You are on page 1of 13

Journal of Consumer Research, Inc.

The Effect of Package Shape on Consumers’ Judgments of Product Volume: Attention as a


Mental Contaminant
Author(s): Valerie Folkes and Shashi Matta
Source: Journal of Consumer Research, Vol. 31, No. 2 (September 2004), pp. 390-401
Published by: The University of Chicago Press
Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/10.1086/422117 .
Accessed: 04/01/2015 06:51

Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at .
http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp

.
JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of
content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms
of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org.

The University of Chicago Press and Journal of Consumer Research, Inc. are collaborating with JSTOR to
digitize, preserve and extend access to Journal of Consumer Research.

http://www.jstor.org

This content downloaded from 137.195.150.201 on Sun, 4 Jan 2015 06:51:22 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
The Effect of Package Shape on Consumers’
Judgments of Product Volume: Attention as a
Mental Contaminant
VALERIE FOLKES
SHASHI MATTA*

A series of experiments examined how a container’s shape can bias judgments


of product quantity. Packages that have shapes that are perceived as attracting
more attention are also perceived to contain a greater volume of a product than
same-sized packages that attract less attention. The disparity in attention leads to
“mental contamination” of the volume judgment. The bias holds for different sets
of containers, for containers placed in different contexts, and for containers with
contents varying in desirability. Habituation to an unusual container that attracts
attention can reduce the effect, as can viewing containers with a disliked content.

P ackages come in all shapes and sizes, complicating the


ability of consumers to make accurate judgments about
the amount of a product. Some package sizes vary because
leading to bias in volume estimation. Hence, the notion
suggested by previous research that consumers make volume
judgments by mentally comparing one physical dimension
of the nature of the product (e.g., produce, meat), but for of a container against that of another container may be true
others the basis for the variation is not obvious (e.g., con- for only some situations.
sider the myriad sizes and shapes of shampoos). Consumers
can easily overcome the challenge of visually assessing vol-
umes contained within a variety of shapes because most THEORETICAL BACKGROUND
product labels provide amount information. When a con-
sumer wishes to compare product volumes, an obvious so- As with previous marketing research on volume judg-
lution is to simply read the label and compare standard units ments, our interest lies in visual perception of package size.
(e.g., compare fluid ounces). In general, visual input dominates other modalities in per-
However, previous research has documented that shoppers ception (Posner, Nissen, and Klein 1976). Although con-
often do not expend the seemingly minimal effort to read sumers shop with their eyes, they apparently exert little
product label and price information (Cole and Balasubra- effort to search for volume information on package labels
manian 1993; Dickson and Sawyer 1990). The purpose of (e.g., Cole and Balasubramanian 1993). The dependence on
our research is to explore biases in visual judgments of visual assessment of volume has led researchers to examine
package volume. Surprisingly, the basic question of how judgments for the kinds of containers people encounter
consumers assess product volume has just begun to be ex- while purchasing and consuming products.
amined. Our research extends that literature by identifying Since survival depends on the ability to discriminate
an external and often irrelevant factor to a container’s size among stimuli, it is not surprising that visual judgments of
that can mentally contaminate a consumer’s volume judg- size are often made easily and automatically. For example,
ment. When consumers pay greater attention to one con- consumers seem to have no difficulty discriminating be-
tainer than to another container with the same volume, that tween a quart-size milk carton and a half-gallon milk carton.
differential attention contaminates their volume judgment, As a general rule, discriminating between objects becomes
more difficult as the magnitude of the difference decreases
(e.g., Banks, Mermelstein, and Yu 1982). The difficulty is
*Valerie Folkes is professor of marketing, Marshall School of Busi-
ness, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, CA 90089-1421
illustrated in research examining two-dimensional figures
(folkes@marshall.usc.edu). Shashi Matta is a doctoral candidate in with the same area. When consumers compare the size of
marketing at the University of Southern California (matta@marshall two shapes (e.g., a rectangle and a circle of the same area),
.usc.edu). The authors appreciate the insightful comments of C. W. they make systematic errors. Krider, Raghubhir, and Krishna
Park and Allison Johnson on an earlier draft of this manuscript and
are grateful for the help of George Eapen in conducting this research.
(2001) suggest that consumers judge the area of a shape by
comparing across the most salient linear dimension, weigh-
390

䉷 2004 by JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH, Inc. ● Vol. 31 ● September 2004


All rights reserved. 0093-5301/2004/3102-0014$10.00

This content downloaded from 137.195.150.201 on Sun, 4 Jan 2015 06:51:22 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
PACKAGE SHAPE AND VOLUME CONTENTS 391

ing the initial dimension more heavily than the second di- rule. To confirm that size influences perceived attention for
mension. Differential weight leads to bias favoring which- similarly shaped packages, we conducted a small-scale
ever shape is largest on the initial dimension. study. Each of 16 female students viewed two cans with
Obviously, considering an additional dimension increases different volumes and with the labels masked by white paper.
the difficulty of judging a shape’s volume compared to its When asked, “Which of these two containers attracts your
area. Further increasing the difficulty of consumers’ volume attention more?” all 16 students reported that the 16 oz. can
judgments is that many package shapes are irregular, with attracted their attention more than the 12 oz. can.
varying widths. For example, the classic Coke bottle’s hour- The covariance of attention and size may cause the overall
glass shape complicates the assessment of even its mean attention-attracting properties of a container’s shape to bias
width. Given that consumers have developed a variety of or mentally “contaminate” volume judgments. External and
shortcuts to conserve mental effort in decision making, they irrelevant factors frequently intrude upon and contaminate
are likely to simplify these volume judgments. judgments of objects, yet they are unrecognized because
people often are unable to identify the basis for a judgment
The Height Heuristic in Judging Package Volume (Wilson and Brekke 1994). An individual might be partic-
ularly unlikely to recognize the intrusion of a perceptual
Raghubir and Krishna (1999) found that consumers use factor on a perceptual judgment. When judging size, people
the height of the container or its elongation to simplify are accustomed to relying on their senses to make quick
volume judgments. A container’s height predicted volume judgments without introspecting about why an object ap-
judgments better than or about as well as models that in- pears larger than another. For example, people accept that
cluded width or depth measurements. When containers are a half-gallon milk carton appears larger than a quart without
tall or elongated, they are perceived as having more of a analyzing why it does so. Further, attention can intrude upon
product than containers that are shorter or squat in shape. size judgments because attention can be directed to objects
Both Krider et al. (2001) and Raghubir and Krishna automatically, without a consumer having to consciously
(1999) maintain that consumers simplify the size judgment deliberate over it. An individual might not even notice that
task by using a single dimension at a time (although Ra- he or she is comparing across packages in their attention-
ghubir and Krishna’s study 1 results suggest that consumers attracting abilities because comparative evaluations are so
use only one dimension, height, rather than each of the ingrained as to be spontaneous (for a review, see Mussweiler
three). Yet, focusing on a single dimension seems incon- 2003). Hence, a consumer might simply conclude that one
sistent with research that suggests that people pay attention package “seems bigger” without quite knowing why.
to objects at a holistic level rather than to a particular di- In sum, when consumers compare the volume of two
mension. Visual attention is directed to objects as a whole similarly sized packages, that judgment may be contami-
rather than to areas of interest in a visual field (see gestalt nated by a factor that typically covaries with size differ-
theories of attention vs. spatial theories of attention, e.g., ences—that one container attracts more attention than the
Baylis and Driver 1993; Bloch 1995). other. If one of the two containers attracts more attention,
consumers may misattribute the greater attention they sub-
Attention Attraction and Size Judgments for jectively experience as being paid to that package to a size
Irregular Shapes difference. This intrusion may occur unconsciously so that
consumers do not recognize its influence.
Our research focuses on irregular container shapes (e.g.,
tapered beverage bottles as opposed to more regularly H1: A package shape that attracts more attention from
shaped canned goods) because previous research does not a consumer is judged as containing a greater vol-
seem to provide an adequate explanation for how consumers ume of a product compared to a same-sized pack-
simplify those volume judgments when packages are similar age shape that attracts less attention.
in size. We propose that consumers holistically compare an
irregularly shaped container to another container. If the con-
tainers are similar in size, the one that attracts attention more Overview of the Research
is judged to be larger.
This bias is due to the covariation of the attention directed We conducted a series of experiments examining biases
to a stimulus with its size. Through experience with a variety in consumers’ judgments of the volume of products con-
of stimuli, people probably have learned that they can ho- tained in packages that were available in the marketplace.
listically and easily gauge which of two shapes is larger Study 1 tests hypothesis 1 by asking consumers to judge
when the magnitude of the difference is great. A perceptual the volume of containers differing in their attention-attract-
sensation that covaries with that size assessment is differ- ing properties. This field experiment provides some evidence
ential attention—large shapes generally attract attention of the ecological validity of our findings, while also facil-
more than small shapes. For example, the larger the ad, the itating comparisons with a previous volume estimation study
more likely it is to be seen (Finn 1988; Gronhaug, Kvita- that used similar stimuli. Study 2 manipulated exposure to
stein, and Gronmo 1991). Large containers probably also the containers to provide support for attention as the me-
attract more attention than small containers, as a general diator of the effect, as well as indicating the magnitude of

This content downloaded from 137.195.150.201 on Sun, 4 Jan 2015 06:51:22 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
392 JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

the error. Study 3 explored cognitive responses to containers, the same type of beverage but one bottle had more attention-
investigating the relationship between thoughts about con- attracting properties than the control bottle. For two of the
tainer shape and volume. Study 4 aimed to identify a mod- pairs, the less attention-attracting bottle was also taller.
erator of the effect—liking for the product. Within each pair, one container attracted attention more be-
Most of the experiments used a paired comparisons meth- cause it had a more unusual shape for the product class than
odology, a well-established procedure in psychology (Wed- the control container. In a context of more familiar objects,
ell 1995) and one that has been used in past consumer re- novel stimuli capture attention rapidly and automatically
search on volume judgments (Raghubir and Krishna 1999). (Johnston et al. 1990). The way that the more unusual con-
Consumers compared a pair of packages—a control package tainer was novel varied across pairs for the purpose of testing
and a package that attracted more attention. In most of our the generality of the effect. For the 10 oz. apple juice pair,
studies the container that attracted less attention was either the shorter bottle should have attracted attention because it
of similar height or taller and contained the same volume was different from other packages in its squat, apple-like
of the product. Although the paired comparisons method- shape. For the 16 oz. lemonade pair, the shorter, more squat
ology involves presenting participants with two packages lemonade bottle had an unusual rippled or jagged contour
simultaneously, attention should be directed to one object instead of the smooth contour of the taller, more elongated
at a time. People find it difficult to attend to two objects control bottle. For the 20 oz. lemonade pair, the container
simultaneously (Baylis and Driver 1993; Duncan 1984). As height was similar but one bottle was more unusual than
with other cognitive resources, people have limited attention the other. The more unusual bottle was more curvaceous
to allocate to objects in their surroundings. One object at- and was not radially symmetrical whereas the other, the
tracts attention at the expense of another. control bottle, was not as curvaceous and was radially sym-
Further, comparisons between two alternatives seem to metrical. To create a greater hurdle for observing the con-
be common when consumers choose products. Eye-tracking tamination effect, the more unusual 20 oz. bottle was glass
research shows that shoppers screen a grocery product class but the usual bottle was plastic. Raghubir and Krishna (1999,
quickly, spending relatively more time directing their atten- study 1) found that glass containers were judged to have a
tion to two or three alternatives (though the total mean search smaller volume than were plastic containers.
time is less than half a minute) (Russo and Leclerc 1994). To facilitate comparisons with previous research, the
Hence, the paired comparisons procedure is an appropriate package characteristics described in table 1 correspond to
method. many of those used by Raghubir and Krishna to describe
their study 1 containers. All comparisons were made for
STUDY 1 cylindrical containers holding beverages, partially to create
a higher hurdle for dismissing the height effect. Raghubir
We conducted a field experiment in which we asked par- and Krishna (1999) described their height effect inter-
ticipants to make volume judgments when one package changeably as an elongation effect (the ratio of height to
shape attracted more attention than another. To investigate maximum diameter/width) so two of our control containers
the generality of the effect, volume judgments were made were both taller and more elongated.
for three pairs of containers. Some of these same containers
were used in subsequent studies, so study 1 serves an im- Pretest of Attention Attraction. To confirm that the
portant purpose in establishing that people perceive the par- three container pairs differentially attracted attention, a pre-
ticular experimental stimuli used as differing in the extent test was conducted in which we asked participants which
to which they attract attention and in their perceived volume. container attracted their attention more. Thirty-three people
A grocery store setting was selected to provide confidence (10 university employees and 23 students) were paid for
that hypothesis 1 holds in a shopping situation in which their participation. Participants viewed each of the three
consumers might make volume judgments. container pairs with the ordering of the pairs randomized.
After viewing a pair of bottles (with brand labels removed
Method and counterbalanced positioning), participants were given a
questionnaire asking them, “Which of these two containers
The experiment was a 3 # 2 # 2 between-subjects de- attracts more attention?” and “Which of these containers has
sign, varying stimulus pair, context and the direction of the more volume?”
size comparison (greater volume vs. lesser volume). The As intended, for each of the three pairs the majority of
stimuli were three different pairs of packages. The two con- participants reported that the unusual containers attracted
texts were different locations in the supermarkets that pro- their attention more than their counterparts (78.79% for the
vided a background against which the stimuli were placed. 20 oz. bottles, 78.79% for the 16 oz. bottles, and 69.70%
The third independent variable controlled for the way the for the 10 oz. bottles; see table 2). Consistent with hypothesis
question was posed by asking either which one of the pair 1, the majority of participants (84.84%, 81.82%, and
had a greater volume or which had a lesser volume. 72.73%, respectively) reported that the unusual containers
Respondents made paired-comparison judgments within had a greater volume. The differences are significant within
the same type of product for two products—lemonade and each pair for each of the three measures (see table 2).
apple juice. Each pair of bottles held the same volume of Participants were asked how much they liked lemonade/

This content downloaded from 137.195.150.201 on Sun, 4 Jan 2015 06:51:22 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
PACKAGE SHAPE AND VOLUME CONTENTS 393

TABLE 1

DESCRIPTION OF STIMULI: PRETEST AND STUDY 1

Content of container
Characteristics of container 20 oz. lemonade 16 oz. lemonade 10 oz. apple juice

Commonality of shape Usual Unusuala Usual Unusual Usualb Unusual


Container materialc Plastic Glass Plastic Plastic Glass Glass
Height of the container (cm) 23.0 23.0 22.0 17.0 18.5 11.0
Level of liquid in the container (cm) 18.5 19.7 17.8 15.2 15.2 8.3
Maximum diameter (cm) 7.2 7.7 6.0 7.3 6.0 8.2
Minimum diameter (cm) 2.8 3.8 2.6 4.0 2.6 3.8
Height to maximum diameter ratio 3.2 3.0 3.7 2.3 3.1 1.4
Height to minimum diameter ratio 8.4 6.0 8.5 4.3 7.2 2.9
Shape (max. diameter ⫺ min. diameter) 19.8 14.8 11.4 10.8 12.0 19.0
Lateral surface area (cm2) 357.1 414.4 417.4 303.2 248.7 243.9
Total surface area (cm2) 403.7 471.8 473.6 357.2 282.4 371.0
Volume of the container (cm3) 601 607 485 478 305 305
a
The unusual bottle was radially asymmetric, being somewhat triangular in shape in its middle section but round at the top and bottom sections. The usual bottle
was radially symmetric. Radial asymmetry accounts for the unusual container’s equivalent volume and greater fill level but greater diameter compared to the usual
container.
b
This bottle was a brand—Martinelli’s apple juice in a glass bottle—used as a stimulus in Raghubir and Krishna’s (1999) study 1. Although the volume of our
Martinelli bottle was similar to their Martinelli bottle (305 ml vs. 315 ml, respectively), the dimensions of our bottle differs from theirs.
c
Raghubir and Krishna (1999, study 1) found greater volume estimates for plastic containers than for glass containers, independent of the height effect. Increases
in height and their shape measure (maximum diameter — minimum diameter) independently predicted volume estimates to a similar extent. An alternative regression
model showed that elongation (height/maximum diameter) predicted volume estimates.

apple juice and their attitude toward lemonade/apple juice


(1 p like very much, 9 p not at all; 1 p positive, 9 p
negative). Those items were combined to form a product-
liking measure (r p .94). Means were below the midpoint,
suggesting that participants liked both products, but partic- TABLE 2
ipants liked lemonade more than apple juice (M p 3.9 vs. PERCENT CHOOSING CONTAINER FOR EACH PAIR OF
M p 4.7, respectively), t(32) p ⫺4.56, p ! .001. (The ef- STIMULI: RESULTS OF PRETEST AND STUDY 1
fects of liking for the product are examined in study 4.)
Container size
Procedure. The participants in the main study were 240
adult grocery shoppers (132 females and 108 males) who Dependent measure 20 oz. 16 oz. 10 oz.
were recruited from two urban supermarkets. The experi-
menter waited for an unaccompanied, adult grocery shopper Pretest, more attention (%):
Unusual container 78.79 81.82 69.70
to approach where he was standing in an empty grocery Usual container 21.21 18.18 30.30
aisle. When the shopper was nearby, he pointed to the pair N 33 33 33
of bottles and asked the shopper, “Which of these two bottles x2 (1) 10.94* 13.36* 5.12*
has a greater/lesser volume of the lemonade/apple juice? Pretest, more volume (%):
Unusual container 84.85 78.79 72.72
Which of these two bottles has more/less of the lemonade/ Usual container 6.06 9.09 15.15
apple juice?” The experimenter noted the person’s response Both are same volume 9.09 12.12 12.12
and gender. Shoppers responded to the question in about 3 N 33 33 33
to 10 seconds. The shoppers were randomly assigned to one x2 (2) 39.46* 35.09* 23.09*
of the twelve conditions, making a paired-comparison judg- Cramer’s Va .82 .93 .93
Study 1, more volume:
ment for one of the three pairs of bottles. Unusual 95.0 97.5 87.5
Usual 2.5 2.5 7.5
Both are same 2.5 0 5.0
Results and Discussion N 40 40 40
x2 68.45* 36.10* 52.85*
Shoppers judged the containers that captured more atten- Study 1, less volume (%):
tion to be larger, even when the usual container was also Unusual 12.5 12.5 15.0
taller (see table 2). Two of the more unusual bottles were Usual 75.0 82.5 82.5
also more complex stimuli in that their contours were more Both are same 12.5 5.0 2.5
N 40 40 40
varied, raising the possibility that complexity rather than or x2 31.25* 43.85* 44.45*
in addition to novelty enhanced attention (Berlyne 1960; a
Cramer’s V-statistic is calculated for a 2 (attention) # 3 (size judgment)
Wohlwill 1975). However, the short apple juice bottle’s di- contingency table for the pretest data.
mensions suggest less complexity than the taller bottle, yet *p ! .05.

This content downloaded from 137.195.150.201 on Sun, 4 Jan 2015 06:51:22 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
394 JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

the majority of participants judged it both larger and more unusual product as larger might be explained by a slightly
attention-attracting. That finding suggests that shape com- different process than that which we proposed. Perhaps in-
plexity is not essential for judgments to be biased. attention to the usual container led to an inaccurate assessment
of its size. The shoppers might have made the judgments
Effects of Container Shape versus Specific Dimen- hurriedly to avoid delaying their own shopping and ignored
sions. An alternative explanation for the results is that the less visually interesting, usual container. However, even
participants inferred that the unusual products contained if shoppers made the judgments quickly, the pretest results
more because of their greater diameter. For our unusual make it unlikely that insufficient time devoted to scrutiny
bottles to be shorter than the same-sized usual bottles, the explains the effect. Pretest respondents were comfortably
unusual bottles had to be wider. Because our results showed seated and could view the containers at their leisure before
that respondents judged shorter containers to have greater estimating the amounts. Yet, they judged the unusual con-
volume, the results could also be interpreted as showing that tainers to be larger. Nevertheless, study 2 aimed to replicate
consumers perceive wider containers to be larger. the results in a laboratory setting that lacked the distraction
That explanation is unlikely based on theoretical and em- of a shopping task and provided plenty of time for respondents
pirical reasons. First, the complexity of many three-dimen- to view the containers.
sional packages, such as those examined here, makes con-
sumers more likely to direct their attention holistically rather STUDY 2
than to an individual container dimension when making vol-
ume judgments. Even if the shoppers failed to attend ho- Study 2 focused on attention as the underlying reason for
listically, there is no evidence to suggest that they would why volume judgments of novel containers are biased. This
focus on width. Empirical research indicates that height ac- experiment manipulated the extent to which an unusual con-
counts for volume estimates about as well or better than tainer attracted attention by increasing the length of exposure
models that include width measurements (Raghubir and to it. People habituate to novel stimuli so that the unfamiliar
Krishna 1999). becomes familiar over time and the attention paid to the
If one disregards Raghubir and Krishna’s results and en- stimuli decreases (Berlyne 1971). Extended exposure to the
tertains the notion that diameter influenced our participants’ unusual container should reduce the perception that it is
volume judgments, the evidence for the diameter effect is larger by reducing the subjective sense that the unusual con-
unimpressive. The usual bottles did have a smaller minimum tainer more successfully competes for the viewer’s attention
diameter than the unusual bottles. However, the notion that to the usual container. Hence, we hypothesize the following:
people identified one bottle as bigger on the basis of the
size of its smallest diameter seems incompatible with the H2: Habituation to a novel package shape decreases
task. It is more plausible to think that the maximum diameter the tendency to estimate a greater volume of the
was used as a heuristic for volume judgments. Yet, even product compared to that of a same-sized, more
that seems unlikely. The difference in maximum diameter usual package shape.
for the pairs was smallest for the 20 oz. bottle (.5 cm), but
the greatest percent of participants (84.85%) chose the un-
usual bottle in that pair as the largest. It is doubtful that Method
such a small difference in size would have such a great
impact when assessed visually. Participants were 64 female graduate students and uni-
Although neither of the two individual diameter measures versity staff, who were paid $5.00. The study was a 2 #
seem to account for the effect of unusual shapes on volume 2 between-subjects experiment manipulating exposure (ha-
judgments, it is important to rule out the effect of another bituation vs. no habituation) and shape of the container that
diameter measure used by Raghubir and Krishna (1999) to is shown first (unusual vs. usual shape). The containers were
make the case that our results are truly novel. They found the 16 oz. lemonade bottles from study 1.
that a measure they termed “shape” (maximum diameter ⫺ In the habituation condition, each participant was exposed
minimum diameter) predicted increases in their participants’ to either the unusual or the usual container while being
size judgments independent of container height. However, briefed about an unrelated study that involved listening to
table 1 shows that their shape measure does not explain our an audio recording. The women completed that study in
results. The difference between the maximum and minimum about 15 min. Then the experimenter explained that the next
diameter is much larger for the usual container than for the task entailed comparisons of volume. The participant was
unusual container in the 20 oz. pair, is similar for the 16 told that the container that was on the table held 16 oz. of
oz. pair, and is much smaller for the usual container than lemonade. The experimenter then placed the second con-
the for unusual container in the 10 oz. pair. Hence, the effect tainer next to the first one (placement position varied) and
of our attention-attracting containers on volume judgment asked the participant to estimate the volume of the second
does not appear to have been identified previously. container.
Women in the nonhabituation exposure conditions were
Accuracy as an Alternative Explanation. Although the not exposed to any container prior to the unrelated study.
results support hypothesis 1, the tendency to perceive the Instead, on completing the unrelated study, the participant

This content downloaded from 137.195.150.201 on Sun, 4 Jan 2015 06:51:22 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
PACKAGE SHAPE AND VOLUME CONTENTS 395

was shown both the containers simultaneously. Half the .001). No significant differences were found across condi-
women were told that the usual container had 16 oz. of tions for the extent to which the usual container attracted
lemonade and were asked to write their estimate of the un- attention (see table 3). Habituation should have had little
usual container’s lemonade. The other half were told that impact on attention to the usual container because it lacked
the unusual container had 16 oz. of lemonade and were asked novelty.
to write their estimate of the usual container’s lemonade.
The numerical anchor was given because pretesting indi- Volume Estimates. The volume estimates were always
cated participants were poor at estimating amounts in stan- made with the knowledge that one container held 16 oz.
dard units without one. Analyses revealed a significant main effect of container
As a manipulation check, the women were asked to in- shape, which was qualified by a significant interaction of
dicate the extent to which each container attracted their at- container shape and habituation (F(1, 63) p 237.05, p !
tention on a nine-point scale (“not at all” vs. “very much”). .001; F(1, 63) p 10.51, p ! .005, respectively). Consistent
Their attention to the unusual container should be greater with study 1, participants in the nonhabituation conditions
in the no-habituation condition than in the habituation con- estimated the unusual container’s volume to be significantly
dition because habituation would decrease the container’s greater than that of the usual container (M p 18.50 oz. vs.
perceived novelty. The mean attention directed to the usual M p 12.75 oz., respectively; F(1, 63) p 173.68, p ! .001).
container should not differ depending on exposure time be- As predicted by hypothesis 2, habituation to the unusual
cause the usual container was not novel. container increased the volume estimate of the usual con-
tainer as compared to no-habituation to the unusual container
(M p 14.31 oz. vs. M p 12.75 oz.); F(1, 63) p 12.82,
Results p ! .001). Hence, habituation to the unusual container has
The volume estimates and the manipulation check for at- the effect of reducing the perceived difference between the
tention were analyzed using ANOVA and planned contrasts. two containers. The mean difference between the 16 oz.
unusual container and the usual container was only 1.64 oz.
Manipulation Check. As intended, habituation de- when participants habituated to the unusual container but
creased mean ratings of attention paid to the unusual con- was 3.25 oz. when participants did not habituate to it. This
tainer but not to the usual container (see table 3). Analysis finding is consistent with the notion that lengthy exposure
of variance of the attention attracted by the unusual container (habituation) decreases the unusual container’s novelty and
revealed a significant main effect for habituation and for so reduces the relative attention directed to the unusual con-
shape, which was qualified by a shape by habituation in- tainer when it is compared to the usual container. Yet, even
teraction (F(1, 63) p 16.58, p ! .001; F(1, 63) p 20.85, lengthy exposure does not eliminate the bias. Few respon-
p ! .001; and F(1, 63) p 18.66, p ! .001, respectively). Ta- dents accurately estimated that the containers were equal
ble 3 shows that the women who habituated to the unusual (table 3).
container believed that it attracted significantly less attention The usual container already attracted less attention and
when compared to those who habituated to the usual con- so extended exposure to the usual container should not
tainer (M p 5.19 vs. M p 7.44, F(1, 63) p 39.47, p ! change the perception that it is smaller than the unusual
.001). Additionally, participants who habituated to the un- container. Hence, when told that the usual container holds
usual container believed that it attracted significantly less 16 oz., the unusual container’s volume should be perceived
attention when compared to those in the nonhabituation con- as larger regardless of exposure time. Consistent with that
ditions (M p 5.19 vs. M p 7.34, F(1, 63) p 35.21, p ! logic, the mean estimate of the unusual container’s volume

TABLE 3

EFFECT OF HABITUATION ON ATTENTION AND VOLUME ESTIMATES: RESULTS OF STUDY 2

Habituation No habituation
Usual container is Unusual container is Usual container is Unusual container is
Dependent measure anchor anchor anchor anchor

Attention to the usual 4.19 4.88 4.25 4.38


(.91) (.89) (1.18) (1.20)
Attention to the unusual 7.44 5.19 7.38 7.31
(.96) (.83) (1.09) (1.14)
Estimated volume in fluid ounces 18.06a 14.31b 18.50a 12.75b
(1.34) (1.20) (1.37) (1.00)
% correctly estimating 12.5 12.5 6.25 0
N 16 16 16 16
NOTE.—Standard deviations are in parentheses.
a
Participants were told that the volume of the usual container was 16 oz., and they were asked to estimate the volume of the unusual container.
b
Participants were told that the volume of the unusual container was 16 oz., and they were asked to estimate the volume of the usual container.

This content downloaded from 137.195.150.201 on Sun, 4 Jan 2015 06:51:22 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
396 JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

in the habituation condition was similar to that in the no nation of their judgments, correctly perceiving that the con-
habituation condition (see table 3). tainers were the same size even though they were not told
which container attracted more attention. Most informed
Discussion consumers were accurate, as contrasted with study 2 habit-
uation respondents who merely increased their looking time.
Study 2 replicates the study 1 findings that consumers over- At best, the latter underestimated the amount by a mean of
estimate the volume of an unusually shaped container when 10.56% (with 12.5% being accurate).
compared to a more usual shape (when both are presented
simultaneously). When told that the usual bottle held 16 oz.,
consumers overestimated the unusual container’s content by
STUDY 3
15.63%. When told that the unusual bottle held 16 oz., con- Study 3 investigated cognitive responses to containers
sumers underestimated the usual container by 20.31%. How- differing in their tendency to attract attention. Mental con-
ever, habituation to the unusual container reduced the per- tamination occurs because people often form evaluations
ceived difference in volume between same-sized containers. without analyzing the mental processes that lead to the eval-
Extended exposure to the unusual container should have re- uation (Wilson and Brekke 1994). Due to limited access to
duced its novelty and so its ability to attract attention when one’s mental processes, consumers are unlikely to describe
its size is compared to the usual container. In fact, habituation differential attention as the mechanism explaining their vol-
to the unusual container did reduce the perceived difference ume estimates when asked to explain their volume judg-
in volume, consistent with hypothesis 2. ments. However, if differential attention mentally contam-
The results suggest that errors occurred even though par- inates volume judgments at even a low level of awareness,
ticipants were not distracted by a shopping task, as might some evidence of that process might surface in analyses of
have been true for the study 1 shoppers. It seems unlikely cognitive responses. Thoughts about an unusually shaped
that the lengthy exposure to the container in the habituation container should be strongly related to thoughts about size.
condition would have increased overall accuracy because Further, those thoughts about shape should be more acces-
respondents did not know that they should allocate the extra sible for an unusual than a usual shape.
time to assessing size. Further, once they were asked for Related information is connected by associative pathways
estimates, no pressure was exerted on participants to make so that, if one concept is activated, related concepts are also
those judgments quickly. In all conditions, respondents were activated (for a review of spreading activation models of
allowed to gaze at the containers for as long as they wished. memory, see Markman 1999). The mental contamination
Hence, an extra 15 min. of exposure to a container while explanation suggests that unusual shapes and larger sizes
the individual is engaged in an unrelated task seems unlikely are connected because they both attract attention. Alterna-
to increase accuracy. tively, thoughts about container shape may be associated
Study 2 results might lead one to question whether ac- conceptually with thoughts about container size simply be-
curate volume estimates are possible. We examined this is- cause both are physical properties of products. If so,
sue in a small-scale study that used procedures similar to thoughts about shape should accompany thoughts about size
study 2’s no-habituation condition, but we informed re- regardless of whether the shape attracts attention. Conse-
spondents of the bias. A common means to correct for the quently, the activation of that concept (shape) would influ-
unwanted influence of extraneous factors is to inform people ence judgments of size (see Higgins, Rholes, and Jones
of the direction of the mental process that taints their judg- 1977; Higgins 1996). In sum, mental contamination suggests
ments (Wilson and Brekke 1994). We characterize the at- the following:
tention-volume bias as contamination because it seems un-
likely that consumers would desire differential attention H3: More consumers think about the shape and the
directed toward objects to bias their size judgments. People size of unusual containers than think about the
aware of the bias should want to correct for it. shape and size of usual containers. Further, shape
The experimenter informed 36 undergraduate students and size are more top-of-mind thoughts about un-
that “people, when asked to judge which of two containers usual than usual containers.
has more volume, pick the one that attracts their attention
more.” The students were shown the pair of 16 oz. containers Method
used in earlier studies. Half of the students were told that
the usual bottle contained 16 oz., and were asked to estimate Of the 64 respondents, 49 were undergraduate students
the volume of the unusual bottle, whereas the other half who participated for extra credit and 15 were graduate stu-
were told that the unusual bottle contained 16 oz. and were dents who voluntarily participated. They provided thought
asked to estimate the volume of the usual bottle. Mean es- listings in response to the 16 oz. lemonade containers used
timates of the amounts were not significantly different: in studies 1 and 2. The experiment was a between-subjects
M p 15.83 oz. for the usual bottle, with 88.89% accurately design with half the respondents shown the tall container
estimating 16 oz. versus M p 16.17 oz. for the unusual and the other half shown the shorter, unusual container.
bottle, with 83.33% being accurate. Those results suggest Study 3 did not present both containers simultaneously be-
that the students could exercise control over the contami- cause that would increase the difficulty of identifying the

This content downloaded from 137.195.150.201 on Sun, 4 Jan 2015 06:51:22 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
PACKAGE SHAPE AND VOLUME CONTENTS 397

TABLE 4

THOUGHTS IN RESPONSE TO CONTAINER SHAPE: RESULTS OF STUDY 3

Percent mentioning Mean order of listing


Attribute Usual container Unusual container x2 Usual container Unusual container F df

Price 100.00 87.50 .27 2.88 3.68 3.34 1, 58


Expiration date 96.88 87.50 .15 3.39 4.29 1.80 1, 57
Brand 90.63 90.63 0 2.48 2.79 .45 1, 56
Natural 100.00 81.25 .62 3.91 4.57 .89 1, 56
Ingredients 100.00 78.13 .86 4.19 6.00 8.37* 1, 55
Nutrition 84.38 87.50 .02 5.67 6.00 .27 1, 53
Color 68.75 75.00 .09 5.14 6.71 4.92* 1, 44
Size 43.75 87.50 4.7* 7.29 3.25 48.99* 1, 40
Shape 28.13 75.00 6.82* 9.44 5.71 27.45* 1, 31
*p ! .05.

stimulus eliciting respondents’ thoughts. The disadvantage the mean highest rank of the attribute for each container (see
is that exposure to a single container rather than the pair table 4). Considering that the containers lacked price, brand,
should make it more difficult to detect differences in and content information, it is not surprising that those attrib-
thoughts about shape and size. Differences in attention di- utes surfaced in respondents’ thoughts. The only attributes
rected to each stimulus should make differences in the that differed across containers on both the percent of respon-
shapes more salient. However, because the shorter container dents mentioning the attribute and the rank of the attribute
was more unusual for the product category than the other, listing were container size and container shape. The unusual
its shape should attract attention even in the absence of the container elicited more thoughts about size and shape and
comparison product. Consumers should be able to use an elicited them earlier than did the usual container.
internal standard to compare shapes with other containers Further, more respondents reported that they thought
in the marketplace. about both attributes in response to the unusual container
After being shown a lemonade bottle, the students were compared to the usual container. Whereas only 9.3% of the
asked to read the following, “If you encounter this prod- respondents mentioned both size and shape when they
uct in the grocery store and you were interested in buying viewed the less attention-attracting container, 62.5% of the
a bottle of lemonade, what factors would come to your respondents mentioned both size and shape when they
mind when considering this product? Please list these viewed the more attention-attracting container (x 2 (1) p
factors as they come to your mind.” Respondents listed 12.57, p ! .001). The results indicate that the unusually
from five to 15 factors. Two judges who were unaware shaped container not only makes these two factors of con-
of the hypotheses and the previous studies independently tainer size and shape more accessible when the product is
coded the thought listings. They differed on five (1%) of being evaluated but also makes these two factors more
the total of 487 thoughts classified, with disagreements closely related in the consumer’s mind. The difference be-
resolved by a third judge. The attributes were coded into tween conditions indicates that the two attributes are not
one of the following nine categories: price, expiration always linked, which suggests that mental contamination
date, brand naturalness (e.g., natural juice, artificial, occurs in response to shapes that attract attention.
freshly squeezed, made from concentrate), nutritional in-
formation (e.g., percentage of vitamins, calories, fat), Limitations of Thought Listings. As a method, thought
color of the lemonade, container shape (e.g., unusual listings have the advantage of detecting processes at low
shape, easy to carry, appealing, wide, long), and container levels of awareness. A disadvantage is that respondents did
size (e.g., volume, amount of lemonade contained). Only not elaborate much about the thoughts they had about size
6.57% of the thoughts did not fit into one of these cat- and shape. Further, other attributes were mentioned prior to
egories. Each attribute was assigned a rank depending on size and shape. One might question whether attention-at-
where it appeared on the student’s list (e.g., the first item tracting shapes influence product judgments except when
was given a value of one, the second item was given a respondents are asked about size.
value of two). If a student listed more than one attribute Shape Effects in the Absence of Priming of Volume
that was coded as the same category, the value assigned Judgments. To provide additional evidence that shape in-
to the first appearance of the attribute was taken into fluences volume perceptions, we conducted a study in which
account and the other was ignored. size was not specifically primed as the respondent’s judg-
ment task. This study used similar procedures to that in study
Results and Discussion 1, but it asked 25 graduate students to select which of the
The thought listings were analyzed by identifying the per- two containers used in study 3 was the better buy. The
cent of respondents mentioning the attribute and by comparing containers were described as holding similar tasting lem-

This content downloaded from 137.195.150.201 on Sun, 4 Jan 2015 06:51:22 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
398 JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

onade and being available for the same price in the grocery ments resulting in a shape by product desirability interaction
store. The experimenter then asked, “Which of these two (predicted as a moderator by hypothesis 4) or a desirability
bottles of lemonade is a better buy?” The majority (88%) main effect (predicted as an alternative explanation by hy-
selected the unusual bottle as the better buy, a finding that pothesis 5). One might also argue that a different type of
is significantly different from chance (x 2 (1) p 14.44, p ! shape by desirability interaction would be observed due to
.001). Hence, attention-attracting shapes affect size judg- differences in motivation to be accurate. Perhaps consumers
ments even when consumers are not asked specifically to correctly judge the volume of a bottle containing a desirable
compare volume. product since they allocate more cognitive resources to that
task. If so, differences in volume estimates should be greater
when the product is undesirable than when desirable. How-
STUDY 4 ever, that pattern seems unlikely because Raghubir and
Study 4 explored the effect of the container’s content as Krishna (1999, study 2) found no effect on judgments when
a possible moderator of volume judgments. When consum- they increased participants’ motivation to estimate volume
ers initially encounter packages that contain desirable prod- accurately.
ucts, the incentive to allocate cognitive resources to the
judgment task should be greater than when consumers ini-
tially encounter packages that contain undesirable products. Method
A desirable product might be selected for purchase in the
future and so has more personal relevance, whereas an un-
desirable product would not be sought out. People devote The design of study 4 was a 2 # 2, manipulating
less cognitive resources and attention to tasks that lack per- container shape and product desirability as between-sub-
sonal relevance (Celsi and Olson 1988; Renninger and Woz- jects variables. Sixty undergraduate students, who were
niak 1985). Further, eye-movement research indicates that paid $5.00 to participate in a taste test, initially completed
consumers fixate longer on a preferred brand when they are a questionnaire. The same two questions used in study 1
shown an array of brands from the same category (Pieters to measure liking for the product category were combined
and Warlop 1999). to form a category-liking measure for lemonade (r p
When people rule out consuming the product because it .86). After completing those items, each student was
is disliked, the overall attention paid to the volume judgment given a 4 oz. cup of lemonade. Half tasted a popular
task should be less than when the product is liked. This brand of lemonade (desirable product condition) and the
should make the absolute difference in attention allocated other half tasted sour lemonade (undesirable product con-
to the unusual bottle more difficult to detect. The differences dition). Next, the experimenter showed the participant a
in amount of attention may be subtle, reducing the perceived container (the unusual container for half the participants
difference in volume. In short, the effect of attention-at- and the more usual container for the other half) filled
tracting shapes should be greater when consumers like the with lemonade. They were told that this was a brand of
product than when they dislike the product. lemonade already in the market with a flavor similar to
the lemonade that they had just tasted. To provide par-
H4: Volume estimates of a more attention-attracting ticipants with a numerical anchor, they were told that it
container and a less attention-attracting container held 16 oz. Participants were then shown a second bottle
are more discrepant when consumers like the con- (the stimulus object) that purportedly contained the lem-
tent than when they dislike the content. onade they had just tasted. That bottle was described as
one that the manufacturer was considering in which to
Alternatively, a halo effect may account for the volume sell the product. This second container was always a dif-
bias. Perhaps the study 1 findings reflect greater liking for ferent shape from the first but held the same amount of
the unusual containers and respondents’ inferences that those lemonade. If the anchor was the unusually shaped con-
liked containers held more desirable contents than did the tainer then the second container (the stimulus object) was
more usual containers. We addressed this concern by point- the taller, usual shaped container, and vice versa.
ing out to the study 4 participants that the contents of both The questionnaire requested an estimate of the volume of
bottles were similar. Study 4 also manipulated product de- the stimulus object in fluid ounces. Participants were told
sirability to directly test the halo effect explanation. If a halo that they could use the competitive product, which contained
effect influences size judgments, then consumers will be- 16 oz., as a reference point. Students’ liking for the specific
lieve that bottles containing a desirable product are larger lemonade they tasted was measured with two items (r p
than bottles containing an undesirable product, regardless .96) that were similar to the items for liking toward the
of container shape. category of lemonade in general collected at the beginning
H5: Consumers’ volume estimates are greater for con- of the experiment. The next question asked which of the
tainers with desirable products than for containers two containers they found more attractive. About half
with undesirable products. (46.67%) considered the usual shaped container more at-
tractive, and the remainder (53.33%) considered the unusual
In short, package content might influence volume judg- shaped container more attractive.

This content downloaded from 137.195.150.201 on Sun, 4 Jan 2015 06:51:22 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
PACKAGE SHAPE AND VOLUME CONTENTS 399

Results usual bottle and the unusual bottle contained 12.92% more
than the 16 oz. usual bottle.
The results were analyzed using 2 # 2 ANOVAs with Finally, contrasts show that the overestimation of the un-
shape (usual vs. unusual) as one factor and desirability of the usual bottle containing desirable lemonade was greater com-
drink (desirable vs. undesirable) as the other factor. Ratings pared to the overestimation of the unusual bottle containing
of the lemonade itself were examined to confirm that the undesirable lemonade (M p 20.20 vs. M p 18.07;
desirable drink was liked more than the undesirable drink, as F(1, 59) p 13.71, p ! .001). Underestimation of the usual
well as to confirm that lemonade was a positively valued bottle containing desirable lemonade was greater compared
product. As intended, the mean liking for lemonade in general to the underestimation of the usual bottle containing unde-
indicates a positive attitude toward the product (M p 2.64 sirable lemonade (M p 12.13 vs. M p 13.80; F(1, 59) p
on a nine-point scale). Also as intended, liking for the tasted 8.37, p ! .005). In sum, the discrepancy between the differ-
lemonade differed depending on which formula they tasted ently shaped bottles was greater when participants thought
(M p 3.35 for the desirable product and M p 7.15 for the the two bottles contained a desirable product than when they
undesirable product). A 2 # 2 analysis of variance of liking thought the two bottles contained an undesirable product.
for the tasted lemonade showed only a significant main effect
of desirability (F(1, 59) p 216.60, p ! .001). Participants
liked the desirable lemonade similarly to their liking of lem-
Discussion
onade in general but liked the undesirable lemonade much The results support hypotheses 1 and 4 and find no support
less (mean difference is .60 for the delicious condition vs. for hypothesis 5. The bottle that attracted more attention
4.62 for the sour condition). A 2 # 2 analysis of variance was perceived as larger regardless of the content. However,
of the difference between each participant’s liking for the the difference between the two bottles was perceived as
product and liking for the tasted product shows only a main greater when it contained a desirable product. That finding
effect of desirability of the tasted product (F(1, 59) p is consistent with the notion that the discrepancy in attention
75.53, p ! .001). paid to the unusual container compared to the common con-
The volume estimates were always made in relation to tainer is more obvious to consumers when they allocate
knowledge that one bottle contained 16 oz. If the unusual ample cognitive resources to the judgment task. If consum-
bottle was judged, then the usual bottle was described as ers pay minimal attention to the task, the difference between
16 oz., and vice versa. Hypothesis 4 was tested using a 2 the attention allocated to the unusual container and that al-
# 2 ANOVA of the volume estimates. The ANOVA re- located to the usual container should be more subtle and so
vealed a significant main effect for container shape and a harder for the viewer to detect. However, the absence of
significant product desirability by shape interaction process measures makes our interpretation somewhat
(F(1, 29) p 229.04, p ! .001, and F(1, 29) p 21.74, p ! speculative.
.001, respectively). The shape effect replicates the previous Estimates of the usual container’s volume contradict the
findings that the attention-attracting unusual container is per- notion that a halo effect leads consumers to judge the size
ceived as larger than the less attention-attracting, usual con- of desirable objects to be greater than the size of undesirable
tainer. Participants estimated a greater volume for the un- objects (hypothesis 5 not supported). The volume estimates
usual container when the usual container was the anchor, are opposite from those predicted in the usual bottle con-
and they estimated a lesser volume for the usual container dition. Participants thought that the usual bottle contained
when the unusual container was the anchor. more of the undesirable lemonade than of the desirable lem-
The interaction indicates that the effect is moderated by onade (M p 13.80 oz. vs. M p 12.13 oz.). Perceptions of
product desirability. The discrepancy between containers is the bottles’ attractiveness help rule out a halo effect for the
perceived as greater when both containers held a desirable container as opposed to the container’s content. Biases might
product (M p 20.20 oz. in the unusual vs. M p 12.13 oz. have arisen because the unusual bottle was perceived as
in the usual when both contained the desirable product; more attractive and so was assumed to possess additional
M p 18.07 in the unusual versus 13.80 in the usual when positive attributes, such as greater volume. However, at-
both contained the undesirable product). When participants tractiveness did not differ significantly for the containers in
were told that the unusual bottle contained 16 oz. of a de- study 4, ruling out aesthetics as an explanation.
sirable product, participants estimated that the taller bottle
contained 12.13 oz. (24.17% less). When participants were GENERAL DISCUSSION
told that the taller bottle contained 16 oz. of the desirable
product, participants estimated that the unusual bottle con- The results of the experiments 1, 2, and 4 suggest that
tained 20.20 oz. (26.25% more). The means are significantly containers that attract more attention are perceived as larger
different, (M p 12.13 vs. M p 20.20; F(1, 59) p 195.96, in volume than same-sized containers that attract less atten-
p ! .001). The discrepancy between the usual and the un- tion, even when the latter is taller (hypothesis 1). Study 1
usual container was smaller but significantly different when showed that the effect occurred in a shopping environment
both held undesirable lemonade (M p 13.80 vs. M p (the grocery store). The effect decreased when the con-
18.07; F(1, 59) p 54.82, p ! .001). Respondents estimated tainer’s attention-attracting properties were diminished
the usual bottle contained 13.75% less than the 16 oz. un- through habituation (study 2, hypothesis 2). Further, the dis-

This content downloaded from 137.195.150.201 on Sun, 4 Jan 2015 06:51:22 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
400 JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

crepancy in volume judgments increased when the bottles volume effect. However, a halo effect might influence vol-
held a desirable product compared to when the bottles con- ume perceptions when package aesthetics are varied.
tained an undesirable product (study 4, hypothesis 4). The Future research should explore the complex relationship
volume bias is likely to influence purchase decisions because among quantity assessments, attitudes toward the product,
people believe that the container that attracts attention is a and exposure to the product. Liking for the product and its
better buy than a container that is the same size but attracts container may influence attention directed toward the con-
less attention (see the study reported in study 3’s discussion). tainer over time, potentially decreasing mental contamina-
The overall results are consistent with the notion that tion. Consumers may habituate to containers of favored
volume judgments are contaminated by the attention that an brands. Yet, they may also develop a habit of purchasing
unusual container attracts. Consumers may confuse their liked products and be less likely to compare the container
greater attention to one bottle over the other as due to its to which they are loyal to other brands. Further complicating
larger size. Study 3 found that size and shape are associated the relationship between evaluation and attention is the well-
in people’s minds, particularly for the unusual container established mere exposure effect—people like neutral or
(hypothesis 3). Nevertheless, consumers may not be aware mildly pleasant but unfamiliar stimuli more as they gain
that they apply an attentional heuristic to volume judgments. familiarity with them. Whereas familiarity may decrease per-
Mental contamination of judgments can occur below levels ceptions of novelty, it might increase liking for a container.
of awareness (Nisbett and Wilson 1977; Wilson and Brekke Volume judgments are intricately related to the amount
consumed, with consumers learning about quantities from
1994). We characterize the attention-volume bias as mental
their product usage and motivated to judge sizes from prod-
contamination because consumers seem unlikely to want to
ucts they wish to consume. Biases occur in the amount one
believe that unusual shapes increase their size judgments.
allocates for usage depending on judgments of the amount
If it were a voluntary inferential rule, consumers would in a container (Folkes, Martin, and Gupta 1993). Clarifying
consciously learn to judge the volume larger when one shape these relationships will provide interesting avenues for fu-
attracts more attention, similar to other inferential rules that ture research.
could be justified as rational (e.g., that the price of a product Whereas our research focuses on a perceptual contaminant
reflects its quality). Yet, when we informed respondents of of a perceptual judgment, the conditions under which con-
the direction of the way that attention could taint their judg- sumers rely on semantic size cues (e.g., a label identifying
ments, the vast majority of respondents provided accurate the container as a family size) rather than or in addition to
volume estimates, correcting errors that emerged in our other perceptual cues deserve further scrutiny. Consumers may
studies. It is unclear whether the effect depends on people consciously use such semantic cues, as contrasted with the
perceiving a difference in attention directed to two stimuli contamination process we propose for the attentional bias.
or whether a difference in attention intrudes upon judgments The effects of other cues consumers commonly encounter
without the perception of a disparity. when making volume judgments also should be pursued
There is no reason to assume that shape is the only at- (e.g., the price discounts associated with size differences
tention-attracting property of products that can lead to a size and unit price information), as well as other dependent mea-
bias. We have limited the scope of this article to product sures. Finally, identifying the boundary conditions of the
shape to provide a link with the only previous consumer contamination effect may provide insight into the underlying
research on volume judgments. Our results provide no sup- processes. Contamination due to differential attention may
port for the height heuristic for judging volume proposed decrease as the discrepancy between a pair of containers
by Raghubir and Krishna (1999). However, it is possible increases. For example, consumers should be able to judge
that the height heuristic is used when containers do not differ a 16 oz. container as greater than a 8 oz. container, even if
in their attention-attracting abilities. As to the salience hy- the smaller package is more novel. Hence, contamination
pothesis, the logic behind the salient linear dimension size may occur only when volume differences are not immedi-
heuristic (Krider et al. 2001) addresses the simpler area ately apparent. Yet, considering that numerical estimates of
judgments and does not seem to apply to the irregular three- sizes differed by as much as a fourth in our studies, the
dimensional shapes we examined. Moreover, Krider et al. potential magnitude of the volume bias suggests that it de-
have not incorporated into their theorizing how consumers serves more attention from researchers.
take into account dimensional variation (e.g., irregular or
nonuniform shapes that lack a uniform width or height). In [Dawn Iacobucci served as editor and Gita Johar served
sum, although previous research may explain how consum- as associate editor for this article.]
ers arrive at size judgments for simple, regular shapes, our
research suggests that other processes are involved in judg- REFERENCES
ments of more complex figures.
Banks, William P., Robin Mermelstein, and Howard Yu (1982),
Whereas our studies ruled out several potential effects of “Discriminating among Perceptual and Symbolic Stimuli,”
shape on volume, it is possible that those effects might be Memory and Cognition, 10 (3), 265–78.
observed in other circumstances. For example, study 4 in- Baylis, Gordon C. and Jon Driver (1993), “Visual Attention and
dicates that container attractiveness does not account for the Objects: Evidence for Hierarchical Coding of Location,” Jour-

This content downloaded from 137.195.150.201 on Sun, 4 Jan 2015 06:51:22 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
PACKAGE SHAPE AND VOLUME CONTENTS 401

nal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Per- by Novel Stimuli,” Journal of Experimental Psychology: Gen-
formance, 19 (3), 431–70. eral, 119 (4), 397–411.
Berlyne, Daniel E. (1960), Conflict, Arousal, and Curiosity. New Krider, Robert, Priya Raghubir, and Aradhna Krishna (2001), “Piz-
York: McGraw-Hill. zas: Pi or Square? Psychophysical Biases in Area Compari-
——— (1971), “Novelty and Attention: Controls for Retinal Ad- sons,” Marketing Science, 20 (4), 405–25.
aptation and for Stimulus-Response Specificity,” Psychon- Markman, Arthur B. (1999), Knowledge Representation, Mahwah,
omic Science, 25 (6), 349–51. NJ: Erlbaum.
Bloch, Peter (1995), “Seeking the Ideal Form: Product Design and Mussweiler, Thomas (2003), “Comparison Processes in Social Judg-
Consumer Response,” Journal of Marketing, 59 (July), 16–29. ment: Mechanisms and Consequences,” Psychological Review,
Celsi, Richard L. and Jerry C. Olson (1988), “The Role of In- 110 (July), 472–89.
volvement in Attention and Comprehension Processes,” Jour- Nisbett, Richard E. and Timothy Wilson (1977), “Telling More
nal of Consumer Research, 15 (September), 210–24. than We Can Know: Verbal Reports on Mental Processes,”
Cole, Catherine and Siva K. Balasubramanian (1993), “Age Dif- Psychological Review, 84 (3), 231–58.
ferences in Consumers’ Search for Information: Public Policy Pieters, Rik and Luk Warlop (1999), “Visual Attention during
Implications,” Journal of Consumer Research, 20 (June), Brand Choice: The Impact of Time Pressure and Task Mo-
157–69. tivation” International Journal of Research in Marketing, 16
Dickson, Peter and Alan G. Sawyer (1990), “The Price Knowledge (February), 1–16.
and Search of Supermarket Shoppers,” Journal of Marketing, Posner, Michael, Mary J. Nissen, and Raymond Klein (1976), “Vi-
54 (July), 42–53. sual Dominance: An Information-Processing Account of Its
Duncan, John (1984), “Selective Attention and the Organization
Origins,” Psychological Review, 83 (2), 157–71.
of Visual Information,” Journal of Experimental Psychology:
Raghubir, Priya and Aradhna Krishna (1999), “Vital Dimensions
General, 113 (4), 501–17.
in Volume Perception: Can the Eye Fool the Stomach?” Jour-
Finn, Adam (1988), “Print Ad Recognition Readership Scores: An
nal of Marketing Research, 36 (August), 313–26.
Information Processing Perspective,” Journal of Marketing
Research, 25 (May), 168–77. Renninger, K. Ann and Robert Wozniak (1985), “Effects of Interest
Folkes, Valerie S., Ingrid M. Martin, and Kamal Gupta (1993), on Attentional Shift, Recognition, and Recall in Young Chil-
“When to Say When: Effects of Supply on Usage,” Journal dren,” Developmental Psychology, 21 (4), 624–32.
of Consumer Research, 20 (December), 467–77. Russo, J. Edward and France Leclerc (1994), “An Eye-Fixation
Gronhaug, Kjell, Olav Kvitastein, and Gronmo (1991), “Factors Analysis of Choice Processes for Consumer Nondurables,”
Moderating Advertising Effectiveness as Reflected in 333 Journal of Consumer Research, 21 (September), 274–90.
Tested Advertisements,” Journal of Advertising Research, 31 Wedell, Douglas H. (1995), “Contrast Effects in Paired Compar-
(5), 42–50. isons: Evidence for Both Stimulus-Based and Response-
Higgins, E. Tory (1996), “Knowledge Activation: Accessibility, Based Processes,” Journal of Experimental Psychology: Hu-
Applicability, and Salience,” in Social Psychology: Handbook man Perception and Performance, 21 (5), 1158–73.
of Basic Principles, ed. E. Tory Higgins and Arie W. Krug- Wilson, Timothy and Nancy Brekke (1994), “Mental Contamination
lanski, New York: Guilford, 133–68. and Mental Correction: Unwanted Influences on Judgments and
Higgins, E. Tory, William S. Rholes, and Carl R. Jones (1977), Evaluation,” Psychological Bulletin, 116 (1), 117–42.
“Category Accessibility and Impression Formation,” Journal Wohlwill, Joachim F. (1975), “Children’s Responses to Meaningful
of Experimental Social Psychology, 13 (2), 141–54. Pictures Varying in Diversity: Exploration Time vs. Prefer-
Johnston, William A., Kevin J. Hawley, Steven H. Plewe, John ence,” Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 20 (3),
M. Elliott, and M. Jann DeWitt (1990), “Attention Capture 341–51.

This content downloaded from 137.195.150.201 on Sun, 4 Jan 2015 06:51:22 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

You might also like