You are on page 1of 14

International Journal of Nonprofit and Voluntary Sector Marketing

Int. J. Nonprofit Volunt. Sect. Mark. 13: 237–250 (2008)


Published online 11 September 2007 in Wiley InterScience
(www.interscience.wiley.com) DOI: 10.1002/nvsm.326

The efficacy of anti-smoking


advertisements: the role of source,
message, and individual
characteristics
Sridhar Samu 1* and Namita Bhatnagar 2y
1
Indian School of Business, ISB Campus, Gachibowli, Hyderabad, India
2
University of Manitoba, Manitoba, Canada

 This research investigates the effects of direct and indirect sources of anti-smoking
messages. Specifically, it examines the direct influence of advertised messages and the
indirect effect of the subsequent discussion.
 Two studies examine the role of: (i) Source characteristics (i.e., messages disseminated
through mass media and subsequently via discussion by friends or strangers); (ii)
Message characteristics (i.e., messages that induce either low or high fear); (iii) Individual
characteristics (i.e., gender based differences within the target audience) in attitude
formation towards smokers, the act of smoking, propensity to smoke, and the likelihood of
being influenced.
 Message efficacy is found to vary by gender, type of ad appeal, as well as group
membership of ad discussants. Implications for design of anti-smoking campaigns are
derived.
Copyright # 2007 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

Introduction other parts of the world. An estimated


46.2 million Americans and 5.4 million Cana-
The economic and health-related costs of
dians currently smoke and the highest pro-
smoking have led to a substantial amount of
portion of smokers belong to the ‘young
research to determine methods to reduce,
adults’ (18–24 years) segment (CTUMS,
prevent, and eliminate smoking (Block and
2005). Hence, there is a critical need to
Keller, 1995; Keller and Block, 1996; Pech- increase understanding of factors that can
mann et al., 2003). Smoking remains wide-
contribute to reduction/cessation of smoking
spread in North America and is growing in among young adults.
Research on the effectiveness of anti-
smoking ads have focused on message type
*Correspondence to: Sridhar Samu, Assistant Professor of and recipient characteristics—e.g., variations
Marketing, Indian School of Business, ISB Campus, Gachi- in the level of fear arousal (high/low), type of
bowli, Hyderabad 500032, India. fear (physical/social, short-term/long-term),
E-mail: sridhar_samu@isb.edu
y
Namita Bhatnagar is Assistant Professor of Marketing and message format, smoking status, and gender
Ross Johnson Fellow at the University of Manitoba. (Block and Keller, 1995; Keller and Block,

Copyright # 2007 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Int. J. Nonprofit Volunt. Sect. Mark., August 2008
DOI: 10.1002/nvsm
238 Sridhar Samu and Namita Bhatnagar

1996; Smith and Stutts, 2003). There is some discussion of anti-smoking messages to aug-
evidence that the unconverted (e.g., smokers) ment the direct message.
discount fear-based messages when elaborat- This research seeks to identify potential
ing on the consequences, while the converted factors that mitigate consumers’ propensity to
(e.g., non-smokers) are influenced by appeals smoke, their attitudes towards smoking and
that arouse high fear (Keller, 1999). Anecdotal smokers, as well as their willingness to
evidence, garnered through focus group and advocate abstinence or cessation to others.
in-depth interviews, suggests that non-smokers The major focus is on advertising appeals that
adopt more extreme views after exposure to create different levels of fear arousal and
anti-smoking messages and become motivated increase the effectiveness of anti-smoking
to engage smokers in cautionary discussions messages, along with the effects of peers
(Price et al., 1987; Aronson and Gonzales, and individual factors in mediating the effec-
1990; Fisher and Misovich, 1990). Such tiveness of such messages. In this paper, the
discussions may enhance or mitigate the direct effects of the anti-smoking ads are examined
effect of health communications by influen- along with the effect of discussion of these ads
cing the amount of discounting or reactance by other people. This approach combines the
engaged in by smokers. Whether such inter- individual level influence with the social
ventions reinforce or weaken the original aspect of group behavior and is a significant
message, and variations in persuasiveness contribution towards the anti-smoking litera-
based on the source of such interventions ture. It is important to consider the combined
(e.g., in-group vs. out-group members), are effect of these three factors rather than the
aspects that have not been explored in individual effects due to the synergistic effect
sufficient depth. between them.
Peer and opinion leader effects have been of Two studies examine variations in the
enduring interest in research on negative efficacy of anti-smoking messages given: (1)
behaviors (e.g., tobacco consumption, sexual differences in the levels of fear arousals; (2)
activity) especially in terms of conformity to differences in the source of the discussion
peer pressure (e.g., Santor et al., 1999; about anti-smoking ads; and (3) gender
Pechmann and Knight, 2002) and advocacy differences. Study 1 investigates the effects
(Howard et al., 2000). Research indicates a of level of fear arousal and gender on the
strong group influence on negative behavior effectiveness of anti-smoking messages.
like alcohol (Urberg et al., 1997; Maxwell, Study 2 includes the additional effect of
2002), smokeless tobacco (Evans and Raines, discussion of anti-smoking messages by friends
1990), and drugs (Mounts and Steinberg, or strangers.
1995), and could be a key factor in anti-
smoking campaigns as well. Peer effects have
generally been studied for their negative Theoretical background
influence on other members in the group
Impact of fear arousal on processing
(Pechmann and Knight, 2002), but positive
of anti-smoking messages
effects may also be possible (Aronson and
Gonzales, 1990; Weisse et al., 1990; Mounts It is well known that media portrayals help
and Steinberg, 1995). The positive effect on shape pro and anti-smoking sentiments.
the basis of social influence may strengthen the Positive media depictions of smoking in earlier
mass media communication by creating a buzz decades are now being tempered by caution-
involving the discussion of anti-smoking ary messages. A majority of consumers turn to
messages. This additional influence of group media for health-related information (Block
membership could lead to improvement on and Keller, 1995) and it is possible to inoculate
the efficacy of existing anti-smoking messages. youth by showing anti-smoking ads before
Public policy makers may be able to initiate a scenes depicting smokers in a positive light

Copyright # 2007 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Int. J. Nonprofit Volunt. Sect. Mark., August 2008
DOI: 10.1002/nvsm
The efficacy of anti-smoking advertisements 239

(Pechmann and Shih, 1999). Fear-inducing tend to discount health-related messages that
messages are commonly used within public are too fearful (Keller, 1999). Message formats
health campaigns even though there is con- that contain consequences followed by recom-
flicting evidence for its effectiveness (Block mended behaviors are more effective for
and Keller, 1995). To resolve such conflicts, non-smokers, but presenting recommen-
researchers have examined the types of fear dations prior to consequences works best in
appeals (Pechmann et al., 2003), the format of the case of smokers (Keller, 1999). Significant
fear messages (Keller, 1999), the extent of fear gender differences have also been found across
arousal (Hovland et al., 1953), and target appeal type (Maldonado et al., 2003). Females
audience characteristics such as gender (Smith are more persuaded by long-term health
and Stutts, 2003) and smoking status (Keller, appeals and males by short-term cosmetic
1999). appeals (Smith and Stutts, 2003). Further,
Conflicting evidence for the impact of fear factors such as importance of the issue,
appeals may be rooted in the variety of ways perceived likelihood that the negative out-
that risk appraisals can be made—e.g., in come would actually occur, and the belief that
terms of health, social or interpersonal risks a change of behavior would affect the feared
(Rogers, 1983); short-term or long-term; outcome, also serve to enhance the effect of
physical or cosmetic (Smith and Stutts, fear appeals (Glascoff, 2000).
2003)—and the weight consumers put on
them. Researchers on healthy behaviors draw
heavily on the protection motivation model
Impact of gender on processing
(Rogers, 1983) to understand how people
of anti-smoking messages
process fear-inducing communications and
become motivated to protect themselves. Females value empathy and are traditionally
According to this framework, threat messages assigned a submissive and subordinate com-
generally consist of the consequences of munal role while males emphasize values such
harmful behaviors and recommendations for as self-respect and social recognition and are
minimizing them. On being exposed to fearful assigned a dominant agentic role (Hall, 1984;
communications, people engage in two types Wymer and Samu, 2002). Given a focus on the
of appraisals: threat appraisal (i.e., about the self and others in the case of females, and
severity/vulnerability of the risk) and coping primarily on the self for males (Hupfer, 2002),
appraisal (i.e., about the ability to engage in gender differences in processing anti-smoking
recommended behaviors/the likelihood that messages can be predicted. The Selectivity
engaging in recommended behaviors would Hypothesis (Meyers-Levy, 1989; Meyers-Levy
actually decrease the risk). With a few and Maheswaran, 1991) postulates that while
exceptions (Pechmann et al., 2003), appli- men are more likely to respond to broad
cations of this model have been restricted to messages; women are more inclined to closely
mass-communicated fear messages. This is examine the details of message content.
limiting, given the number of ways in which Further, if women are more apt to elaborate
fear can be felt and there is evidence that than men (Meyers-Levy and Sternthal, 1991),
information is processed differently depending they may also make more extensive use of
on the type of perceived risk. For example, message cues than males. Thus women may be
Pechmann et al. (2003) found that messages more likely to process and understand
containing social disapproval rather than messages, leading to a higher inclination to
physical health consequences were more persuade others.
effective for adolescents. Men and women differ in item-specific
Further, low-to-moderate levels of fear versus relational processing (Hunt and
arousal are most persuasive (Hovland et al., Einstein, 1981). While relational processing
1953), and smokers (but not non-smokers) involves focusing on shared themes among

Copyright # 2007 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Int. J. Nonprofit Volunt. Sect. Mark., August 2008
DOI: 10.1002/nvsm
240 Sridhar Samu and Namita Bhatnagar

information, item-specific processing concen- other men or groups (Fisher and Dube, 2005).
trates on the characteristics of a specific Social fear is a low agency emotion and it can
message. Given the male tendency to be be expected that men would be more sensitive
agentic and self-focused, men are more to ad appeals with social fear. However,
inclined toward unique message attributes physical fear is a high agency appeal and so
that affect them personally while women are men may not be as sensitive as women. A
more likely to consider all aspects of a message second factor to consider is caring for others
and draw out similarities from multiple which is conceptualized as a higher agency
messages. When exposed to a series of emotion and gender differences can be
anti-smoking ads, women should be able to expected here as well with women being
identify the common theme more easily than more sensitive to such emotions compared to
men. Further, females will be more inclined to men (Fisher and Dube, 2005). Hence,
elaborate and extrapolate the messages to find
relevance for themselves and others (Putrevu, H2: Males will be influenced by anti-smoking
2001). The willingness among females to pay messages that arouse low levels of fear to
attention to self and others and to care for their a greater extent than messages that
well being should translate into higher arouse high levels of fear; females will
susceptibility to internalize and pass along be influenced by anti-smoking messages
messages that encourage healthy behaviors. that arouse high levels of fear to a greater
Hence, extent than messages that arouse low
levels of fear.
H1: Females will be influenced by anti-
smoking messages to a greater extent
than males.
Impact of discussion and
group membership

Impact of gender and message appeal


Previous literature on smoking has looked at
the effect of ad and peer effects in tandem and
on processing of anti-smoking
separately (e.g., Pechmann and Ratneshwar,
messages
1994; Pechmann and Knight, 2002), primarily
A number of themes such as disease and death, in terms of the influence that peer attitudes and
endangerment to others, cosmetic effects, and behaviors have on consumers. Group effects
smokers’ negative life circumstances dominate have been studied in terms of the negative
anti-smoking ads (Pechmann et al., 2003). effects of peer behavior encouraging other
Short-term cosmetic fear appeals (low fear group members to engage in similar behavior
arousal) are more effective for males while (Pechmann and Knight, 2002). Little has been
long-term health fear appeals (high fear done in terms of examining the role of peer
arousal) are more effective for females (Smith attitudes towards anti-smoking ads in the
and Stutts, 2003). It might also be that low fear formation of attitudes towards smoking. Anec-
would be more effective for males because dotal evidence suggests significant amounts of
they value social recognition (Wymer and discussion surrounding ads, which might lead
Samu, 2002). Females also tend to process to the reinforcement or discounting of anti-
messages in depth (Myers-Levy and Sternthal, smoking messages. It may be important to look
1991) and should be less likely to discount at the combined effect of ad discussion and
adverse health-related claims. exposure to anti-smoking ad.
Both men and women react to emotional When reminded of their group membership,
messages in ads in very similar ways in private; group mind-set becomes salient and consu-
however, for emotional appeals, men were mers make decisions that minimize the risk of
more sensitive than women in the presence of negative outcomes to themselves and their

Copyright # 2007 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Int. J. Nonprofit Volunt. Sect. Mark., August 2008
DOI: 10.1002/nvsm
The efficacy of anti-smoking advertisements 241

group (Briley and Wyer, 2002). Social influ- fear ads will be more strongly reinforced by
ence theory explains how people compare in-group rather than out-group while no
themselves to their social group to determine differences are expected for low fear ads.
appropriate behaviors in a given situation.
There is a strong peer influence among teens to H4: Anti-smoking messages that arouse high
initiate risk behaviors such as smoking, but levels of fear will be more effective when
such influence was less than the individuals’ friends rather than strangers discuss
direct effect on such risky behaviors (Maxwell, them; there will be no differences for
2002). These findings also suggest that the anti-smoking messages that arouse low
social context plays a role in determining levels of fear.
which risk behaviors may be pursued. How-
ever, this peer influence could lead to
reduction or cessation of some types of risk Impact of gender and group
behaviors. Other research has shown that membership
peers can influence in a positive direction, The final interaction, between gender and
both in promoting healthful behaviors and in group membership, is of great interest to
discouraging negative behaviors (Weisse et al., marketers. Women are more socially oriented
1990; Hansen and Graham, 1991). Peers’ and would be more influenced by the group
smoking behavior along with extant attitudes they belong to than men (Washburn-Ormachea
about health had the power to affect post-ad et al., 2004). Hence,
attitudes about smoking (Quester, 1998).
Hence, peers do have an impact on the H5: Discussion of anti-smoking messages by
behavior of adolescents and teens. friends will have a greater impact on
Peer tobacco use is an important mediator females than males; there will be no
between environmental/family factors and use gender differences when strangers dis-
of tobacco products (Adamczyk-Robinette et al., cuss anti-smoking messages.
2002) and it clearly indicates that peer behavior,
and by extension peer influence, can play a
significant role in the individual’s behavior. Study 1
Participants and overview
H3: Consumers are more likely to be
influenced by anti-smoking messages The design was a 2 (fear arousal: high/low)  2
when friends rather than strangers (gender: male/female) between-subjects design
discuss these messages. where fear arousal was manipulated and gender
was measured. One hundred and two under-
Since the type of ad appeal will be evaluated graduate business students participated in the
based on the source of discussion, in-group study for course credit. Participants, randomly
would be a more dependable source of assigned to either the high fear or low fear
information than out-group. People have more arousal condition, were asked to view anti-
positive views and give preferential treatment smoking ads and then fill out a questionnaire. Of
to members of the same group (Tajfel and the participants 48.1% were male and 51.9%
Turner, 1979) and this bias would increase were females, and 94.2% were under 25 years of
when their group becomes salient (Huddy, age. The sample consisted of 8.7% smokers,
2004). Consumers are also more sensitive to 18.3% ex-smokers, and 73.1% non-smokers.
in-group members when exposed to emotional
appeals (Fisher and Dube, 2005). When an
Stimuli
in-group member discusses a high versus low
fear appeal, this will be more relevant and The level of fear arousal was manipulated by
personal. Hence, it is hypothesized that high asking respondents to view two anti-smoking

Copyright # 2007 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Int. J. Nonprofit Volunt. Sect. Mark., August 2008
DOI: 10.1002/nvsm
242 Sridhar Samu and Namita Bhatnagar

print ads on the computer (see http://www. four seven-point scales anchored by very
tobaccofree.org/images/Utter%20fool%20web. unafraid/very afraid, relaxed/tense, calm/agi-
jpg and http://www.takingontobacco.org/qofm/ tated, and restful/excited (a ¼ 0.94). Partici-
0202/virginiaslims.html sample ads). It was ex- pants’ smoking status and age were included in
pected that ads dealing with the risk of early the analysis as potential covariates, but were
death and the risk of requiring a tracheotomy dropped as they did not have significant
would induce high levels of fear, and ads dealing effects.
with claims that the tobacco industry hides the
truth from consumers and that smokers appear
Results and discussion
foolish would induce low levels of fear. The
order in which the ads were shown was Variation in the level of fear arousal induced by
counterbalanced. Following this, respondents the anti-smoking ads was successfully manipu-
were asked to respond to questions related to lated. Fear arousal in the low fear condition
the dependent variables, manipulation check, was significantly lower (Mlow fear ¼ 2.50) than
and covariates. in the high fear condition (Mhigh fear ¼ 3.54;
F1, 100 ¼ 10.68, p < 0.005).
MANOVA was used to test H1 and H2.
Dependent variables, manipulation
The results were significant for Ad
check, and covariates
Appeal (F4, 95 ¼ 3.451, p ¼ 0.011) and gender
After exposure to the anti-smoking ads, (F4, 95 ¼ 4.07, p ¼ 0.004). Hypothesis 1 was
respondents’ propensity toward smoking, partially supported—females displayed a
attitude toward smoking, attitude toward significantly lower propensity to smoke
smokers, and perceived susceptibility to anti- (Mfemale ¼ 2.03, Mmale ¼ 2.79; F1, 98 ¼ 4.84,
smoking messages were measured. Propensity p < 0.05), had less favorable attitudes toward
toward smoking was measured by averaging smoking (Mfemale ¼ 1.99 and Mmale ¼ 2.63;
responses to three seven-point scales F1, 98 ¼ 4.59, p < 0.05), and had higher perce-
(a ¼ 0.78): ‘I often have an urge to smoke’; ptions of susceptibility to anti-smoking ads
‘If smoking were not unhealthy, I would (Mfemale ¼ 6.05, Mmale ¼ 4.75, F1, 97 ¼ 15.67,
smoke’; ‘Even under extreme stress, I would p < 0.000) than males following exposure to
not smoke’ (reverse scored). Attitude toward anti-smoking messages. However, no signifi-
the act of smoking was measured by averaging cant gender differences were found in
responses to four seven-point Likert scales beliefs about the acceptability of smoking
(a ¼ 0.92) anchored by extremely pleasant/ (Mmale ¼ 4.92, Mfemale ¼ 5.15, F1, 98 ¼ 0.59, ns).
extremely unpleasant, extremely good/extre- Hypothesis 2 was not supported, but beliefs
mely bad, extremely favorable/extremely unfa- about the acceptability of smoking were signi-
vorable, and extremely acceptable/extremely ficantly lower after exposure to high versus
unacceptable. General beliefs that it is okay to low fear ads (Mlow fear ¼ 5.35, Mhigh fear ¼ 4.72,
smoke were gauged by using one seven-point F1, 98 ¼ 4.53, p < 0.05).
scale item: ‘As long as people do not smoke
near others it is okay for them to do so’
Discussion for study 1
(1 ¼ Strongly Disagree, 7 ¼ Strongly Agree). A
single item measured the degree to which Results of study 1 suggest that gender and fear
participants believed they were susceptible to arousal influence perceptions of anti-smoking
anti-smoking ads: ‘When I am exposed to ads messages. Women were more influenced than
that point out the dangers of smoking I have a men after exposure to anti-smoking ads.
lower urge to smoke’ (1 ¼ Strongly Disagree, Further, in support of meta-analytic con-
7 ¼ Strongly Agree). clusions (Glascoff, 2000), beliefs that it is
After exposure to the two ads, participants okay for people to smoke were found to be
expressed their fear arousal by responding to stronger when fear arousal was low. Hypoth-

Copyright # 2007 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Int. J. Nonprofit Volunt. Sect. Mark., August 2008
DOI: 10.1002/nvsm
The efficacy of anti-smoking advertisements 243

esis 2—that women would be more influ- (measured as in study 1, a ¼ 0.82 and
enced by highly fearful ads and men by less a ¼ 0.85, respectively), beliefs about the
fearful ones—was not supported in study 1. acceptability of smoking, and perceived susce-
Multi-item measures in place of single-item ptibility to anti-smoking ads were measured.
measures of response variables are used in a Respondents indicated their beliefs that it is
follow-up study for a more rigorous test of this acceptable to smoke by responding to five
hypothesis. seven-point scales (a ¼ 0.80): ‘I have a problem
with people who smoke’; ‘I believe that it is
okay for people to smoke’; ‘I believe that it
Study 2 is okay for people to smoke in private places’; ‘As
Study 2 extended study 1 and looked at the long as people do not smoke near others it is
impact of group membership of consumers okay for them to do so’ (1 ¼ strongly disagree,
that discussed the claims made in the anti- and 7 ¼ strongly agree); and ‘I find people who
smoking ads (i.e., in-group vs. out-group). smoke to be extremely: (1 ¼ unattractive, and
7 ¼ attractive). Respondents expressed their
perceived susceptibility to anti-smoking ads
Participants and overview by responding to the statement (a ¼ 0.81):
Hypotheses 1–5 were tested in a 2 (fear arousal ‘When I am exposed to ads that point out the
by anti-smoking ad: low/high)  2 (group dangers of smoking’, on four seven-point scales:
membership of ad discussant: friend/ ‘I am willing to listen to the message’, ‘I believe
stranger)  2 (gender: male/female) between- that it influences me’, ‘I have a lower urge to
subjects design where fear arousal and group smoke’, and ‘I use this information to influence
membership were manipulated and gender other people’, (1 ¼ strongly disagree, and
was measured. One hundred and forty-three 7 ¼ strongly agree).
undergraduate business students participated The manipulation of group membership was
in the experiment for course credit in an tested by measuring categorical responses to
introductory marketing course. the statement: ‘Who talked to you about the
anti-smoking ad?’ (1 ¼ a friend, 2 ¼ a stranger,
and 3 ¼ don’t recall). The success of the fear
Stimuli arousal manipulation was assessed using
the same scales as in study 1 (a ¼ 0.91).
Participants in each of the conditions were Respondents’ age and their self-designated
asked to see two anti-smoking print ads similar smoking status (1 ¼ smoker, 2 ¼ non-smoker,
to study 1. After viewing the ads, subjects were 3 ¼ ex-smoker) were included as covariates in
presented with paper-based scenarios (devel- the analysis as in study 1.
oped through a pre-test) asking them to
imagine that either a friend (in-group) or
stranger (out-group) was discussing the
message contained in the ads (see Table 1 Results and discussion
for script). Following this, subjects responded Both manipulations of fear arousal and group
to the dependent variables, manipulation membership of ad discussants were successful.
check, and covariates. Respondents exposed to high fear ads reported
a higher level of fear arousal (M high ¼ 3.93)
than subjects in the low fear condition
Dependent measures, manipulation
(M low ¼ 2.96, F1, 109 ¼ 12.58, p < 0.001). For
check, and covariates
group membership, 80% (i.e., 114 of the 143
Multi-item scales were used to measure subjects) of all participants correctly identified
single-item measures from study 1. Propensity the source of message discussion as either
to smoke and attitude toward smoking friend or stranger. Participants that failed the

Copyright # 2007 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Int. J. Nonprofit Volunt. Sect. Mark., August 2008
DOI: 10.1002/nvsm
244 Sridhar Samu and Namita Bhatnagar

Table 1. Experimental stimuli for group membership of discussant of anti-smoking ads in study 2

Discussion by a friend Discussion by a stranger

Your close friend sitting next to you also The person sitting next to you (a stranger) also
notices the ads. After seeing the ads, your notices the ads. After seeing the ads, this person
friend says, ‘these ads are extremely scary! says, ‘these ads are extremely scary! I remember
I remember reading that tobacco companies reading that tobacco companies are very dishonest
are very dishonest about the consequences of about the consequences of smoking—people are
smoking—people are totally unable to make totally unable to make informed choices about
informed choices about smoking. Research has smoking. Research has shown that once people
shown that once people start smoking, it is start smoking, it is almost impossible to quit’
almost impossible to quit’
‘I am very confident that smoking is extremely ‘I am very confident that smoking is extremely
harmful, and that it causes serious health problems. harmful, and that it causes serious health problems.
I don’t think that anti-smoking ads exaggerate the I don’t think that anti-smoking ads exaggerate the
consequences of smoking’ consequences of smoking’
‘Smokers tend to be shunned and are extremely ‘Smokers tend to be shunned and are extremely uncool.
uncool. Also, the cost of cigarettes has dramatically Also, the cost of cigarettes has dramatically increased
increased and most smokers find it difficult to and most smokers find it difficult to keep up. I hope
keep up. I hope that more smokers would see these that more smokers would see these ads and quit smoking’
ads and quit smoking’

manipulation check were not included in the Mfemale ¼ 5.32, F1, 104 ¼ 4.02, p < 0.05) than
remaining analyses. Within this sample, 62% of males following exposure to anti-smoking ads.
the respondents were male and 38% were Further, partial support for Hypothesis 2
females. Fourteen per cent reported being was found. Exposure to anti-smoking ads
smokers, 72% identified themselves as non- influenced perceptions of susceptibility to
smokers, and 14% as ex-smokers. these messages and fear arousal was found
MANOVA was used to test the impact of the to significantly interact with gender (F1,
independent variables on the various depen- 104 ¼ 4.08, p < 0.05) (See Figure 1). Females
dent measures and covariates. The results were (Mfemale ¼ 5.79) believed that they would be
significant for gender (F4, 101 ¼ 2.85, p ¼ 0.02) more influenced than did males (Mmale ¼ 4.79,
and the ad appeal  gender interaction (F4, F1, 104 ¼ 8.37, p < 0.005) when high fear was
101 ¼ 2.45, p ¼ 0.05). Smoking status and age aroused by the ads. No gender differences,
did not have a significant impact and were however, were found in perceptions of
dropped from subsequent analyses. susceptibility for low fear ads (Mmale ¼ 4.82,
Key results in support of hypothesis 1 in Mfemale ¼ 4.84, F1, 104 ¼ 0.00, ns).
study 1 were replicated. Significant main The interaction of fear arousal and gender on
effects of gender on propensity to smoke, propensity to smoke was significant (F1, 104 ¼
attitude toward smoking, beliefs about 3.93, p < 0.05). There were no significant
the acceptability of smoking, and perceived differences in propensities to smoke when
susceptibility to anti-smoking ads were males and females were exposed to less fearful
found. More specifically, females indicated ads (Mmale ¼ 2.68, Mfemale ¼ 2.38, F1, 104 ¼
lower propensity to smoke (Mmale ¼ 2.90, 0.21, ns). On the other hand, females displayed
Mfemale ¼ 2.06, F1, 104 ¼ 6.96, p < 0.05), lower a significantly lower propensity to smoke
beliefs about the acceptability of smoking when exposed to high fear ads (Mmales ¼ 3.22,
(Mmale ¼ 4.01, Mfemale ¼ 3.25, F1, 104 ¼ 9.27, Mfemales ¼ 1.74, F1, 104 ¼ 11.02, p < 0.005) (See
p < 0.005), lower attitudes toward smoking Figure 2).
(Mmale ¼ 2.50, Mfemale ¼ 1.80, F1, 104 ¼ 9.68, The interaction of fear arousal and gender on
p < 0.005), and higher perceptions of suscepti- beliefs about acceptability of smoking was
bility to anti-smoking messages (Mmale ¼ 4.82, significant (F1, 104 ¼ 7.76, p < 0.01). Males

Copyright # 2007 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Int. J. Nonprofit Volunt. Sect. Mark., August 2008
DOI: 10.1002/nvsm
The efficacy of anti-smoking advertisements 245

6.5
Average perceptions of susceptibility to anti 6 Women, 5.79

5.5

5 Women, 4.84
Men, 4.79
smoking messages

4.5 Men, 4.82


Women
4
Men
3.5
3

2.5

1.5
Low High
Fear Arousal

Figure 1. Impact of fear arousal and gender on perceived susceptibility to anti-smoking ads in study 2. This figure is
available in colour online at www.interscience.wiley.com/journal/nvsm

believed that it is acceptable to smoke to a (Mmale ¼ 3.71, Mfemale ¼ 3.64, F1, 104 ¼ 0.03, ns)
greater extent than females after viewing high (See Figure 3).
fear ads (Mmale ¼ 4.28, Mfemale ¼ 2.86, F1, 104 ¼ In support of H3, group membership of
17.56, p < 0.000); no differences between men anti-smoking ad discussants had a significant
and women were found for low fear ads effect on attitude toward smoking and a

3.5

3.3 Men, 3.22


3.1
Average propensity to smoke

2.9

2.7 Men, 2.68


Women
2.5
Men
2.3 Women, 2.38

2.1

1.9

1.7 Women, 1.74

1.5
Low High
Fear Arousal

Figure 2. Impact of fear arousal and gender on propensity to smoke study 2. This figure is available in colour online at
www.interscience.wiley.com/journal/nvsm

Copyright # 2007 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Int. J. Nonprofit Volunt. Sect. Mark., August 2008
DOI: 10.1002/nvsm
246 Sridhar Samu and Namita Bhatnagar

4.5
Men, 4.28

4
Average Beliefs about the Acceptability of

Men, 3.71

3.5 Women, 3.64


Smoking

Women
3
Men
Women, 2.86

2.5

1.5
Low High
Fear Arousal

Figure 3. Impact of fear arousal and gender on beliefs about the acceptability of smoking in study 2. This figure is
available in colour online at www.interscience.wiley.com/journal/nvsm

marginally significant influence on perceived Discussion for study 2


susceptibility to anti-smoking ads. Respon-
Results of study 2 replicated key findings in
dents reported lower attitudes toward smok-
study 1 on gender differences in message
ing after an in-group member discussed the
processing. Women displayed less positive
message versus when an out-group member
attitudes towards smoking, had lower beliefs
(Mfriend ¼ 1.84, Mstranger ¼ 2.45, F1, 104 ¼ 7.39,
about acceptability of smoking, and had a
p < 0.01). In addition, perceived susceptibility
lower propensity to smoke than males. They
to anti-smoking ads was higher when friends
also believe that they would be more open to
rather than strangers discussed the messages
anti-smoking messages, be persuaded by them,
contained within these ads (Mfriend ¼ 5.28,
and that they would use this information to
Mstranger ¼ 4.86, F1, 104 ¼ 2.89, p < 0.10).
persuade others. Other gender effects due to
Finally, results indicated partial support for
variations in fear arousal were also found,
H4. The interaction of fear arousal and group
perhaps due to the richer multi-item response
membership of message discussants was mar-
measures used here.
ginally significant (F1, 104 ¼ 3.51, p < 0.10).
The main effects of gender should be
When friends discussed high fear ads, the
interpreted with the results of the interactions
influence on perceived susceptibility to anti-
that men seemed to discount the high fear ads
smoking messages was significantly higher than
more than the low fear ads. As expected,
when strangers discussed them (Mfriend ¼ 5.74,
friends had a higher influence than strangers
Mstranger ¼ 4.83, F1, 104 ¼ 6.60, p < 0.05). Further,
and high fear ads had a stronger influence than
no significant differences were found when
low fear ads.
friends or strangers discussed low fear ads
(Mfriend ¼ 4.84, Mstranger ¼ 4.85, F1, 104 ¼ 0.01, Overall discussion
ns). Figure 4 illustrates these results. No support
and conclusions
was found for H5—that friends are more
successful when discussing anti-smoking ads in This research has important theoretical and
the case of women. practical implications. It introduces peer

Copyright # 2007 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Int. J. Nonprofit Volunt. Sect. Mark., August 2008
DOI: 10.1002/nvsm
The efficacy of anti-smoking advertisements 247

5.8
Friends, 5.74
Average Perceived Susceptibility to the Ads

5.6

5.4

5.2
Friends
Strangers
5
Strangers, 4.85
Strangers, 4.83
4.8
Friends, 4.84
4.6

4.4

4.2
Low High
Fear Arousal

Figure 4. Impact of fear arousal and group membership of anti-smoking ad discussants on perceived susceptibility to
the ads in study 2. This figure is available in colour online at www.interscience.wiley.com/journal/nvsm

effects of others-on-self in terms of discussions tion of public service campaigns. Second,


about anti-smoking ads. This role of consumers discussions among peers about ads, and not
as recipients of mass media and peer mediated only the ads themselves, have an impact on
messages of public service campaigns has not attitudes and beliefs about smoking. Under-
previously been addressed. Given that discus- standing the source and content of these
sions surrounding public service announce- discussions is therefore important. It may be
ments are widespread, the combined effect of possible to generate peer discussion of anti-
these two sources is something that warrants smoking ads in order to increase the effective-
closer scrutiny. ness of such messages. It can be seen that peer
Results indicate interesting practical impli- effects have a strong influence on responses
cations. First, significant gender differences in from consumers and should be a priority when
message efficacy were found. Women have a deciding on an appropriate mechanism to
lower urge to smoke and hold less favorable convey anti-smoking messages. Further, dis-
attitudes towards smokers and the act of cussion among peers also depends on the type
smoking. They are also more susceptible to of ads with high fear ads being more effective
anti-smoking messages—this is especially true when discussed with friends, suggesting that
for ads that generate high levels of fear. In peer discussion of public service ads can be an
addition to being more receptive to smokin- effective weapon in the fight against tobacco
g-related information, women are also more consumption. Hence, it is important to
willing to persuade others to quit smoking. acknowledge the interaction between gender,
This may be a manifestation of the traditional type of appeal, and discussion among peers,
role of nurturers and caretakers assigned to and that the combined effect of some of the
women. Results of these studies therefore factors has more impact than just the direct effect.
indicate that gender should be an important Finally, greater budget/funding at the state/
consideration in the design and implementa- federal level could be allocated for health

Copyright # 2007 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Int. J. Nonprofit Volunt. Sect. Mark., August 2008
DOI: 10.1002/nvsm
248 Sridhar Samu and Namita Bhatnagar

communications that target people in general willingness to post non-smoking signs in their
and not just smokers. The focus of messages residence, willingness to attend anti-smoking
aimed at non-smokers should be to motivate rallies, and willingness to make anti-smoking
them to engage in abstinence related dialogs speeches in front of various audiences (e.g.,
with at-risk individuals. Further, these same vs. opposite gender audiences, friendly
messages have to be crafted in such a manner vs. hostile audiences, audiences comprising
that the right combination of fear level is acquaintances vs. strangers). These measures,
created and care is taken to ensure that the considered as actionable by the target popu-
target group is chosen after due consideration. lation, will provide greater insights into a
It is also important that the right media are variety of inter-personal actions that people
used to reach the targeted consumers. From a can engage in to combat the spread of tobacco
practitioner perspective, it is important to consumption.
understand the potential for mass-message
enhancement or distortion as it enters the
peer discussion process, and to gain further Biographical notes
understanding of factors that contribute to the
former and detract from the latter. Sridhar Samu (Ph.D., Indiana University) is
Assistant Professor of Strategic Marketing at
the Indian School of Business, Hyderabad,
India. His research interests are in the area
Limitations and future research of non-profit and social marketing, branding,
While this research makes a beginning in and information processing.
identifying the influence of peers/strangers in Namita Bhatnagar is Assistant Professor of
enhancing ad messages, much more needs to Marketing, I.H. Asper School of Business, Uni-
be done to understand and manage this effect. versity of Manitoba, Canada (Ph.D. from Uni-
In addition to examining the influence of the versity of North Carolina at Chapel Hill). Dr.
source of this discussion, future research also Bhatnagar’s research interests lie in consumer
needs to look at the impact when there is psychology, persuasive communications, and
support or opposition for public service ads. social and health marketing.
The role of time lags and gender within this
process can also be examined. Second, based
on work by Pinel (1999), scales to measure References
how groups differ on the extent to which they
Adamczyk-Robinette SL, Fletcher AC, Wright K.
expect to be stereotyped by others (i.e., stigma
2002. Understanding the authoritative parentin-
consciousness) need to be developed. This has
g-early adolescent tobacco use link: the mediat-
important implications for how people per-
ing role of peer tobacco use. Journal of Youth
ceive and evaluate messages from members of
and Adolescence 31(4): 311–318.
the same or different groups. Finally, the role of Aronson E, Gonzales MH. 1990. Alternative social
individual characteristics such as interpersonal influence processes applied to energy conserva-
influence susceptibility, venturesomeness, and tion. In Social Influence Processes and Preven-
susceptibility to peer influence also need to be tion, Edwards J, Tindale RS, Heath L, Posavac EJ
examined. (eds). Plenum Press: New York; 301–326.
Some limitations also need to be acknowl- Block L, Keller PA. 1995. When to accentuate the
edged. The current study focuses primarily on negative: the effects of perceived efficacy and
the impact of fear appeals, gender, and group message framing on intentions to perform a
membership on specific dependent variables. health-related behavior. Journal of Marketing
These measures need to be broadened to Research 32: 192–203.
include additional variables such as people’s Briley DA, Wyer RS, Jr. 2002. The effect of group
willingness to take a non-smoking pledge, membership salience on the avoidance of nega-

Copyright # 2007 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Int. J. Nonprofit Volunt. Sect. Mark., August 2008
DOI: 10.1002/nvsm
The efficacy of anti-smoking advertisements 249

tive outcomes: implications for social and con- of Marketing Science Review 6(4). Available
sumer decisions. Journal of Consumer Research http://www.amsreview.org/articles/hupfer03-
29: 400–415. 2002.pdf
Canada Tobacco Use Monitoring Survey CTUMS. Keller PA. 1999. Converting the unconverted: the
2000–2005. http://www.cdha.nshealth.ca/pub- effect of inclination and opportunity to discount
lichealth/ctumsTrends.pdf. Accessed on health-related fear appeals. Journal of Applied
14 October 2006. Psychology 84: 403–415.
Evans RI, Raines BE. 1990. Applying a social Keller PA, Block LG. 1996. Increasing the persua-
psychological model across health promotion siveness of fear appeals: the effect of arousal and
interventions: cigarettes to smokeless tobacco. elaboration. Journal of Consumer Research 22:
In Social Influence Processes and Prevention, 448–459.
Edwards J, Tindale RS, Heath L, Posavac EJ Maldonado R, Tansuhaj P, Muehling DD. 2003. The
(eds). Plenum Press: New York; 143–158. impact of gender on ad processing: a social pers-
Fisher RJ, Dube L. 2005. Gender differences in pective. Academy of Marketing Science Review
responses to emotional advertising: a social desir- 7(4). Available at http://www.amsreview.org/
ability perspective. Journal of Consumer articles/maldonado03-2003.pdf
Research 31: 850–858. Maxwell KA. 2002. Friends: the role of peer influ-
Fisher JD, Misovich SJ. 1990. Social influences and ence across adolescent risk behaviors. Journal of
AIDS-preventive behavior. In Social Influence Youth and Adolescence 31(4): 267–277.
Processes and Prevention, Edwards J, Tindale Meyers-Levy J. 1989. Gender differences in infor-
RS, Heath L, Posavac EJ (eds). Plenum Press: mation processing: a selectivity interpretation. In
New York; 39–70. Cognitive and Affective Responses to Advertis-
Glascoff DW. 2000. A meta analysis of fear appeals: ing, Cafferata P, Tybout A (eds). Lexington:
implications for effective public health cam- Lexington, MA; 219–260.
paigns. Marketing Health Services 20(4): 35. Meyers-Levy J, Maheswaran D. 1991. Exploring
Hall JA. 1984. Nonverbal Sex Differences: Com- differences in males’ and females’ processing
munication Accuracy and Expressive Style. strategy. Journal of Consumer Research 18:
John Hopkins University Press: Baltimore. 63–70.
Hansen WB, Graham JW. 1991. Preventing, alco- Meyers-Levy J, Sternthal B. 1991. Gender differ-
hol, marijuana, and cigarette use among adoles- ences in the use of message cues and judgments.
cents: one year results of the adolescent alcohol Journal of Marketing Research 28: 84–96.
prevention trial. Preventive Medicine 20: Mounts NS, Steinberg L. 1995. An ecological
414–430. analysis of peer influence on adolescent grade
Hovland CI, Janis IK, Kelley HH. 1953. Com- point average and drug use. Developmental Psy-
munication and Persuasion. Yale University chology 31(6): 915–922.
Press: New Haven, CT. Pechmann C, Knight SJ. 2002. An experimental
Howard KA, Rogers T, Howard-Pitney B, Flora JA, investigation of the joint effects of advertising
Norman GJ, Ribisl K. 2000. Opinion leaders’ and peers on adolescents’ beliefs and intentions
support for tobacco control policies and participa- about cigarette consumption. Journal of Consu-
tion in tobacco control activities. American mer Research 29: 5–19.
Journal of Public Health 90(8): 1282–1287. Pechmann C, Ratneshwar S. 1994. The effects of
Huddy L. 2004. Contrasting theoretical approaches anti-smoking and cigarette advertising on young
to intergroup relations. Political Psychology adolescents’ perceptions of peers who smoke.
25(6): 947–967. Journal of Consumer Research 21: 236–251.
Hunt RR, Einstein GO. 1981. Relational and item- Pechmann C, Shih C-F. 1999. Smoking in movies
specific information in memory. Journal of Ver- and antismoking advertisements before movies:
bal Learning and Verbal Behavior 20: 497–514. effects on youth. Journal of Marketing 63:
Hupfer M. 2002. Communicating with the agentic 1–13.
woman and the communal man: are stereotypic Pechmann C, Zhao G, Goldberg ME, Reibling ET.
advertising appeals still relevant? Academy 2003. What to convey in antismoking advertise-

Copyright # 2007 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Int. J. Nonprofit Volunt. Sect. Mark., August 2008
DOI: 10.1002/nvsm
250 Sridhar Samu and Namita Bhatnagar

ments to adolescents: the use of protection stance abuse. Journal of Youth and Adolescence
motivation theory to identify effective message 29(2): 163–182.
themes. Journal of Marketing 67: 1–18. Smith KH, Stutts MA. 2003. Effects of short-term
Pinel EC. 1999. Stigma consciousness: the psycho- cosmetic versus long-term health fear appeals in
logical legacy of social stereotypes. Journal of anti-smoking advertisements on the smoking
Personality and Social Psychology 76(1): behavior of adolescents. Journal of Consumer
114–128. Behavior 3(2): 157–177.
Price LL, Feick LF, Higie RA. 1987. Information Tajfel H, Turner J. 1979. An integrative theory of
sensitive consumers and market information. intergroup conflict. In The Social Psychology of
The Journal of Consumer Affairs 21(2): Inter-Group Relations, Austin WG , Worchel S
328–341. (eds). Brooks/Cole: Monterey, CA; 33–47.
Putrevu S. 2001. Exploring the origins and infor- Urberg KA, Degirmencioglu SM, Pilgrim C. 1997.
mation processing differences between men and Close friend and group influence on adolescent
women: implications for advertisers. Academy cigarette smoking and alcohol use. Developmen-
of Marketing Science Review 6(1). Available tal Psychology 33(6): 834–844.
at www.amsreview.org/articles/putrevu10-2001. Washburn-Ormachea JM, Hillman SB, Sawilosky
pdf (accessed 21 July 2005). SS. 2004. Gender and gender-role orientation
Quester PG. 1998. Antecedents of anti-smoking differences on adolescents coping with peer
advertisings’ effectiveness: a bi-cultural study. stressors. Journal of Youth and Adolescence
Journal of International Consumer Marketing 33(1): 31–40.
10(4): 29–48. Weisse CS, Nesselhof-Kendall SEA, Fleck-Kandath
Rogers RW. 1983. Cognitive and physiological pro- C, Basum A. 1990. Psychosocial aspects of AIDS
cess in fear appeals and attitude change: a revised prevention among heterosexuals. In Social Influ-
theory of protection motivation. In Social Psycho- ence Processes and Prevention, Edwards J,
physiology: A Source Book, Cacioppo J, Petty R Tindale RS, Heath L, Posavac EJ (eds). Plenum
(eds). Guilford Press: New York; 153–176. Press: New York; 15–38.
Santor DA, Messervey D, Kusumakar V. 1999. Wymer W, Jr, Samu S. 2002. Volunteer service as
Measuring peer pressure, popularity, and con- symbolic consumption: gender and occupational
formity in adolescent boys and girls: predicting differences in volunteering. Journal of Market-
school performance, sexual attitudes, and sub- ing Management 18: 971–989.

Copyright # 2007 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Int. J. Nonprofit Volunt. Sect. Mark., August 2008
DOI: 10.1002/nvsm

You might also like