You are on page 1of 15

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/257464319

Development and Content Validation of the Pediatric Eating Assessment Tool


(Pedi-EAT)

Article  in  American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology · October 2013


DOI: 10.1044/1058-0360(2013/12-0069) · Source: PubMed

CITATIONS READS
27 2,165

8 authors, including:

Suzanne M Thoyre Britt Frisk Pados


University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill Boston College, USA
75 PUBLICATIONS   1,408 CITATIONS    52 PUBLICATIONS   346 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Jinhee Park Hayley Henrikson Estrem


Boston College, USA University of North Carolina at Wilmington
44 PUBLICATIONS   430 CITATIONS    23 PUBLICATIONS   214 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Ankyloglossia/Tongue Tie & Feeding View project

We are doing a large cross-cultural study concerning adaptation and resilience in families of individuals with Down syndrome. We followed the process you and your
colleagues outlines in a number of other papers, but we would also like to read and cite this paper. View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Jinhee Park on 28 August 2014.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


AJSLP

Research Article

Development and Content Validation


of the Pediatric Eating Assessment
Tool (Pedi-EAT)
Suzanne M. Thoyre,a Britt F. Pados,a Jinhee Park,a Hayley Estrem,a Eric A. Hodges,a
Cara McComish,a Marcia Van Riper,a and Kimberly Murdochb

Purpose: In this article, the authors describe the Results: Experts rated the total scale CVI > .90 for both
development and content validation of a parent-report relevance and clarity; item CVI ranged from .67 to 1.0 for
measure of problematic eating behaviors: the Pediatric relevance and .5 to 1.0 for clarity. Analysis of each item
Eating Assessment Tool (Pedi-EAT). with low scores, along with experts’ and parents’ feedback,
Method: In Phase I, items were generated from parents’ resulted in refinement of the items, scoring options, and
descriptions of problematic feeding behaviors of children, directions. Experts and parents added additional items.
review of literature, and review of existing eating-related Readability after refinements was acceptable at less than
instruments. In Phase II, interdisciplinary experts on pediatric a 5th-grade level.
eating behaviors rated the items for clarity and relevance Conclusion: The Pedi-EAT was systematically developed
using content validity indices (CVI) and provided feedback and content validated with input from researchers, clinicians,
on the comprehensiveness of the instrument. In Phases III and parents.
and IV, 2 groups of parents of children with and without
feeding difficulties participated in cognitive interviews to
gain respondent feedback on content, format, and item Key Words: eating behavior, content validity, parent report,
interpretation. The authors analyzed interviews using matrix instrument development, cognitive interviews, feeding
display strategies. difficulty

T
he eating behaviors children express reflect problem exists (Rogers, Magill-Evans, & Rempel, 2012;
codependent interactions among intrinsic properties Thoyre & Van Riper, 2010), and clinicians are often faced
of the child, the environment, and dimensions of with a problem that may be difficult to differentiate from
the task itself (Thelen & Smith, 1994). Problematic eating typical behavior of children (Kerzner, 2009). Comprehensive
behaviors, such as refusal of specific or all foods, gagging measurement of eating behaviors is a necessary component
during mealtime or in anticipation of eating, avoidance of of the diagnostic process (Williams et al., 2009) and critical
specific food textures, or insistence on food being offered in a to moving clinical care and empirical study of childhood
certain way, pose significant challenges for children, families, eating problems forward.
and health care providers (Arvedson, 2008; Linscheid, 2006; In this article, we describe the conceptual framework
Stoner, Bailey, Angell, Robbins, & Polewski, 2006). As and development of a new instrument, the Pediatric Eating
problematic behaviors repeat over time, they can become Assessment Tool (Pedi-EAT). We present content validity
entrenched and difficult to change, and nutrition and growth testing of the instrument by two methods: (a) testing of the
often suffer (Williams, Riegel, & Kerwin, 2009). Families relevance and clarity of items and comprehensiveness of the
report it can be difficult to gain a clinician’s agreement that a Pedi-EAT by interdisciplinary clinical and research experts and
(b) content validation by parents of children with and with-
out feeding difficulty using cognitive interviews. We present
a
The University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill readability testing and plans for further psychometric testing.
b
Cheshire Speech and Voice Center, Raleigh, NC
Correspondence to Suzanne Thoyre: thoyre@email.unc.edu
Background and Conceptual Framework
Editor: Carol Scheffner Hammer
Associate Editor: Jane Mertz Garcia Many families seek assistance in solving feeding issues
Received July 22, 2012 of their young children. Recent published estimates indicate
Revision received January 10, 2013
Accepted June 29, 2013 Disclosure: The authors have declared that no competing interests existed at the
DOI: 10.1044/1058-0360(2013/12-0069) time of publication.

American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology • 1–14 • A American Speech-Language-Hearing Association 1

Downloaded From: http://ajslp.pubs.asha.org/ by a University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill User on 02/14/2014
approximately 20% of families of typically developing Eating behaviors develop within a complex system that
children have feeding concerns (Mascola, Bryson, & is self-organizing and drawn to stable states through pattern
Agras, 2010; Wright, Parkinson, Shipton, & Drewett, 2007). formation when emergent behaviors achieve intended out-
This number increases to as high as 80% when children have comes (Humphry, 2002; Thelen, 2005) and as neuronal con-
developmental disabilities (Gal, Hardal-Nasser, & Engel- nections become more defined and preferred (Knudsen, 2004).
Yeger, 2011; Sullivan et al., 2000), such as autism (Emond, Eating behaviors therefore reflect capacities and the patterns
Emmett, Steer, & Golding, 2010; Martins, Young, & Robson, of behavioral organization that have become familiar and
2008; Rogers et al., 2012) or cerebral palsy (Erkin, Culha, successful. Previous experiences with food and mealtime,
Ozel, & Kirbiyik, 2010; Wilson & Hustad, 2009). Feeding surrounded by performance-limiting capacities, and specific
difficulties are common among children with genetic disorders environmental and task constraints drive the behaviors that
(Cooper-Brown et al., 2008; Dobbelsteyn, Peacocke, Blake, emerge. For example, children who have difficulty chewing
Crist, & Rashid, 2008) and multiple medical conditions, have several behavioral options when foods that require
such as past history of prematurity (DeMauro, Patel, Medoff- chewing are offered. They may refuse the food, swallow the
Cooper, Posencheg, & Abbasi, 2011; Samara, Johnson, food without adequately chewing, expel or spit the food out,
Lamberts, Marlow, & Wolke, 2010; Törölä, Lehtihalmes, or soften the food by sucking on or storing it in the cheek
Yliherva, & Olsén, 2012), congenital heart disease (Medoff- or roof of mouth (Morris & Klein, 2000). Each of these
Cooper, Naim, Torowicz, & Mott, 2010), gastrointestinal behaviors, however different, can reflect difficulty chewing.
problems (Wu, Franciosi, Rothenberg, & Hommel, 2012), Which behavior emerges is a function of multiple factors:
cystic fibrosis (Sheehan et al., 2012), and Type 1 diabetes the behavior’s stability given the specific current constraints,
(Patton, Williams, Dolan, Chen, & Powers, 2009). the behavioral solutions perceived to be effective based on
In all feeding difficulties, the specific eating behaviors past success, and the behavioral patterns that have become
exhibited are central features of the problem (Williams et al., strengthened over time.
2009). A wide range of eating behaviors can be exhibited, An adequate measure of problematic eating behaviors
even within the same condition; and many conditions share needs to include the wide range of behaviors that children
the same problematic behaviors (Berlin, Lobato, Pinkos, may exhibit and be applicable to the measure’s various
Cerezo, & LeLeiko, 2011). Broad categorization or clas- intended functions, including clinical assessment, family-
sification of a feeding problem, such as “food refusal,” can provider communication, and research.
have multiple etiologies (Berlin et al., 2011; Bryant-Waugh,
Markham, Kreipe, & Walsh, 2010) and therefore offers
Measurement of Problematic Eating Behaviors
limited diagnostic value. Alternatively, thorough descrip-
tion of problematic behaviors provides the basis for deter- in Children
mining the most likely etiology of the problem and is Given the variety of intended functions and users of a
essential for the development of targeted and effective measure of problematic eating behaviors, several features are
interventions. A means of measuring these behaviors is important to consider. Direct observation of the child or
therefore essential. report by a valid observer, with an aim to identify the range
Dynamic systems theory guides our understanding of behaviors children exhibit, is essential. In addition, the
of the development of eating behaviors during childhood measurement method needs to be clinically useful, family-
(Thelen & Smith, 1994). Problematic behaviors are viewed friendly, and psychometrically sound. Finally, the method
as an emergent property of the continuous interaction of needs to support repeated measurement to evaluate change
multiple subsystems within the child, the environment, and over time, response to treatment, and yield quick results with
features of the task itself. Because subsystems are dynamic, minimal time and cost.
interacting, and changing as the result of development, Measurement of behavior can be accomplished through
learning, experience, and health, the expression of problem- systematic observation, using reliable coding schemes or
atic eating behaviors may change over time (Davids, Button, rating scales with trained observers, or through report by
& Bennett, 2008; Rogers et al., 2012; Rommel, De Meyer, an observer who is directly involved in feeding the child on
Feenstra, & Veereman-Wauters, 2003). Furthermore, per- a regular basis, such as a parent. Systematic observation
formance capacities intrinsic to children constrain or shape has psychometric strength; however, capturing a full descrip-
the possibilities of their eating behavior (Davids et al., 2008; tion of problematic eating behaviors may require repeated
Humphry, 2002). Capacities include neurological, oral motor, observations over a period of time and across settings (Piazza-
and gross and fine motor development; sensory regulation; Waggoner, Driscoll, Gilman, & Powers, 2008; Young &
cognitive abilities; and health, including cardiac, gastro- Drewett, 2000). This presents time and cost burdens and
intestinal, metabolic, and respiratory functioning. Some con- diminishes the usefulness of systematic observation for
straints will more strongly restrict behavior than others. For frequent assessment. Moreover, the lag between systematic
example, children with cardiac disease who have limited observation and the availability of a resulting report would
energy for exercise may take a long time to eat (Medoff- limit clinical utility. Alternatively, in clinical settings, practi-
Cooper et al., 2010) or require shorter, more frequent feedings tioners often adopt a homegrown and untested method for
(Hartman & Medoff-Cooper, 2012). Limited energy reserves, observing and describing problematic eating behaviors using
in this case, can be a rate-limiting constraint on eating. a checklist and/or interview schedule. Although these tools

2 American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology • 1–14

Downloaded From: http://ajslp.pubs.asha.org/ by a University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill User on 02/14/2014
have some clinical utility, they lack the psychometric testing clear (DeVellis, 2012). As such, content validity focuses on
needed for research use or widespread acceptance. the strength of the instrument itself and is the first step in
Parent report of the child’s problematic eating be- instrument development (DeVellis, 2012). Content validity is
haviors has several advantages. Parents are more likely than accomplished through a multistage, quantitative, and qual-
an outside observer to be able to report frequency of a wider itative process, including (a) instrument development that
range of eating behaviors that occur across the settings the includes domain identification and item generation repre-
child naturally encounters. As an example, Piazza-Waggoner sentative of the identified domains of the construct being
et al. (2008) compared the agreement between parent report measured (DeVellis, 2012; Lynn, 1986); (b) a quantified
of child mealtime behaviors with systematic observation of evaluation of the instrument’s content by experts in the field
those behaviors during an average of three meals observed (Lynn, 1986); and (c) systematic qualitative review of the
in the child’s home. There was significant agreement between instrument for clarity, relevance, and completeness by the
the two observation methods for some behaviors (e.g., length intended respondents (Willis, 2005).
of meal or child leaving table) but little agreement for others We evaluated the strength of the content validity of
(e.g., whines, cries, or tantrums at feeding time or refuses to eight available parent-report instruments that measure
eat meals but requests food immediately after the meal ). Some problematic eating behaviors of children (see Table 1).
behaviors are not elicited with every instance of eating, and Although all eight instruments included items related to
the child’s behavior may be altered by unfamiliar mealtime the construct of problematic eating behaviors, several of
settings and/or unfamiliar observers (Haidet, Tate, Divirgilio- the instruments mixed constructs such as parent feeding
Thomas, Kolanowski, & Happ, 2009). Because parent- strategies, parent feelings, or family mealtime interactions.
report measures are relatively inexpensive and place minimal This makes the operationalization of problematic eating
burden on the family, they permit frequent measurement, behaviors imprecise and the ability to isolate the frequency
making them ideal for tracking the development of eating and severity of the child’s eating behaviors difficult. One
problems and response to feeding interventions over time. instrument is designed for use with children with autism
In addition, results of parent-report measures have the (Brief Autism Mealtime Behavior Inventory [BAMBI];
potential for immediately available results, which further Lukens & Linscheid, 2008), thus limiting its use to a narrow
strengthens their utility. audience. Although some of the instruments were specifi-
Parent-report instruments, however, are not without cally designed to be brief, none of the instruments measure
limitations. They are a measure of parents’ perception of the range of problematic eating behaviors families have
their child’s behavior, and this comes with inherent bias reported (e.g., Marquenie, Rodger, Mangohig, & Cronin,
that needs to be considered in the construction of the in- 2011; Thoyre & Van Riper, 2010).
strument and during interpretation of results. Parents may Items on the reviewed instruments were derived from
be motivated to magnify problematic behaviors to gain a clinical experience, studies reported in the literature at the
clinician’s attention. In contrast, they may underreport the time of their development, and through a process of bor-
frequency of behaviors that they have become accustomed rowing from prior instruments addressing children’s eating
to. Although parent-report scales have been found to be behaviors. Although these are excellent sources for item
a valid means of measuring early child communication skills generation, none of the instruments generated items from
(Määttä, Laakso, Tolvanen, Ahonen, & Aro, 2012) and parent interviews. Because parents are the intended re-
behaviors representing developmental risk (Martin et al., spondents of the instrument, these instruments’ items may
2012), others note minimal congruence between teachers and lack parents’ perspectives and the language parents typically
parents or clinicians and parents when rating behavioral use.
problems (De Los Reyes & Kazdin, 2005). There is no Finally, only one of the instruments (BAMBI)
definitive way to judge which reporter is able to provide a reported any assessment of content validity. Items of the
more accurate measure of behavior because there is no gold BAMBI were reviewed by clinical experts; however, no
standard for identifying problematic behaviors. Different standardized process for this review was reported (Lukens &
reporters may have different interpretations and definitions Linscheid, 2008). When the Screening Tool of Feeding
of what constitutes “problematic” (Dirks, De Los Reyes, Problems Applied to Children (STEP-CHILD; Seiverling,
Briggs-Gowan, Cella, & Wakschlag, 2012). Given their Hendy, & Williams, 2011) was adapted from the STEP, no
utility yet potential for bias, it is essential that parent-report content validity assessment was reported, although the
measures be designed to minimize error and have high original STEP for adults was judged by clinical experts for
content validity (DeVellis, 2012; Drennan, 2003). comprehensiveness and appropriateness (Matson & Kuhn,
2001). Neither the BAMBI nor the STEP-CHILD had
quantified content expert assessments; thus, they do not meet
Evaluation of the Content Validity
the standards set for content validity testing and may, more
of Available Instruments accurately, be referred to as having face validity (Lynn,
Content validity is defined as the degree of confidence 1986). Of all available instruments, none were developed
that the instrument’s items adequately represent the con- with both quantified expert and qualitative parent evaluation
struct being measured and that all elements of the instrument of content validity. The available instruments therefore do
(directions, response options, and items) are relevant and not meet standards for content validity for research purposes

Thoyre et al.: Pediatric Eating Assessment Tool 3

Downloaded From: http://ajslp.pubs.asha.org/ by a University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill User on 02/14/2014
4 American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology • 1–14

Table 1. Review of existing parent-report pediatric eating behavior instruments and content validation procedures.

Item generation Content validity testing


Review Adaptation Quantified Qualitative
Instrument name Construct(s) Content Parent Literature of existing of existing Review by evaluation review by
(author) of interest Item content experts interview review instrument(s) instrument experts by experts parents

Children’s Eating Eating and mealtime Child: Food preferences, X X


Behavior Inventory problems: child, motor skills, behavioral
(Archer et al., 1991) parent, and family compliance (28 items)
domains Parent/family system:
Child behavioral
controls, cognitions
and feelings about
feeding, impact on
family members,
mutuality between
family members,
family mealtime
interactions (12 items)
Behavioral Pediatrics Child behaviors Child: mealtime behaviors, X X
Feeding Assessment associated with restrictiveness of diet
Scale—Child poor nutritional (25 items)
Behaviors subscale intake: child
(Crist et al., 1994) domains
Pediatric Assessment Severity of feeding Description of reliance X X
Scale for Severe difficulty when on tube (2 items);
Feeding Problems also tube-fed: eating behaviors
(Crist et al., 2004) child and parent (8 items); quality of
domains life for child/parent
(4 items); acceptance
of food texture and
temperature (9 items)
About Your Child’s Behavioral and [Factor-derived] resistance X X
Eating—Revised parent–child to eating (parent and
(Davies et al., 2007) interactional child, 11 items),
mealtime positive mealtime
difficulties environment (5 items),
and parent aversion
to mealtime (4 items)
Brief Autism Mealtime Mealtime behavior [Factor-derived] limited X X X
Behavior Inventory problems in variety (8 items),
(BAMBI; Lukens & children with food refusal (5 items),
Linscheid, 2008) autism features of autism
(5 items)

(table continues)

Downloaded From: http://ajslp.pubs.asha.org/ by a University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill User on 02/14/2014
Table 1 (Continued).

Item generation Content validity testing


Review Adaptation Quantified Qualitative
Instrument name Construct(s) Content Parent Literature of existing of existing Review by evaluation review by
(author) of interest Item content experts interview review instrument(s) instrument experts by experts parents

Mealtime Behavior Mealtime behavior [Factor-derived] food X


Questionnaire problems refusal/avoidance
(Berlin et al., (12 items), food
2010) manipulation (7 items),
mealtime aggression/
distress (9 items), and
choking/ gagging/
vomiting (3 items)
Montreal Children’s Feeding problems Oral motor, oral sensory, X
Hospital Feeding in children appetite, maternal
Scale (Ramsay et al., concerns, mealtime
2011) behaviors, maternal
strategies, family
reactions (14 items)
Screening Tool of Child feeding [Factor-derived] X X
Feeding Problems behavior chewing problems
Thoyre et al.: Pediatric Eating Assessment Tool

Applied to Children problems (3 items), rapid eating


(STEP-CHILD; (3 items), food refusal
Seiverling et al., (3 items), food selectivity
2011) (2 items), vomiting
(2 items), stealing food
(2 items)

Note. Data are presented for revised tool versions when available.
5

Downloaded From: http://ajslp.pubs.asha.org/ by a University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill User on 02/14/2014
(Knafl et al., 2007; Lynn, 1986). Although the instruments eating, gags with foods that need to be chewed ). Items were
may demonstrate reliability, we cannot know with certainty selected to capture the range of eating-related behaviors
the degree to which the items are relevant, clear, and rep- observed in children ages 6 months and older and currently
resentative of the construct. Furthermore, whether parents being offered solid food (nonliquids), as recommended by the
interpret the items as intended and find the instruments to be American Academy of Pediatrics (Eidelman et al., 2012), with
clear and comprehensive have not been reported. acknowledgment that some behaviors will not be relevant
at all ages. Items were selected from three sources: (a) an
available set of interviews with parents of children with a wide
range of feeding problems, which included descriptions of
Method their child’s eating behaviors; (b) literature descriptions of
Based on DeVellis’ (2003) guidelines for scale devel- problematic eating behaviors during childhood at the time
opment, development of the Pedi-EAT was conducted in of item generation; (c) and examination of the eating
four phases, with readability adjustments made before and behavior items of available instruments.
after each phase of validation by parents, as illustrated in Secondary analysis of focus groups and individual
Figure 1. interviews conducted by Thoyre and Van Riper (2010)
provided input about problematic eating behaviors as
described by parents of children with Down syndrome.
Phase I: Item Generation Children with Down syndrome, as in most conditions with
The Pedi-EAT was designed to be a comprehensive associated comorbidities, display a range of feeding diffi-
parent-report measure of eating behaviors parents may culties (Van Riper & Thoyre, 2006). Thoyre and colleagues
observe with their children—both positive (e.g., enjoys eating, recruited participants in three states (North Carolina, Ohio,
acts hungry before meals, is willing to feed him- or herself ) and and Utah) through support groups, pediatric specialty
problematic (e.g., eats too fast, breathes faster or harder when clinics, and word-of-mouth. Focus groups averaged four

Figure 1. Phases of development of the Pedi-EAT. aVan Riper & Thoyre, 2006.

6 American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology • 1–14

Downloaded From: http://ajslp.pubs.asha.org/ by a University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill User on 02/14/2014
participants per group. Prior to the interview, participants no relevance/clarity, 4 indicating high relevance/clarity).
were asked to subjectively assess whether their child had any Participants were given the opportunity to provide feedback
feeding difficulties, and if “yes,” to rate severity on a scale on the wording of items and to suggest additional items.
of none, mild, moderate, or severe. Content validity indices (CVIs) were calculated at both
Additional items for the Pedi-EAT were generated the item level (I-CVI) and at the scale level (S-CVI) for
from a review of English language literature and examina- both relevance and clarity (Lynn, 1986; Polit & Beck, 2006).
tion of existing measures of problematic eating behaviors of For each item, an I-CVI for relevance was calculated as the
children. We searched PubMed and CINAHL databases proportion of content experts who rated the item moder-
for feeding and eating behaviors of children at high risk for ately or highly relevant (Polit & Beck, 2006). An I-CVI for
feeding problems, including, but not limited to, children clarity was calculated using the same process. The S-CVI
with congenital heart disease, Down syndrome, prematurity, was calculated as the average of the I-CVIs for all of the
autism spectrum disorder, and cerebral palsy. We then items on the scale (Polit & Beck, 2006). Acceptable limits
performed a content analysis of descriptive clinical articles for I-CVIs and for the S-CVI are dependent on the number
(e.g., Cooper-Brown et al., 2008; Hyman, 1994; Mason, of content experts surveyed. With more than six experts, the
Harris, & Blissett, 2005; Norris & Hill, 1994), research minimum acceptable I-CVI is .78 (Lynn, 1986). An I-CVI
reports (e.g., Carruth & Skinner, 2002; Mercado-Deane et al., of less than .78 indicates that the item is either irrelevant or
2001), and books (e.g., Kedesdy & Budd, 1998; Morris & unclear and changes need to be made to remove the item,
Klein, 2000), looking specifically for normal and abnormal make the item more relevant, or improve clarity. At the
behaviors related to eating. The following instruments ad- scale level, with more than six experts, the acceptable S-CVI
dressing childhood eating behaviors available at the time of is .90 or higher (Polit & Beck, 2006).
item development were reviewed: Children’s Eating Behavior Five of the Pedi-EAT investigators conducted sys-
Questionnaire (Wardle, Guthrie, Sanderson, & Rapoport, tematic item-by-item analysis of expert ratings and feedback.
2001), Behavioral Pediatrics Feeding Assessment Scale— They made decisions to eliminate or revise items, separate an
Child Behaviors subscale (Crist et al., 1994), Pediatric item into two or more items, retain an item unchanged, or
Assessment Scale for Severe Feeding Problems (Crist, add suggested items.
Dobbelsteyn, Brousseau, & Napier-Phillips, 2004), Children’s
Eating Behavior Inventory (Archer, Rosenbaum, & Streiner,
Phase III: Content Validation by Parents of Children
1991), BAMBI (Lukens & Linscheid, 2008), About Your
Child’s Eating—Revised ( Davies, Ackerman, Davies, With Feeding Difficulty
Vannatta, & Noll, 2007), and the Behavioral Assessment The third phase of instrument development involved
Scale of Oral Functions in Feeding (Stratton, 1981). establishing content validity of the Pedi-EAT by the target
respondents of the instrument—that is, parents of children
with feeding difficulties. Cognitive interviewing, a widely
Phase II: Content Validity Testing by
used technique to learn how respondents of questionnaires
Interdisciplinary Clinical and Research Experts process and respond to items, was used in this phase
The second phase of instrument development involved (Drennan, 2003; Knafl et al., 2007; Willis, 2005).
validation of the Pedi-EAT content by professionals with Parents who identified having a child over the age of
expertise in pediatric eating behaviors. A panel of 18 clinical 6 months with feeding difficulty were recruited from a feeding
and research content experts from four countries represent- disorders clinic, several websites for parents of children with
ing a range of disciplines, including psychology, speech- disabilities, and by word of mouth. Once parents consented,
language pathology, nutrition, and occupational science, we arranged an interview of approximately 1 hr in length.
were invited to review the Pedi-EAT for relevance and clarity Parents completed a demographic questionnaire. During the
of the items and comprehensiveness of the instrument. interview, each item of the Pedi-EAT was read aloud, and
Although the content experts were cross-disciplinary, the parents were asked to state their interpretation of the meaning
majority of the invited participants were speech-language of the item. Parents were also asked whether the directions
pathologists. Experts were selected on the basis of history of were clear, if the response options were understandable, and
publication, presentations, and research and /or clinical how easy or difficult they thought it would be to complete
expertise. the Pedi-EAT. Verbal probing was used to clarify responses
We sent invitations to participate via e-mail; the (Willis, 2005). Finally, parents were invited to share sug-
messages contained an attached file of the Pedi-EAT, a gestions for improving the instrument, including additional
description of the instrument’s purpose, and a link to an items that would better capture behaviors seen in their own
online survey. Survey responses were received anonymously; child. No compensation for participating was provided.
we did not request demographic data about the respondents. All interviews were audio-recorded and summarized in
A reminder email was sent 1 month following the initial a matrix format. Prior to analysis, a second team member
invitation. Once initiated, the survey directed the participant verified the transcribed data by listening to the interview
to examine each item of the Pedi-EAT and provide two recording. Five of the Pedi-EAT investigators conducted
ratings, one for the item’s relevance, and a second for the systematic column-by-column analysis (Miles & Huberman,
item’s clarity, using 4-point ordinal scales (1 indicating 1994). Problem categories, such as “unclear meaning” and

Thoyre et al.: Pediatric Eating Assessment Tool 7

Downloaded From: http://ajslp.pubs.asha.org/ by a University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill User on 02/14/2014
“redundancy” were created based on parent feedback (Knafl complete the Pedi-EAT, note the length of time for com-
et al., 2007). Decisions were made by the team to retain pletion, and indicate items needing clarification. During the
an item unchanged, revise, delete, split an item into two, interview, we again used verbal probing to elicit information
or add an item. about the ease of completing the questionnaire, the clarity
of the directions, and appropriateness of response options
Phase IV: Content Validation by Parents of Children (Willis, 2005). Items that were changed after the Phase III
With and Without Feeding Difficulty cognitive interviews were then reviewed, and participants
were asked to provide their interpretation of the items’
After analysis of the Phase III cognitive interviews, meaning. Probes were used to clarify parents’ responses.
it became clear that a second round of cognitive interviews Again, parents were invited to share any additional thoughts
was needed. The comprehensiveness and interpretation of and suggest additional items. As in Phase III, the interviews
the newly revised items needed to be evaluated, and infor- were digitally audio-recorded, summarized into a matrix
mation was needed on parents’ experience of completing the format by the interviewer, and verified by a second team
instrument. In addition, because the instrument may be used member using the audio recording. Seven of the Pedi-EAT
with parents of children with and without feeding difficulty, investigators replicated the analytic procedures from the
it was necessary to assess whether parents of children without Phase III cognitive interviews (Miles & Huberman, 1994).
feeding difficulty understood the items in the same way as did
parents of children with feeding difficulty.
We recruited parents of children over the age of Results
6 months who were orally feeding at least some solid foods.
Parents self-identified whether their child had feeding dif- Phase I: Item Generation
ficulty (yes/no); no clinical diagnosis was required. We aimed Thirty-seven caregivers of children with Down syn-
to have an equal balance of parents of children with and drome (33 mothers, 3 fathers, and 1 care provider) participated
without feeding difficulty represented in the sample. Invita- in the focus group or individual interviews (see Table 2), as
tions to participate were posted on local parenting Listservs, described by Thoyre and Van Riper (2010). Participants
including parent-run support groups for families with children reported that the target child (ages 9 months through 13 years)
with conditions that often include feeding challenges. Re- had an average of 5 (range 1–13) eating behaviors that were
cruitment also involved participant word of mouth. Efforts challenging. Two children were reported to have severe
were made to increase the income range and diversity of the feeding difficulty, 32 had moderate difficulty, and 3 had no
subjects compared with the Phase III cognitive interviews or mild feeding difficulty. Thirty-four participants expressed
by posting invitations to participate in targeted public loca- concern that the child was underweight, and 29% were
tions and a feeding disorders clinic. offering enhanced calorie formula. The most commonly
As in the Phase III of cognitive interviews, once reported difficulties were selectivity about types of foods
parents consented, we arranged an interview of approxi- eaten, delayed initiation of chewing or difficulty with chewing,
mately 1 hr in length. No compensation for participating avoidance of textured foods mixed with smooth foods, lack
was provided. Prior to the interview, parents were asked to of interest in eating, difficulty swallowing, selectivity about

Table 2. Demographics of samples by phase of development.

Phase IV cognitive interviews


Phase I item generation Phase III cognitive interviews: No feeding difficulty Feeding difficulty
Variable interviewsa ( n = 37) feeding difficulty ( n = 9) ( n = 10) ( n = 10)

Mean age of child in 2.7 (0.8–13) 4.3 (0.8–10.1) 4.1 (1.6–7.8) 4.8 (1.4–9.7)
years (range)
Difficulty breast or 59% 100% 30% 40%
bottle-feeding
Current tube feeding 5% 55% 0% 0%
Race/ethnicity of child
White 92% 77% 50% 90%
African American 5% 11% 30% 0%
Self-defined 3% African / Native American 11% African / Native American 20% White/ Hispanic 10% White/ Brazilian
Mean family income > $60 (< $20 to > $60) $70–$80 ($30 to > $100) $50–$60 (< $20 to > $100) $70–$80 ($20 to > $100)
(range)b
Family type: 94% 88% 100% 90%
Two-parentc

Note. Phase I = item generation; Phase III = content validation by parents of children with feeding difficulty; Phase IV = content validation by
parents of children with and without feeding difficulty. aVan Riper & Thoyre, 2006. bIncome in thousands of dollars; the upper limit the parent could
select was > $60,000 in Phase 1 and > $100,000 in the remaining three samples. cIncludes step-parent and same-sex parents.

8 American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology • 1–14

Downloaded From: http://ajslp.pubs.asha.org/ by a University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill User on 02/14/2014
how foods are offered, delayed skill development, and (28%) did not meet the criteria of .78 for relevance and /or
spitting up. clarity (Lynn, 1986). The S-CVI was acceptable for both
A list of 44 items was generated from the interviews. relevance (.93) and clarity (.90; Polit & Beck, 2006). Table 3
We added 39 additional items from the literature review and provides examples of items rated as having low relevance
25 items from the review of existing instruments for a total and /or clarity and illustrates the decisions and rationale for
of 108 items. All items were written to be concise and to the item’s management.
begin with the phrase “My childI .” We avoided language Of the 30 items with low I-CVI scores, five had both
of interpretation; respondents reported observable behaviors low relevance and low clarity, five had low relevance, and
rather than the meaning of the behavior, their perception of 20 had low clarity. Item-by-item analysis of problematic
the child’s intentions, or their interpretation of the etiology items and expert feedback (both on the survey and written)
of the behavior. For example, an item may state “my child resulted in the following decisions. Seventeen items were
stops eating after a few bites ” rather than “my child seems eliminated due to low relevance or redundancy. Eleven
to get full before eating enough.” Approximately half of additional items were determined to be inconsistent with the
the items are worded positively and half negatively. The conceptual purpose of the Pedi-EAT and were eliminated.
response format began as a five-point balanced response Five of these items asked parents to interpret what their
scale with the following options: never, almost never, some- children were capable of doing (e.g., can drink from a straw,
times, often, and almost always. Directions at the beginning is able to sit up without support during feeding), and six items
of the instrument asked parents to think back over a typical described the adequacy of diet (e.g., my child eats foods with
2-week period and to check the response that best described protein [beans, cheese, meat, fish]). A total of 39 items were
their child. The research team chose the 2-week time frame revised to improve clarity or grammar. Three items were de-
to be large enough to detect the frequency of current termined to be measuring two behaviors each; therefore, they
behaviors yet small enough to facilitate accurate parent were each split into two items. Four new suggested items
recall and enable the instrument to be used for repeated were added. The revised Pedi-EAT consisted of 87 items.
measurements of progress over time.
Phase III: Content Validation by Parents of Children
Phase II: Content Validity Testing by With Feeding Difficulty
Interdisciplinary Clinical and Research Experts Nine parents of children between the ages of 9 months
Thirteen of the 18 invited experts provided feedback and 10 years with feeding difficulty participated in the
on the Pedi-EAT. Nine participated in the content validity Phase III cognitive interviews. See Table 2 for demographics
survey, and four provided written feedback only. Ratings of the sample. Over half were caring for children with an
for item clarity were omitted by one respondent; therefore, enteral feeding tube, and all reported an early origin of their
content relevance of each item was rated by nine experts, and child’s feeding problems.
the clarity of each item was rated by eight. Column-by-column analysis of the summarized
The I-CVIs for relevance ranged from .67 to 1.0, and data from the cognitive interviews led to the revision of
the I-CVIs for clarity ranged from .50 to 1.0. Thirty items 33 items; elimination of four items; and, in response to

Table 3. Examples of eliminated, reworded, separated, and retained items.

I-CVI
Decision Sample items Relevance Clarity Rationale or revision

Eliminated My child wants to eat. .75 .86 Low I-CVI for relevance and redundant with other items.
My child sighs during mealtime. .63 .71 Low I-CVI (both for relevance and clarity).
My child can drink from a straw. .89 1.00 Represents a skill rather than an eating behavior.
Reworded My child drinks between mouthfuls. .67 .75 My child needs to take a drink between mouthfuls.
My child is particular about how food 1.00 .50 My child insists on food being offered in a certain way
is offered. (such as how food is arranged on the plate or what
dishes or spoon or fork is used).
My child over-fills his/her mouth. .78 .88 My child puts too much food in at one time.
Separated My child spills food or liquid out of mouth 1.00 1.00 My child spills food out of mouth while eating.
while eating. My child spills liquid out of mouth while eating.
My child will eat warm or cold food. .89 .50 My child eats warmed food.
My child eats cold food.
Retained My child will touch food with his/her hands. .67 .75 Critical eating behavior described in the literature related
to sensory modulation disorders.
My child pauses in the middle of eating to .75 1.00 Critical eating behavior described in the literature related
take a rest. to physiologic functioning.

Note. I-CVI = Item Content Validity Index.

Thoyre et al.: Pediatric Eating Assessment Tool 9

Downloaded From: http://ajslp.pubs.asha.org/ by a University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill User on 02/14/2014
parents’ suggestions and new literature, the addition of from two readability formulas was applied to the content to
12 items. Fifty items were interpreted by parents as intended achieve a more valid and reliable estimate. At each time
and therefore were retained without modification. Parents point, the SMOG and Flesch-Kincaid formulas estimated
commented that the Pedi-EAT was straightforward and the reading level to be less than fifth grade.
reasonable in length. The PediEAT was found to be of
limited use for children who were not eating at least some
solid foods. The directions for completion of the Pedi-EAT Discussion
were revised to improve clarity, and the resulting Pedi-EAT The Pedi-EAT was developed to measure the presence
consisted of 95 items. or absence and frequency of a comprehensive set of discrete
behaviors related to eating. Through a systematic process
Phase IV: Content Validation by Parents of Children of item development (Phase I), clinical and research expert
With and Without Feeding Difficulty review (Phase II), and cognitive interviews with parents
Twenty parents of children between the ages of (Phases III and IV), we have gathered evidence to support the
17 months and 10 years participated in the Phase IV content validity of the Pedi-EAT.
cognitive interviews. Half of the parents identified their In Phase I, the use of focus groups and individual
children as having feeding difficulties. Compared with interviews to generate an initial set of items for the Pedi-EAT
Phase III, participants in Phase IV were slightly more grounded the early development of the Pedi-EAT within the
diverse in both income and race/ethnicity (see Table 2). perspective of parents, the intended respondents (Thoyre &
Phase IV led to the revision of seven items, abbrevi- Van Riper, 2010). The focus group format was particularly
ation of 38 items, elimination of three items, and the addition useful. As parents described problematic eating behaviors,
of five items. The resulting Pedi-EAT consisted of 97 items. they clarified their descriptions for one another, often
The directions were further revised to focus parents on comparing specific behaviors and differentiating between
reporting their children’s typical, current behavior. Parents’ similar-sounding feeding problems. We were alerted to terms
postcompletion feedback resulted in our changing the re- or phrases that families disagreed on the meaning of as well
sponse format to a six-point balanced response scale with as descriptions that they readily agree upon. Forty-one
the following options: never, almost never, sometimes, often, percent of the Pedi-EAT items were developed from these
almost always, and always. Parents of children without interviews. The remaining items were derived from litera-
feeding difficulty were less familiar with descriptions of prob- ture and review of available instruments measuring eating
lematic eating behaviors; we revised several items based on behaviors of children.
this feedback. The average length of time to complete the In Phase II, quantified content validity of the Pedi-
Pedi-EAT for these 20 parents was 16 min. Figure 2 illustrates EAT was established using systematic CVI processes, defined
the format of the final Pedi-EAT. by Lynn (1986) and Polit and Beck (2006). The Pedi-EAT
was found to have acceptable content relevance and clarity by
the recommended number of content experts (Lynn, 1986).
Readability Testing The expert CVI process was informative; all but two
Readability of the Pedi-EAT was tested before and items with I-CVI below acceptable levels were adjusted
after each phase of cognitive interviews. As recommended or eliminated according to the expert ratings and written
by Friedman and Hoffman-Goetz (2006), an average score feedback. The two retained items (will touch food with his/her

Figure 2. Illustration of the final format of the Pediatric Eating Assessment Tool.

10 American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology • 1–14

Downloaded From: http://ajslp.pubs.asha.org/ by a University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill User on 02/14/2014
hands and pauses in the middle of eating to take a rest) had Limitations
both clinical support and subsequent parental support.
The process of establishing content validity of the Pedi-
The content experts strengthened the comprehen-
EAT had several limitations. The secondary data source
siveness of the Pedi-EAT by suggesting additional items.
of interviews accessed to generate items in Phase I (focus
Moreover, their comments further clarified the Pedi-EAT
group and individual interviews) represented a single group of
conceptually. Sensory items were found to reflect observed
children with Down syndrome. Field, Garland, and Williams
sensory behaviors rather than caregiver perceptions of the
(2003) found that, within a sample of children referred for
child’s experience of sensory input during feeding. Motor
feeding problems (n = 349), those with Down syndrome
items were likewise aligned to reflect what parents observe
had more oral motor delays—as evidenced by problems with
their children choosing or willing to do rather than what
chewing, moving the tongue, and lip closure—and were
children can do physically. Measuring parents’ perceptions
more selective of the type of foods eaten. The items derived
of what their children are able to do (i.e., motor skill) would
from the Phase I interviews may have oversampled in these
mix constructs and stray from the intent of the Pedi-EAT.
areas. Most of the participants rated their children as
Items that asked parents to rate the quality of their children’s
having moderate feeding difficulty. Although this response
diet were also eliminated. Although it is important to un-
indicates most would likely be able to describe a number of
derstand the impact of problematic eating behaviors, the
problematic eating behaviors, the range of behaviors may
Pedi-EAT is not designed to measure the adequacy of diet.
have been limited. However, the items from the parent in-
Separate assessments of motor skill and diet would augment
terviews were augmented by two other sources and consti-
the Pedi-EAT in future studies to increase understanding
tuted only a subset of the total number of items. A second
of factors that contribute to or are associated with prob-
limitation was the representativeness of the expert sample for
lematic eating behaviors of children.
Phase II. All invited experts had extensive research and/or
Cognitive interview methodology improved the con-
clinical experience, but the majority of those invited were
tent validity of the Pedi-EAT by using parents’ experience
speech pathologists. Because we do not know the disciplines
and expertise with their own children to shape the instru-
of the participants, there is potential for selection bias and
ment, ensuring that directions, items, and response options
overrepresentation of this area of expertise. A third limitation
were understood in the way intended. Phase III focused on
was the representativeness of the samples of parents who
assessment of parents’ interpretation of the items and eval-
participated in the cognitive interviews. Although the samples
uation of the comprehensiveness of the instrument, whereas
represented parents of children with a diverse range of eating
Phase IV focused on parents’ experience of completing the
behaviors, the majority were White, lived in two-parent
Pedi-EAT. A wide range of parents for whom the instrument
households, and had a family income greater than $50,000
is intended participated: five parents of children who were
per year. Eating behaviors of concern to low-income, non-
partially tube feeding, 19 of children with feeding difficulties,
White, single-parent families may be underrepresented; the
and 10 of children with no feeding difficulties. The Pedi-EAT
understanding of the items may be inadequately tested within
was found to be optimal for children eating at least some
these groups. Finally, the PediEAT has been validated with
solid foods. Although the instrument had a less than fifth-
families of children through the age of 10, and this sets the
grade reading level, parents asked that the language of the
upper age limit. However, because some children with de-
Pedi-EAT be simplified to improve the readability and speed
velopmental disabilities continue to make progress toward
with which they could complete the tool. For example, they
optimization of motor capabilities and lessening of difficult
asked that the phrase “his/her” be changed to “their” and for
behaviors into their second decade or longer (Buckley, Bird,
longer phrases to be shortened. Parents asked that questions
Sacks, & Archer, 2006), the instrument may be appropriate
with some similarity be grouped together to facilitate dif-
for a higher age.
ferentiating among them (e.g., gags with foods that need to be
chewed, gags with textured food like coarse oatmeal, gags with
smooth foods like pudding) and that examples be added for Future Development of the Pedi-EAT
several items. Several words were consistently suggested to The next step in the development of the Pedi-EAT is
substitute for another. For example, parents preferred the to determine its factor structure and to assess reliability
word drool to salivate and throws up to vomits. (internal consistency) on the basis of data obtained from a
Parents of children with feeding difficulties confirmed large, heterogeneous sample of parents of children with and
the relevance and comprehensiveness of the items, and without feeding difficulties. Construct validity and test–retest
parents of children without eating difficulties assisted in reliability should also be evaluated. Once the factor structure
the phrasing of items so that items were understandable has been defined (and items eliminated that do not sufficiently
to them as well. By having parents complete the Pedi-EAT load or cross-load too closely), cutoffs will need to be established
in Phase IV, we learned that the directions were not clear for children of various ages at risk for feeding problems.
enough and that a wider range of response options was
necessary. Parents suggested that the response options be
repeated at the top of each page and that in the directions Conclusion
we underline the phrase “at this time” to highlight that the We developed the Pedi-EAT within the framework
instrument is asking about current behaviors. of dynamic systems theory using a systematic process of

Thoyre et al.: Pediatric Eating Assessment Tool 11

Downloaded From: http://ajslp.pubs.asha.org/ by a University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill User on 02/14/2014
instrument development and validation. The design of Cooper-Brown, L., Copeland, S., Dailey, S., Downey, D.,
the Pedi-EAT as a parent-report instrument is one of its Petersen, M. C., Stimson, C., & Van Dyke, D. C. (2008).
strengths as parents interact with their children during Feeding and swallowing dysfunction in genetic syndromes.
Developmental Disabililties Research Reviews, 14, 147–157.
mealtime across different settings and during different times
doi:10.1002/ddrr.19
of the day, qualifying them to report on their children’s Crist, W., Dobbelsteyn, C., Brousseau, A. M., & Napier-Phillips, A.
typical eating behaviors. To our knowledge, this is the first (2004). Pediatric Assessment Scale for Severe Feeding Problems:
measure of problematic eating behaviors in children that has Validity and reliability of a new scale for tube-fed children.
taken a systematic, quantitative approach to expert content Nutrition in Clinical Practice, 19, 403–408. doi:10.1177/
validation as well as a systematic, qualitative approach to 0115426504019004403
evaluating content validity with parents. The Pedi-EAT has Crist, W., McDonnell, P., Beck, M., Gillespie, C. T., Barrett, P., &
content validity with future users and respondents of the Mathews, J. (1994). Behavior at mealtimes and the young child
instrument: experiential, clinical, and research experts. The with cystic fibrosis. Journal of Developmental & Behavioral
Pediatrics, 15, 157–161. doi:10.1097/00004703-199406000-00001
research team plans further psychometric evaluation of
Davids, K., Button, C., & Bennett, S. (2008). Dynamics of skill
the instrument. acquisition: A constraints-led approach. Champaign, IL: Human
Kinetics.
Davies, W. H., Ackerman, L. K., Davies, C. M., Vannatta, K., &
Acknowledgments Noll, R. B. (2007). About your child’s eating: Factor structure
This research was supported by the National Institutes of and psychometric properties of a feeding relationship measure.
Health, National Institute of Nursing Research Center for Research Eating Behaviors, 8, 457–463. doi: 10.1016/j.eatbeh.2007.01.001
on Chronic Illness Grant P30 NR05046 (awarded to the first and De Los Reyes, A., & Kazdin, A. E. (2005). Informant discrepancies
seventh authors), Institutional National Research Service Award in the assessment of childhood psychopathology: A critical
T32 NR007091 to support doctoral fellows in the study of Inter- review, theoretical framework, and recommendations for further
ventions for Preventing and Managing Chronic Illness (awarded to study. Psychological Bulletin, 131, 483–509.
the second and fourth authors), and Individual National Research DeMauro, S. B., Patel, P. R., Medoff-Cooper, B., Posencheg, M., &
Service Award 5F31NR011262 (awarded to the second author). Abbasi, S. (2011). Postdischarge feeding patterns in early-
Additional support was provided by the Royster Society of Fellows and late-preterm infants. Clinical Pediatrics, 50, 957–962.
(awarded to the third author). We would like to acknowledge and doi:10.1177/0009922811409028
express gratitude to the content experts and families who contrib- DeVellis, R. F. (2003). Scale development: Theory and applications
uted to the development of the Pedi-EAT. (2nd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
DeVellis, R. F. (2012). Scale development: Theory and applications
(3rd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Dirks, M. A., De Los Reyes, A., Briggs-Gowan, M., Cella, D., &
References Wakschlag, L. S. (2012). Annual research review: Embracing not
Archer, L. A., Rosenbaum, P. L., & Streiner, D. L. (1991). The erasing contextual variability in children’s behavior—theory and
Children’s Eating Behavior Inventory: Reliability and validity utility in the selection and use of methods and informants in
results. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 16, 629–642. developmental psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology
doi:10.1093/jpepsy/16.5.629 and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 53, 558–574. doi:10.1111/
Arvedson, J. C. (2008). Assessment of pediatric dysphagia and j.1469-7610.2012.02537.x
feeding disorders: Clinical and instrumental approaches. Dobbelsteyn, C., Peacocke, S. D., Blake, K., Crist, W., & Rashid, M.
Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews, 14, 118–127. (2008). Feeding difficulties in children with CHARGE syndrome:
doi:10.1002/ddrr.17 Prevalence, risk factors, and prognosis. Dysphagia, 23, 127–135.
Berlin, K. S., Davies, W. H., Silverman, A. H., Woods, D. W., doi:10.1007/s00455-007-9111-6
Fischer, E. A., & Rudolph, C. D. (2010). Assessing children’s Drennan, J. (2003). Cognitive interviewing: Verbal data in the
mealtime problems with the Mealtime Behavior Question- design and pretesting of questionnaires. Journal of Advanced
naire. Children’s Health Care, 39, 142–156. doi:10.1080/ Nursing, 42, 57–63. doi:10.1046/j.1365-2648.2003.02579.x
02739611003679956 Eidelman, A. I., Schanler, R. J., Johnston, M., Landers, S., Noble, L.,
Berlin, K. S., Lobato, D. J., Pinkos, B., Cerezo, C. S., & LeLeiko, Szucs, K., & Viehmann, L. (2012). Breastfeeding and the use
N. S. (2011). Patterns of medical and developmental comor- of human milk. Pediatrics, 129, e827–e841. doi:10.1542/peds.
bidities among children presenting with feeding problems: A 2011-3552
latent class analysis. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Emond, A., Emmett, P., Steer, C., & Golding, J. (2010). Feeding
Pediatrics, 32, 41–47. doi:10.1097/DBP.0b013e318203e06d symptoms, dietary patterns, and growth in young children
Bryant-Waugh, R., Markham, L., Kreipe, R. E., & Walsh, B. T. with autism spectrum disorders. Pediatrics, 126, e337–e342.
(2010). Feeding and eating disorders in childhood. International doi:10.1542/peds.2009-2391
Journal of Eating Disorders, 43, 98–111. doi:10.1002/eat.20795 Erkin, G., Culha, C., Ozel, S., & Kirbiyik, E. G. (2010). Feeding
Buckley, S., Bird, G., Sacks, B., & Archer, T. (2006). A comparison and gastrointestinal problems in children with cerebral palsy.
of mainstream and special education for teenagers with Down International Journal of Rehabilitation Research, 33, 218–224.
syndrome: Implications for parents and teachers. Down Syn- doi:10.1097/MRR.0b013e3283375e10
drome Research and Practice, 9, 54–67. doi:10.3104/reports.295 Field, D., Garland, M., & Williams, K. (2003). Correlates of specific
Carruth, B. R., & Skinner, J. D. (2002). Feeding behaviors and other childhood feeding problems. Journal of Paediatrics and Child
motor development in healthy children (2–24 months). Journal Health, 39, 299–304. doi:10.1046/j.1440-1754.2003.00151.x
of the American College of Nutrition, 21, 88–96. Retrieved from Friedman, D. B., & Hoffman-Goetz, L. (2006). A systematic review
http://www.jacn.org of readability and comprehension instruments used for print and

12 American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology • 1–14

Downloaded From: http://ajslp.pubs.asha.org/ by a University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill User on 02/14/2014
web-based cancer information. Health Education and Behavior, Mason, S. J., Harris, G., & Blissett, J. (2005). Tube feeding in
33, 352–373. doi:10.1177/1090198105277329 infancy: Implications for the development of normal eating
Gal, E., Hardal-Nasser, R., & Engel-Yeger, B. (2011). The re- and drinking skills. Dysphagia, 20, 46–61. doi:10.1007/
lationship between the severity of eating problems and intellec- s00455-004-0025-2
tual developmental deficit level. Research in Developmental Matson, J. L., & Kuhn, D. E. (2001). Identifying feeding problems
Disabilities, 32, 1464–1469. doi:10.1016/ j.ridd.2010.12.003 in mentally retarded persons: Development and reliability of
Haidet, K. K., Tate, J., Divirgilio-Thomas, D., Kolanowski, A., & the Screening Tool of Feeding Problems (STEP). Research in
Happ, M. B. (2009). Methods to improve reliability of video- Developmental Disabilities, 22, 165–172. doi:10.1016/S0891-
recorded behavioral data. Research in Nursing & Health, 4222(01)00065-8
32, 465–474. doi:10.1002/nur.20334 Medoff-Cooper, B., Naim, M., Torowicz, D., & Mott, A. (2010).
Hartman, D. M., & Medoff-Cooper, B. (2012). Transition to home Feeding, growth, and nutrition in children with congenitally
after neonatal surgery for congenital heart disease. Maternal malformed hearts. Cardiology in the Young, 20(Suppl. 3),
Child Nursing, 37, 95–100. doi:10.1097/ NMC.0b013e318241dac1 149–153. doi:10.1017/S1047951110001228
Humphry, R. (2002). Young children’s occupations: Explicating the Mercado-Deane, M. G., Burton, E. M., Harlow, S. A., Glover, A. S.,
dynamics of developmental processes. American Journal of Deane, D. A., Guill, M. F., & Hudson, V. (2001). Swallowing
Occupational Therapy, 56, 171–179. doi:10.5014/ajot.56.2.171 dysfunction in infants less than 1 year of age. Pediatric
Hyman, P. E. (1994). Gastroesophageal reflux: One reason why Radiology, 31, 423–428. doi:10.1007/s002470100456
baby won’t eat. Journal of Pediatrics, 125, S103–S109. Miles, M., & Huberman, A. (1994). Qualitative data analysis: An
doi:10.1016/S0022-3476(05)82933-6 expanded sourcebook (2nd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Kedesdy, J. H., & Budd, K. S. (1998). Childhood feeding disorders: Morris, S. E., & Klein, M. D. (2000). Pre-feeding skills: A
Biobehavioral assessment and intervention. Baltimore, MD: comprehensive resource for mealtime development (2nd ed.).
Brookes. Tucson, AZ: Therapy Skill Builders.
Kerzner, B. (2009). Clinical investigation of feeding difficulties in Norris, M. K., & Hill, C. S. (1994). Nutritional issues in infants and
young children: A practical approach. Clinical Pediatrics, 48, children with congenital heart disease. Critical Care Nursing
960–965. doi:10.1177/0009922809336074 Clinics of North America, 6(1), 153–163. Retrieved from
Knafl, K., Deatrick, J., Gallo, A., Holcombe, G., Bakitas, M., http://www.elsevier.com
Dixon, J., & Grey, M. (2007). The analysis and interpretation Patton, S. R., Williams, L. B., Dolan, L. M., Chen, M., & Powers,
of cognitive interviews for instrument development. Research S. W. (2009). Feeding problems reported by parents of young
in Nursing & Health, 30, 224–234. doi:10.1002/nur.20195 children with Type 1 diabetes on insulin pump therapy and their
Knudsen, E. I. (2004). Sensitive periods in the development of the associations with children’s glycemic control. Pediatric Diabetes,
brain and behavior. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16, 10, 455–460. doi:10.1111/j.1399-5448.2009.00506.x
1412–1425. doi:10.1162/0898929042304796 Piazza-Waggoner, C., Driscoll, K. A., Gilman, D. K., & Powers,
Linscheid, T. R. (2006). Behavioral treatments for pediatric feeding S. W. (2008). A comparison using parent report and direct
disorders. Behavior Modification, 30, 6–23. doi:10.1177/ observation in young children with cystic fibrosis: Implications
0145445505282165 for practical and empirically based behavioral assessment in
Lukens, C. T., & Linscheid, T. R. (2008). Development and vali- routine clinical care. Children’s Health Care, 37, 38–48.
dation of an inventory to assess mealtime behavior problems doi:10.1080/02739610701766875
in children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Polit, D. F., & Beck, C. T. (2006). The Content Validity Index:
Disorders, 38, 342–352. doi:10.1007/s10803-007-0401-5 Are you sure you know what’s being reported? Critique and
Lynn, M. R. (1986). Determination and quantification of content recommendations. Research in Nursing & Health, 29, 489–497.
validity. Nursing Research, 35, 382–385. doi:10.1097/00006199- doi:10.1002/nur.20147
198611000-00017 Ramsay, M., Martel, C., Porporino, M., & Zygmuntowicz, C.
Määttä, S., Laakso, M.-L., Tolvanen, A., Ahonen, T., & Aro, T. (2011). The Montreal Children’s Hospital Feeding Scale: A
(2012). Developmental trajectories of early communication brief bilingual screening tool for identifying feeding problems.
skills. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 55, Paediatrics & Child Health, 16, 147–151. Retrieved from
1083–1096. doi:10.1044/1092-4388(2011/10-0305) http://www.elsevier.com
Marquenie, K., Rodger, S., Mangohig, K., & Cronin, A. (2011). Rogers, L. G., Magill-Evans, J., & Rempel, G. R. (2012). Mothers’
Dinnertime and bedtime routines and rituals in families with challenges in feeding their children with autism spectrum
a young child with an autism spectrum disorder. Australian disorder—Managing more than just picky eating. Journal of
Occupational Therapy Journal, 58, 145–154. doi:10.1111/ Developmental and Physical Disabilities, 24, 19–33. doi:10.1007/
j.1440-1630.2010.00896.x s10882-011-9252-2
Martin, A. J., Darlow, B. A., Salt, A., Hague, W., Sebastian, L., Rommel, N., De Meyer, A. M., Feenstra, L., & Veereman-Wauters, G.
Mann, K., . . . Inis Trial Collaborative Group. (2012). Identifi- (2003). The complexity of feeding problems in 700 infants
cation of infants with major cognitive delay using parental and young children presenting to a tertiary care institution.
report. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 54, 254– Journal of Pediatric Gastroenterology and Nutrition, 37, 75–84.
259. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8749.2011.04161.x doi:10.1097/00005176-200307000-00014
Martins, Y., Young, R. L., & Robson, D. C. (2008). Feeding and Samara, M., Johnson, S., Lamberts, K., Marlow, N., & Wolke, D.
eating behaviors in children with autism and typically develop- (2010). Eating problems at age 6 years in a whole population
ing children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, sample of extremely preterm children. Developmental Medicine
38, 1878–1887. doi:10.1007/s10803-008-0583-5 and Child Neurology, 52, e16–e22. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8749.2009.
Mascola, A. J., Bryson, S. W., & Agras, W. S. (2010). Picky eating 03512.x
during childhood: A longitudinal study to age 11 years. Eating Seiverling, L., Hendy, H. M., & Williams, K. (2011). The Screening
Behaviors, 11, 253–257. doi:10.1016/j.eatbeh.2010.05.006 Tool of Feeding Problems Applied to Children (STEP-CHILD):

Thoyre et al.: Pediatric Eating Assessment Tool 13

Downloaded From: http://ajslp.pubs.asha.org/ by a University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill User on 02/14/2014
Psychometric characteristics and associations with child and low birth weight (ELBW). Infant Behavior and Development,
parent variables. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 32, 35, 187–194. doi:10.1016/j.infbeh.2012.01.005
1122–1129. doi:10.1016/j.ridd.2011.01.012 Van Riper, M., & Thoyre, S. (2006, August). Feeding challenges for
Sheehan, J., Massie, J., Hay, M., Jaffe, A., Glazner, J., Armstrong, young children with Down syndrome and their families. Paper
D., & Hiscock, H. (2012). The natural history and predictors of presented at the meeting of the World Down Syndrome
persistent problem behaviours in cystic fibrosis: A multicentre, Congress, Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada.
prospective study. Archives of Disease in Childhood, 97, 625–631. Wardle, J., Guthrie, C. A., Sanderson, S., & Rapoport, L. (2001).
doi:10.1136/archdischild-2011-301527 Development of the Children’s Eating Behaviour Questionnaire.
Stoner, J. B., Bailey, R. L., Angell, M. E., Robbins, J., & Polewski, K. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42, 963–970.
(2006). Perspectives of parents/guardians of children with doi:10.1111/1469-7610.00792
feeding/swallowing problems. Journal of Developmental and Williams, K. E., Riegel, K., & Kerwin, M. L. (2009). Feeding
Physical Disabilities, 18, 333–353. doi:10.1007/s10882-006-9020-x disorder of infancy and early childhood: How often is it seen
Stratton, M. (1981). Behavioral Assessment Scale of Oral Functions in feeding programs? Children’s Health Care, 38, 123–136.
in Feeding. American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 35, doi:10.1080/02739610902813302
719–721. Retrieved from http://www.aota.org Willis, G. B. (2005). Cognitive interviewing: A tool for improving
Sullivan, P. B., Lambert, B., Rose, M., Ford-Adams, M., Johnson, A., questionnaire design. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
& Griffiths, P. (2000). Prevalence and severity of feeding and Wilson, E. M., & Hustad, K. C. (2009). Early feeding abilities in
nutritional problems in children with neurological impairment: children with cerebral palsy: A parental report study. Journal
Oxford Feeding Study. Developmental Medicine & Child of Medical Speech-Language Pathology, 17, 31–44. Retrieved
Neurolology, 42, 674–680. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8749.2000.tb00678.x from http://go.galegroup.com
Thelen, E. (2005). Dynamic systems theory and the complexity of Wright, C. M., Parkinson, K. N., Shipton, D., & Drewett, R. F.
change. Psychoanalytic Dialogues, 15, 255–283. doi:10.1080/ (2007). How do toddler eating problems relate to their eating
10481881509348831 behavior, food preferences, and growth? Pediatrics, 120,
Thelen, E., & Smith, L. B. (1994). A dynamic systems approach to the e1069–e1075. doi:10.1542/peds.2006-2961
development of cognition and action. Cambridge, MA: Bradford / Wu, Y. P., Franciosi, J. P., Rothenberg, M. E., & Hommel, K. A.
MIT Press. (2012). Behavioral feeding problems and parenting stress in
Thoyre, S., & Van Riper, M. (2010, February). Figuring it out: eosinophilic gastrointestinal disorders in children. Pediatric
Parents’ reports of how they help their toddlers with Down Allergy and Immunology, 23, 730–735. doi:10.1111/j.1399-3038.
syndrome learn how to eat. Paper presented at the annual meeting 2012.01340.x
of the Southern Nursing Research Society, Austin, TX. Young, B., & Drewett, R. (2000). Eating behaviour and its variability
Törölä, H., Lehtihalmes, M., Yliherva, A., & Olsén, P. (2012). in 1-year-old children. Appetite, 35, 171–177. doi:10.1006/appe.
Feeding skill milestones of preterm infants born with extremely 2000.0346

14 American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology • 1–14

Downloaded From: http://ajslp.pubs.asha.org/ by a University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill User on 02/14/2014
View publication stats

You might also like