur (Varecia variegata) could serve the same role for relaxed selection for some traits as certain behaviours
the eastern rain forest of Madagascar. A large captive important for survival in the wild tend to lose their sig-
population of Varecia, from 21 founders, had been well nificance in the artificial, captive environment. Captive-
managed since the 1960s (Porton, 1992). Thus in the breeding for conservation is comparable to the early
early 1990s the Madagascar Fauna Group (MFG), stages of domestication. Thus, behavioural change
began to concentrate their efforts towards an experi- should be expected, and behaviour may depart further
mental re-stocking of Varecia into the Betampona from that in natural conditions as the number of cap-
Reserve. Betampona is one of the few fragments of the tive-born generations increases. Kummer and Kurt
once extensive eastern lowland rain forest. It provides (1965) found that wild hamadryas baboons (Papio
refuge for 11 lemur species, including the indri (Indri hamadryas) exhibit behaviours not observed in captiv-
indri), simpona (Propithecus diadema diadema) and ity, and vice versa (see also Klein and Klein, 1971 for
aye-aye (Daubentonia madagascariensis). Surveys in the spider monkey, Ateles geoffroyi). However, there is
1990 and 1991 estimated the wild Varecia population little or no evidence that domestication has caused
at 35 individuals (Welch and Katz, 1992), too small to behaviours to be eliminated or that the basic structure
be viable in the long-term (Franklin, 1980). The of motor patterns for behaviour has been altered
results of population simulation modelling exercies (Miller, 1977). Encouragingly for captive-breeding pro-
indicated that the long-term viability of the Betam- grammes, the characteristic perceptual capabilities of
pona Varecia sub-population would be significantly most species seem to remain highly stable under
enhanced by the release of a relatively few individuals domestication (Kretchmer and Fox, 1975; Heaton,
from the captive sub-population (Seal, 1997). How- 1976). Behavioural differences between wild and
ever, the question remained as to whether such ani- domestic stocks are quantitative in character and best
mals could adapt to life in the wild, integrate and explained by shifting thresholds based on genotypic
ultimately reproduce with the existing Varecia sub- changes, the effects of the captive environment on
population. behavioural development, or both (Price, 1984). Early
There is evidence to suggest that animals bred in cap- experience appears to be more important than the
tivity may lose the skills and abilities necessary for sur- effects of domestication in the development of beha-
vival in the wild (Frankham et al., 1986). In fish, for viours, for example, antipredator behaviours (Spiegel et
example, there is strong evidence of genetic adaptation al., 1974).
to the captive environment (e.g. Johnsson and Abra- The evidence above suggests that special efforts will
hams, 1991), presumably effected by relaxation in selec- be required to maintain the behavioural skills necessary
tive pressures. Evidence for mammalian species is for survival in the wild within a captive sub-population.
mainly anecdotal (Price, 1984; Frankham et al., 1986), Originally a welfare issue, environmental enrichment
but if physical and social environments differ con- has, with the realisation of the potential role of zoos in
siderably from those in nature it can be predicted that a conservation, become vital for the maintenance of nat-
similar phenomenon may occur. ural behaviour in captive populations (Shepherdson,
Problems associated with the release of captive-bred 1994). Increasingly, the importance of providing
mammals were experienced in the golden lion tamarin endangered species in captivity with naturalistic envir-
re-introduction programme (Kleiman et al., 1986). onments, allowing the expression of a full range of nat-
Many of the animals that were released were unable to ural behaviours, has been realised.
perform key natural activities adequately. Conversely This paper will examine if captive-bred Varecia retain
captive-bred brush-tailed phascogales (Phascogale the skills to survive in the wild, and compare the effects
tapoatafa) re-introduced to the wild encountered no of differing captive conditions on the ability to adapt to
such problems (Soderquist and Serena, 1994). In general their natural habitat. The implications of the findings
very low survival rates are reported for untrained cap- from this study for the future of captive-breeding for
tive-reared animals released in nature (e.g. Wood-Gush conservation will be discussed.
and Duncan, 1976).
Research into animal domestication exposes a num-
ber of potential problems facing captive-breeding and 2. Methods
subsequent re-introduction of endangered species.
Compared with the wild, captive animals experience 2.1. Study site
different resources, limitations to physical and psycho-
logical space, the inability to express behaviours fully The Betampona Reserve is situated between 17 150 –
and the proximity of objects formerly associated with 17 550 S and 49 120 –49 150 E about 40 km north-west of
fear (Kohane and Parsons, 1988). The first indication of the major port of Toamasina on the east coast of
evolutionary change under domestication is likely to be Madagascar. Betampona covers an area of 2228 ha with
A. Britt et al. / Biological Conservation 115 (2003) 319–327 321

altitude ranging from 275 to 650 m above sea level Set 1a: Individuals with considerable experience (at
(Razokiny, 1985). The climate is humid tropical with a least 6 months) in a free-range environment during
mean annual rainfall of 2857 mm from 1998 to 2001. their early developmental phase.
There are no dry months; rainfall peaks from January Set 1b: Individuals with limited experience in a free-
to March and June to August, while October and ranging environment. Early development phase spent
November are relatively dry. Temperature falls mark- in a cage environment.
edly between June and September. The forest at Betam- Set 2a: Individuals less than 5 years old at release.
pona is dominated by trees of the families Lauraceae, Set 2b: Individuals more than 5 years old at release.
Moraceae, Euphorbiaceae, Clusiaceae, Sapotaceae, Myr-
taceae, Arecaceae, Liliaceae and Burseraceae; lianas of the 2.3. Data collection and analysis
families Dillenidae and Apocyanaceae; and numerous
epiphytes of the families Aspleniaceae and Orchidaceae Data were collected through long-term monitoring
(B.R. Iambana, personal communication, Andrianar- of the released Varecia by the MFG field team at
isata, 1995). The canopy height averages 20–25 m and is Betampona from November 1997 to January 2002.
very broken, with occasional large emergents of > 30 m. All the released Varecia were fitted with radio-collars
The terrain is steep with numerous ridges. and comparative detailed behavioural data were col-
lected from both the released and resident wild
2.2. Study animals Varecia to allow investigation of the adaptation of
the released animals. These data will be presented
Three releases were carried out: November 1997—an elsewhere.
established group from Duke University Primate Center Statistical analyses of the limited data set were
(three males, two females); November 1998—a male undertaken using SPSS 10.0 for Windows. Mean survi-
was released with two females and a third female was val times and standard errors of the mean were calcu-
released with a male from 1997; January 2001—a lated for each group. Survival differences to date
mother and three offspring (three males, one female) between each pair of groups were investigated for sta-
(Britt et al., 1998, 2000a,b, 2001). The sex ratio of these tistical significance using Mann–Whitney U-tests. Due
groups was force of circumstance, i.e no scientific ration- to the small sample sizes involved the significance level
ale was involved in the make-up of the three groups. was set at P < 0.01 for all tests performed.
November was selected as the best time for release being
prior to the cyclone season (January–March) and a time
of good fruit availability. However the third release 3. Results
took place in January due to a reassessment of release
strategy following significant losses to predation in 3.1. Survival
2000. A ‘soft’ release strategy was adopted, which
involved the provision of supplementary food in baskets The mean survival time for Set 1a animals was 21.22
suspended within the canopy, recapture of individuals if months  5.75 versus 20.75 months  1.32 for Set 1b.
they strayed from the release area and veterinary assis- The difference in survival time between the two sets was
tance if necessary. The cessation of supplementary not significant (U=13.5, z=0.706, exact two-tailed
feeding for the first release group occurred roughly 2 P=0.503).
months post-release as the released lemurs were at the The mean survival time for Set 2a animals was 23.60
time completely ignoring the food provided, however it months  8.33 versus 19.50 months  4.16 for Set 2b.
was reinstated on two later occasions due to concerns The difference in survival time between the two sets was
over weight loss. The second release group were provi- not significant (U=17.5, z=0.372, exact two-tailed
sioned throughout their time in the forest as they were P=0.724).
completely unwilling to range far enough to encounter The mean survival time for males was 25.14
sufficient natural food resources. Provisioning for the months  5.98 versus 16.00 months  5.71 for females.
third release group was ended 2 months after release– The difference in survival time between males and
again they were ignoring the provisioned food provided. females was not significant (U=13.5, z=0.657, exact
However, it was reinstated during the austral winter two-tailed P=0.530).
again due to concerns over weight loss. This group has
since survived for over a year without supplementary 3.2. Survival without supplementary feeding
food.
The captive-bred Varecia released were considered as The mean survival time without supplementary food
two sets on the basis of experience in free-ranging for Set 1a animals was 9.89 months  2.99 versus 4.00
environments (Sets 1a and 1b), age (Sets 2a and 2b) and months  2.00 for Set 1b. The difference in survival time
gender (males and females). (See Tables 1–3) without supplementary feeding between the two sets
322 A. Britt et al. / Biological Conservation 115 (2003) 319–327

Table 1
Details of the nine Set 1a Varecia released at Betampona

Individual Studbook no. Birth date Institution Free-range experience (months) Date of release

Male 1a 536m 24 April 1985 D.U.P.C.b 54 10 November 1997


Male 2 1322m 31 March 1993 D.U.P.C. 43 10 November 1997
Male 3 1745m 14 April 1996 D.U.P.C. 18 10 November 1997
Male 5 1974m 7 May 1999 D.U.P.C. 12 18 January2001
Male 6 2065m 13 May 2000 D.U.P.C. 6 18 January 2001
Male 7 2066m 13 May 2000 D.U.P.C. 6 18 January 2001
Female 1 591f 8 April 1986 D.U.P.C. 138 10 November 1997
Female 2 1120f 3 April 1991 D.U.P.C. 78 10 November 1997
Female 3 1376f 1 May 1993 W.C.S.c St. Catherine’s 15 20 November 1998
a
Born free-range, but removed to cage for 8 years
b
D.U.P.C.=Duke University Primate Center.
c
W.C.S.=Wildlife Conservation Society, St. Catherine’s Island, Georgia.

Table 2
Details of the four Set 1b Varecia released at Betampona

Individual Studbook no. Birth date Institution Free-range experience (months) Date of release

Male 4 1169m 26 April 1991 Los Angeles Zoo 3 25 November 1998


Female 4 1170f 1 May 1991 Hogle Zoo 2 25 November 1998
Female 5 1171f 1 May 1991 Hogle Zoo 2 25 November 1998
Female 6 1382f 2 May 1993 Santa Ana Zoo 24 18 January 2001

Table 3 ferox), accounting for five of the seven losses (Britt et


Individuals in Sets 2a and 2b al., 2001) (see Table 4). It is hypothesised that the
Set 2a Set 2b released Varecia were completely naı̈ve of the threat
posed by fossa making them particularly vulnerable to
Male 2 Male 1
capture. None of these predation events has been wit-
Male 3 Male 4
Male 5 Female 1 nessed by MFG personnel.
Male 6 Female 2
Male 7 Female 3 3.4. Contribution to the wild population
Female 4
Female 5
Male 1 and female 1 reproduced in October 1998, but
Female 6
the infant(s) did not survive. The pair reproduced again
in October 1999, successfully raising triplets (two male,
was not significant (U=11.5, z=1.023, exact two- one female) to beyond weaning (Britt et al., 2000b). To
tailed P=0.330). date one of the triplets, a male is surviving. Male 2 has
The mean survival time without supplementary food integrated into a wild group. He is strongly suspected to
for Set 2a animals was 13.40 months  4.47 versus 4.75 have sired a single infant with a wild female in October
months  1.68 for Set 2b. The difference in survival time 2002. Female 6 was observed to mate with a wild male
without supplementary feeding between the two sets in July 2002. In October, she produced twins which she
approaches significance (U=7.5, z=1.866, exact two- has successfully raised beyond weaning. Thus to date
tailed P=0.065). matings between the released captive-bred Varecia
The mean survival time without supplementary food and the resident population have resulted in the birth
for males was 11.57 months  3.48 versus 2.80 of three infants, all of which have survived beyond
months  1.59 for females. The difference in survival weaning.
time without supplementary feeding between males and
females approaches significance (U=3.0, z=2.388,
exact two-tailed P=0.018). 4. Discussion

3.3. Causes of death The results to date illustrate that there is no sig-
nificant difference in survival between animals with
The most common cause of death for the released early experience in a free-ranging situation and those
Varecia was predation by the fossa (Cryptoprocta without, between young animals and mature animals, or
A. Britt et al. / Biological Conservation 115 (2003) 319–327 323

Table 4
Dates and causes of death for captive-bred Varecia following release to the wild at Betampona

Individual Set Release Date Date of death Cause of death Post release survival (months)

Male 1 1a/2b 10 November 1997 October 2000 Fossa 35


Female 1 1a/2b 10 November 1997 July 2000 Fossa 32
Female 2 1a/2b 10 November 1997 February 1998 Fossa 3
Male 3 1a/2a 10 November 1997 August 1998 Weight loss 8
Female 3 1a/2b 20 November 1998 February 1999 ? 3
Male 4 1b/2b 25 November 1998 November 2000 Fossa 23
Female 5 1b/2b 25 November 1998 June 2000 Fossa 19

between males and females. However, differences Varecia spend excessive time on the ground, making
between young and mature animals and males and them especially vulnerable to fossa predation. By hous-
females in survival without supplementary feeding ing small, muzzled dogs in the same enclosure to harass
approached significance. Young animals and males sur- the Varecia, ground use would be discouraged. How-
vived longer without supplementary feeding than ever, fossa are excellent climbers and particularly skilled
mature animals or females. These apparent differences at moving rapidly in an arboreal environment (Wright
are likely to be an artefact of the small number of ani- et al., 1997). As none of the predation events has been
mals released. witnessed at Betampona, it is not known whether these
It is difficult as yet to draw any firm conclusions from occurred on the ground or in the trees. The fact that 5
this study as the statistical calculations are dependent out of 13 Varecia fell victim to fossa predation suggests
on the eventual fate of the five animals that are still that fear of this animal may not be innate in Varecia. It
surviving and due to the low sample size of animals can be concluded that avoidance of fossa is a learned
released. behaviour, i.e. infants learn from the example of their
All Set 1a animals have been observed to have some parents. This is perhaps one reason to aim for rapid
degree of contact with wild individuals. While the social integration of released animals into wild groups, in
dynamics of Varecia appears to be variable between order that they can learn from the example of their wild
populations (Morland, 1991; White, 1991; Balko, 1998), conspecifics (see below).
at Betampona the species exists in loose communities The only other known cause of death is that of a
composed of relatively stable sub-groups (personal obser- young group 1a male. He probably died as a result of
vation). As in the study by White et al. (1993) females injuries sustained in a fall, but it is likely that the fall
remain within their natal territory, and males disperse, was a result of his weak and malnourished condition.
although not necessarily outside of their community. This first alerted the monitoring team to the apparent
Adult mortality of the released captive-bred Varecia problems the released Varecia experience in maintaining
has been high (53.8%) (see Table 4) in comparison to body weight during the austral winter. The male
that recorded for the wild study group during the same weighed 2.1 kg when located. Shortly after another male
period (10%). Only one wild adult has apparently dis- was found in a collapsed state, weighing 2.4 kg (mean
appeared since 1997. The major cause of death for the adult Varecia body weight typically falls within the
released Varecia has been predation by the fossa (C. range of 3.0–3.5 kg). A non-invasive weighing system
ferox). To the best of our knowledge no adult wild has since been developed to monitor body weights
Varecia at Betampona have been taken by fossa during throughout the winter period. It is suspected that wild
the same period. It appears that captive-bred Varecia Varecia experience similar weight loss during the winter
are particularly vulnerable to predation. Four of the five months. The causes of this weight loss are likely to be a
predation events occurred within a 6-month period complex interaction of various factors, including
from June to November 2000. It is hypothesised that thermo-regulatory stress and reduced food availability
one fossa began to specialise in Varecia during this per- and quality. However, it is also likely that wild Varecia
iod, perhaps having discovered that these naı̈ve animals are better able to cope with winter conditions through a
were relatively easy to capture in comparison with the variety of adaptive behaviours and through information
wild population. A similar phenomenon is reported by learnt from parents and other group members.
Butynski et al. (1990) concerning the effect of one leo- This seasonal loss of condition provided justification
pard (Panthera pardus) on a population of Gorilla gor- (with hindsight) for the ‘soft’ release strategy adopted at
illa berengei. No further predation by fossa has occurred Betampona. ‘Soft’ releases are financially more costly
since November 2000. than ‘hard’ releases, but in the long-term are more likely
Could captive-bred Varecia be trained to avoid pre- to be successful (Cowlishaw and Dunbar, 2000). The
dation by fossa? One suggestion is that captive-bred success of ‘soft’ releases can be improved by a training
324 A. Britt et al. / Biological Conservation 115 (2003) 319–327

or rehabilitation phase to encourage independence from Although Varecia in general (and indeed the majority of
humans. Rehabilitation involves active coaching to lemur species) are disappearing throughout Madagascar
develop skills in foraging, antipredator and rearing because of competition with humans for space, at
behaviour. Although not involving active coaching, Betampona this is not the case. The reserve has been in
‘boot camp’ training at DUPC and St. Catherine’s existence since 1927 (Andriampianana and Peyrieras,
Island allowed the release candidates to experience tra- 1972), and while the vast majority of the surrounding
velling using natural substrates, foraging for natural forest has been cleared, the reserve has remained rela-
food resources and the presence of potential predators tively untouched. In recent times the main threat to the
(e.g. raptor species). The rehabilitation phase of the Betampona Varecia has not been loss of habitat per se,
golden lion tamarin project systematically trained the but small-scale hunting by local people. Evidence of this
tamarins how to feed, avoid danger and locomote using was provided in the early 1990s by the discovery of
new substrates (Kleiman et al., 1986). numerous snares and shotgun cartridges (personal
Many primate behaviours are learned and standard observation). The effects of the removal of even one or
learning periods may be many years in length. This two individuals from such a small population could be
presents a particular problem for captive-bred animals significant. The presence of the MFG research team at
that have never experienced the wild (e.g. Yeager, Betampona has acted as a significant deterrent to hunt-
1997—orang-utans). The longest surviving Varecia ing. Thus it was felt that the immediate threat to the
released at Betampona is a male who has integrated into Betampona Varecia could be controlled, if not removed
a wild group. Evidence suggests that it is male Varecia completely.
who leave their natal territory and search out a new Can the Betampona project be judged a success?
group (White et al., 1993). Thus it would perhaps be a Kleiman et al. (1991) suggest two differing measures of
preferable to release only young, sexually mature males success depending on the ecology of the species released.
on the periphery of existing wild groups, in order to For ‘K-selected’ species the simple post-release survival
increase the chances and rapidity of reproduction with of re-introduced individuals can be taken as an indica-
the existing population. It can be hypothesised that by tion of success. However, for ‘r-selected’ species re-
encouraging such rapid integration the released animals introductions can only be regarded as successful if there
will learn vital survival skills from their wild conspecifics. is reproductive output and infant survival during the
The released Varecia were not from the same clade as early years. Project Betampona adopted the criteria for
the Betampona population. It appears that the entire success suggested by Kleiman et al. (1991) for ‘r-selec-
captive population derives from the Ranomafana/Man- ted’ species. In addition, given that the release pro-
ombo clade (Wyner et al., 1999). Hence the question of gramme at Betampona aimed to reinforce the existing
the genetic compatibility of the stock released is raised. wild Varecia population, further indicators of success
One can argue that this may be beneficial for the include integration into wild groups and reproduction
Betampona population through introducing new genetic with wild individuals.
material. Equally there could be a risk of genetic pollu- The survival rate to date at Betampona (38.5%)
tion and outbreeding depression. There exists the poten- compares very favourably with that achieved by the
tial for the loss through hybridisation of specific golden lion tamarin release programme. In addition
adaptations to the local environment (Templeton, 1986), successful integration and reproduction with the wild
e.g., seasonality and food resources. However, at population has occurred. This experiment has demon-
Betampona it was felt that the population was so small strated that captive-bred Varecia can survive and
that the risk of introducing animals that may not be reproduce in the wild, but is the release of captive-bred
genetically compatible in order to save the population animals the best means to reinforce an isolated popula-
was justified. Indeed, hybridisation of races within a tion? Data from translocations of wild primates
species may result in hybrid vigour (Wayne et al., 1994). demonstrate that such approaches can succeed across a
The dangers of disturbing the existing wild population range of primate ecological niches (Cowlishaw and
of Varecia at Betampona, e.g., through the introduction Dunbar, 2000). Re-introductions of captive-bred ani-
of disease (Woodford and Rossiter, 1994; Cunningham, mals are very expensive. Kleiman et al. (1991) estimated
1996), were considered. Even if, after the rigorous that each surviving golden lion tamarin re-introduced to
veterinary screening of release candidates, a disease was the wild cost $22,563. Is this a cost-effective use of con-
introduced to the Betampona Varecia, it would only servation resources? There are two schools of thought.
affect one small, isolated sub-population, and would First, there are those who consider that the money would
pose no threat to the metapopulation as a whole. What be better spent on in situ habitat protection (Mackinnon
could potentially be learned from the release was felt to and Mackinnon, 1991). The other school emphasises the
justify this small risk. additional benefits which arise from re-introduction
If the original cause of decline has not been removed programmes that make a significant contribution to
restocking or re-introductions should not proceed. conservation (Stuart, 1991). The cost of releasing each
A. Britt et al. / Biological Conservation 115 (2003) 319–327 325

Varecia in Betampona may be significant, but the spin- her tremendous efforts on the project’s behalf. We
off benefits for the conservation of all the flora and fauna would like to specially thank staff of the San Francisco
within the reserve must be considered. For example, Zoo, Durrell Wildlife Conservation Trust, Zoological
methods of captive propagation for indri (Indri indri) and Society of London (ZSL), Marwell Zoo, Philadelphia
simpona (P. diadema diadema) are still to be developed, Zoo, St. Louis Zoo, Wildlife Conservation Society,
and yet populations of these species at Betampona can Zurich Zoo, Institute for the Conservation of Tropical
benefit from the increased protection of the reserve Environments, Duke University Primate Center and the
afforded by the release programme for captive-bred Friends of DUPC. This work would not have been
Varecia. Thus, captive-breeding is indirectly contribut- possible without generous grants from Mr. John Cleese,
ing to the conservation of these species. the Margot Marsh Biodiversity Foundation, the Thelma
Remember though that one aim of Project Betam- Doelger Charitable Trust, the American Zoological
pona was to reinforce the existing, isolated Varecia Association Conservation Endowment Fund and the
population. This may have been better and less expen- Disney Wildlife Conservation Fund. A.B. would like to
sively achieved by translocating animals locally from personally thank Dr. Robin Crompton and Liverpool
‘doomed’ patches of forest, for example, the nearby University for providing him with an honorary research
classified forest of Antanamalaza (Britt et al., 1999). fellowship and a work base in the UK. Thanks also to
Primate translocations have a good potential as a con- Dr. Glyn Davies (ZSL), and Dr. David Britt for com-
servation tool provided suitable habitat exists (Cow- menting on and suggesting improvements to the original
lishaw and Dunbar, 2000) and also have a higher manuscript. Finally the greatest thanks must go to the
success rate than re-introductions of captive-bred ani- dedicated research team at Betampona, past and pre-
mals (Griffith et al., 1989). However it is unlikely that sent: Ianesy Fidel, Jean-Noel, Ramialison Shuman Eric,
translocations would capture the public imagination or Razafy Celestin, Razanadahy Arsene, Talata Pierre,
attract sufficient funding in comparison to a far more Tevomanana Guy Celestin, Tsirindahy, Georges and
charismatic release of captive-bred animals to the wild. Iambana Bernard Richardson—‘Misaotra betsaka
Thus we face a dilemma, while translocations are more jiaby!’
likely to succeed, they are perhaps less likely to receive
funding, and while re-introductions are less likely to
succeed and may well be more expensive, they are more References
likely to attract funding. Eight of the 10 primate trans-
locations reviewed by Cowlishaw and Dunbar (2000) Andriampianina, J., Peyrieras, A., 1972. Les réserves naturelles inté-
can be regarded as successful. Finally, a word of cau- grales de Madagascar. In: Comptes Rendus de la Conférence Inter-
tion—it is important not to lose sight of perhaps the nationale sur la Conservation de la Nature et de ses Resources à
most effective means of conservation—preserving habi- Madagascar, Tananarive, Madagascar, 7–11 October 1970. IUCN,
Gland, Switzerland and Cambridge, UK, pp.103–123.
tat and avoiding its fragmentation. The effects of a Andrianarisata, M., 1995. Inventaire des Plantes consommes par les
restocking event were modelled for a metapopulation of Varecia variegata variegata dans la Réserve Naturelle de Betampona
Cercopithecus mitis in Natal (Swart and Lawes, 1996). et Etude de leurs Abondonces. Unpublished Report for Missouri
While restocking improved the probability of persis- Botanical Gardens, Antananarivo, Madagascar.
tence in the short-term ( < 500 years), corridors were a Balko, E.A., 1998. A Behaviorally Plastic Response to Forest Com-
position and Logging Disturbance by Varecia variegata variegata in
more effective management strategy for extending per- Ranomafana National Park, Madagascar. PhD Thesis, State Uni-
sistence beyond 500 years. On this evidence we must versity of New York, Syracuse, NY.
consider very carefully how best to make use of avail- Beck, B.B., Kleiman, D.G., Dietz, J.M., Castro, I., Carvalho, C.,
able funds for conservation. Martins, A., Rettberg-Beck, B., 1991. Losses and reproduction in
reintroduced golden lion tamarins Leontopithecus rosalia. Dodo 27,
50–61.
Britt, A., Welch, C., Katz, A., 1998. The first release of captive-bred
Acknowledgements lemurs into their natural habitat. Lemur News 3, 8–11.
Britt, A., Axel, A., Young, R., 1999. Brief surveys of two classified
Project Betampona would like to acknowlege Mada- forests in Toamasina Province, eastern Madagascar. Lemur News 4,
gascar’s Association Nationale pour la Gestion des 25–27.
Britt, A., Katz, A., Welch, C., 2000a. Project Betampona: conserva-
Aires Protégées (ANGAP), the Ministère des Eaux et tion and re-stocking of black and white ruffed lemurs (Varecia vari-
Fôrets and the Circonscription Scolaire de Toamasina- egata variegata). In: Roth, T.L., Swanson, W.F., Blattman, L.K.
II for their technical support and collaboration. We are (Eds.), Proceedings of the Seventh World Conference on Breeding
grateful for the continuing support and assistance of all Endangered Species, 22–26 May 1999, Cincinnati, OH, pp. 87–94.
Madagascar Fauna Group (MFG) member institutions Britt, A., Welch, C., Katz, A., 2000b. Ruffed Lemur release update.
Lemur News 5, 36–38.
who generously provide funds, equipment, technical Britt, A., Welch, C., Katz, A., 2001. The impact of Cryptoprocta ferox
expertise and moral support for our work. We are on the Varecia variegata variegata re-stocking project at Betam-
indebted to MFG Chief of Staff, Dr. Eva Sargent, for pona. Lemur News 6, 35–37.
326 A. Britt et al. / Biological Conservation 115 (2003) 319–327

Butynski, T.M., Werikhe, S.E., Kalina, J., 1990. Status, distribution tropical region. In: Benirschke, K. (Ed.), Primates: the Road to
and conservation of the mountain gorilla in the Gorilla Game Self-sustaining Populations. Springer-Verlag, New York, pp. 221–
Reserve, Uganda. Primate Conservation 11, 31–41. 240.
Coimbra-Filho, A.F., Mittermeier, R.A., 1977. Conservation of the Mittermeier, R.A., 1988. Primate diversity and the tropical forest. In:
Brazilian lion tamarins (Leontopithecus rosalia). In: Prince, Rainier Wilson, E.O. (Ed.), Biodiversity. National Academy Press,
III., Bourne, G.H. (Eds.), Primate Conservation. Academic Press, Washington, DC, pp. 145–154.
New York, pp. 59–95. Morland, H.S., 1991. Preliminary report on the social organisation of
Cowlishaw, G., Dunbar, R., 2000. Primate Conservation Biology. ruffed lemurs (Varecia variegata variegata) in a northeast Mada-
University of Chicago Press, Chicago. gascar rain forest. Folia Primatologica 56, 157–161.
Cunningham, A.A., 1996. Disease risks of wildlife translocations. Porton, I., 1992. International Studbook for the Ruffed Lemur, Var-
Conservation Biology 10, 349–353. ecia variegata variegata and Varecia variegata rubra. St. Louis
Dietz, J.M., Dietz, L.A., Nagagata, E.Y., 1994. The effective use of Zoological Park, St. Louis, USA.
flagship species for conservation. In: Olney, P.J.S., Mace, G.M., Price, E.O., 1984. Behavioural aspects of animals domestication.
Feistner, A.T.C. (Eds.), Creative Conservation. Chapman and Hall, Quarterly Review of Biology 59, 1–31.
London, pp. 32–49. Razokiny, 1985. Monographie de la Réserve naturelle de Betampona.
Durrell, L., Mallinson, J., 1987. Reintroduction as a political and Unpublished. Report for la Direction des Eaux et Forêts, Toama-
educational tool for conservation. Dodo 24, 6–19. sina, Madagascar.
Frankham, R., Hemmer, H., Ryder, O.A., Cothran, E.G., Soulé, Seal, U., 1997. Population simulation modeling of Varecia variegata
M.E., Murray, N.D., Snyder, M., 1986. Selection in captive popu- variegata. Unpublished Report for Madagascar Fauna Group.
lations. Zoo Biology 5, 127–138. Seal, U.S., Foose, T.J., Ellis, S., 1994. Conservation Assessment
Franklin, I.R., 1980. Evolutionary change in small populations. In: and Management Plans (CAMPs) and Global Captive Action
Soulé, M.E., Wilcox, B.A. (Eds.), Conservation Biology: An Evo- Plans (GCAPs). In: Olney, P.J.S., Mace, G.M., Feistner, A.T.C.
lutionary-Ecological Perspective. Sinauer Associates Inc, Sunder- (Eds.), Creative Conservation. Chapman and Hall, London, pp.
land, MA, pp. 135–149. 312–325.
Griffith, B., Scott, J.M., Carpenter, J.W., Reed, C., 1989. Transloca- Shepherdson, D., 1994. The role of environmental enrichment in the
tion as a species conservation tool: status and strategy. Science 245, captive breeding and reintroduction of endangered species. In:
477–480. Olney, P.J.S., Mace, G.M., Feistner, A.T.C. (Eds.), Creative Con-
Heaton, M.B., 1976. Developing visual function in the red junglefowl servation. Chapman and Hall, London, pp. 167–177.
embryo. Journal of Comparative Physiological Psychology 90, 53–56. Soderquist, T.R., Serena, M., 1994. An experimental reintroduction
Johnsson, J.L., Abrahams, M.V., 1991. Interbreeding with domestic for brush-tailed phascogales (Phascogale tapoatafa): the interface
strain increases foraging under threat of predation in juvenile steel- between captivity and the wild. In: Olney, P.J.S., Mace, G.M.,
head trout (Oncrohynchus mykiss). Canadian Journal of Fisheries Feistner, A.T.C. (Eds.), Creative Conservation. Chapman and Hall,
and Aquatic Science 48, 243–247. London, pp. 431–438.
Kleiman, D.G., Beck, B.B., Dietz, J.M., Dietz, L.A., Ballou, J.D., Spiegel, R., Price, E.O., Huck, U.W., 1974. Differential vulnerability
Coimbra-Filho, A.F., 1986. Conservation program for the golden of wild, domestic and hybrid Norway rats to predation by great
lion tamarin captive research and management, ecological studies, horned owls. Journal of Mammalogy 55, 386–392.
educational strategies and reintroduction. In: Benirschke, K. (Ed.), Stuart, S.N., 1991. Re-introductions: to what extent are they needed?
Primates: the Road to Self-sustaining Populations. Springer-Verlag, In: Gipps, J.H.W. (Ed.), Beyond Captive Breeding: Re-introducing
New York, pp. 959–980. Endangered Mammals to the Wild. Symposia of the Zoological
Kleiman, D.G., Beck, B.B., Dietz, J.M., Dietz, L.A., 1991. Costs of a Society of London No. 62. Clarendon Press, Oxford, pp. 27–37.
reintroduction and criteria for success: accounting and account- Swart, J., Lawes, M.J., 1996. The effect of habitat patch connectivity
ability in the Golden Lion Tamarin Conservation Program. In: on samango monkey (Cercopithecus mitis) metapopulation persis-
Gipps, J.H.W. (Ed.), Beyond Captive Breeding: Re-introducing tence. Ecological Modelling 93, 57–74.
endangered mammals to the wild. Symposia of the Zoological Templeton, A.R., 1986. Coadaptation and outbreeding depression. In:
Society of London No. 62. Clarendon Press, Oxford, pp. 125–142. Soulé, M.E. (Ed.), Conservation Biology: The Science of Scarcity
Klein, L., Klein, D., 1971. Aspects of social behaviour in a colony of and Diversity. Sinauer Press, Sunderland, MA, pp. 105–116.
spider monkeys, Ateles geoffroyi. International Zoo Yearbook 11, Wayne, R.K., Bruford, M.W., Girman, D., Rebholz, W.E.R., Sun-
175–181. nucks, P., Taylor, A.C., 1994. Molecular genetics of endangered
Kohane, M.J., Parsons, P.A., 1988. Domestication: evolutionary species. In: Olney, P.J.S., Mace, G.M., Feistner, A.T.C. (Eds.),
change under stress. Evolutionary Biology 23, 31–48. Creative Conservation. Chapman and Hall, London, pp. 92–117.
Kretchmer, K., Fox, M.W., 1975. Effects of domestication on animal Welch, C., Katz, A., 1992. Survey and census work on lemurs in the
behaviour. Veterinary Record 96, 102–108. natural reserve of Betampona in Eastern Madagascar with a view to
Kummer, H., Kurt, F., 1965. A comparison of social behaviour restocking. Dodo 28, 45–58.
in captive and wild hamadryas baboons. In: Vagtborg, H. (Ed.), White, F.J., 1991. Social organisation, feeding ecology and reproduc-
The Baboon in Medical Research. University of Texas Press, tive strategy of ruffed lemurs, Varecia variegata. In: Ehara, A.,
Austin. Takenaka, O., Iwamoto, M. (Eds.), Primatology Today, Proceed-
Mackinnon, K., Mackinnon, J., 1991. Habitat protection and re- ings of the XIII Congress of the IPS, Nagoya and Kyoto 1990.
introduction programmes. In: Gipps, J.H.W. (Ed.), Beyond Captive Elsevier Science, Amsterdam.
Breeding: Re-introducing endangered mammals to the wild. Sym- White, F.J., Balko, E.A., Fox, E.A., 1993. Male transfer in captive
posia of the Zoological Society of London No. 62. Clarendon Press, ruffed lemurs, Varecia variegata variegata. In: Kappeler, P.M.,
Oxford, pp. 173–198. Ganzhorn, J.U. (Eds.), Lemur Social Systems and Their Ecological
Mayr, E., 1963. Animal Species and Evolution. Harvard University Basis. Plenum Press, New York, pp. 41–49.
Press, Cambridge, MA. Wood-Gush, D.G.M., Duncan, I.J.H., 1976. Some behavioural
Miller, D.B., 1977. Social displays of mallard ducks (Anas platyrhynchus): observations on domestic fowl in the wild. Applied Animal Ethol-
effects of domestication. Journal of Comparative Physiological ogy 2, 255–260.
Psychology 91, 221–232. Woodford, M.H., Rossiter, P.B., 1994. Disease risks associated with
Mittermeier, R.A., 1986. Primate conservation priorities in the Neo- wildlife translocation projects. In: Olney, P.J.S., Mace, G.M.,
A. Britt et al. / Biological Conservation 115 (2003) 319–327 327

Feistner, A.T.C. (Eds.), Creative Conservation. Chapman and Hall, Wyner, Y.M., Amato, G., Desalle, R., 1999. Captive breeding,
London, pp. 178–200. reintroduction, and the conservation genetics of black and white
Wright, P.C., Heckscher, S.K., Dunham, A.E., 1997. Predation on ruffed lemurs, Varecia variegata variegata. Molecular Ecology 12
Milne-Edward’s Sifaka (Propithecus diadema edwardsi) by the Fossa (Suppl. 1), S107–S115.
(Crytoprocta ferox) in the rain forest of Southeastern Madagascar. Yeager, C.P., 1997. Orangutan rehabilitation in Tanjung Putting
Folio Primatologica 68, 34–43. National Park, Indonesia. Conservation Biology 11, 802–805.

You might also like